Next Article in Journal
Is Wild Marine Biota Affected by Microplastics?
Next Article in Special Issue
Cats Did Not Change Their Problem-Solving Behaviours after Human Demonstrations
Previous Article in Journal
Phosphoproteomic Analysis of the Jejunum Tissue Response to Colostrum and Milk Feeding in Dairy Calves during the Passive Immunity Period
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Cat–Owner Relationship: Validation of the Italian C/DORS for Cat Owners and Correlation with the LAPS
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sociality of Cats toward Humans Can Be Influenced by Hormonal and Socio-Environmental Factors: Pilot Study

1
Laboratory of Human-Animal Interaction and Reciprocity, Azabu University, Sagamihara 252-5201, Japan
2
Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo 102-8471, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2023, 13(1), 146; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13010146
Submission received: 28 October 2022 / Revised: 29 December 2022 / Accepted: 29 December 2022 / Published: 30 December 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Cats Behaviors, Cognition and Human-Cat Interactions)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Cats are the most widely kept companion animal in the world. Various factors influence the sociality of cats. Here, we investigated whether the hormonal status of cats, and the age at which they began living with a human, affected their behaviors toward humans. The results showed that male cats that began living with a human earlier had more contact with humans. In addition, males with lower testosterone levels had more contact with humans. The results of this pilot study suggest that testosterone levels and the timing of when cats begin living with humans modulate affinity behavior of male cats toward humans.

Abstract

Individual differences in the sociality of cats are influenced by inherited and environmental factors. We recently revealed that hormones can make a difference in intraspecies social behavior. It remains unclear whether cat behavior toward humans is modulated by hormones. Therefore, we analyzed the relationship between cat behavior and their basal hormone concentrations after spending time together with human experimenters. In addition, we analyzed the relationship between cat behavior and the timing of when the individual cats began living with a human because the sociality of cats could be dependent on their developmental experiences. The results showed that male cats that began living with humans earlier had more contact with an experimenter. In addition, individual male cats with low testosterone levels were more likely to interact with an experimenter. These findings of this pilot study suggest that the sociality of male cats toward humans is affected by testosterone and the age at which they begin to live with humans.

1. Introduction

Animals live in relationships with other members of their species, as well as with different species. Cats, in particular, live in the same space with humans and have a close relationship through their interactions based on various senses [1]. Cats have many natural instincts that promote human interaction [1]. In vocal communication, for example, meowing has changed to communicate more effectively with humans. Although meowing is typically only used by mothers and kittens among cats [2], they also meow to attract human attention, even after they become adult animals [3]. Visual communication has also developed between cats and humans [1]. Cats follow human gaze in food selection tasks for referential information [4], and eyeblink synchronization, an indicator of smooth communication, has also been observed [5,6,7].
Although cats have evolved various behaviors toward humans, there is large individual variability. Although cats have evolved various behaviors toward humans, there is large individual variability owing to selection preference that favors appearance over sociality [8]. These differences in behavior are influenced by genetic and environmental factors. The question of whether genetic factors influence animal behavior has been analyzed by investigating behavioral differences between breeds, and supported by the responses to questionnaires [9,10]. In addition, coat color, which is genetically determined, has been studied in relationship to behavior in cats [11,12,13].
Furthermore, genetic variation can lead to differences in the development of the hormone system in cats and the amount of basal hormone secretion, which could influence cat behavior. Another factor that influences cat behavior toward humans, such as early childhood experiences [14,15,16,17,18,19,20]. Cats receiving 30–40 min of handling per day when they are kittens display a greater affinity towards humans [16]. In addition, the number of handlers that kittens are exposed to affects their affinity towards humans as adult cats [16,17]. We previously showed that intraspecific behavior in cats is modulated by hormones, and it is possible that their behavior toward humans is also modulated by hormones. Carlstead et al. showed that cat urinary cortisol is negatively correlated with hiding from human behaviors. In addition, concentrations of urinary cortisol [21] and oxytocin [22] have been shown to change depending on how humans care for cats. Finally, aggressive behavior decreases in cats after castration [23,24], which may be due to decreased testosterone concentration.
Although this was a pilot study due to the limited number of sample size, we aimed to clarify whether the behavior of cats toward humans is influenced by the age at which cats begin living with humans and/or by cat hormone levels. We recorded the age at which the cats began to live with humans, their behavior while spending time with human experimenters, and measured their hormone levels. We hypothesized that (1) the younger a cat begins to live with humans, the greater the affinity they would have with humans as adult cats, (2) individual cats with lower testosterone concentrations would avoid experimenters less than that of cats with higher testosterone concentrations, (3) individual cats with lower cortisol concentrations would avoid experimenters less than that of cats with higher cortisol concentrations, and (4) individual cats with lower oxytocin concentrations would exhibit lower group boundaries, interact with others, and develop more friendly interactions with experimenters.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Cat Subjects

Cats living in a cat café or shelter participated in this experiment, and all of the cats were rescued cats looking for adoptive homes. There were 4 male and 11 female cats aged 20.0 ± 12.6 months of age; all of the male cats were castrated. Detailed information about each cat is shown in Table S1.

2.2. Experimental Environment

For the café cats, the experiment was conducted in an 8.6 m2 room of the cat café. Because the shelter cats had been kept at Azabu University for 3 months for another experiment, we used a 10 m2 room at the Companion Dog Laboratory of Azabu University as the experimental room for the shelter cats. Within each room, there was a sofa for the human experimenters and a cat bed or cat tower for cats to choose where they wanted to rest. The environment of the two rooms was almost the same and both rooms were new places for the cats.

2.3. Behavioral Observation Flow-Chart

We investigated the behavior of the cats toward the experimenter over a period of 2 h. A flow-chart of the experiment is shown in Figure 1. First, the cat was introduced to the experimental room with the experimenter petting and talking to the cat for 10–20 min to get used to the room. Second, after the cat walked freely and explored the room, the experimenter left the room and the cat spent 5 min alone. Third, the experimenter re-entered the room and spent 2 h with the cat. During this time, the experimenter read a book or worked on a computer and was not allowed any interaction with the cat, such as petting, talking to, or looking at the cat. The experimenters included two males and two females who had met the subject cat at least five times, but less than ten times. One recording camera (HDR-AS50R, SONY) was installed on the ceiling of each room and a second camera was used in the case of blind areas.

2.4. Behavioral Analysis

The behavior of cats toward experimenters was recorded. The following behaviors were analyzed: rubbing against experimenter, being on the same sofa with experimenter, touching, meowing, sniffing, staying near the door, greeting score, and following score. Greeting and following were scored based on the Strange Situation Test [25]. The following score was calculated when the experimenter left the room once after the familiarization period, and the greeting score was calculated when the experimenter re-entered the room. Other behaviors during the reunion were recorded by continuous sampling. The definition of each behavior is listed in Table S2.

2.5. Hormone Assay

Cat urine samples were collected with cotton immediately after urination using a two-layer toilet between 7:00 a.m. and 10:00 a.m. A total of 101 urine samples (6.7 ± 3.6 samples/cat) were collected, and the experimenter was able to determine which cat excreted the urine in each case. Urine samples were collected during the month before and after the experiment. Basal hormone concentrations for each individual cat were obtained by averaging samples.

2.5.1. Cortisol Concentrations

Cortisol concentrations were measured using an enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA). We prepared the ELISA-plate using a mouse IgG-Fc fragment antibody (A90-131A, Bethyl Laboratories, Montgomery, TX, USA). The secondary antibody was diluted 500-fold and dispensed in 100 µL aliquots. After overnight incubation at 22–25 °C, the liquid in each well was discarded. Subsequently, a phosphate buffer containing 0.1% bovine serum albumin (BSA) was dispensed in 200 µL aliquots to all wells. After incubation for 30 min at 22–25 °C, the plates were stored at 4 °C in the dark until used in the assay.
Undiluted urine samples were dispensed into the wells of the ELISA-plate. An anti-cortisol antibody (ab1949; Abcam, Cambridge, UK) diluted 200,000-fold was used as a primary antibody. Cortisol-3-CMO-HRP (FKA403, COSMO, Dublin, Ireland) diluted 10,000-fold was used as a horseradish peroxidase (HRP). 15 µL standard samples, 15 µL urine samples and 100 µL primary and 100 µL secondary antibodies were dispensed into each well. After incubation for at least 6 h, the liquid in each well was discarded and the plate was washed four times using a plate washer. Thereafter, 150 μL of substrate buffer was dispensed into all wells. The reaction was stopped by adding 50 μL of 4 N H2SO4. Absorbance was measured at a wavelength of 450 nm using a microplate reader (Model 680XR, Bio-Rad Laboratories, Inc., Hercules, CA, USA).

