Next Article in Journal
Effects of Dietary Supplementation with Honeybee Pollen and Its Supercritical Fluid Extract on Immune Response and Fillet’s Quality of Farmed Gilthead Seabream (Sparus aurata)
Next Article in Special Issue
Increasing Environmental Complexity by Providing Different Types of Litter and Perches during Early Rearing Boosts Coping Abilities in Domestic Fowl Chicks
Previous Article in Journal
Correction: De Francesco et al. In Vitro Characterization of Canine Microfragmented Adipose Tissue Non-Enzymatically Extracted from the Thigh and Lumbar Regions. Animals 2021, 11, 3231
Previous Article in Special Issue
Similarity in Temporal Movement Patterns in Laying Hens Increases with Time and Social Association
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Effect of Hard Pecking Enrichment during Rear on Feather Cover, Feather Pecking Behaviour and Beak Length in Beak-Trimmed and Intact-Beak Laying Hen Pullets

by
Paula Elizabeth Baker
1,*,
Christine Janet Nicol
2 and
Claire Alexandra Weeks
3
1
Laying Hen Welfare Forum, 22 City Road, London EC1Y 2AJ, UK
2
Royal Veterinary College, Hawkshead Lane, Brookmans Park, Hatfield AL9 7TA, UK
3
Bristol Veterinary School, University of Bristol, Langford House, Bristol BS40 5DU, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2022, 12(6), 674; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12060674
Submission received: 27 January 2022 / Revised: 22 February 2022 / Accepted: 23 February 2022 / Published: 8 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Behaviour and Welfare of Laying Hens)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Laying hens may peck each other, painfully pulling out feathers and damaging flesh. There are many potential reasons and no obvious solutions, so the sharp tip of the beak is often carefully removed after hatching to limit the damage inflicted. Scientist have found it is useful to provide hens with lots of other things to forage in and peck at. Our study compared the effect of providing pans containing an abrasive material designed to attract pecking behaviour to eight treatment flocks of young hens, with eight flocks acting as controls. Each group had four beak-trimmed and four intact-beak flocks paired by farm. The pans significantly reduced the side and top beak lengths in samples of birds measured at 6–7 weeks and 10–11 weeks of age, and beak growth appeared to be linear. Thus, provision of abrasive material in pans effectively blunted beaks of younger birds. By the end of rear (14–15 weeks), birds seemed to lose interest in pecking at the pans, so there was no difference between treatment and control flocks, indicating scope for redesign to retain their interest.

Abstract

Injurious pecking, commonly controlled by beak trimming (BT), is a widespread issue in laying hens associated with thwarted foraging. This controlled study compared the effect in intact and beak-trimmed pullets of providing pecking pans to eight treatment flocks from six weeks of age. Flocks (mean size 6843) comprised eight British Blacktail, six Lohmann Brown and two Bovans Brown. All young birds (6–7 weeks) pecked more frequently at the pecking pans (mean 40.4) than older pullets (mean 26.0, 23.3 pecks/bird/min at 10–11 weeks and 14–15 weeks, respectively) (p < 0.005). There was no effect on feather pecking or plumage cover. Mean side-beak length and mean top-beak lengths were shorter in treatment flocks at 6–7 weeks and 10–11 weeks (p < 0.001). Intact-beak treatment flocks had shorter mean side-beak length at 10–11 weeks (p < 0.001) and at 14–15 weeks (p < 0.05) and mean top-beak length at 6–7 weeks (p < 0.05) and at 10–11 weeks (p < 0.05). BT treatment flocks had shorter side-beak and top-beak lengths at 6–7 weeks and at 10–11 weeks (p < 0.001). Beak lengths showed linear growth, with individual bird variation indicating a potential for genetic selection. The study demonstrated that abrasive material can reduce beak length in pullets.

1. Introduction

Injurious pecking (IP) is an umbrella term for inter-bird pecking behaviours including severe feather pecking (SFP), vent pecking (VP), cannibalistic pecking (CP) and toe pecking (TP) [1]. The forceful pulling of feathers of conspecifics that occurs during SFP not only causes pain and distress [2] but can lead to substantial feather loss and be associated with cannibalistic behaviour, vent pecking and a decline in egg production [1,2] as well as disease and high mortality [2,3]. Birds with poor feather cover due to IP may consume up to 40% more feed [4]. The risk of IP is multifactorial, with the environment, nutrition and genetics all having a role [5,6], making IP hard to manage in commercial flocks [5,7].
A further form of inter-bird pecking is gentle feather pecking (GFP), which comprises repeated pecks at the edges of feathers—often aimed at the tip of the tail and sometimes other areas. GFP results in minor damage of little welfare concern [5]. The occurrence of GFP in pullets is not a good predictor for subsequent SFP or other forms of IP in older birds, but there can be a positive association between GFP and SFP in birds of the same age [8]. Both GFP and SFP behaviours can be witnessed during the rearing period. GFP has been reported in week old chicks [9] and can peak around three to four weeks of age [10]. The first signs of SFP in young pullets have been observed as pecking around the preen gland and oily feathers near the tail [11]. SFP has been observed in flocks at 10 weeks of age [12].
Beak trimming (BT) remains one of the most effective management tools to reduce the impact of IP [1,13,14], as blunted beaks cause less damage and pain to the recipient bird. Plumage damage is also reduced, as beak trimmed birds tend to perform less destructive feather pecking [15,16]. Issues related to BT remain controversial worldwide and still provoke a great deal of debate from an animal welfare perspective [1,17].
There are two methods: the hot blade technique (HB), still widely used in some countries, and infra-red beak trimming (IRBT), which has replaced HB trimming of day-old chicks in the UK and in Europe [18,19].
Hot-blade trimming involves the removal of up to one third of the maxilla (upper) and (lower) mandible [1,17], which can include the horny skin structure of keratinized epidermis [11,18]. This technique can result in short and long-term pain, causing tissue damage, nerve injury and the development of neuromas and pain-related behaviours when trimmed at five weeks of age [20,21], as well as reduced feed intake and a decline in growth rate [22].
IRBT is recognised as less damaging with fewer adverse consequences, suggesting a low risk of long-term pain [23]. However, IRBT is associated with short-term reductions in feeding and ground pecking behaviour and reduced weight gain observed in the first few days [21,24,25]. Although these effects resolve by the time birds are two to four weeks of age [26,27], there is some adverse impact of IRBT on birds. Thus, effective management of IP behaviour would obviate the need to perform BT.
There is increasing awareness of the impact of the environment and management during rear on the development of IP in the subsequent laying period [2,3]. Young birds kept in barren environments with limited enrichment and foraging opportunities may exhibit fear, reduced cognitive ability and be attracted to peck at feathers [28,29,30]. In contrast, the provision of high-quality litter and other enrichments from an early age can direct foraging and exploratory behaviour towards harmless substrates and has been shown to reduce subsequent overall feather pecking [2], SFP [31] and VP [32] on the laying farm. Despite the influence of the rearing period, adult hen behaviour remains responsive to the environment, and the continued provision of appropriate foraging opportunities for adult birds is important [33]. A key approach to reducing the risk of IP in adult birds is to provide appropriate substrates with the aim of satisfying foraging motivation [34]. Several other management strategies, for example those that address lighting, diet or use of the range, can also be effective. In recognition of this complexity, the effectiveness of management packages that combine a range of strategies in a holistic attempt to prevent or reduce injurious pecking (IP) has been assessed [31,35]. One study of commercial free-range laying hens provided 53 treatment flocks with a bespoke management package and compared their behaviour and performance with 47 control flocks where no changes were suggested [31]. IP and plumage damage were notably lower within the treatment flocks and less SFP was observed. All types of feather pecking were reduced by increasing the number of management strategies in place [31]. A further study found that the concurrent provision of a variety of objects designed to attract pecking behaviour and the provision of outside shelters to encourage range use resulted in a reduction in both GFP and SFP [35].
A complementary approach with a different focus is to try to maintain birds with “naturally” blunted beaks, which may be achieved by providing hard materials as pecking substrates. Commercially available durable pecking blocks and stones have two potential functions, first, by reducing or blunting beaks to avoid beak trimming and, second, by providing a type of enrichment. Some studies have examined the effects of providing hard materials as part of a package of changes. For example, Pettersson et al. [35] included pecking pans as part of a resource package on 14 laying farms and considered them well-used in most flocks. As stated earlier, the package as a whole was effective in reducing feather pecking behaviour, but the separate contribution of the pecking pans could not be evaluated. In addition, due to faecal contamination and perching activity, it was found that pecking bouts significantly decreased over time [35]. When foraging and pecking enrichments such as pecking stones, pecking blocks and lucerne bales were combined with a reduction in stocking density (SD) in laying hen chicks from 22.9 birds/m2 to 18.1 birds/m2, a significant reduction in GFP and SFP was seen. On average, the highest rate of enrichment pecking per bird per 3 min was observed for lucerne bales, followed by pecking stones and pecking blocks, but, again, the separate effect of the pecking stones could not be evaluated [16,32].
Whilst the studies mentioned above have included hard pecking materials, they have not examined whether these materials are effective in blunting birds’ beaks via abrasion. Abrasion could be an alternative to BT in laying hens, but this has only been examined in a handful of studies. Fiks-van Niekerk and Elson [36] placed an abrasive material inside a feeding trough at rear (from 6 weeks) and chain feeder troughs at lay (from 18 weeks). Birds’ beaks were measured at four-week intervals with vernier callipers for length and curvature by tape measure. The continuous exposure to the abrasive materials throughout rear and lay was effective and resulted in consistent shortening of the beaks by an average of 1 mm. However, the abrasive material was less effective when provided only during the rearing period, and it was difficult to standardise the measurement of the beak tip (hook) on immature birds. Iqbal and others [1] likewise explored the effect of pecking stones on beak length, plumage condition, toe length, production and mortality from 16 weeks until 66 weeks of age. Morrissey et al. [30] investigated the effect of beak blunting boards as part of a package of other enrichments but found no effect on the beak length or beak sharpness on the upper maxilla. Further research [37] in a replicated smaller study with beak blunting boards and cuttlebone found that at 29, 35 and 40 weeks of age, hens pecked at a cuttlebone more than beak blunting boards, performed less feather pecking behaviour and had less plumage damage. In addition, there was a suggestion that cuttlebone might reduce beak length (p = 0.070), but there were no effects on other beak measures and no treatment effects of enrichment or location on any measure. However, pecks at the extra enrichment generally decreased over time. In other studies, pecking stones have been more successful as beak blunting tools; for example, in turkeys after six months of access to pecking stones, the beaks of about 80% of treatment birds resembled those of beak trimmed conspecifics [38].
In recent years, there has been a shift in the technique used to measure hen beak characteristics. Previous work in other avian species has revealed diversity in beak length, depth and strength in Darwin’s finches [39] using landmark-based geometric morphometrics. This methodology has more recently also been used for chickens and turkeys [30,37,40,41]. Although precise, with reference points identified on the upper beak, lower beak, overhang and angle of the beak tip, the method is very time consuming and was not feasible for the current project conducted on a commercial farm. Thus, studies have generally focused on enrichment preference in older laying hens and have been undertaken in small group in-house experiments [30,37], with few studies conducted on young pullets at different age ranges. The potentially differential effects of hard pecking materials on intact-beak and beak-trimmed birds in a commercial setting have not been considered. The aim of our current study was therefore to conduct a controlled investigation in commercial flocks of the effects of a hard pecking material on the behaviour and beak characteristics of beak-trimmed and intact-beak laying hen genotypes during rear (from 6 to 15 weeks of age).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Animals and Housing

Sixteen UK commercial rearing flocks were studied between September 2015 and December 2016. Half of the 16 flocks were infra-red beak trimmed (IRBT) at day old at the hatchery and the other half were intact-beak flocks (i.e., not trimmed). Strains were British Blacktail (BBT) (n = 8), Lohmann Brown (LB) (n = 6) and Bovans Brown (BB) (n = 2). Flock size ranged from 3300 to 11,000 and the average flock size was 6843 (see Table 1). All flocks were reared in loose housing systems with wood shavings or shredded paper as the floor substrate. Most of the flocks had A-frame perches, except for two, which had a raised slatted system (flocks 5 and 6). All flocks had chain feeders. Four flocks were reared on organic mash feed and the others were reared on standard commercial mash. No flocks had access to the outside. Welfare and behaviour assessments took place over three visits: 6–7 weeks of age, 10–11 weeks and 14–15 weeks of age. Eight treatment flocks were supplied with pecking pans (Vencomatic UK) and eight were paired control flocks reared on the same farm Flocks were allocated to pecking pan or no pecking pan treatment groups based on matching strains and beak status (and reared concurrently as sister flocks on each farm). The experimental design consisted of four intact treatment, four intact control, four trimmed treatment and four trimmed control flocks.
Pecking pans (Figure 1) comprised a green plastic pan feeder with a detachable grey base. The pan contained a hard block composed principally of sand, cement and oyster shell. Each treatment flock was given one pecking pan per 500 birds, and these evenly distributed on the litter area throughout the rearing house. Pecking pans were placed the day before the first observations commenced, at around 6–7 weeks, to allow the birds to acclimatise to them, and they remained for the rest of the rearing period.

2.2. Behavioural and Welfare Assessments

2.2.1. Pecking Pan Interaction

Focal sampling techniques were used to record usage of the pecking pan in treatment flocks. To encourage the birds to make an initial approach to the pans, two handfuls of organic mixed corn were sprinkled into each pan the afternoon before data collection took place on the first visit at 6–7 weeks of age. Observations were made on randomly selected pecking pans and the quadrat around the pan was an estimated 1 m2 area. A 2-min habitation period allowed the birds to become accustomed to the presence of the observer, who was sited unobtrusively some distance away. The number of pecks to the pecking pan was observed for two individual birds, 2 min per bird, 4 min in total. If either of these birds discontinued pecking and left the area, the observation ended, and its time was recorded.

2.2.2. Use of Pecking Pans

An initial aim was to record use of the pecking pans at each age of 6–7, 10–11 and 14–15 weeks and to classify usage as minimum, medium and maximum wear, with photographs taken to aid evaluation. However, it proved difficult to assess wear and compare or weigh the pecking substrate throughout the three visits. This was due to the substrate not being standardised in each pecking pan and because, from time to time, the pecking pan itself would contain bird excrement. Thus, this part of the trial was abandoned, and data not used.

2.2.3. Feather Pecking Behaviour

Observations of feather pecking behaviour were recorded at each age (6–7, 10–11 and 14–15 weeks) between the hours of 08.30 a.m. and 12.00 noon and generally matched for time of day across all visits. Five-minute observations of behaviour were recorded in 2 m2 quadrats in nine randomly selected areas of the house (total of 45 min) to give a representative sample. A 2-min habituation period allowing the birds to become accustomed to the presence of the observer was used and observations were taken at a 1 m distance to minimise bird disturbance. The number of birds in the observation area was counted before and after the observation to calculate the average number present during each observation period. Observations included all bouts of gentle feather pecking (GFP), where a bout was defined as continuous pecking until another behaviour was performed or the behaviour stopped for a 5 s gap [42]. All individual instances of severe injurious pecking (SFP), vent pecking (VP), cannibalistic pecking (CB) and aggressive pecking (AP) were also recorded (see Table 2 for definitions).

2.3. Feather Scoring and Plumage Condition Assessment

Plumage condition was assessed at the end of the rearing period (bird age at 14/15 weeks). This time point was selected as young chicks and pullets go through many moults throughout rear. Twelve birds per area (108 birds in total) were visually scored during the morning behaviour observations in nine areas of the house on the litter area. Birds were not handled in order to minimise flock disturbance. Pullets were randomly selected by counting three birds to the right of the first bird focused upon. Feather cover was assessed and scored for five areas of the body—neck, back, tail, rump and wings—using a five-point scale and photographs for reference, a method similar to Bright and others (2006) (0 = No damage to 4 = Severe damage to skin and very large, injured areas (>5 cm2 traumatized)). Birds that were selected for beak measurements (45 in total) were further inspected for plumage condition using the same five-point scale (Table 3).

2.4. Measurement of Beak Length

A sample of the birds (30–45 per visit at 6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks and 14–15 weeks) was selected from different areas of the rearing house for beak measurement. Birds were randomly selected using the method described above. Birds were caught in the afternoon so the flock behaviour during observations would not be influenced by catching and handling of the sampled birds. Birds were gently restrained by one person and wrapped in a disposable cloth to restrain their wings so they could be easily handled. The top of the right leg of each bird was marked with a black dot with a permanent marker pen to ensure that these birds would not be recaptured throughout the study. Beak length was measured with vernier callipers using methodology slightly adjusted from that of Van de Weerd (2005) [36] (see Table 4 and Figure 2 and Figure 3). During the study, it became apparent that it was difficult to take accurate hook measurements and to standardise the technique, so these data were not used. Each measurement was repeated three times in succession to provide a mean length with an estimated accuracy of ±0.5 mm.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

The data collected from the study were evaluated for significant relationships using statistical tests in IBM SPSS version 26. General linear modelling (GLM) was used, with boot-strapping procedures where appropriate to account for variable data distributions, to examine effects of treatment (pecking pans provided or not), beak status (intact or trimmed) and bird age at (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks and 14–15 weeks), treated as a repeated measure. A flock was taken to be the independent unit for analysis for measures of bird pecking behaviour towards pans or other birds and for plumage condition, as these measures would be highly influenced by the behaviour of other birds in each flock. Pecking activity directed towards pecking pans was analysed for treatment flocks with the model including beak status and bird age. Bird pecking behaviour at the pans was corrected for the length of time for each focal bird and is presented as a pecking rate (pecks/bird/min).
GLM repeated measures analyses were used for analysis of feather pecking (SFP and GFP) in both treatment and control flocks, with models including beak status and bird age. For each flock, a mean value of the nine observations of feather pecking behaviour was calculated for each visit: visit 1 at 6–7 weeks, visit 2 at 10–11 weeks and visit 3 at 14–15 weeks. Plumage condition on non-handled birds was assessed only at the end of the rearing period, and so a simple model examined the effect of treatment and beak status. Comparable simple statistical tests were performed on the 45 birds/flock which were plumage scored during handling for beak measurements.
Analysis of bird beak characteristics was performed at the individual bird level using data for side length and top of beak for each of the 45 birds sampled at each age (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks and 14–15 weeks). Normality tests showed that none of the data were normally distributed even after transformation, therefore, non-parametric statistical analyses were used. Mann–Whitney U tests were used to compare side-beak length and top-beak length between intact and IRBT birds across age (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks and 14–15 weeks), and patterns of beak growth were plotted for each breed. Further Mann–Whitney U tests were conducted to explore the effect of pecking pan provision on side-beak and top-beak length. A Bonferroni adjustment was used to reduce the possibility of incorrectly rejecting the null hypothesis (Type 1 error) because independent tests were being performed simultaneously on a single dataset (15 tests 0.05/15 = 0.003).

3. Results

3.1. The Effects of Age on Pecking Pan Use in Intact and Beak Trimmed Flocks

There was a reduction in pecking pan activity (peck rate) with age for the eight flocks with access to the pecking pans, with young birds at 6–7 weeks pecking more often (mean 40.4) compared to 10–11 weeks (mean 26.0) and 14–15 weeks (mean 23.32), pecks/bird/min: F = 25.51; df = 1,6; p = 0.003 (Figure 4). However, there was no significant difference between intact or beak trimmed flocks (F = 1.44; df = 1,6; p = 0.27).

3.2. The Effect of Pecking Pan Presence on Feather Pecking Behaviour

No significant treatment differences in scan observations of SFP were found in birds with or without access to the pecking pans, nor were there beak status effects. For SFP, overall mean pecks/5 min scan were 0.241 (0.016–0.465) with the provision of pans versus a mean of 0.526 (0.285–0.767) with no pans (F = 3.57; df = 1,12; p = 0.08). SFP appeared to increase linearly with bird age (F = 7.77; df = 1,12; p = 0.016). There were no age or treatment effects on GFP behaviour. The frequency of GFP was 2.58 (se 0.52) for beak trimmed birds and 1.12 (se 0.48) for intact beak birds (F = 4.21; df = 1,12; p = 0.06).

3.3. The Effect of Pecking Pan Presence in Intact and Beak Trimmed Flocks on Plumage Condition

Visual scoring of plumage condition at 14–15 weeks of age showed no significant differences across all body regions between intact flocks and BT flocks with and without a pecking pan. There was little plumage damage, and none seen in the wings or rump region (Table 5).
Analysis of the flock averages from detailed feather scoring of handled birds confirmed no significant effect of beak status or pecking pan provision on plumage in any of the body regions (Table 6). Again, there was no plumage damage to the rump region.

3.4. The Effects of Beak Status and Age on Beak Length

Unsurprisingly, intact beaks were significantly longer than trimmed beaks across all ages. This was seen in both mean side-beak length and mean top-beak length p < 0.001) (Table 7 and Table 8). The pattern of beak length increases with age, irrespective of pecking pan provision, and can be seen for each strain in (Figure 5 and Figure 6). A linear rate of increase in mean side-beak length and mean top-beak length was seen in BBT and LB birds, but a growth curve provided a better description of the rate of increase for IRBT BB birds (Figure 5 and Figure 6). There were no intact beak BB flocks in the study.

3.5. The Effect of Pecking Pan Presence on Beak Length

When individual birds from all treatment flocks were considered together, irrespective of beak status, mean side-beak length and mean top-beak length were significantly shorter with a pecking pan present at 6–7 weeks and 10–11 weeks (Table 9 and Table 10). No significant differences were found for beak measurements at 14–15 weeks (Table 9 and Table 10).
Intact and IRBT birds were next considered separately. For intact birds, there was a significant shortening effect of pecking pans on mean side-beak length at 10–11 weeks (U = 10,859; p < 0.001) and at 14–15 weeks (U = 12,301; p = 0.006) and on mean top-beak length at 6–7 weeks (U = 16,414; p = 0.026) and at 10–11 weeks (U = 12,202; p = 0.004). For IRBT birds, there was a significant shortening effect of pecking pans on mean side-beak length at 6–7 weeks (U = 6378; p < 0.001) and at 10–11 weeks (U = 10,819; p < 0.001), with similar effects on mean top-beak length at 6–7 weeks (U = 6800; p < 0.001) and 10–11 weeks (U = 10,840; p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

Our study, in common with others [35,37], found that the use of pecking material declined with age. It could be that the pecking pans needed to be placed at an earlier age, at a day old [29], as enrichment can retain interest up until at least 17 weeks of age.
However, we considered that placing the pecking pans early in rear could be detrimental, as the beaks of chicks are soft, and we had concerns that the hardness of the substrate inside the pecking pan could potentially damage the chicks’ beaks. The same pecking pan used in our study was also used in laying flocks [35]. These authors recorded that the pans were used by the birds for the first 15 weeks of lay, but the mean bout duration of pecking at the substrate of 4 s at 25 and 40 weeks decreased over time. Informal observations in our study noted that as the birds matured, the pecking pan was frequently used as a perch rather than a pecking enrichment. The loss of interest in both studies could also be attributed to other factors, such as conspecifics blocking access to the pecking material within the pans and to faecal contamination, indicating the need for refinement of presentation of pecking material. In our study, the maintenance of the pecking pans was the sole responsibility of the farmer. The upkeep varied between farms, with excrement usually not removed, possibly because farmers did not regard the pans as feeding equipment.
Other studies suggest that young chicks and pullets prefer enrichments that they can play with and manipulate, such as string [29]. It is possible that in our study the young birds found the pecking substrate too hard to satisfy their needs to express foraging behaviour. It is also possible that beak-treated birds were more reluctant to peck at something hard owing to short- or long-term pain [22]. Research has shown that feeding habits are different and feed intake is reduced in beak-treated birds [17,22].
We examined whether IP and plumage condition differed between the three strains, finding no significance differences for all areas scored (neck, back, tail and wings). However, relatively low levels of IP were observed and, in general, the flocks had good plumage cover and quality.
It was hypothesized that treatment flocks provided with pecking pans would show reduced levels of feather pecking, yet no differences were observed, and neither were there any significant effects of beak treatment, strain or age. This finding contrasts with other studies, which show that providing some form of enrichment reduces IP during the rearing and laying period [2,31,32]. However, in our study, overall rates of feather pecking were low, many of the sheds had good friable litter and some of the sheds were stocked with a greater space allowance, providing more room for the birds to perform foraging activity, ground scratching and dust bathing behaviour. Most flocks were given the opportunity to pursue priority behaviours (i.e., foraging, perching and dustbathing [42,43,44], perhaps to a greater extent than in some other studies. Supporting this argument are results from works that show that providing enrichments in combination with lower SD can reduce both gentle and severe feather pecking behaviour [7,16,32]. We found no difference in the plumage condition of intact and beak-treated flocks. This unexpected finding is in contrast to other reports which have found that beak trimmed birds have better plumage condition at rear [2,15,44], despite IP activity [45]. This could be explained by the low rate of IP observed in our study, which is a positive finding, as young pullets commonly start to peck around the preen gland and oily feathers near the tail before they are transferred to the laying house [11]. One reason for the relative lack of effects of treatment, strain or beak status on IP may be that short periods of observation at a given time of day were not sufficient to accurately reflect levels of IP, and the sample size was too small to detect differences because this analysis treated whole flocks as independent units in the analysis. Plumage damage is also not solely related to IP and may also reflect moulting [46] or abrasion by furnishings in the shed [38].
Few studies have considered whether pecking enrichments that supply hard substrates can reduce beak sharpness in laying hens, with only three previous studies on birds during the rearing stage. Our study measured beaks at the individual bird level and confirmed, as expected, intact beaks were longer than trimmed beaks. Beak length generally increased with age, except in BB birds, where growth between age 6 and 7 weeks and 10 and 11 weeks was relatively low, followed by a greater increase between 10 and 11 weeks and 14 and 15 weeks of age. Variation between individual birds in beak length tended to decrease with age. Pecking pans had a significant shortening effect on side-beak length and top-beak length at 6–7 weeks of age and at 10–11 weeks. Finding significantly shorter beaks at 6–7 weeks was unexpected, as the pans had been placed just the day before observations commenced. This could therefore be due to a random effect of uncontrolled differences between housing or flocks. However, it is also possible that the pecking pans had a very rapid effect, shortening the birds’ beaks dramatically in just a few hours or days. In support, other authors [36] found that abrasive material in the feed trough worked immediately at the start of the experiment at 6 weeks of age. It was considered that this rapid effect could arise through a combination of the novelty of the abrasive materials and the softness of the beaks of young birds.
At 10–11 weeks, both mean side-beak length and mean top-beak length were significantly shorter in the birds provided with a pecking pan when birds of all beak status were considered together, and separately for both intact and IRBT birds. By 14–15 weeks, there was no significant difference in the two measurements when all birds were considered together, but the side-beak length of the intact birds remained shorter at this final visit when pans were provided. One explanation for the reduced effect of pans at the older age is that the birds’ beaks are continually growing and becoming stronger, so blunting may not have been achieved as easily as when the chicks were younger, having much softer beaks, or, more likely, blunting needs to occur continuously in growing birds and the use of the pans had substantially declined.

5. Conclusions

This study has highlighted factors that are likely to be important when considering a long-lasting enrichment to enhance bird welfare. It is the first to investigate, on multiple rearing farms, the potential benefit of blunting the bird’s beak naturally through normal levels of wear. This was achieved by provision of pecking pans containing an abrasive material and provided as an intended environmental enrichment. Evaluation of the results and methodology has given rise to questions to be addressed for future work based upon the improvements to the design and presentation of the pecking pan enrichment device and to some of the methods developed in this study. Despite some limitations in methodology, this study demonstrates that pecking pans for chicks/pullets may provide an environmental enrichment, with the potential added benefit of blunting the bird’s beak naturally through normal level of wear with a very quick onset of effect at 6–7 weeks and continuing to 10–11 weeks of age for all birds, and through to 14–15 weeks for birds with intact beaks. It remains to be determined whether the effect could be extended by presenting the abrasive material differently so that it remained clean and accessible. Furthermore, these preliminary results indicate genotypic variation in beak characteristics, which gives scope for selecting for genotypes with blunter, less damaging beaks.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.J.N. and C.A.W.; methodology, P.E.B., C.A.W. and C.J.N.; software, not applicable; validation, P.E.B., C.A.W. and C.J.N.; formal analysis, P.E.B. and C.J.N.; investigation, P.E.B.; resources, not applicable; data curation, P.E.B. and C.J.N.; writing—original draft preparation, P.E.B.; writing—review and editing, C.A.W. and C.J.N.; visualization, not applicable; supervision, C.A.W. and C.J.N.; project administration, C.A.W.; funding acquisition, C.A.W. and C.J.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Vencomatic Poultry Ltd., who also supplied the pecking pans free of charge, for which we are most grateful.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study had ethical approval from the University of Bristol ethics committee. Reference UIN UB/15/053.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data on beak measurements are available at the Dryad open access repository: doi:10.5061/dryad.2bvq83bs3.

Acknowledgments

The authors would also like to express appreciation to Stonegate and Noble Foods, who gave PB the opportunity to collect the data on their rearing farms.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Iqbal, Z.; Drake, K.; Swick, A.; Taylor, R.S.; Perez-Maldonado, R.A.; Ruhnke, I. Effect of Pecking Stones and Age on Feather Cover, Hen Mortality, and Performance in Free-Range Laying Hens; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2020; Volume 99, pp. 2307–2314. [Google Scholar]
  2. Lambton, S.L.; Knowles, T.G.; Yorke, C.; Nicol, C.J. The Risk Factors Affecting the Development of Gentle and Severe Injurious Pecking in Loose Housed Laying Hens; International Society for Applied Ethology: Edinburgh, UK, 2010; pp. 32–42. [Google Scholar]
  3. Campbell, D.L.M.; De Hass, E.N.; Lee, C. A Review of Environmental Enrichment for Laying Hens during Rearing in Relation to Their Behavioral and Physiological Development; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2019; Volume 98, pp. 9–28. [Google Scholar]
  4. Blokhuis, H.J.; Van Niekerk, T.F.; Bessei, W.; Elson, A.; Guémené, D.; Kjaer, J.B.; Levrino, G.A.M.; Nicol, C.J.; Tauson, R.; Weeks, C.A. The LayWel project welfare implications of changes in production systems for laying hens. Worlds Poult. Sci. J. 2007, 63, 101–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Nicol, C.J.; Bestman, M.; Gilani, A.M.; De Haas, E.N.; De Jong, I.C.; Lambton, S.; Wagenaar, J.P.; Weeks, C.A.; Rodenburg, T.B. The prevention and control of feather pecking: Application to commercial systems. Worlds Poult. Sci. J. 2013, 69, 775–778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Rodenburg, T.; Van Krimpen, M.; De Jong, I.; De Haas, E.; Kops, M.; Riedstra, B.; Nordquist, R.; Wagenaar, J.; Bestman, M.; Nicol, C. The prevention and control of feather pecking in laying hens: Identifying the underlying principles. Worlds Poult. Sci. J. 2013, 69, 361–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Finj, L.B.; van der Staay, J.; Vivian, F.; Goerlich-Jansson, C.; Saskia, S. Importance of Basic Research on the Causes of Feather Pecking in Relation to Welfare. Animals 2020, 10, 213. [Google Scholar]
  8. Van Staaveren, N.; Harlander, A. Cause and prevention of injurious pecking in chickens. In Understanding the Behavioiur and Improving the Welfare of Chickens; Nicol, C., Ed.; Burleigh Dodds Science Publishing: Cambridge, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  9. Gilani, A.M.; Knowles, T.G.; Nicol, C.J. The effect of rearing environment on injurious pecking in young and adult laying hens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 148, 54–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. De Haas, E.N.; Blokhuis, J.E.; de Jong, I.C.; Kemp, B.; Jancsak, A.M.; Rodenburg, T. Predicting feather damage in laying hens during the laying period. Is it the past or is it the present? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 160, 75–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Appleby, M.C.; Mench, J.A.; Hughes, B.O. Poultry Behaviour and Welfare; CABI Publishing: Oxfordshire, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  12. Lambton, S.L. The Factors Affecting the Development of Injurious Pecking in Laying. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  13. Glatz, P.C. Beak trimming methods—Review. Asian-Australas. J. Anim. Sci. 2000, 13, 1619–1637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Nicol, C.J. Behavioural Biology of Chickens; CABI: Wallingford, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  15. Fiks-van Niekerk, T.G.; Jong De, I. Mutilations in poultry in European poultry production systems. Lohmann Inf. 2007, 42, 35–46. [Google Scholar]
  16. Liebers, C.J.; Schwarzer, A.; Erhard, M.; Schmidt, P.; Louton, H. The influence of environmental enrichment and stocking density on the plumage and health conditions of laying hen pullets. Poult. Sci. 2019, 98, 2474–2488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gentle, M.J. Beak trimming in poultry. Poult. Sci. 1986, 42, 268–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Cheng, H. Morphopathological changes and pain in beak trimmed laying hens. Worlds Poult. Sci. J. Assoc. 2006, 62, 41–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Spoolder, H.; Schöne, M.; Bracke, M. Initiatives to Reduce Mutilations in EU Livestock Production. 2016. Available online: http://edepot.wur.nl/37496 (accessed on 2 December 2017).
  20. Gentle, M.J.; Breward, J. The bill tip organ of the chicken (Gallus gallus var. domesticus). J. Anat. 1986, 145, 79–85. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  21. Marchant-Forde, R.M.; Fahey, A.G.; Cheng, H.W. Comparative effects of infrared and one-third hot-blade trimming on beak topography, behavior, and growth. Poult. Sci. 2008, 87, 1474–1483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Kuenzel, W.J. Neurobiological Basis of Sensory Perception: Welfare Implications of Beak Trimming. Poult. Sci. 2007, 86, 1273–1282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. McKeegan, D.E.F.; Philbey, A.W. Chronic neurophysiological and anatomical changes associated with infrared beak treatment and their implications for laying hen welfare. Anim. Welf. 2012, 21, 207–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Angevaare, M.J.; Prins-Franz, S.; van der Staay, J.; Nordquist, R.E. The effect of maternal care and infrared beak trimming on development, performance, and behavior of Silver Nick hens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 140, 70–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hartcher, K.M.; Tran, K.T.N.; Wilkinson, S.J.; Hemsworth, P.; Thomson, P.; Cronin, G.M. The effects of environmental enrichment and beak-trimming during the rearing period on subsequent feather damage due to feather-pecking in laying hens. Poult. Sci. 2015, 94, 852–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. García, J.; Mandalawi, H.A.; Fondevila, G.; Mateos, G.G. Influence of beak trimming and inclusion of sodium butyrate in the diet on growth performance and digestive tract traits of brown-egg pullets differing in initial body weight. Poult. Sci. 2019, 98, 3937–3949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Struthers, S.; Classen, H.; Gomis, S.; Crowe, T.; Schwean-Lardner, K. The impact of beak tissue sloughing and beak shape variation on the behavior and welfare of infrared beak-treated layer pullets and hens. Poult. Sci. 2019, 98, 4269–4281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Jones, R.; Carmichael, N.L.; Rayner, E. Pecking preferences and pre-dispositions in domestic chicks: Implications for the development of environmental enrichment devices. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2000, 69, 291–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Jones, R.; McAdie, T.; McCorquodale, C.; Keeling, L. Pecking at other birds and at string enrichment devices by adult laying hens. Br. Poult. Sci. 2002, 43, 337–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Morrissey, K.L.H.; Brocklehurst, S.; Baker, L.; Widowski, T.M.; Sandilands, V. Can Non-Beak Treated Hens be Kept in Commercial Furnished Cages? Exploring the Effects of Strain and Extra Environmental Enrichment on Behaviour, Feather Cover, and Mortality. Animals 2016, 6, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  31. Lambton, S.L.; Nicol, C.J.; Friel, M.; Main, D.C.J.; Mckinstry, J.L.; Sherwin, C.M.; Walton, J.; Weeks, C.A. A bespoke management package can reduce the levels of injurious pecking in loose housed laying hen flocks. Vet. Rec. 2013, 172, 423–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Zepp, M.; Louton, H.; Erhard, M.; Schmidt, P.; Helmer, F.; Schwarzer, A. The influence of stocking density and enrichment on the occurrence of feather pecking and aggressive pecking behavior in laying hen chicks. J. Vet. Behav. 2018, 24, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Nicol, C.J.; Lindberg, A.C.; Phillips, A.J.; Pope, S.J.; Wilkins, L.J.; Green, L.E. Influence of prior exposure to wood shavings on feather pecking, dustbathing and foraging in adult laying hens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2001, 73, 141–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Schreiter, R.; Damme, K.; von Eberhard, B.; Vogt, I.; Klunker, M.; Freick, M. Effects of litter and additional enrichment elements on the occurrence of feather pecking in pullets and laying hens—A focused review. Vet. Med. Sci. 2019, 3, 184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Pettersson, I.; Weeks, C.A.; Nicol, C.J. Provision of a resource package reduces feather pecking and improves ranging distribution on free-range layer farms. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 195, 60–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Fiks-van Niekerk, T.G.C.M.; Elson, H.A. Abrasive Devices to Blunt the Beak Tip; Glatz, P., Ed.; Poultry Welfare Issues, Beak Trimming; Nottingham University Press: Nottingham, UK, 2004; pp. 127–131. [Google Scholar]
  37. Morrissey, K.L. Behavioural and Nutritional Management of Non-Beak Treated Hens Housed in Furnished Cages. Ph.D. Thesis, The University of Guelph, Guelph, Canada, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  38. Icken, W.; Cavero, D.; Schmutz, M. Selection on Beak Shape to Reduce Feather Pecking in Laying Hens. 2017. Available online: http://www.ltz.de/en/news/lohmann-information/4-Selection-on-beak-shape-to-reduce-feather-pecking-in-laying-hens.php (accessed on 1 August 2021).
  39. Soons, J.; Herrel, A.; Genbrugge, A.; Aerts, P.; Podos, J.; Adriaens, D.; de Witte, Y.; Jacobs, P.; Dirckx, J. Mechanical stress, fracture risk and beak evolution in Darwin’s ground finches (Geospize). Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sciences. 2010, 365, 1093–1098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Osborn, A. Individual Variation in the Beak Shape of Laying Hens by the End of the Laying Period. Master’s Thesis, 2014. Unpublished work. [Google Scholar]
  41. Dalton, H.A.; Torrey, S.; Wood, B.J.; Widowski, T.M.; Guerin, M.T. Analysis of Beak Shape in Domestic Turkeys Using Landmark-Based Geometric Morphometrics; Poultry Science Association Annual Meeting: Louisville, Kentucky, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  42. Weeks, C.; Nicol, C. Behavioural needs, priorities and preferences of laying hens. World’s Poult. Sci. J. 2006, 62, 296–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Kjaer, J.B.; Sorensen, P. Feather pecking and cannibalism in free-range laying hens affected by genotype, dietary level of methionine plus cystine, light intensity during rearing and age at first access to the range area. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 76, 21–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Riber, A.B.; Hinrichsen, L.K. Welfare Consequences of Omitting Beak Trimming in Barn Layers. Front. Vet. Sci. 2017, 4, 222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Blokhuis, H.J.; Van Der Haar, J.W. Effects of Floor Type During Rearing and of Beak Trimming on Ground Pecking and Feather Pecking in Laying Hens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1989, 22, 359–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Bestman, M.; Koene, P.; Wagenaar, J.-P. Influence of farm factors on the occurrence of feather pecking in organic reared hens and their predictability for feather pecking in the laying period. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 121, 120–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Photograph of the Vencomatic Pecking Pan.
Figure 1. Photograph of the Vencomatic Pecking Pan.
Animals 12 00674 g001
Figure 2. Beak length was measured with vernier callipers using methodology slightly adjusted from that of Fiks van Niekerk and Elson (2005).
Figure 2. Beak length was measured with vernier callipers using methodology slightly adjusted from that of Fiks van Niekerk and Elson (2005).
Animals 12 00674 g002
Figure 3. The black line on each image indicates the measurement that was taken on live birds, from left to right, top length and side length.
Figure 3. The black line on each image indicates the measurement that was taken on live birds, from left to right, top length and side length.
Animals 12 00674 g003
Figure 4. The decline with age in the pecking behaviour for eight flocks that were provided with a pecking pan (mean pecks/bird/min).
Figure 4. The decline with age in the pecking behaviour for eight flocks that were provided with a pecking pan (mean pecks/bird/min).
Animals 12 00674 g004
Figure 5. Growth curve of side-beak length for intact and IRBT flocks for each strain. (a) British blacktail birds with intact beaks; (b) British blacktail birds with trimmed beaks; (c) Lohmann Brown birds with intact beaks; (d) Lohmann Brown birds with trimmed beaks; (e) Bovan Brown birds with trimmed beaks. There were no Bovan Brown flocks with intact beaks in the study.
Figure 5. Growth curve of side-beak length for intact and IRBT flocks for each strain. (a) British blacktail birds with intact beaks; (b) British blacktail birds with trimmed beaks; (c) Lohmann Brown birds with intact beaks; (d) Lohmann Brown birds with trimmed beaks; (e) Bovan Brown birds with trimmed beaks. There were no Bovan Brown flocks with intact beaks in the study.
Animals 12 00674 g005aAnimals 12 00674 g005b
Figure 6. Growth curve of top-beak length for intact and IRBT flocks for each strain. (a) British blacktail birds with intact beaks; (b) British blacktail birds with trimmed beaks; (c) Lohmann Brown birds with intact beaks; (d) Lohmann Brown birds with trimmed beaks; (e) Bovan Brown birds with trimmed beaks. There were no Bovan Brown flocks with intact beaks in the study.
Figure 6. Growth curve of top-beak length for intact and IRBT flocks for each strain. (a) British blacktail birds with intact beaks; (b) British blacktail birds with trimmed beaks; (c) Lohmann Brown birds with intact beaks; (d) Lohmann Brown birds with trimmed beaks; (e) Bovan Brown birds with trimmed beaks. There were no Bovan Brown flocks with intact beaks in the study.
Animals 12 00674 g006aAnimals 12 00674 g006b
Table 1. Flock information and pecking pan provision for all sixteen experimental groups.
Table 1. Flock information and pecking pan provision for all sixteen experimental groups.
Flock No.Size GenotypeBeak StatusPecking Pans (1 Per 500 Birds) or Controls
110,000British BlacktailIRBTNo pecking pan
210,000British BlacktailIntactNo pecking pan
39000Lohmann BrownIntactPecking Pan
411,000Lohmann BrownIRBTPecking Pan
59000Bovans BrownIRBTPecking Pan
68000Bovans BrownIRBTNo pecking pan
73600Lohmann BrownIntactNo pecking pan
83600British BlacktailIRBTPecking Pan
910,000Lohmann BrownIRBTNo pecking pan
108000British BlacktailIRBTPecking Pan
116400Lohmann BrownIntactPecking Pan
126500British BlacktailIntactPecking Pan
134100British BlacktailIntactNo pecking pan
143400Lohmann BrownIRBTNo pecking pan
153300British BlacktailIntactPecking Pan
163600British BlacktailIntactNo Pecking Pan
Table 2. Ethogram of feather pecking behaviour.
Table 2. Ethogram of feather pecking behaviour.
BehaviourDefinition
GFPSoft gentle feather pecking, without pulling and removal of feathers
SFPTo peck or pull out the feathers of other birds with force
APForceful pecking directed at the head and neck region
VPPecking directly at the vent area
CBPecking at exposed skin creating wounds, leading to cannibalistic pecking
Table 3. Description of feather scoring.
Table 3. Description of feather scoring.
0Well feathered body part; feathers intact
1Slight damage with feathers ruffled but body part completely or almost completely covered
2Severe damage to feathers but localised naked areas (<5 cm2)
3Severe damage to feathers and large naked areas (>5 cm2)
4Severe damage to feathers, (>5 cm2) naked area and haemorrhage due to broken skin
Table 4. Description of beak measurements see Figure 2 and Figure 3.
Table 4. Description of beak measurements see Figure 2 and Figure 3.
Beak MeasurementsDescription
Top of the beakTop of the beak, at the frontal feather tract margin to tip
Right side of the beakNares to the tip of the beak
Table 5. Means and confidence intervals of visual flock feather scores in intact and beak trimmed birds with and without access to pecking pans. There were no significant differences.
Table 5. Means and confidence intervals of visual flock feather scores in intact and beak trimmed birds with and without access to pecking pans. There were no significant differences.
Body RegionBeak StatusPecking Pan Provision
IntactTrimmedPansNo Pans
Neck0.40 (0.02–0.06)0.03 (0.01–0.04)0.02 (0.01–0.04)0.04 (0.02–0.07)
Back0.00 (0.00–0.03)0.01 (0.01–0.02)0.02 (0.01–0.02)0.01 (0.00–0.02)
Tail0.32 (0.00–0.03)0.01 (0.27–0.38)0.31 (0.27–0.35)0.34 (0.27–0.40)
Table 6. Flock mean feather scores (confidence intervals) in handled birds comparing beak status and pecking pan provision. There were no significant differences.
Table 6. Flock mean feather scores (confidence intervals) in handled birds comparing beak status and pecking pan provision. There were no significant differences.
Body Region Beak StatusPecking Pan Provision
IntactTrimmedPansNo Pans
Neck0.10 (0.05–0.15)0.06 (0.03–0.09)0.09 (0.05–0.12)0.07 (0.02–0.12)
Back0.04 (0.00–0.00)0.02 (0.01–0.00)0.024 (0.00–0.04)0.04 (0.00–0.88)
Tail0.04 (0.37–0.62)0.45 (0.32–0.59)0.432 (0.32–0.55) 0.51 (0.38–0.66)
Wings0.03 (0.00–0.06)0.03 (0.00–0.74)0.01 (0.04–0.03)0.05 (0.01–0.09)
Table 7. Median and inter-quartile range (IQR) of side-beak lengths (mm) for intact and IRBT flocks for all ages (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks, 14–15 weeks).
Table 7. Median and inter-quartile range (IQR) of side-beak lengths (mm) for intact and IRBT flocks for all ages (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks, 14–15 weeks).
Age (Weeks)Median (IQR) Side-Beak Length—Intact FlocksMedian (IQR) Side Beak-Length—IRBT FlocksMann–Whitney UStandardized Test Statisticp-Value
n = 360n = 330
6–712.99 (4.38)7.99 (2.66)19,649−15.23<0.001
10–1115.54 (1.25)11.25 (3.76)9263−19.19<0.001
14–1517.32 (1.10)13.42 (1.52)256421.75<0.001
Table 8. Median and inter-quartile range (IQR) of top-beak lengths (mm) for intact and IRBT flocks for all ages (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks, 14–15 weeks).
Table 8. Median and inter-quartile range (IQR) of top-beak lengths (mm) for intact and IRBT flocks for all ages (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks, 14–15 weeks).
Age (Weeks)Median (IQR) Top-Beak Length—Intact FlocksMedian (IQR) Top Beak-Length—IRBT FlocksMann–Whitney UStandardized Test Statisticp-Value
n = 360n = 330
6–714.73 (5.70)7.99 (2.66) 19,649−15.23<0.001
10–1118.33 (2.20)13.72 (5.12)10,391−18.71<0.001
14–1520.23 (1.54)16.48 (1.90)3414−21.43<0.001
Table 9. Median and inter-quartile range (IQR) of side-beak lengths (mm) for birds of all beak status, showing effect of pecking pan provision at (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks, 14–15 weeks).
Table 9. Median and inter-quartile range (IQR) of side-beak lengths (mm) for birds of all beak status, showing effect of pecking pan provision at (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks, 14–15 weeks).
Age (Weeks)Median (IQR) Side-Beak Length with Pan Median (IQR) Side Beak-Length with No Pan Mann–Whitney UStandardized Test Statisticp-Value
n = 360n = 330
6–79.12 (5.43)10.13 (5.18)50,838−3.274<0.001
10–1112.04 (6.18)13.97 (4.10)46,046−5.106<0.001
14–1515.16 (1.98)15.21 (4.14)57,659−0.6660.506
Table 10. Median and inter-quartile range (IQR) of top-beak lengths (mm) for birds of all beak status, showing effect of pecking pan provision at (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks, 14–15 weeks).
Table 10. Median and inter-quartile range (IQR) of top-beak lengths (mm) for birds of all beak status, showing effect of pecking pan provision at (6–7 weeks, 10–11 weeks, 14–15 weeks).
Age (Weeks)Median (IQR) for Top Length with PanMedian (IQR) for Top Length with No PanMann–Whitney UStandardized Test Statisticp-Value
n = 360n = 330
6–710.20 (6.64)12.22 (5.50)53,608−2.2140.027
10–1115.10 (7.40)16.45 (4.47)48,490−4.171<0.001
14–1518.51 (3.78)18.32 (3.87)62,1441.0490.294
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Baker, P.E.; Nicol, C.J.; Weeks, C.A. The Effect of Hard Pecking Enrichment during Rear on Feather Cover, Feather Pecking Behaviour and Beak Length in Beak-Trimmed and Intact-Beak Laying Hen Pullets. Animals 2022, 12, 674. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12060674

AMA Style

Baker PE, Nicol CJ, Weeks CA. The Effect of Hard Pecking Enrichment during Rear on Feather Cover, Feather Pecking Behaviour and Beak Length in Beak-Trimmed and Intact-Beak Laying Hen Pullets. Animals. 2022; 12(6):674. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12060674

Chicago/Turabian Style

Baker, Paula Elizabeth, Christine Janet Nicol, and Claire Alexandra Weeks. 2022. "The Effect of Hard Pecking Enrichment during Rear on Feather Cover, Feather Pecking Behaviour and Beak Length in Beak-Trimmed and Intact-Beak Laying Hen Pullets" Animals 12, no. 6: 674. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12060674

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop