An Analysis of Audio Information Streaming in Georg Philipp Telemann’s Sonata in C Major for Recorder and Basso Continuo, Allegro (TWV 41:C2)
Abstract
1. Introduction
- Receiving sound stimuli from the environment;
- Converting incoming sounds into neural impulses within the hearing organ;
- Encoding the physical characteristics of the received acoustic waves;
- Transmitting neural impulses to the auditory centres located in the cerebral cortex.
2. Terminology Basics
- The listener’s attention and focus;
- The particular musical information on which the listener is focused at any given moment;
- The division of perceived sounds into two or more layers, such as the main melody and harmony (i.e., accompanying or background sounds);
- The presence of masking sounds that may distract the listener’s attention, potentially causing the main figure in the music to become background or foreground;
- Shifts of attention towards different perceptual elements in the music, whereby similarity between features may result in the listener’s focus switching and the formation of entirely new perceptual shapes.
3. Equipment Used During the Listening Sessions
- A desktop computer with a Windows 10 64-bit operating system. The computer was assembled from the following parts:
- ◦
- Processor: AMD Ryzen Threadripper 2990WX, 32 × 3.0 GHz, 64 threads;
- ◦
- Motherboard: AsRock X399 Taichi;
- ◦
- RAM: G.Skill TridentZ, 64 GB, DDR4, 3200 MHz, CL 14;
- ◦
- Power supply: be quiet! Power Zone, 1000 W;
- ◦
- Graphics card: Sapphire Radeon RX 590 NITRO+, 8 GB, GDDR5.
- A pair of studio monitors: Sveda Audio Dapo V2, in a d’Appolito configuration, in which the tweeter is placed centrally between a pair of parallel, identical mid-bass speakers with twin audio crossovers (Holmes 2006; Self 2018; Verdults 2019).
- A subwoofer: Sveda Audio Wombat 15C V2, with a 15-inch aluminium-alloy diaphragm, tuned to the Sveda Audio Dapo V2 studio monitors.
- Audio interface (serving as the sound card): Steinberg UR824.
- Cables: Klotz MC5000.
- Plugs: Neutrik NC3MXX and NC3FXX.
4. Listening Method, Listening Sessions, and Audio Material
- The diverse acoustics of different concert halls;
- The sonorous inconsistency of various instruments;
- Differences present in each performance and interpretation of musical works.
5. Analysis of Georg Philipp Telemann’s Sonata in C Major for Recorder and Basso Continuo, TWV 41:C2, Allegro
- The first note of each sequence is the most accented (Fletcher and Munson 1937; Rogers and Bregman 1998);
- The rapid repetition of G5 strongly segregates the sound material;
- The quick succession of G5 notes makes it easier for the listener to distinguish the notes of different pitches.
- A greater diversity of tones with dissimilar pitches is required for the sequence to split into two perceptual streams.
- In these measures, there is a constant continuation of the melodic line (at times descending, at others ascending), yet the listener’s mind still combines the incoming stimuli into a single stream (Jackendoff and Lerdahl 2006; Ockelford 2004; Palmer 2002; Shepard and Levitin 2002).
- The larger intervals that occur between the tones are incidental in these measures; therefore, a second stream cannot form in the listener’s mind, as there are no sounds that would continue the pattern necessary for the formation of a perceptual stream.
- The composer intertwines measures in which the formation of two perceptual streams occurs with those in which only one stream is present. This can produce intriguing perceptual impressions in the listener’s mind.
- Several different types of sound segregation into two streams can be observed, even though the musical material itself is not particularly complex.
- It demonstrates how a melody within a single voice can be constructed—using specific compositional techniques—to deliberately cause either the “tearing apart” of sounds (segregation into two perceptual streams) or their “integration” into a single perceptual stream.
- This material serves as an excellent example for illustrating psychoacoustic phenomena related to auditory scene analysis, namely, the segregation and integration of sounds. It may provide listeners with a fundamental understanding of this perceptual phenomenon, making it a valuable educational resource for students or individuals undergoing psychoacoustic tests in the future.
6. Discussion of Results
7. Conclusions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Baker, Kevin L., Sheila M. Williams, and Roderick I. Nicolson. 2000. Evaluating Frequency Proximity in Stream Segregation. Perception & Psychophysics 62: 81–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barlach, Laura. 2021. The Mindset of Innovation: Contributions of Cognitive and Social Psychology. Journal of Psychological Research 3: 16–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bregman, Albert S. 1990. Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bregman, Albert S. 2002. The auditory scene. In Foundations of Cognitive Psychology. Edited by Daniel J. Levitin. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 213–48. [Google Scholar]
- Bregman, Albert S. 2004. Auditory Scene Analysis. In International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences. Edited by Neil J. Smelzer and Paul B. Baltes. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 940–42. [Google Scholar]
- Bregman, Albert S., and Pierre A. Ahad. 1996. Demonstrations of Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Available online: http://webpages.mcgill.ca/staff/Group2/abregm1/web/downloadstoc.htm (accessed on 12 April 2025).
- Bregman, Albert S., Connie Colantonio, and Pierre A. Ahad. 1999. Is a common grouping mechanism involved in the phenomena of illusory continuity and stream segregation? Perception & Psychophysics 61: 195–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chartrand, Jan-Pierre, and Pascal Belin. 2006. Superior voice timbre processing in musicians. Neuroscience Letters 405: 164–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, Dalia, and Shlomo Dubnov. 1997. Gestalt phenomena in musical texture. In Music, Gestalt, and Computing. Studies in Cognitive and Systematic Musicology. Edited by Marc Leman. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 386–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Collection Musicale en Format Numérique. n.d. Georg Philipp Telemann—Sonate C-Dur. Available online: https://s9.imslp.org/files/imglnks/usimg/1/10/IMSLP330401-PMLP41848-Telemann_GP_-_Sonate_C-Dur,_TWV_41,C2_-_EN2014-105.PDF (accessed on 25 January 2025).
- Deutsch, Diana. 1974. An Auditory Illusion. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 55: 18–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deutsch, Diana. 1975. Musical Illusions. Scientific American 233: 92–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deutsch, Diana. 1983. The Octave Illusion in Relation to Handedness and Familial Handedness Background. Neuropsychologia 21: 290–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deutsch, Diana. 1992. Paradoxes of Musical Pitch. Scientific American 267: 88–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fine, Philip A., and Brian C. J. Moore. 1993. Frequency analysis and musical ability. Music Perception 11: 39–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fletcher, Harvey, and Wilden A. Munson. 1937. Relation between loudness and masking. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 9: 78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holmes, Thom. 2006. The Routledge Guide to Music Technology. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humięcka-Jakubowska, Justyna. 2006. Scena Słuchowa Muzyki Dwudziestowiecznej. Poznań: Rhythmos. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff, Ray, and Fred Lerdahl. 2006. The Capacity for Music: What is it, and What’s Special About it? Cognition 100: 33–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacobson, Loma S., Lola L. Cuddy, and Andera R. Kilgour. 2003. Time tagging: A key to musician’s superior memory. Music Perception 20: 307–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jain, Saransh, Nuggehalli Puttareviyah Nataraja, and Vijaya Kumar Narne. 2022. The Effect of Subjective Fatigue on Auditory Processing in Musicians and Nonmusicians. Music Perception 39: 309–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kilian, Marlena, and Małgorzata Cichocka. 2011. Muzykoterapia w rehabilitacji dzieci niewidomych i słabo widzących—Założenia teoretyczne (część 1). Szkoła Specjalna 4: 245–57. [Google Scholar]
- Kraus, Nina, and Bharath Chandrasekaran. 2010. Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11: 599–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leclerc, Charles, Dave Saint-Amour, Marc E. Lavoie, Maryse Lassonde, and Franco Lepore. 2000. Brain functional reorganization in early blind humans revealed by auditory event-related potentials. NeuroReport 11: 545–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGookin, David K., and Stephen A. Brewster. 2004. Understanding concurrent earcons: Applying auditory scene analysis principles to concurrent earcon recognition. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception 1: 130–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meyer, Martin, Stefan Elmer, Maya Ringli, Mathias S. Oechslin, Simon Baumann, and Lutz Jancke. 2011. Long-term exposure to music enhances the sensitivity of the auditory system in children. European Journal of Neuroscience 34: 755–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Micheyl, Christophe, Robert P. Carlyon, Rhodri Cusack, and Brian C. J. Moore. 2005. Performance measures of auditory organization. In Physiology, Psychoacoustics, and Models. Edited by Daniel Pressnitzer, Alain de Cheveigne, Stephen McAdams and Lionel Collet. New York: Springer, pp. 202–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mizgalski, Gerard. 1959. Podręczna Encyklopedia Muzyki Kościelnej. Poznań, Warszawa and Lublin: Księgarnia Św. Wojciecha. [Google Scholar]
- Moore, Brian C. J., and Hedwig E. Gockel. 2012. Properties of auditory stream formation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 367: 919–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nęcka, Edward, Jarosław Orzechowski, and Błażej Szymura. 2006. Psychologia Poznawcza. Warszawa: Akademica Wydawnictwo SWPS/PWN. [Google Scholar]
- Ockelford, Adam. 2004. On Similarity, Derivation and The Cognition of Musical Structure. Psychology of Music 32: 23–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oxenham, Andrew J., Brian J. Fligor, Christine R. Mason, and Gerald Kidd, Jr. 2003. Informational masking and musical training. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 114: 1543–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Palmer, Stephen E. 2002. Organizing objects and scenes. In Foundations of Cognitive Psychology. Edited by Daniel J. Levitin. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 189–211. [Google Scholar]
- Pantev, Christo, and Sybille C. Herholz. 2011. Plasticity of the human auditory cortex related to musical training. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35: 2140–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parbery-Clark, Alexandra, Adam Tierney, Dana L. Strait, and Nina Kraus. 2012. Musicians have fine-tuned neural distinction of speech syllables. Neuroscience 219: 111–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parbery-Clark, Alexandra, Erika Skoe, and Nina Kraus. 2009a. Musical experience limits the degradative effects of background noise on the neural processing of sound. Journal of Neuroscience 29: 14100–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parbery-Clark, Alexandra, Erika Skoe, Carrie Lam, and Nina Kraus. 2009b. Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear and Hearing 30: 653–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rammsayer, Thomas, and Eckart Altenmüller. 2006. Temporal information processing in musicians and nonmusicians. Music Perception 24: 37–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rogers, Wendy L., and Albert S. Bregman. 1998. Cumulation of the tendency to segregate auditory streams: Resetting by changes in location and loudness. Perception & Psychophysics 60: 1216–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosiński, Adam. 2020. Perception of Sound via Auditory Image Analysis, Made in the Conceptual Context of the Philosophy of Music. In Humanitarian Corpus. Kyiv: M.P. Drahomanov National University of Pedagogy Academic Press, Vinnytsia: TVORY, Vol. 2, Issue 35. pp. 101–7. [Google Scholar]
- Rosiński, Adam. 2021. Wpływ wykształcenia muzycznego na grupowanie dźwięków sekwencji ABA-ABA w rytm galopujący. In Przestrzenie Akustyki. Professional Acoustics. Edited by Adam Rosiński. Olsztyn: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Warmińsko-Mazurskiego w Olsztynie, pp. 53–70. [Google Scholar]
- Rosiński, Adam. 2023. Influence of Music Education and Pitch Scales on the Grouping of the AB-AB Sequence Sounds. In Przestrzenie Akustyki. Professional Acoustics. Edited by Adam Rosiński. Olsztyn: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Warmińsko-Mazurskiego w Olsztynie, vol. 2, pp. 53–74. [Google Scholar]
- Schulze, Katrin, Stefan Zysset, Karsten Mueller, Angela D. Friederici, and Stefan Koelsch. 2011. Neuroarchitecture of verbal and tonal working memory in nonmusicians and musicians. Human Brain Mapping 32: 771–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Self, Douglas. 2018. The Design of Active Crossovers, 2nd ed. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shamma, Shihab A., and Christophe Micheyl. 2010. Behind the scenes of auditory perception. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 20: 361–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shamma, Shihab A., Mounya Elhilali, and Christophe Micheyl. 2011. Temporal coherence and attention in auditory scene analysis. Trends in Neurosciences 34: 114–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shepard, Roger N., and Daniel J. Levitin. 2002. Cognitive psychology and music. In Foundations of Cognitive Psychology. Edited by Daniel J. Levitin. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 503–14. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder, Joel S., Suh-Kyung Lee, Olivia L. Carter, Erin E. Hannon, and Claude Alain. 2008. Effects of context on auditory stream segregation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 34: 1007–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steiger, Howard, and Albert S. Bregman. 1982. Negating the effects of binaural cues: Competition between auditory streaming and contralateral induction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8: 602–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strait, Dana L., Alexandra Parbery-Clark, Emily Hittner, and Nina Kraus. 2012. Musical training during early childhood enhances the neural encoding of speech in noise. Brain & Language 123: 191–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Vlis, Bram. 2013. Semantic Connections. Explorations, Theory and a Framework for Design. Eindhoven: Technical University Eindhoven. Available online: https://pure.tue.nl/ws/portalfiles/portal/4040718/762658.pdf (accessed on 10 April 2025).
- Vecera, Shaun P. 2000. Toward a biased competition account of object-based segregation and attention. Brain and Mind 1: 353–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verdults, Vincent. 2019. Optimal Audio and Video Reproduction at Home: Improving the Listening and Viewing Experience. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winkler, István, Susan L. Denham, and Israel Nelken. 2009. Modeling the Auditory Scene: Predictive Regularity Representations and Perceptual Objects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 532–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Załazińska, Aneta. 2016. Obraz, Słowo, Gest. Kraków: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego. [Google Scholar]
Sound Source Location | Frequency Band [Hz] | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
63 | 125 | 250 | 500 | 1000 | 2000 | 4000 | 8000 | |
1 | 0.50 | 0.45 | 0.52 | 0.48 | 0.42 | 0.44 | 0.47 | 0.45 |
2 | 0.46 | 0.46 | 0.51 | 0.48 | 0.42 | 0.45 | 0.45 | 0.42 |
3 | 0.50 | 0.48 | 0.52 | 0.45 | 0.44 | 0.43 | 0.42 | 0.43 |
4 | 0.52 | 0.45 | 0.53 | 0.45 | 0.47 | 0.50 | 0.52 | 0.54 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Rosiński, A. An Analysis of Audio Information Streaming in Georg Philipp Telemann’s Sonata in C Major for Recorder and Basso Continuo, Allegro (TWV 41:C2). Arts 2025, 14, 76. https://doi.org/10.3390/arts14040076
Rosiński A. An Analysis of Audio Information Streaming in Georg Philipp Telemann’s Sonata in C Major for Recorder and Basso Continuo, Allegro (TWV 41:C2). Arts. 2025; 14(4):76. https://doi.org/10.3390/arts14040076
Chicago/Turabian StyleRosiński, Adam. 2025. "An Analysis of Audio Information Streaming in Georg Philipp Telemann’s Sonata in C Major for Recorder and Basso Continuo, Allegro (TWV 41:C2)" Arts 14, no. 4: 76. https://doi.org/10.3390/arts14040076
APA StyleRosiński, A. (2025). An Analysis of Audio Information Streaming in Georg Philipp Telemann’s Sonata in C Major for Recorder and Basso Continuo, Allegro (TWV 41:C2). Arts, 14(4), 76. https://doi.org/10.3390/arts14040076