2.5.2. Testosterone Concentrations

Testosterone concentrations were also measured using ELISA and the same plates as used for cortisol assay. After washing the plate three times, urine samples were dispensed into the wells. Urine samples with higher concentrations were diluted 2-fold with a phosphate buffer containing 0.1% BSA before dispensing. An anti-testosterone 3 CMO antibody (ab35878, Abcam) diluted 25,000-fold was used as a primary antibody, and a mouse IgG-Fc fragment antibody (A90-131A, Bethyl Laboratories) diluted 500-fold was used as secondary antibody. The HRP used in this process was Testosterone-3-CMO-HRP (FKA101, COSMO) that was diluted 10,000-fold. 25 µL standard samples and 25 µL urine samples were dispensed in each well. Then, 100 µL primary antibody, 100 µL secondary antibody and 100 µL HRP were dispensed. After incubation for at least 6 h, the liquid in the plate was discarded and the plates were washed four times using a plate washer. Thereafter, 150 μL of substrate buffer was dispensed into all wells. The reaction was stopped by adding 50 μL of 4 N H2SO4. Absorbance was measured at a wavelength of 450 nm using a microplate reader.

2.5.3. Oxytocin Concentrations

We used a commercially available oxytocin ELISA kit (ADI-901-153A-0001, ENZO, New York, NY, USA) for the assay. Urine samples diluted 50-fold with the assay buffer in the kit were dispensed into the wells of the ELISA-plates. 15 µL standard samples and 15 µL urine samples were dispensed in each well. A primary antibody and HRP were dispensed in 50 µL aliquots in each well. After incubation at 4 °C for 18–24 h, the substrate solution was dispensed in 200 µL aliquots in all wells. After incubation at 22–25 °C for 1 h, 50 μL of stop solution was added to each well. Absorbance was measured at a wavelength of 405 nm using a microplate reader.

2.5.4. Creatinine Concentration

A creatinine standard samples and the urine samples diluted 100-fold with distilled water were dispensed into a 96-well microplate (AS ONE Co., Ltd., Osaka, Japan) at 100 μL each, followed by 50 μL of 1 M NaOH and 50 μL of 1 g/dL trinitrophenol. The absorbance was measured at a wavelength of 490 nm using a microplate reader after the plate was left at room temperature (22–25 °C) for 20 min. The samples were used undiluted. To adjust for variations in urine concentration, all hormone concentrations were indexed against the creatinine concentration.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using R version 3.5.1. Linear regression model and correlation analysis were conducted to investigate the relationship between cat behavior toward humans and their age in months and hormone concentrations. Each social behavior towards humans was analyzed using a linear model (LM) and the lmer function in the lme4 package version 1.1.10. Testosterone concentration, cortisol concentration, oxytocin concentration, age at experiment, and age at which cats began to live with humans were entered as fixed factors. Correlation analysis was conducted separately for males and females since an analysis including interactions between sex and other factors could not be performed due to the lack of samples. In the correlation analysis, spearman’s rank correlation coefficient was used. The significance level was adjusted because the analysis was repeated five times with months and hormones for a behavior (significance was set at p < 0.01).

3. Results

3.1. Effects of Age and Hormones on Cats’ Behaviors toward Humans

None of the behaviors examined was affected by age or hormone concentrations (Table S3).

3.2. Correlation of Male Cats’ Behavior toward Humans with Age and Hormones

We then examined correlations between behavior, age, and hormone concentrations separately by sex. As a result, the age at which male cats began living with humans correlated negatively with sharing sofa (Figure 2A; rs = −1.000, p < 0.001), contact (Figure 2B; rs = −1.000, p < 0.001), rubbing (Figure 2C; rs = −1.000, p < 0.001), tail up (Figure 2D; rs = −1.000, p < 0.001), and following (Figure 2E; rs = −1.000, p < 0.001). In males, testosterone concentrations correlated negatively with sharing sofa (Figure 3A; rs = −1.000, p < 0.001), contact (Figure 3B; rs = −1.000, p < 0.001), rubbing (Figure 3C; rs = −1.000, p < 0.001). In addition, there was a positive correlation between the age at which the cats began living with humans and testosterone concentrations (Figure S1; rs = 1.000, p < 0.001). The results of the correlation between all behaviors, hormones, and age in months are shown in Tables S4 and S5.

3.3. Correlation of Female Cats’ Behavior toward Humans with Age and Hormones

In females, there was no correlation between behaviors towards humans and age or hormone concentrations.

4. Discussion

In this study, we investigated the relationship between the behavior of cats toward humans and their age and hormone concentrations to determine whether behavior toward humans is influenced by the age at which cats begin living with humans and/or their hormone levels. We found that the earlier a male cat began living with humans and the lower the testosterone level, the more likely the cats were to interact with experimenters. Only the results in males were consistent with our original hypothesis.
A decrease in testosterone leads to a decrease in aggression. The relationship has been reported in various species, including rodents, e.g., [26,27,28], primates, e.g., [29,30,31,32], and canidae, e.g., [33]. Lower testosterone levels may have made the cats more tolerant of other individual cats and increased their contact with humans. This association was only observed in males, which may have been influenced by sexual differentiation of the brain. For example, the brain of rats is sexually undifferentiated until about one week after birth. Androgen action during the perinatal period results in masculinization and defeminization of their brain [34,35,36,37,38]. Individuals that develop masculine brains during this period depend on testosterone for their behavior even after growth [39]. Additionally, in cats, the sexual differentiation of the brain during fetal life might have resulted in different outcomes for males and females. In addition, androgen precursors are also secreted from the adrenal glands in both sexes and converted to testosterone in the peripheral tissues [40]. Although all of the male cats were castrated, the testosterone produced by their adrenal glands could have affected their behavior. In addition, it has been reported that neutered males are friendlier to humans than neutered female [41]. The sex differences in hormone-behavior associations shown in this study may help us understand sex differences in behavior toward humans.
Individual male cats that began to live with a human later in life spent more time at the door, avoiding humans, which is consistent with previous studies. Early handling has shown positive effects on behavior in several studies of cat behavior towards humans [16,18,19,20]. The results of this study support these studies that the timing of first human contact during development is an important factor in a cat’s socialization with humans. There is another possibility that the cats beginning to live with humans early in their lives had lower testosterone baselines, which may have affected the cats’ behavior toward humans, since there was also a relationship between the age at which they began to live with humans and their testosterone levels.
Since cortisol and oxytocin are related to the behavior of cats toward other cats [42,43], we predicted that these hormones would also affect the behavior of cats toward humans. Our results did not support the prediction. Cortisol is also associated with anxiety and aggression in cats [43,44], but none of the individuals in this study showed excessively anxiety or aggression toward humans. In a previous study examining the relationship between fear responses to humans and cortisol, there was no relationship between them [45]. These may be the reasons why there was no correlation between cortisol and social behavior. In addition, oxytocin acts on a specific individual, especially the owner, and its effects depend on whom it interacts with. In dogs, oxytocin is increased by interaction with owners and familiar humans [46,47,48,49,50,51]. In this experiment, the experimenter met more than five times with each individual cat. It is possible that the effect of oxytocin is not present in these experimenters or in the entire category of humans. In addition, the first human encounter for cats is important for the subsequent relationship because it makes clear the other’s motivations and behavioral strategies. Because some differences in cat behavior occur based on the attributes of humans [3], investigating the behavior of cats toward a variety of humans, including strangers and familiar people, will lead to further understanding of cat social behaviors.
Most rarely a single hormone influences behavior; multiple hormones influence behavior integratedly. For example, testosterone secretion in the testis is affected by cortisol in an in vitro study of cats [52]. Other hormones, such as serotonin, also influence cat behavior [53], but the relationships between serotonin, oxytocin and testosterone are unknown.
In previous study, sociality with humans can be explained by paternal inheritance [54,55]. McCune examined how childhood experiences of interaction with humans influence later behavior, and whether these influences are affected by paternity [53]. As a result, individual cats that had a father who was friendly to humans, and experienced human contact from an early age, approached humans earlier and spent more time with humans than those who had not experienced human contact or had an unfriendly father [54]. These observations indicate that cat sociality toward humans is influenced by both environmental and inherited factors. Therefore, in the future, more profound insights into the sociality of cats with humans may be gained by examining the genetic background of individual cats and their behavior.
Although it has been shown that testosterone concentrations and the age at which they began living with humans may influence the behaviors of males toward humans, there are some limitations of this pilot study. One of the most concerns is the small sample size. In particular, only four males were sampled, which is not a sufficient number of data to account for all cats. Additional experiments need to be conducted to confirm reproducibility with more samples. Second, it is possible that the indicator of when cats began to live with humans is not appropriate. Although we used the age in months when the cats were rescued as an indicator of when they began interacting with humans, they might have been interacting with humans even before they were rescued. It will be necessary to conduct experiments on individual cats so that we can accurately determine when they start interacting with humans.

5. Conclusions

In this pilot study, there was no relationship between hormones or age and behaviors toward humans in male and female cats overall. In only male cats, the lower testosterone and the earlier they began to live with humans, the more interactions they had with humans. These results suggest one possibility that socialization of male cats with humans is influenced by both experience in kittenhood and testosterone, although reproducibility needs to be confirmed. Research on the relationship between cat behavior and endocrine status could be key to revealing the evolution of cat sociality.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani13010146/s1, Figure S1: Correlation between age at beginning to live with humans and testosterone levels; Table S1: Information of cats; Table S2: Analyzed behaviors and their definitions; Table S3: Relationship between behavior toward humans, the age in months, and hormones. SS: Sum of Squares, df: degree of freedom; Table S4 Correlation between behaviors towards humans, age in months, and hormones in female; Table S5: Correlation between behaviors towards humans, age in months, and hormones in male.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.K., S.T., T.K. and M.N.; data curation, H.K., H.T. and I.S.; formal analysis, H.K. and S.T.; funding acquisition, H.K., S.T., T.K. and M.N.; investigation, H.K., H.T. and I.S.; methodology, H.K., T.K. and M.N.; project administration, T.K. and M.N.; supervision, T.K. and M.N.; visualization, H.K.; writing—original draft, H.K., S.T., T.K. and M.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, and Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology of Japan, grant numbers 20J14760 (H.K.), 19J01485 and 22J40175 (S.T.), 18H02489 and 19K22823 (M.N.), and 19H00972 (T.K.).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The animal study protocol was approved by the Animal Experiment Committee of Azabu University (No. 180410-1 and No. 210325-12).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available within the article and in the Supplementary Materials.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Koyasu, H.; Kikusui, T.; Takagi, S.; Nagasawa, M. The Gaze Communications Between Dogs/Cats and Humans: Recent Research Review and Future Directions. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 613512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Bradshaw, J.; Cameron-Beaumont, C. The Signalling Repertoire of the Domestic Cat and Its Undomesticated Relatives. In The Domestic Cat: The Biology of Its Behaviour, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000; pp. 67–93. [Google Scholar]
  3. Mertens, C.; Turner, D.C. Experimental Analysis of Human-Cat Interactions during First Encounters. Anthrozoös 1988, 2, 83–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Pongrácz, P.; Szapu, J.S.; Faragó, T. Cats (Felis Silvestris Catus) Read Human Gaze for Referential Information. Intelligence 2019, 74, 43–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Humphrey, T.; Proops, L.; Forman, J.; Spooner, R.; McComb, K. The Role of Cat Eye Narrowing Movements in Cat-Human Communication. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 16503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Humphrey, T.; Stringer, F.; Proops, L.; McComb, K. Slow Blink Eye Closure in Shelter Cats Is Related to Quicker Adoption. Animals 2020, 10, 2256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Koyasu, H.; Goto, R.; Takagi, S.; Nagasawa, M.; Nakano, T.; Kikusui, T. Mutual Synchronization of Eyeblinks between Dogs/cats and Humans. Curr. Zool. 2021, 68, 229–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Finka, L.R. Conspecific and Human Sociality in the Domestic Cat: Consideration of Proximate Mechanisms, Human Selection and Implications for Cat Welfare. Animals 2022, 12, 298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hart, B.L. Selecting the Best Companion Animal: Breed and Gender Specific Behavioral Profiles. In Pet Connection: Its Influence on Our Health and Quality of Life; Center to Study Human-Animal Relationships and Environments; University of Minnesota: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 1984; pp. 180–193. [Google Scholar]
  10. Fogle, B. The Cats Mind-Understanding Your Cats Behaviour; Howell Books: Toronto, ON, Canada, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  11. Ledger, R.; O’Farrell, V. Factors Influencing the Reactions of Cats to Humans and Novel Objects. In Proceedings of the 30th International Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology, Guelph, ON, Canada, 14–17 August 1996; Colonel KL Campbell Centre for the Study of Animal Welfare: Guelph, ON, Canada, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  12. Stelow, E.A.; Bain, M.J.; Kass, P.H. The Relationship Between Coat Color and Aggressive Behaviors in the Domestic Cat. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2016, 19, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Wilhelmy, J.; Serpell, J.; Brown, D.; Siracusa, C. Behavioral Associations with Breed, Coat Type, and Eye Color in Single-Breed Cats. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 13, 80–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Wilson, M.; Warren, J.M.; Abbott, L. Infantile Stimulation, Activity, and Learning by Cats. Child Dev. 1965, 36, 843–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mendl, M.T. Effects of Litter Size and Sex of Young on Behavioural Development in Domestic Cats. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  16. Karsh, E.B. The Effects of Early and Late Handling on the Attachment of Cats to People. In The Pet Connection; Globe Press: New York, NY, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  17. Collard, R.R. Fear of Strangers and Play Behavior in Kittens with Varied Social Experience. Child Dev. 1967, 38, 877–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Karsh, E.B.; Turner, D.C. The Human-Cat Relationship. In The Domestic Cat: The Biology of Its Behaviour; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1988; pp. 159–177. [Google Scholar]
  19. Casey, R.A.; Bradshaw, J.W.S. The Effects of Additional Socialisation for Kittens in a Rescue Centre on Their Behaviour and Suitability as a Pet. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 114, 196–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Turner, D.C. A Review of over Three Decades of Research on Cat-Human and Human-Cat Interactions and Relationships. Behav. Process. 2017, 141, 297–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Carlstead, K.; Brown, J.L.; Strawn, W. Behavioral and Physiological Correlates of Stress in Laboratory Cats. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1993, 38, 143–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Nagasawa, T.; Ohta, M.; Uchiyama, H. The Urinary Hormonal State of Cats Associated with Social Interaction with Humans. Front. Vet. Sci. 2021, 8, 680843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Hart, B.L.; Barrett, R.E. Effects of Castration on Fighting, Roaming, and Urine Spraying in Adult Male Cats. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 1973, 163, 290–292. [Google Scholar]
  24. Tarttelin, M.F.; Hendriks, W.H.; Moughan, P.J. Relationship between Plasma Testosterone and Urinary Felinine in the Growing Kitten. Physiol. Behav. 1998, 65, 83–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Topál, J.; Gácsi, M.; Miklósi, Á.; Virányi, Z.; Kubinyi, E.; Csányi, V. Attachment to Humans: A Comparative Study on Hand-Reared Wolves and Differently Socialized Dog Puppies. Anim. Behav. 2005, 70, 1367–1375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Lumia, A.R.; Thorner, K.M.; McGinnis, M.Y. Effects of Chronically High Doses of the Anabolic Androgenic Steroid, Testosterone, on Intermale Aggression and Sexual Behavior in Male Rats. Physiol. Behav. 1994, 55, 331–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Motelica-Heino, I.; Edwards, D.A.; Roffi, J. Intermale Aggression in Mice: Does Hour of Castration after Birth Influence Adult Behavior? Physiol. Behav. 1993, 53, 1017–1019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Melloni, R.H., Jr.; Connor, D.F.; Hang, P.T.; Harrison, R.J.; Ferris, C.F. Anabolic-Androgenic Steroid Exposure during Adolescence and Aggressive Behavior in Golden Hamsters. Physiol. Behav. 1997, 61, 359–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Higley, J.D.; Mehlman, P.T.; Higley, S.B.; Fernald, B.; Vickers, J.; Lindell, S.G.; Taub, D.M.; Suomi, S.J.; Linnoila, M. Excessive Mortality in Young Free-Ranging Male Nonhuman Primates with Low Cerebrospinal Fluid 5-Hydroxyindoleacetic Acid Concentrations. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1996, 53, 537–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Mehlman, P.T.; Higley, J.D.; Fernald, B.J.; Sallee, F.R.; Suomi, S.J.; Linnoila, M. CSF 5-HIAA, Testosterone, and Sociosexual Behaviors in Free-Ranging Male Rhesus Macaques in the Mating Season. Psychiatry Res. 1997, 72, 89–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Kalin, N.H. Primate Models to Understand Human Aggression. J. Clin. Psychiatry 1999, 60 (Suppl. 15), 29–32. [Google Scholar]
  32. Rejeski, W.J.; Brubaker, P.H.; Herb, R.A.; Kaplan, J.R.; Koritnik, D. Anabolic Steroids and Aggressive Behavior in Cynomolgus Monkeys. J. Behav. Med. 1988, 11, 95–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Creel, S.; Creel, N.M.; Mills, M.G.L.; Monfort, S.L. Rank and Reproduction in Cooperatively Breeding African Wild Dogs: Behavioral and Endocrine Correlates. Behav. Ecol. 1997, 8, 298–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Meaney, M.J.; Stewart, J.; Poulin, P.; McEwen, B.S. Sexual Differentiation of Social Play in Rat Pups Is Mediated by the Neonatal Androgen-Receptor System. Neuroendocrinology 1983, 37, 85–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Isgor, C.; Sengelaub, D.R. Effects of Neonatal Gonadal Steroids on Adult CA3 Pyramidal Neuron Dendritic Morphology and Spatial Memory in Rats. J. Neurobiol. 2003, 55, 179–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Isgor, C.; Sengelaub, D.R. Prenatal Gonadal Steroids Affect Adult Spatial Behavior, CA1 and CA3 Pyramidal Cell Morphology in Rats. Horm. Behav. 1998, 34, 183–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Joseph, R.; Hess, S.; Birecree, E. Effects of Hormone Manipulations and Exploration on Sex Differences in Maze Learning. Behav. Biol. 1978, 24, 364–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Zuloaga, D.G.; Puts, D.A.; Jordan, C.L.; Breedlove, S.M. The Role of Androgen Receptors in the Masculinization of Brain and Behavior: What We’ve Learned from the Testicular Feminization Mutation. Horm. Behav. 2008, 53, 613–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Kikusui, T. The Role of Sex Spectrum Differences in Reproductive Strategies and the Endocrine Mechanisms Underlying It. In Spectrum of Sex: The Molecular Bases that Induce Various Sexual Phenotypes; Tanaka, M., Tachibana, M., Eds.; Springer Nature: Singapore, 2022; pp. 165–179. ISBN 9789811953590. [Google Scholar]
  40. Shea, J.L.; Wong, P.-Y.; Chen, Y. Free Testosterone: Clinical Utility and Important Analytical Aspects of Measurement. Adv. Clin. Chem. 2014, 63, 59–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Hart, B.L.; Hart, L.A. Your Ideal Cat: Insights into Breed and Gender Differences in Cat Behavior; Purdue University Press: Lafayette, IN, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  42. Koyasu, H.; Takahashi, H.; Yoneda, M.; Naba, S.; Sakawa, N.; Sasao, I.; Nagasawa, M.; Kikusui, T. Correlations between Behavior and Hormone Concentrations or Gut Microbiome Imply That Domestic Cats (Felis Silvestris Catus) Living in a Group Are Not like “groupmates”. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0269589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Finkler, H.; Terkel, J. Cortisol Levels and Aggression in Neutered and Intact Free-Roaming Female Cats Living in Urban Social Groups. Physiol. Behav. 2010, 99, 343–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Gourkow, N.; LaVoy, A.; Dean, G.A.; Phillips, C.J.C. Associations of Behaviour with Secretory Immunoglobulin A and Cortisol in Domestic Cats during Their First Week in an Animal Shelter. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 150, 55–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Siegford, J.M.; Walshaw, S.O.; Brunner, P.; Zanella, A.J. Validation of a Temperament Test for Domestic Cats. Anthrozoös 2003, 16, 332–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Odendaal, J.S.; Meintjes, R.A. Neurophysiological Correlates of Affiliative Behaviour between Humans and Dogs. Vet. J. 2003, 165, 296–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Rehn, T.; Handlin, L.; Uvnäs-Moberg, K.; Keeling, L.J. Dogs’ Endocrine and Behavioural Responses at Reunion Are Affected by How the Human Initiates Contact. Physiol. Behav. 2014, 124, 45–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Hritcu, L.D.; Horhogea, C.; Ciobica, A.; Spataru, M.C.; Spataru, C.; Kis, A. Conceptual Replication of Canine Serum Oxytocin Increase Following a Positive Dog-Human Interaction. Rev. Chim. 2019, 70, 1579–1581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. MacLean, E.L.; Gesquiere, L.R.; Gee, N.R.; Levy, K.; Martin, W.L.; Carter, C.S. Effects of Affiliative Human–Animal Interaction on Dog Salivary and Plasma Oxytocin and Vasopressin. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mitsui, S.; Yamamoto, M.; Nagasawa, M.; Mogi, K.; Kikusui, T.; Ohtani, N.; Ohta, M. Urinary Oxytocin as a Noninvasive Biomarker of Positive Emotion in Dogs. Horm. Behav. 2011, 60, 239–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Handlin, L.; Hydbring-Sandberg, E.; Nilsson, A.; Ejdebäck, M.; Jansson, A.; Uvnäs-Moberg, K. Short-Term Interaction between Dogs and Their Owners: Effects on Oxytocin, Cortisol, Insulin and Heart Rate—An Exploratory Study. Anthrozoös 2011, 24, 301–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Genaro, G.; Franci, C.R. Cortisol Influence on Testicular Testosterone Secretion in Domestic Cat: An in Vitro Study. Pesqui. Vet. Bras. 2010, 30, 887–890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Cools, A.R. Serotonin: A Behaviorally Active Compound in the Caudate Nucleus of Cats. Isr. J. Med. Sci. 1973, 9, 5–16. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  54. McCune, S. The Impact of Paternity and Early Socialisation on the Development of Cats’ Behaviour to People and Novel Objects. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1995, 45, 109–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Reisner, I.R.; Houpt, K.A.; Erb, H.N.; Quimby, F.W. Friendliness to Humans and Defensive Aggression in Cats: The Influence of Handling and Paternity. Physiol. Behav. 1994, 55, 1119–1124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Experimental procedure.
Figure 1. Experimental procedure.
Animals 13 00146 g001
Figure 2. Correlation between behavior towards experimenters and age at keeping with humans (months). (AE) indicate the relationship between age at beginning to live with humans and the following behaviors, (A) Sharing sofa, (B) Contact, (C) Rubbing, (D) Tail up, (E) Following.
Figure 2. Correlation between behavior towards experimenters and age at keeping with humans (months). (AE) indicate the relationship between age at beginning to live with humans and the following behaviors, (A) Sharing sofa, (B) Contact, (C) Rubbing, (D) Tail up, (E) Following.
Animals 13 00146 g002
Figure 3. Correlation between behavior towards experimenters and testosterone concentrations. (AC) indicate the relationship between testosterone concentration and the following behaviors, (A) Sharing sofa, (B) Contact, (C) Rubbing.
Figure 3. Correlation between behavior towards experimenters and testosterone concentrations. (AC) indicate the relationship between testosterone concentration and the following behaviors, (A) Sharing sofa, (B) Contact, (C) Rubbing.
Animals 13 00146 g003
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Koyasu, H.; Takahashi, H.; Sasao, I.; Takagi, S.; Nagasawa, M.; Kikusui, T. Sociality of Cats toward Humans Can Be Influenced by Hormonal and Socio-Environmental Factors: Pilot Study. Animals 2023, 13, 146. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13010146

AMA Style

Koyasu H, Takahashi H, Sasao I, Takagi S, Nagasawa M, Kikusui T. Sociality of Cats toward Humans Can Be Influenced by Hormonal and Socio-Environmental Factors: Pilot Study. Animals. 2023; 13(1):146. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13010146

Chicago/Turabian Style

Koyasu, Hikari, Hironobu Takahashi, Ikuto Sasao, Saho Takagi, Miho Nagasawa, and Takefumi Kikusui. 2023. "Sociality of Cats toward Humans Can Be Influenced by Hormonal and Socio-Environmental Factors: Pilot Study" Animals 13, no. 1: 146. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13010146

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop