Next Article in Journal
Inclusive Research and the Use of Visual, Creative and Narrative Strategies in Spain
Previous Article in Journal
Association between Neighborhood and Racial Composition of Victims on Fatal Police Shooting and Police Violence: An Integrated Review (2000–2022)
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Multicomponent Intervention Associated with Improved Emotional and Cognitive Outcomes of Marginalized Unemployed Youth of Latin America

by
Cristina Crespo-Andrade
1,*,
Ana Francisca Trueba
1,2,3,
María Sol Garcés
1,3 and
Graham Pluck
3,4
1
Colegio de Ciencias Sociales y Humanidades, Universidad San Francisco de Quito, Quito 170901, Ecuador
2
Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA
3
Instituto de Neurociencias, Universidad San Francisco de Quito, Quito 170901, Ecuador
4
Faculty of Psychology, Chulalongkorn University, Bangkok 10330, Thailand
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Soc. Sci. 2022, 11(4), 155; https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci11040155
Submission received: 30 December 2021 / Revised: 5 March 2022 / Accepted: 14 March 2022 / Published: 1 April 2022

Abstract

:
Mass migration and people seeking political refuge are critical social issues facing Latin America. Ecuador has the largest population of recognized refugees in the region. Youths from a migration background have an increased risk of becoming NEET (Young people not in employment, education, or training). Such youths struggle more with mental health problems than non-NEET peers. Being a refugee, NEET further increases the risk of having mental health problems and may be linked to lower cognitive functioning, which could maintain exclusion and unemployment. This intervention study was performed with a group of young people of different nationalities who were refugees or belonged to other vulnerable groups attending a six-week employability-support intervention in Ecuador. In order to assess the impact of the intervention, a range of measures of executive cognitive function, mental health, and the potential for positive change were used. We found that post-intervention, the group reported significantly less psychological distress and better self-esteem, self-efficacy, and cognitive response inhibition than before the intervention. We conclude that multicomponent interventions may effectively improve the psychological functioning of vulnerable NEET groups in the Latin American context.

1. Introduction

Education and employment are positively related to cognitive and mental health (Chen et al. 2019; Vance et al. 2016; Hergenrather et al. 2018; Modini et al. 2016). Participating in education and employment requires learning new skills, establishing goals and routines, and engaging socially, benefiting well-being by creating occupational and social support and enhancing cognition (Vance et al. 2016; O’Dea et al. 2014). Dropout from education or disengagement of the labor market exposes youth to a greater risk of social exclusion, psychological distress, disability, maladaptive behaviors, illness, and disease (O’Dea et al. 2014; Fernández-Suárez et al. 2016; Hjorth et al. 2016).
According to the International Labor Organization, there is a high rate of young people, more than one in five (22.4%) aged 15–24, neither in employment, education, nor training (NEET). Acknowledging this problem worldwide led to adopting the NEET rate as part of the 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development, as an indicator of progress towards reducing the proportion of the NEET individuals within a population (O’Higgins 2020; Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development et al. 2016; Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development 2021).
NEET rates vary widely across countries, showing the highest rates in lower-middle-income countries and with young women outnumbering young men by a factor of two to one (O’Higgins 2020). NEETs are not a homogeneous group; the causes behind this status are diverse (Tamesberger and Bacher 2014). The Eurofound (2012, 2016) suggested subgroups in the NEET population that include a mix of vulnerable and non-vulnerable young people with different characteristics and needs. The individual risk factors include adverse family environments, low household income or educational levels, disability, living in remote areas, and immigration background (Eurofound 2012; Assmann and Broschinski 2021).

1.1. NEET Mental Health and Cognition

Evidence suggests that NEETs struggle more with mental health problems than non-NEET peers. NEETs may be at greater risk of depression and anxiety (Feng et al. 2015) and severe symptoms of these disorders (Basta et al. 2019; O’Dea et al. 2014). The relationship between mental health and NEET status could be bidirectional: the pre-existence of a mental health condition may constitute a risk factor for becoming NEET, and conversely, mental health problems could be a long-term consequence of NEET status. In a British cohort study, nearly 60% of NEETs had suffered from a mental health problem during childhood or adolescence compared to 35% of non-NEETs (Goldman-Mellor et al. 2015). Moreover, an association between NEET status during adolescence and several mental health outcomes in adulthood is established (Gutiérrez-García et al. 2017; Power et al. 2015). The mental health problems experienced by NEETS are thought to have been exacerbated by the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic (Kvieskienė et al. 2021).
Cognitive and non-cognitive skills negatively correlate with the probability of being NEET. Higher scores in cognitive measures and socio-emotional abilities such as self-esteem, self-efficacy, and locus of control are associated with a lower probability of being NEET (Alvarado et al. 2020). Education has been related to better performance on cognitive measures and further engagement with cognitive activities and challenges (Lövdén et al. 2020; Guerra-Carrillo et al. 2017). Parisi et al. (2012) found that educational and other cognitively charged experiences may benefit brain structure and function, developing neuroprotective effects to face age-related decline. Additionally, education has economic implications as cognitive and non-cognitive skills have labor-market value and impacts individual earnings (De Hoyos et al. 2016). Therefore, the lack of opportunities to develop skills is crucial for future NEET status (Gladwell et al. 2015).

1.2. NEETs in Ecuador

Like other countries in Latin America, Ecuador has a high percentage of people who could be defined as NEET. The total number of people between 15 and 24 years of age who are NEET in Latin America remained practically constant between the beginning and the end of 1992–2014: it went from 19.0 million in 1992 to 18.7 million in 2014 (Tornarolli 2016). Four out of five NEETs in Latin America are females; they more often come from a rural background and belong to the poorest quintile of the wealth distribution (Minujín et al. 2016). Among young Ecuadorians aged between 15 and 24, 19% do not attend any educational center and are unemployed (Buitrón et al. 2018).

1.3. Refugees in Ecuador

As mentioned earlier, being a refugee is a risk factor for becoming NEET. According to the United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees (2020), Ecuador has the largest population of recognized refugees in Latin America, with 69,897 recognized refugees arriving between 1989 and 2020, most of them coming from Colombia and Venezuela. For decades, Colombians were forced to leave their country, escaping the violence of the internal conflict and looking for security in Ecuador (United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees 2020). In 2019, 79.5 million people were forcibly displaced worldwide. Four million of them left Venezuela and now live mainly in Latin America and the Caribbean (United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees 2020). In the first quarter of 2019, around 470,095 Venezuelans lived in Ecuador, almost 3% of Ecuador’s population (Olivieri et al. 2020).
For both groups, but especially for Venezuelans, forced migration increased the vulnerability to ‘labor and sexual exploitation, trafficking, violence, discrimination and xenophobia’ (United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees 2021). Refugees and asylum seekers are among the most vulnerable groups (United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees 2015, 2016). Their vulnerability is linked to experiences before migration due to conflict, violence, or persecution and to post-migration stressors such as exclusion, unemployment, and discrimination (Hameed et al. 2018). As reported in the literature, having a migration background increases the youth person’s likelihood of becoming NEET by 70% (Eurofound 2012). Therefore, the probability that a significant percentage of young refugees in Ecuador are in NEET status is high, becoming a socioeconomic concern.

1.4. Mental Health and Cognition of Refugees

The adverse psychosocial factors experienced during or after migration could negatively impact migrant mental-health status and increase the risk of cognitive impairment (Xu et al. 2018). Most of the current research on the mental health of refugees has shown that they are at higher risk for a variety of psychiatric disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder, anxiety, and depression, due to their experiences before migration (Peterson et al. 2020; Chaplin et al. 2020; Hameed et al. 2018). In addition, post-migration distress, mainly acculturative stress, can worsen mental health symptoms (Hameed et al. 2018).
There is strong evidence that cognitive factors are essential for successful refugee adjustment (Hahn et al. 2019). Xu et al. (2018) identify different pathways that may link migration and cognitive function from current evidence. Socioeconomic status, psychosocial and behavioral traits, and physical and psychological health prior to migration could influence cognitive function (Xu et al. 2018). A study comparing executive cognitive abilities, specifically working memory and inhibitory control, between refugee and non-refugee adolescents, found that poverty was more critical to cognitive function than traumatic experiences (Chen et al. 2019). Therefore, since dropout from education and unemployment are risk factors for poverty, refugees in the NEET situation may also be more susceptible to executive cognitive problems. Executive functions are important considerations because they are both significant predictors of educational and workplace success (Pluck et al. 2019; Pluck et al. 2020b) and are also known to be malleable, being sensitive to behavioral interventions (Stamenova and Levine 2018). For instance, in previous studies, we found that the Hayling Test that measures verbal response suppression was the best predictor of workplace success, specifically sales (Pluck et al. 2020b) as well a predictor of academic performance in college and high school (Pluck et al. 2019). Cognitive switching measured using Trail Making Test of the Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System was a predictor of working hours in populations struggling with mental (McGurk and Mueser 2006) and physical health (Rabkin et al. 2004), and spatial working memory is a predictor of vocational participation (Cairns et al. 2017).

1.5. Relevance

The population examined in this study is doubly vulnerable since they are migrants and have NEET status. As mentioned previously, being a refugee NEET increases the risk of having mental health problems and may impact cognitive ability. Interventions that improve cognitive functioning and mental health may be important ways to break the cycle of social exclusion. Furthermore, in addition to having worse mental health, a study conducted by Buckman et al. (2021) suggests that NEETs had less successful outcomes following psychological interventions in terms of recovery, deterioration, and attrition, indicating that they need early intervention and more sessions.

1.6. Aims of the Study

Previous research has shown that young refugees need diverse activities and practices to promote their socioeconomic inclusion (Pastoor 2017). This study aimed to assess the impact of one such multicomponent employability-support intervention. We assessed the effect of the intervention on the mental health and cognitive abilities of a group of refugees who were in the NEET status in Ecuador (aged 18–36). Our first hypothesis was that our intervention would result in significant improvements in a range of emotional factors such as less psychological distress (General Health Questionnaire-28; GHQ-28), fewer difficulties in emotion regulation (Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale total score; DERS), higher self-efficacy (Generalized Self-efficacy Scale; GSE), higher self-esteem (Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale; RSES), higher locus of control (Rotter’s Locus of Control Scale; LoC), and lower depression and anxiety (Hospital Anxiety Depression Scale; HADS). Our second hypothesis was that our intervention would result in significant improvements in executive function, specifically working memory, cognitive switching, and response suppression. We selected the Hayling Sentence Completion Test to evaluate response suppression, the Trails Making Test to measure cognitive switching, and finally, the Counting Span Test to evaluate working memory, as previous research suggests that these cognitive measures are predictors of academic (Pluck et al. 2019) and workplace success (Cairns et al. 2017; McGurk and Mueser 2006; Pluck et al. 2020b; Rabkin et al. 2004).

2. Material and Methods

2.1. Research Design

This was an opportunistic study on a group of adults (aged 18–36) undertaking a six-week-long psychological intervention to assist them in finding employment or further study. A pre-and post-intervention assessment was employed. The study focused on psychological distress and measures related to self-esteem, self-efficacy, and executive cognitive functioning.

2.2. Participants

Fifty-five people took part in the intervention. Of those 55, 51 (93%) consented to be part of the research study. As this was an opportunistic study, no predetermination of sample size was made; nevertheless, it was anticipated that the recruited sample would be sufficient to detect medium-sized effects at 80% power (Cohen 1992).

2.3. The Intervention

This intervention took place in 2018, and it lasted six weeks. ‘A Ganar: Sin Limites’ is an outreach project with the collaboration of three partners: FUDELA (Fundación de las Americas), United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees, and Universidad San Francisco de Quito.
FUDELA is a private non-profit organization that incorporates sport as a comprehensive training tool. The foundation works to guarantee children, adolescents, young people, and adults in the fulfillment of their rights in recreational, educational, and labor matters, preventing risk situations. FUDELA works in partnership with the United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees, who acts as the angel funder of the foundation’s various programs.
The aim of “A Ganar: Sin Límites” is to provide skills to facilitate entrepreneurship opportunities and employability tools, as well as opportunities to access higher education. The target population is young people of different nationalities who were refugees or belonged to other vulnerable groups. During the summer of 2018, 55 participants from Colombia, Venezuela, Ecuador, and El Salvador were part of the project. In addition, one of the funder conditions was that the project included young Ecuadorian NEETs.
FUDELA attracts participants through social media and information provided by other organizations that work with refugees. The participants completed a survey and then participated in an interview. The project was over a six-week period, comprising 30 days of activities, and took place at the Universidad San Francisco de Quito campus in Cumbayá, Ecuador. The participants were divided into two groups and attended different classes and activities. Smaller groups (around 15 participants) were formed for the sports activities and the psychology classes. The intervention, described in Table 1, had 11 modules, with a total of 235 h of activities, with full-time dedication. The intervention lessons and activities were run by faculty members of various departments of the university and assisted by students.

2.4. Assessments

2.4.1. Self-Report Measures

Six different questionaries were included in the current study. These were selected to assess a range of factors related to mental health and the potential for positive change.
A validated Spanish-language version (Lobo et al. 1986) of the General Health Questionnaire-28 (GHQ-28; Goldberg and Hillier 1979) was included and was graded using the Goodchild and Duncan-Jones (1985) method. Although subscale scores are available, total scores across all items are typically used to quantify psychological distress. The GHQ-28 has been shown to have ‘very good’ internal consistency in an Ecuadorian non-clinical adult sample, with α values ranging from 0.81 to 0.87 (Pluck et al. 2020c). An additional measure of psychological distress was included, the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond and Snaith 1983), in a validated Spanish-language version (Herrero et al. 2003). Again, this is scored on a Likert scale, from 0 to 3 points. We found a ‘reasonable’ level of internal consistency for the HADS total score, at α = 0.73, from a previously reported Ecuadorian non-clinical adult sample (Pluck et al. 2020a). As with the GHQ-28, the HADS has subscale scores available, but research suggests it is essentially unidimensional, measuring psychological distress (Pallant and Tennant 2007). We, therefore, analyzed only the total scores from the GHQ-28 and HADS to test our principal hypotheses regarding mental health. Higher scores on both the GHQ-28 and the HADS indicate greater psychological distress.
We included a validated Spanish-language version (Hervás and Jódar 2008) of the 36-item Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz and Roemer 2004). This is measured on a five-point Likert scale from 1–5 points for a range of different problems, including lack of awareness of emotional states, problems applying appropriate strategies to alleviate negative emotions, and difficulties controlling one’s behavior during strong emotions. The total score appears appropriate for use in the Ecuadorian context, where it has a ‘very good’ internal consistency with α at 0.90 (Reivan-Ortiz et al. 2020). We, therefore, used the total DERS score to test our principal hypothesis regarding emotional regulation skills. Higher scores indicate greater difficulty with emotion regulation.
To measure self-esteem, we used a validated Spanish-language version (Torres 1983) of the Rosenburg Self-esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg 1965). This ten-item scale is scored on a Likert scale, from 1 to 4 points. As with other versions of the RSES, in an Ecuadorian adult sample, it was found to be essentially unidimensional, with ‘very good’ internal consistency at α = 0.84 (Bueno-Pacheco et al. 2020). We, therefore, used the total score to test our principal hypothesis regarding self-esteem. Higher scores on the RSES indicate higher self-esteem. We also used a validated Spanish-language version (Baessler and Schwarzer 1996) of the Generalized Self-efficacy Scale (GSE; Schwarzer and Jerusalem 1995). This is a 10-item questionnaire scored on a Likert scale, from 0 to 3 points. It has ‘very good’ internal consistency in the Ecuadorian context, with α at 0.91 (Bueno-Pacheco et al. 2018).
The final questionnaire used in this study was a Spanish version (Perez García 1984) of Rotter’s Locus of Control Scale (LoC; Rotter 1966).

2.4.2. Executive Function Tests

Three different measures of higher-order cognitive control were employed in this study, based on the findings of Miyake et al. (2000) that there are three basic, somewhat independent, components of executive function. These are working memory, cognitive switching, and response suppression. In addition, all executive tasks employed here were language-based, as such functions are those most susceptible to socioeconomic deprivation (Pluck et al. 2021) and thus may have the greatest potential for remediation.
To measure working memory, we used the Counting Span Test (Conway et al. 2005). Ten separate trials were completed, graded from spans of two items to spans of five items.
To measure cognitive switching, we used the Trail Making Test of the Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System (Delis et al. 2001). This involves two baseline trials, one connecting numbers on an A3-size page and the other connecting letters. A switching trial is also completed in which the participant must alternate between connecting numbers and letters. The crucial statistic is the ratio of completion time on the switching trial to the average completion time on the baseline conditions, as this provides a relatively pure index of cognitive switching (Arbuthnott and Frank 2000). That ratio was used to test our principal hypothesis regarding this aspect of executive function.
To measure response suppression, we used the Hayling Sentence Completion Test (Burgess and Shallice 1996). We used only Part B, which is the test of response suppression, and for convenience, we will refer to this simply as ‘the Hayling’. Fifteen sentences are read aloud to the participant. However, for each sentence, the final word is missing, and the task of the participant is to quickly complete the sentence with a word that makes no sense (i.e., they must suppress the natural response to complete the utterance sensibly). Errors are recorded and scored from 0 points (good performance, a completely unrelated word) to 3 points (bad performance, a word that completes the utterance sensibly). Therefore, the possible score range is 0 to 45, with higher scores indicating greater difficulty with verbal response suppression. Response times for each sentence completion are also recorded.
We used two different Spanish-language versions of the Hayling, one created for research in response to suppression conducted in Cuba (Obeso et al. 2011) and the other created for use in Spain (Pérez-Pérez et al. 2016). In that latter study, the internal consistency of the error score was calculated and found to be ‘very good’ with α at 0.84. Therefore, that error score was used as the principal measure to assess our hypothesis regarding verbal response suppression as an executive function. The two different versions of the Hayling are equivalent, and both were used in this study so that different stimuli materials could be applied at pre-and post-intervention assessments (counterbalanced).

2.4.3. Institutional Review Board Statement

The Bioethics Committee of Universidad San Francisco de Quito reviewed the protocol, including the pre- and post-intervention assessments (application code: 2018-098IN, approval granted 9 May 2018). Written informed consent was taken from all participants who wished to take part. The research was conducted following all local laws and guidance on research provided by the American Psychological Association and by the Declaration of Helsinki of 1975, revised in 2013.

2.4.4. Procedure

All the participants of the intervention attended the campus of Universidad San Francisco de Quito, Ecuador. On the first day, the research aspect was introduced to them in a group meeting, and they were able to ask questions. Then consent forms were circulated. Those who consented were individually assessed on the three different executive function tests in one-to-one sessions with research assistants. The questionaries were distributed in booklet format and completed in a group setting. This was all completed on the first morning of the intervention.
The intervention then began. It lasted for six weeks and comprised of 30 individual days of intervention activities. On the final day, all participants who consented were assessed a second time, using the same questionnaires and cognitive tests. Participants were thanked for their participation in the research and debriefed about the nature of the research. They were able to ask questions and feedback to the researchers at this point. No incentive was given for participation in the research aspect of the intervention.

2.4.5. Statistical Methods

Raw data were used, processed, and analyzed with SPSS version 23. Data were summarized as mean and standard deviation. Parametric inferential analysis was by repeated-measures ANOVA, with effect sizes given as Cohen’s d, calculated as the difference between pre- and post-intervention means divided by the pooled standard deviation (Cohen 1992). Qualitative interpretations of effect sizes are also based on Cohen (1992). For all ANOVA calculations, the residuals were considered normally distributed if the z scores for both skew and kurtosis were below 1.96 (Kim 2013). When non-normal distributions of residuals were observed, raw data were transformed and reanalyzed. When transformation could do produce normally distributed residuals, non-parametric tests were substituted. All statistical null-hypothesis tests were two-tailed. A significance threshold of 0.05 was selected.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic Information of the Sample

Of the 51 participants who completed the study, data were lost on basic demographics from a small proportion. Of those with data available, 30/49 (61%) identified as women, and the mean age was 24.47 (SD = 4.17, range = 18–36, n = 49). Notably, this is a somewhat higher age range than usually defined as NEET (i.e., 18–24), with 19/49 (39%) of our sample being older than 24. For this reason, additional analyses are performed on the main outcome variables when limiting the sample to those aged < 25. Of the 44 cases with ethnicity information available, the majority, 38/44 (86%), described themselves as mestizo (meaning mixed Amerindian and European ancestry). Of the others, 2/49 (5%) self-described as Black, 2/49 (5%) as mixed-race (4%), and 2/49 (5%) as White. The majority, 35/49 (71%), were migrants to Ecuador; the most common origin countries were Venezuela (n = 21) and Colombia (n = 12). All were Spanish speakers and were unemployed and not in education at the time of the intervention.
As we can identify two broad groups within the sample, Ecuadorian nationals (n = 14, 28%) and immigrants to Ecuador (n = 35, 72%), these were compared for both demographic and psychological status variables (i.e., scores on the questionnaires and cognitive tests, pre-intervention). These data are shown in Table 2, where it can be seen that the two sub-groups were similar on demographic variables, with no significant differences. Nor were there any significant differences regarding cognitive test performance. However, there were some significant differences regarding psychological variables as measured by the questionaries: the Ecuadorian participants had more difficulties with emotion regulation (DERS), more external locus of control (LoC), and lower self-esteem scores (RSES) compared to the migrant participants. This may reflect the different life events between the groups.

3.2. Effectiveness of the Intervention

Of the 51 individuals who participated in the study, all had missing data, usually due to not completing the evaluation at the post-intervention stage. The highest data loss was for the executive function tests, in which data were missing for nine individuals (17%), and was lowest for the RSES, in which both pre- and post-intervention data were only missing for three individuals (6%). The actual number of individuals in each pre-post comparison is indicated in Table 3, which also shows average scores for all pre-and post-intervention assessments and the results of null-hypothesis tests of the differences.
From the table, we can see that as a group, the people who completed the intervention showed significantly less psychological distress afterward, measured with either the HADS or the GHQ-28, and in both cases, the effect sizes would be considered ‘medium-sized’. They also reported significantly fewer difficulties with emotion regulation as measured by the DERS scale, but this is technically a ‘small’ effect size. Similarly, post-intervention, participants scored slightly higher for self-esteem; however, this was not much more than a one-point increase in scores on the RSES questionnaire and was qualitatively a ‘small’ effect. Scores on self-efficacy showed only a two-point increase from pre- to post-intervention assessment, but was nevertheless a significant increase, albeit a qualitatively ‘small’ effect. In contrast, there was no significant change in Locus of Control scores.
Next, we examined differences in executive function tests scores associated with the intervention. Although there was no significant change in cognitive switching ability (Trail Making Test), nor in working memory (Counting Span Test), verbal response suppression (Hayling Test) improved significantly, which had a ‘medium’ effect size.
When people are tested on the same assessment twice, there is the possibility that lower scores in the second assessment will be produced by practice effects. Indeed, the principle mental questionnaire measured used here, the GHQ-28, has been shown to have a large repeat-testing effect. Ormel et al. (1989) compared scores on the GHQ-28 completed on two separate dates by a group of psychiatric outpatients for whom psychological condition, as assessed by clinical interview, had not changed. Despite there being no clinical change, scores on the GHQ-28 were almost 19% lower on the second completion.
This repeat-testing effect could possibly explain our significant reduction in GHQ-28 scores. To assess the risk of this, as an exploratory measure, we increased all of the post-assessment GHQ-28 scores by the same level of practice effect reported by Ormel et al. (1989). This increased the mean post-assessment score to 7.29 (SD = 7.02), which is now closer to the pre-intervention mean of 9.70 (5.58). Nevertheless, the lower scores post-intervention compared to pre-intervention remained statistically significantly different, F(1,43) = 9.044, p = 0.004, d = 0.38.
We were not able to assess the other self-report measures in this way, as estimates of the magnitude of retest effects are not available, but if the reduction in psychological distress observed can be attributed to the action of the intervention, it seems reasonable that the other changes in scores can also be interpreted in that way.
In the Hayling data presented in Table 2, error scores are analyzed, which is the normal method with this test. However, as per test instructions, response times (RT) were also recorded. The RT data mirrored that of the error score data. The participants performed faster after the intervention (mean RT = 1.22 s, SD = 0.62) compared to their performance before the intervention (mean RT = 1.92 s, SD = 0.88). This was a significant improvement, F(1,41) = 43.169, p < 0.001, d = 0.93.
Practice effects on RT tasks are known to occur, which could explain the improvement rather than being caused by the intervention. Nevertheless, on 15 trial RT assessments, almost all the practice effect occurs in the first five or six trials, and post hoc removal of those trials can be used to achieve equivalence between first and second test administration (Collie et al. 2003), therefore removing the practice effect. We found that the improvement in the Hayling performance remained even when we removed the first six trials from each administration. Performance was still significantly faster after the intervention (mean RT = 1.34 s, SD = 0.83) compared to before the intervention (mean RT = 2.01 s, SD = 1.04), F(1,41) = 17.985, p < 0.001, d = 0.721. A practice effect, therefore, seems unlikely as the cause of improved verbal response suppression after the intervention.
Finally, our sample was somewhat older than other described samples of NEET individuals, whereas many definitions define NEET as being aged 18 to 24. Only 30/49 61% of our sample were within that range, the other cases, n = 19, being older (range 26–36). To assess whether our results may be biased by our relatively older sample, we repeated all the analyses described above but limited the analyses to individuals under the age of 25. For most of the results, this did not change the significance of the comparisons, the differences that are shown as significant in Table 2 remained significant, and the differences that were not significant in Table 2 remained not significant. The only exception was for emotion regulation scores (DERS), which were significantly different in the full sample, but not significantly different pre- and post-intervention in the age 18–24 subsample (pre-intervention mean = 79.25, SD = 18.53 compared to post-intervention mean = 74.04, SD = 22.06), F(1,27) = 2.143, p = 0.155, d = 0.260).

4. Discussion

This study examined the effectiveness of a multicomponent intervention that focused on providing a variety of skills to prepare a diverse group of NEETs in Ecuador, which included refugees, to successfully re-engage in employment or further their education. We found that after our intervention, the group reported experiencing significantly less psychological distress, higher self-efficacy, and self-esteem and had better cognitive response inhibition than at baseline. A strength of our study compared to previous studies is that we used several outcome measures, including cognitive performance.
Improvement in mental health and associated measures may be an important consequence of the intervention. More specifically, in the context of the aims of the intervention, the reduction in psychological distress after completing the intervention may assist individuals to move into education or employment. A similar case could be made for the observed improvements in self-esteem and self-efficacy. Previous studies have focused on educational program enrolment, re-employment, and salary as the primary outcome measures (Alegre et al. 2015). For example, the main outcome measure of a systematic review that evaluated the outcomes of 18 multicomponent intervention studies saw an increase in the employment rate for those that received the intervention compared to controls (Mawn et al. 2017).
Although there is research indicating that NEETs are at a greater risk of mental health difficulties (Gutiérrez-García et al. 2017), few studies focus on health outcome measures (Nafilyan and Speckesser 2014). One study focused on providing intervention for improving health behaviors in NEETs and found that those that received the intervention were more likely to visit a psychologist (Robert et al. 2019). As with our study, other intervention studies have reported an increase in NEET self-efficacy (Seddon et al. 2013). Other studies have also identified self-efficacy as an important mediator of re-engagement, specifically linked to more job search activity (Fort et al. 2011).
Using different outcome measures can help us further understand underlying mediators for change in this population. Indeed, one study found that excluding non-employment-focused outcomes underestimates the beneficial impact of these interventions, for example, on health (Nafilyan and Speckesser 2014). Our study adds to the existing literature by examining other outcomes measures, including emotional, psychological, and cognitive measures.
The improvement of executive cognitive functions may be of particular interest, as these are seen as core abilities that allow people to control their behavior and succeed in dynamic environments, such as in formal education (Pluck et al. 2016, 2019) or the workplace (Pluck et al. 2020b). In particular, we observed an improvement in verbal response suppression ability, as measured with the Hayling Test. This test has previously been shown to be a better predictor of academic performance in college than a standard intelligence test (Pluck et al. 2016) and predicts academic performance at the high school level (Pluck et al. 2019). Additionally, it predicts the performance of salesmen (Pluck et al. 2020b). Verbal response suppression ability is therefore emerging as a possible cognitive ability closely related to real-life achievement. The observed improvement in response suppression in our NEET sample following the intervention may therefore have practical implications.
Interventions for NEET re-engagement have varied significantly in their focus and scope. Some interventions have focused on changing educational and employment policies to reduce the number of NEETs, while others have focused on NEET prevention with strategies such as individual support (Hutchinson et al. 2016).
Many intervention studies have focused on specific skill training, and others, such as described in the current study, have used a multicomponent approach. For example, previous interventions on NEETs have focused on delivering support, internship placements, vocational, social, and education. The intervention in this study has many of these elements, specifically: industrial quality and productivity, industrial techniques, sales and customer service, sports activities, audiovisual production, financial education, customer services for hotels and restaurants, entrepreneurship, communication, psychology, and job search skills. A systemic review found that more intensive programs with multiple components had a greater impact on employment rate and salary in the long term (Mawn et al. 2017).
A multicomponent intervention approach might be better able to provide skills and support for this group, as it can provide a variety of skills for a group with varying needs. Indeed, the NEETs that participated in our study are a heterogeneous group, including refugees from different countries. This is consistent with what has been found in previous studies. ‘NEET’ in general has always been a global label of young people without employment and not enrolled at school (Furlong 2006; Serracant 2014).
Furthermore, there is research to suggest that it is also important to adapt the intervention to fit the population. Indeed, a recent systematic review indicated that tailoring interventions to specific cultures and minority groups improved outcomes; this includes therapist training, the content of therapy, and how the services are delivered (digitally, face-to-face, over the telephone, group, individual, length of intervention; Arundell et al. 2021). The results of this review paper further highlight the importance of multicomponent interventions that can adapt to meet a specific population’s needs.
We also found that the national participants in the intervention (i.e., Ecuadorians) differed from the migrant participants in several psychological factors. In particular, compared to the migrant participants, the national participants reported more difficulties with emotion regulation and external loci of control, as well as lower self-esteem scores. This likely reflects the different past histories of the two groups. It would have been of interest to compare the pre- and post-intervention scores separately for these groups. However, our sample size was too small to allow it, which should be noted as a limitation of the current study.
The findings in this study need to be considered in light of several other limitations. We did not have a control group, and therefore it is possible that the apparent improvements in mental health and cognitive ability were due to test practice. Our detailed analyses suggested that this was not the case. Nevertheless, such effects cannot be fully ruled out. Another limitation is that we did not use employment or school enrolment as an outcome measure, so we do not know the extent to which our intervention could have an actual effect on employability. Similarly, we cannot determine to what extent this intervention had an enduring effect on participants. Our intervention had multiple components, and our sample was very heterogeneous, and this prevented us from ascertaining which components are essential for change and which are less necessary for subgroups within our NEET sample.
Despite these limitations, this study contributes to our understanding of NEETs in Latin America and potential interventions that can assist this vulnerable population to re-engage in education and employment. However, further research is needed to establish which components of this intervention are most effective for particular subgroups of NEETs and if this intervention could be effective for other cultures or different contexts than that of this study.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.C.-A. and A.F.T.; methodology, A.F.T. and G.P.; formal analysis, G.P.; investigation, C.C.-A.; data curation, C.C.-A. and G.P.; writing—original draft preparation, C.C.-A., A.F.T. and M.S.G.; writing—review and editing, M.S.G.; project administration, C.C.-A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received funding from the Articles Publication Fund of Universidad San Francisco de Quito USFQ.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted following all local laws and guidance on research provided by the American Psychological Association and in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki of 1975, revised in 2013 and approved by the Bioethics Commit-tee of Universidad San Francisco de Quito reviewed the protocol, including the pre- and post-intervention assessments (application code: 2018-098IN, approval granted 9 May 2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the authors. The data has not been placed in a public repository because of restrictions with the ethics committee approval.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Ana María Viteri, Gabriela Romo, Raquel Galindo, María Gloria Medina, and Isabela Lara who assisted with management and data collection on this research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alegre, Miquel Àngel, David Casado, Jordi Sanz, and Federico A. Todeschini. 2015. The impact of training-intensive labour market policies on labour and educational prospects of NEETs: Evidence from Catalonia (Spain). Educational Research 57: 151–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Alvarado, Alfredo, Belén Conde, Rafael Novella, and Andrea Repetto. 2020. NEETs in Latin America and the Caribbean: Skills, aspirations, and information. Journal of International Development 32: 1273–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Arbuthnott, Katherine, and Janis Frank. 2000. Trail Making Test, Part B as a measure of executive control: Validation using a set-switching paradigm. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 22: 518–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Arundell, Laura-Louise, Phoebe Barnett, Joshua E. J. Buckman, Rob Saunders, and Stephen Pilling. 2021. The effectiveness of adapted psychological interventions for people from ethnic minority groups: A systematic review and conceptual typology. Clinical Psychology Review 88: 102063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Assmann, Marie-Luise, and Sven Broschinski. 2021. Mapping young NEETs across Europe: Exploring the institutional configurations promoting youth disengagement from education and employment. Journal of Applied Youth Studies 4: 95–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Baessler, Judith, and Ralf Schwarzer. 1996. Evaluación de la autoeficacia: Adaptación española de la Escala de Autoeficacia General. Ansiedad y Estrés 2: 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  7. Basta, Maria, Stamatis Karakonstantis, Katerina Koutra, Vassilis Dafermos, Antonis Papargiris, Maria Drakaki, Stelios Tzagkarakis, Alexandros Vgontzas, Panagiotis Simos, and Nikos Papadakis. 2019. NEET status among young Greeks: Association with mental health and substance use. Journal of Affective Disorders 253: 210–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Buckman, Joshua E. J., Joshua Stott, Nicole Main, Daniela M. Antonie, Satwant Singh, Syed A. Naqvi, Elisa Aguirre, Jon Wheatley, Mirko Cirkovic, Judy Leibowitz, and et al. 2021. Understanding the psychological therapy treatment outcomes for young adults who are not in education, employment, or training (NEET), moderators of outcomes, and what might be done to improve them. Psychological Medicine, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bueno-Pacheco, Alexandra, Sandra Lima-Castro, Eva Peña-Contreras, Cristina Cedillo-Quizhpe, and Mónica Aguilar-Sizer. 2018. Adaptación al español de la escala de autoeficacia general para su uso en el contexto ecuatoriano. Revista Iberoamericana de Diagnóstico y Evaluación Psicológica 48: 5–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bueno-Pacheco, Alexandra, Sandra Lima-Castro, Paúl Arias-Medina, Eva Peña-Contreras, Mónica Aguilar-Sizer, and Marcela Cabrera-Vélez. 2020. Estructura factorial, invarianza y propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Autoestima de Rosenberg en el contexto Ecuatoriano. Revista Iberoamericana de Diagnóstico y Evaluación 56: 87–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Buitrón, Karina, Verónica Jami, and Yasmín Salazar Méndez. 2018. Los jóvenes ninis en el Ecuador. Revista de Economía del Rosario 21: 39–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Burgess, Paul W., and Tim Shallice. 1996. Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 34: 263–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cairns, Alice J., David J. Kavanagh, Frances Dark, and Steven M. McPhail. 2017. Prediction of vocational participation and global role functioning in help-seeking young adults, from neurocognitive, demographic and clinical variables. Journal of Affective Disorders 221: 158–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Chaplin, Lucia, Lauren Ng, and Cornelius Katona. 2020. Refugee mental health research: Challenges and policy implications. BJPsych Open 6: e102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Chen, Yaojing, Chenlong Lv, Xin Li, Junying Zhang, Kewei Chen, Zhongwan Liu, He Li, Jialing Fan, Ting Qin, Liang Luo, and et al. 2019. The positive impacts of early-life education on cognition, leisure activity, and brain structure in healthy aging. Aging 11: 4923–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Cohen, Jacob. 1992. A power primer. Psychological Bulletin 112: 155–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Collie, Alexander, Maruff Paul, David G. Darby, and Mcstephen Michael. 2003. The effects of practice on the cognitive test performance of neurologically normal individuals assessed at brief test-retest intervals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 9: 419–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Conway, Andrew R. A., Michael J. Kane, Michael F. Bunting, D. Zach Hambrick, Oliver Wilhelm, and Randall W. Engle. 2005. Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 12: 769–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. De Hoyos, Rafael, Halsey Rogers, and Miguel Székely. 2016. Out of School and Out of Work: Risk and Opportunities for Latin America’s Ninis. Washington, DC: World Bank. [Google Scholar]
  20. Delis, Dean C., Edith Kaplan, and Joel H. Kramer. 2001. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D–KEFS). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Eurofound. 2012. NEETs—Young People Not in Employment, Education or Training: Characteristics, Costs and Policy Responses in Europe. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union. [Google Scholar]
  22. Eurofound. 2016. Exploring the Diversity of NEETs. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union. [Google Scholar]
  23. Feng, Zhiqiang, Dawn Everington, Kevin Ralston, Chris Dibben, Gillian Raab, and Elspeth Graham. 2015. Consequences, Risk Factors, and Geography of Young People Not in Education, Employment or Training (NEET); Edinburgh: Scottish Government, p. 88. Available online: https://dera.ioe.ac.uk/24628/1/00487865.pdf (accessed on 8 June 2021).
  24. Fernández-Suárez, Asunción, Juan Herrero, Beatriz Pérez, Joel Juarros-Basterretxea, and Francisco J. Rodríguez-Díaz. 2016. Risk factors for school dropout in a sample of juvenile offenders. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Fort, Isabelle, Flora Jacquet, and Naïs Leroy. 2011. Self-efficacy, goals, and job search behaviors. Career Development International 16: 469–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Furlong, Andy. 2006. Not a very NEET solution: Representing problematic labour market transitions among early school-leavers. Work, Employment and Society 20: 553–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Gladwell, Daniel, Gurleen Popli, and Aki Tsuchiya. 2015. A Dynamic Analysis of Skill Formation and NEET Status. Working Papers 2015016. Sheffield: Department of Economics, The University of Sheffield, Available online: http://www.sheffield.ac.uk/economics/research/serps/articles/2015_016 (accessed on 9 June 2021).
  28. Goldberg, David P., and Valerie F. Hillier. 1979. A scaled version of the General Health Questionnaire. Psychological Medicine 9: 139–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Goldman-Mellor, Sidra, Avshalom Caspi, Louise Arseneault, Nifemi Ajala, Antony Ambler, Andrea Danese, Helen Fisher, Abigail Hucker, Candice Odgers, Teresa Williams, and et al. 2015. Committed to work but vulnerable: Self-perceptions and mental health in NEET 18-year olds from a contemporary British cohort. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 57: 196–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Goodchild, Margaret E., and Paul Duncan-Jones. 1985. Chronicity and the General Health Questionnaire. British Journal of Psychiatry 146: 55–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Gratz, Kim L., and Lizabeth Roemer. 2004. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 26: 41–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Guerra-Carrillo, Belén, Kiefer Katovich, and Silvia A. Bunge. 2017. Does higher education hone cognitive functioning and learning efficacy? findings from a large and diverse sample. PLoS ONE 12: e0182276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Gutiérrez-García, Raúl A., Corina Benjet, Guilherme Borges, Enrique Méndez Ríos, and María Elena Medina-Mora. 2017. NEET adolescents grown up: Eight-year longitudinal follow-up of education, employment and mental health from adolescence to early adulthood in Mexico City. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 26: 1459–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hahn, Elisabeth, David Richter, Jürgen Schupp, Mitja D. Back, Brent Donnellan, and Beth Visser. 2019. Predictors of refugee adjustment: The importance of cognitive skills and personality. Collabra: Psychology 5: 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hameed, Sameena, Asad Sadiq, and Amad U. Din. 2018. The increased vulnerability of refugee population to mental health disorders. Kansas Journal of Medicine 11: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Hergenrather, Kenneth C., Diona Emmanuel, Maureen McGuire-Kuletz, and Scott D. Rhodes. 2018. Employment as a social determinant of health: Exploring the relationship between neurocognitive function and employment status. Rehabilitation Research, Policy, and Education 32: 101–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Herrero, María Jesús, Jordi Blanch, Joseph María Peri, Joan De Pablo, Luis Pintor, and Antonio Bulbena. 2003. A validation study of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) in a Spanish population. General Hospital Psychiatry 25: 277–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hervás, Gonzalo, and Rafael Jódar. 2008. Adaptación al castellano de la Escala de Dificultades en la Regulación Emocional. Clínica y Salud 19: 139–56. [Google Scholar]
  39. Hjorth, Cathrine F., Line Bilgrav, Louise Sjørslev Frandsen, Charlotte Overgaard, Christian Torp-Pedersen, Berit Nielsen, and Henrik Bøggild. 2016. Mental health and school dropout across educational levels and genders: A 4.8-year follow-up study. BMC Public Health 16: 976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Hutchinson, Jo, Vanessa Beck, and Tristram Hooley. 2016. Delivering NEET policy packages? A decade of NEET policy in England. Journal of Education and Work 29: 707–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Kim, Hae-Young. 2013. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: Assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restorative Dentistry & Endodontics 38: 52–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kvieskienė, Giedrė, Ilze Ivanova, Karmen Trasberg, Viktorija Stasytytė, and Eglė Celiešienė. 2021. Modelling of social policy and initiatives under COVID-19: Rural NEET youth case study. Social Sciences 10: 393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Lobo, Antonio, Maria Jesús Pérez-Echeverría, and Jesus Artal. 1986. Validity of the scaled version of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-28) in a Spanish population. Psychological Medicine 16: 135–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Lövdén, Martin, Laura Fratiglioni, M. Maria Glymour, Ulman Lindenberger, and Elliot M. Tucker-Drob. 2020. Education and cognitive functioning across the life span. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 21: 6–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Mawn, Lauren, Emily J. Oliver, Nasima Akhter, Clare L. Bambra, Carole Torgerson, Chris Bridle, and Helen J. Stain. 2017. Are we failing young people not in employment, education or training (NEETs)? A systematic review and meta-analysis of re-engagement interventions. Systematic Reviews 6: 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. McGurk, Susan R., and Kim T. Mueser. 2006. Cognitive and clinical predictors of work outcomes in clients with schizophrenia receiving supported employment services: 4-year follow-up. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research 33: 598–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Minujín, Alberto, Diego Born, María Laura Lombardía, and Enrique Delamónica. 2016. Unpacking the NEETs of Latin America and the Caribbean: Methodological challenges and surprising results. In Child Poverty, Youth (Un)Employment, and Social Inclusion. Edited by Maria Petmesidou, Enrique Delamónica, Christos Papatheodorou and Aldrie Henry-Lee. Stuttgart: Ibidem Press, pp. 121–56. [Google Scholar]
  48. Miyake, Akira, Naomi P. Friedman, Michael J. Emerson, Alexander H. Witzki, Amy Howerter, and Tor D. Wager. 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41: 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Modini, Matthew, Sadhbh Joyce, Arnstein Mykletun, Helen Christensen, Richard A. Bryant, Philip B. Mitchell, and Samuel B. Harvey. 2016. The mental health benefits of employment: Results of a systematic meta-review. Australasian Psychiatry 24: 331–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Nafilyan, Vahé, and Stefan Speckesser. 2014. Estimating the Long-Term Social Benefits of a Programme Aiming to Re-Engage NEETs in Education. Brighton: Institute for Employment Studies, Available online: https://www.sheffield.ac.uk/polopoly_fs/1.679096!/file/2014_B1_2_Nafilyan.pdf (accessed on 7 June 2021).
  51. Obeso, Ignacio, Leonora Wilkinson, Enrique Casabona, Maria Luisa Bringas, Mario Alvarez, Lázaro Alvarez, Nancy Pavón, Maria-Cruz Rodríguez-Oroz, Raúl Macías, Jose A. Obeso, and et al. 2011. Deficits in inhibitory control and conflict resolution on cognitive and motor tasks in Parkinson’s disease. Experimental Brain Research 212: 371–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. O’Dea, Bridianne, Nicholas Glozier, Rosemary Purcell, Patrick D. McGorry, Jan Scott, Kristy-Lee Feilds, Daniel F. Hermens, John Buchanan, Elizabeth M Scott, Alison R Yung, and et al. 2014. A cross-sectional exploration of the clinical characteristics of disengaged (NEET) young people in primary mental healthcare. BMJ Open 4: e006378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. O’Higgins, Niall. 2020. Young People Not in Employment, Education or Training, ILO/SIDA Partnership on Employment. ILO/SIDA Technical Brief no. 3. Geneva: International Labour Organization. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Olivieri, Sergio, Francesc Ortega, Ana Rivadeneira, and Eliana Carranza. 2020. The Labor Market Effects of Venezuelan Migration in Ecuador. Policy Research Working Paper; No. 9336. Washington, DC: World Bank, Available online: https://openknowledge.worldbank.org/handle/10986/34254 (accessed on 8 June 2021).
  55. Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development. 2021. Youth Not in Employment, Education or Training (NEET) (Indicator). Paris: OECD. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development, Development Bank of Latin America, and Economic Commission for Latin America and the Caribbean. 2016. Latin American Economic Outlook 2017 Youth, Skills and Entrepreneurship. Available online: https://www.oecd.org/dev/americas/Overview_LEO2017.pdf (accessed on 11 June 2021).
  57. Ormel, Johan, Maarten W. J. Koeter, and Wim Van den Brink. 1989. Measuring change with the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ). The problem of retest effects. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 24: 227–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Pallant, Julie F., and Alan Tennant. 2007. An introduction to the Rasch measurement model: An example using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS). British Journal of Clinical Psychology 46: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Parisi, Jeanine M., George W. Rebok, Qian-Li Xue, Linda P. Fried, Teresa E. Seeman, Elizabeth K. Tanner, Tara L. Gruenewald, Kevin D. Frick, and Michelle C. Carlson. 2012. The role of education and intellectual activity on cognition. Journal of Aging Research 2012: 416132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Pastoor, Lutine De Wal. 2017. Reconceptualising refugee education: Exploring the diverse learning contexts of unaccompanied young refugees upon resettlement. Intercultural Education 28: 143–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Perez García, Ana María. 1984. Dimensionalidad del constructo ‘Locus of Control’. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada 39: 471–88. [Google Scholar]
  62. Pérez-Pérez, Alicia, Jordi A. Matias-Guiu, Iris Cáceres-Guillén, Teresa Rognoni, María Valles-Salgado, Marta Fernández-Matarrubia, Teresa Moreno-Ramos, and Jorge Matías-Guiu. 2016. The Hayling Test: Development and normalization of the spanish version. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 31: 411–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Peterson, Cynthia, Kalpana Poudel-Tandukar, Kirk Sanger, and Cynthia S. Jacelon. 2020. Improving mental health in refugee populations: A review of intervention studies conducted in the United States. Issues in Mental Health Nursing 41: 271–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Pluck, Graham, Brittany M. Barajas, José L. Hernandez-Rodriguez, and María A. Martínez. 2020a. Language ability and adult homelessness. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 55: 332–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Pluck, Graham, Carlos B. Ruales-Chieruzzi, Edgar J. Paucar-Guerra, María Victoria Andrade-Guimaraes, and Ana F. Trueba. 2016. Separate contributions of general intelligence and right prefrontal neurocognitive functions to academic achievement at university level. Trends in Neuroscience and Education 5: 178–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Pluck, Graham, Cristina Crespo-Andrade, Patricia Parreño, Karla I. Haro, María A. Martínez, and Sarahí C. Pontón. 2020b. Executive functions and intelligent goal-directed behavior: A neuropsychological approach to understanding success using professional sales as a real-life measure. Psychology & Neuroscience 13: 158–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Pluck, Graham, David Villagomez-Pacheco, María Isabel Karolys, María Emilia Montaño-Córdova, and Pamela Almeida-Meza. 2019. Response suppression, strategy application, and working memory in the prediction of academic performance and classroom misbehavior: A neuropsychological approach. Trends in Neuroscience and Education 17: 100121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Pluck, Graham, Marco A. Córdova, Christine Bock, Izan Chalen, and Ana F. Trueba. 2021. Socioeconomic status, executive functions, and theory of mind ability in adolescents: Relationships with language ability and cortisol. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 39: 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Pluck, Graham, Patricia Bravo Mancero, Paola Alexandra Ortíz Encalada, Angélica María Urquizo Alcívar, Claudio E. Maldonado Gavilanez, and Paola Chacon. 2020c. Differential associations of neurobehavioral traits and cognitive ability to academic achievement in higher education. Trends in Neuroscience and Education 18: 100124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Power, Emmet, Mary Clarke, Ian Kelleher, Helen Coughlan, Fionnuala Lynch, Dearbhla Connor, Carol Fitzpatrick, Michelle Harley, and Mary Cannon. 2015. The association between economic inactivity and mental health among young people: A longitudinal study of young adults who are not in employment, education or training. Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine 32: 155–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Rabkin, Judith G., Martin McElhiney, Stephen J. Ferrando, Wilfred Van Gorp, and Shu Hsing Lin. 2004. Predictors of employment of men with HIV/AIDS: A longitudinal study. Psychosomatic Medicine 66: 72–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Reivan-Ortiz, Geovanny Genaro, Patricia Elizabeth Ortiz Rodas, and Patricia Natali Reivan Ortiz. 2020. A brief version of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS): Validity evidence in Ecuadorian population. International Journal of Psychological Research 13: 14–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Robert, Sarah, Lucile Romanello, Sophie Lesieur, Virginie Kergoat, Joël Dutertre, Gladys Ibanez, and Pierre Chauvin. 2019. Effects of a systematically offered social and preventive medicine consultation on training and health attitudes of young people not in employment, education or training (NEETs): An interventional study in France. PLoS ONE 14: e0216226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Rosenberg, Morris. 1965. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  75. Rotter, Julian B. 1966. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied 80: 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Schwarzer, Ralf, and Matthias Jerusalem. 1995. Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale. In Measures in Health Psychology: A User’s Portfolio. Causal and Control Beliefs. Edited by Marie Johnston, Stephen Wright and John Weinman. Winsor: NFER-Nelson, pp. 35–37. [Google Scholar]
  77. Seddon, Fred, Richard Hazenberg, and Simon Denny. 2013. Effects of an employment enhancement programme on participant NEETs. Journal of Youth Studies 16: 503–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Serracant, Pau. 2014. A brute indicator for a NEET case: Genesis and evolution of a problematic concept and results from an alternative indicator. Social Indicators Research 117: 401–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Stamenova, Vessela, and Brian Levine. 2018. Effectiveness of goal management training® in improving executive functions: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 29: 1569–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Tamesberger, Dennis, and Johann Bacher. 2014. NEET youth in Austria: A typology including socio-demography, labour market behaviour and permanence. Journal of Youth Studies 17: 1239–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Tornarolli, Leopoldo. 2016. El Fenómeno de los Ninis en América Latina. CAF—Documento de Trabajo; N° 2016/18. Buenos Aires: CAF, December 30, Available online: http://scioteca.caf.com:8080/xmluicaf/handle/123456789/988 (accessed on 9 June 2021).
  82. Torres, Belén García. 1983. Análisis y Delimitación del Constructo Autoestima. Doctoral thesis, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain. Available online: https://eprints.ucm.es/id/eprint/52859/ (accessed on 8 June 2021).
  83. United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. 2015. Worldwide Displacement Hits All-Time High as War and Persecution Increase. June. Available online: http://www.unhcr.org/558193896.html (accessed on 9 June 2021).
  84. United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. 2016. Global Trends: Forced Displacement in 2015. Available online: https://www.refworld.org/docid/57678f3d4.html (accessed on 9 June 2021).
  85. United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. 2020. Ecuador [Fact Sheet]. Available online: https://www.refworld.org.es/docid/5f691c434.html (accessed on 9 June 2021).
  86. United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. 2021. Venezuela Situation. Retrieved May 9. Available online: https://www.unhcr.org/venezuela-emergency.html (accessed on 8 June 2021).
  87. Vance, David E., Jennifer Bail, Comfort C. Enah, Jennifer J. Palmer, and Anna K. Hoenig. 2016. The impact of employment on cognition and cognitive reserve: Implications across diseases and aging. Nursing: Research and Reviews 6: 61–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Xu, Hanzhang, Allison A. Vorderstrasse, Eleanor S. McConnell, Matthew E. Dupre, Truls Østbye, and Bei Wu. 2018. Migration and cognitive function: A conceptual framework for global health research. Global Health Research and Policy 3: 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Zigmond, Anthony S., and Richard Philip Snaith. 1983. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 67: 361–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Table 1. Module contents and descriptions of the intervention.
Table 1. Module contents and descriptions of the intervention.
ModuleTopicsNumber of Hours
Industrial quality and productivityQuality, safety, use of equipment, basic tools, and the 5 S’s principle.15
Industrial techniques Knowledge in the field of industrial technology to be able to solve basic problems.15
Sales and customer service Techniques to optimize sales, sales process, verbal, and non-verbal communication techniques.15
Sports for development Gamification using sports to develop communication, discipline, respect, teamwork and focus on results 80
Audiovisual production Production of animated shorts and mini documentaries about the program.16
Financial education Financial education for small business: accounting, financial statements, tax, and cash flow, NPV, IRR and other investment rules in all their applications.15
Services for hotels and restaurants New cooking techniques and methods, preparation of a “mise en place”, manipulation of raw materials.15
Entrepreneurship Business Model Canvas as a tool for the beneficiaries to evaluate business proposals and develop the added value of their venture.15
Communication Personal communication and organizational communication (managing social networks for a business, skills in their verbal and non-verbal communication). 15
Psychology Activities focused on self-development, self-esteem, personal skills, introspection, resilience, limiting beliefs, how to stablish healthy limits and take care of oneself. 25
Job search skills Preparation for job interviews, including non-verbal language, clothing, tone, and intonation of the voice. Guidance on design and updating of curriculum vitae. 9
5 S’s: Sort, Set in order, Shine, Standardize, Sustain. NPV: Net present value, IRR: Internal rate of return.
Table 2. Descriptive and inferential statistics comparing the national and migrant participants for demographic and psychological factors (pre-intervention).
Table 2. Descriptive and inferential statistics comparing the national and migrant participants for demographic and psychological factors (pre-intervention).
NationalMigrantSig.Effect Size
Age23.21 (5.00)24.97 (4.16)0.1750.41
Women8/14 (57%)22/35 (63%)0.4770.053 a
Mestizo10/12 (83%)28/32 (88%)0.5290.054 a
GHQ-289.14 (5.26)9.87 (5.75)0.8360.13
HADS10.57 (5.65)12.94 (5.65)0.1960.43
DERS84.79 (17.08)72.48 (16.87)0.0250.74
RSES30.64 (4.36)34.62 (4.50)0.0050.91
GSE21.00 (5.68)24.12 (4.74)0.0580.63
LoC9.86 (2.54)7.41 (2.75)0.0060.93
Trails2.16 (0.50)2.04 (0.61)0.2830.21
Hayling3.64 (3.84)3.60 (3.18)0.2060.01
Counting Span5.43 (1.15)5.57 (1.17)0.6700.12
a = effect size given as Cramer’s V, all other effect sizes are Cohen’s d. GHQ-28 = General Health Questionnaire-28 total score, HADS = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale total score, DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale total score, RSES = Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale total score, GSE = General Self-Efficacy Scale total score, LoC = Locus of Control total score, Trails = Trail Making Test, ratio of switching trial to non-switching trial completion times, Hayling = Hayling Sentence Completion Test (Part B) error score.
Table 3. Descriptive and inferential statistics for the pre-post comparison for all assessments.
Table 3. Descriptive and inferential statistics for the pre-post comparison for all assessments.
MeasurenMean Pre-Score (+SD)Mean Post-Score (+SD)Sig.Effect Size d
GHQ-28449.70 (5.58)5.91 (5.69)<0.0010.68
HADS4612.52 (5.75)8.93 (5.28)<0.0010.66
DERS4776.37 (17.75)69.91 (19.32)0.0020.35
RSES4833.19 (4.73)34.48 (5.23)0.0440.26
GSE4723.21 (5.23)25.19 (4.48)0.0050.41
LoC488.25 (2.90)7.62 (3.22)0.0990.21
Trails422.03 (0.50)2.12 (0.66)0.5660.16
Hayling423.41 (3.08)1.81 (2.04)0.0020.62
Counting Span425.62 (1.19)5.57 (1.15)0.7360.04
GHQ-28 = General Health Questionnaire-28 total score, HADS = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale total score, DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale total score, RSES = Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale total score, GSE = General Self-Efficacy Scale total score, LoC = Locus of Control total score, Trails = Trail Making Test, ratio of switching trial to non-switching trial completion times, Hayling = Hayling Sentence Completion Test (Part B) error score.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Crespo-Andrade, C.; Trueba, A.F.; Garcés, M.S.; Pluck, G. Multicomponent Intervention Associated with Improved Emotional and Cognitive Outcomes of Marginalized Unemployed Youth of Latin America. Soc. Sci. 2022, 11, 155. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci11040155

AMA Style

Crespo-Andrade C, Trueba AF, Garcés MS, Pluck G. Multicomponent Intervention Associated with Improved Emotional and Cognitive Outcomes of Marginalized Unemployed Youth of Latin America. Social Sciences. 2022; 11(4):155. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci11040155

Chicago/Turabian Style

Crespo-Andrade, Cristina, Ana Francisca Trueba, María Sol Garcés, and Graham Pluck. 2022. "Multicomponent Intervention Associated with Improved Emotional and Cognitive Outcomes of Marginalized Unemployed Youth of Latin America" Social Sciences 11, no. 4: 155. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci11040155

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop