Next Article in Journal
Quarries: From Abandoned to Renewed Places
Previous Article in Journal
Land Functions, Rural Space Governance, and Farmers’ Environmental Perceptions: A Case Study from the Huanjiang Karst Mountain Area, China
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Revisiting the Determinants of Pro-Environmental Behaviour to Inform Land Management Policy: A Meta-Analytic Structural Equation Model Application

1
Sustainability Research Institute, School of Earth and Environment, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
2
Rural Economy, Environment and Society Department, Scotland’s Rural College (SRUC), Edinburgh EH9 3JG, UK
3
School of Geography, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Land 2020, 9(5), 135; https://doi.org/10.3390/land9050135
Submission received: 27 March 2020 / Revised: 21 April 2020 / Accepted: 27 April 2020 / Published: 30 April 2020
(This article belongs to the Section Land Socio-Economic and Political Issues)

Abstract

:
Environmental policies in the realm of land management are increasingly focussing on inducing behavioural change to improve environmental management outcomes. This is based, implicitly or explicitly, on theories that suggest that pro-environmental behaviour can be understood, predicted and altered based on certain factors (referred to as determinants of pro-environmental behaviour). However, studies examining the determinants of pro-environmental behaviour have found mixed evidence. It is therefore important that we revisit these theories to assess if the evidence supports their postulations so that a more robust knowledge base can be established to inform land management policies. In this study, we do this using meta-analytic structural equation modelling (MASEM) to explore whether the evidence on determinants of pro-environmental behaviour supports the postulations of some predominantly applied theories of behaviour and/or behaviour change. The study analyses research in four environmental policy areas to identify implications for land management. Evidence from these related environmental areas is expected to provide insights relevant to the land management literature and to allow us to identify the extent to which lessons on pro-environmental behaviour from these other areas can be transferred to the land management context. Our findings suggest a strong evidence base for the Theories of Planned Behaviour and Reasoned Action, Attitude-Behaviour-Context Model, and the Persuasion Theory, but a weak evidence base for the Value-Belief-Norm Theory and the Norm Activation Model. We also found that type of environmental policy area moderates the relationship between different variables. This has key policy implications since, while lessons can be learnt from other environmental policy areas, land management policies aimed at influencing behaviours will need to be tailored to the specific context rather than simply ‘imported’ from other fields. Such context-specific policies may encourage pro-environmental behaviours, and potentially contribute towards improving environmental management outcomes.

1. Introduction

Policies across the environmental sector are increasingly focussing on behavioural change as a means to improve environmental performance [1,2]. Behaviour focussed approaches are particularly prominent in the realm of land management, where policies are increasingly aimed at influencing land managers to take up best management practices to improve water quality, soil and human health and prevent further biodiversity loss [3,4,5,6,7]. Examples of these policies include the National Landcare Programme established in Australia in 1988 [8], the Water Quality Scheme and the Environmental Quality Incentive Programme in the United States of America in 1997, the Monitor Farms Programme established in 1991 in New Zealand [9], the Catchment Sensitive Farming Delivery Initiative started in 2005 in England [10,11] and the Diffuse Pollution Management Strategy established in 2009 in Scotland [12]. These policies acknowledge the role of a wide range of factors in understanding and influencing land managers’ behaviours [4,9,13,14,15]. For instance, they place emphasis on land managers’ awareness and attitudes under the expectation that this may encourage pro-environmental practices [16].
Despite the increasing popularisation of such policies, studies examining the determinants of pro-environmental behaviour in land management have found mixed evidence on their impact (e.g., [17,18,19]). Arguments have been made regarding the limitations of the behavioural theories that (implicitly or explicitly) underpin these policies. For instance, the assumption that awareness of best management practices results in pro-environmental behaviour has been criticised for being deterministic, as this may not apply in every situation [9,16,20].
While these concerns are relevant and could help provide answers that may trigger new policy directions [21], there have been very few studies establishing whether existing evidence supports the postulations of the behavioural theories underpinning land management policies (i.e., policies guiding land use and management). One known study that attempts to establish this is the work of Bamberg and Moser [22]. However, while their study draws evidence from papers that rely on the Theory of Planned Behaviour and the Norm Activation Model, they do not test whether these theories were supported by the evidence or not; they tested a proposed integrative model of pro-environmental behaviour. While such an integrative approach provides a comprehensive understanding of the determinants of pro-environmental behaviour and behavioural change, it does not provide evidence specific to the theories. Additionally, although they acknowledged the potential influence of moderating factors, their study did not explore such factors. This study aims to establish whether the existing empirical evidence supports the predictions of (five) key theories of pro-environmental behaviour, with the particular purpose to improve the information base for land management policy. To do this, we use meta-analytic structural equation modelling (MASEM). The study analyses research in four environmental policy areas to identify implications for land management. Evidence from these related environmental areas is expected to provide insights relevant to the land management literature and to allow us to identify the extent to which lessons on pro-environmental behaviour from these other areas can be transferred to the land management context. We use the term “land management policies” to refer to policies that guide land related activities such as farming, forestry and woodland management. “Environmental management policies” on the other hand refer to policies that regulate all activities (such as recycling and farming) that could impact any aspects of the environment.
This work advances the literature in various ways. Firstly, unlike earlier meta-analytic reviews in the land management field that look only at the individual contributions of awareness, attitudes and situational factors (e.g., [16], this study covers the joint contributions of a larger range of variables (e.g., subjective norms, ascription of responsibility). MASEM allows us to test the mechanisms through which these different variables affect each other (i.e., it considers their mediated relationships) via correlations and multiple regressions [23]. Moreover, the evidence base that we explore goes beyond that of land management only, including other environmental sectors (e.g., recycling) as well as general ecological behaviours more broadly.

2. Theoretical Basis

In this section, we provide an overview of the five main behaviour theories: Persuasion Theory [24,25], Theories of Reasoned Action and Planned Behaviour [26,27], The Norm-Activation Model [28,29], the Value-Belief Norm Theory [30,31,32,33] and the Attitude-Behaviour-Context (ABC) Model [32]1. These theoretical frameworks cite environmental, personal and behavioural characteristics as the major factors in behaviour determination and are frequently applied by researchers trying to understand and explain pro-environmental behaviour [15,23,36,37]. In Figure 1, we present a theoretical framework that joins the various paths (e.g., knowledge-awareness link, awareness-behaviour link, knowledge-behaviour link) postulated in these theories. We use letters to represent the different pathways (e.g., knowledge-awareness link, awareness-behaviour link, knowledge-behaviour link) hypothesised in each of the theories. Therefore, the link between variables also show which determinants are related to the different theories.

2.1. Persuasion Theory

Persuasion Theory is one of the simplest models of behaviour, built on the idea of linear progression. Proponents of this model reasoned that behaviour change is realised through the provision of required information to the targeted actors [24]. It is argued that effective persuasion is contingent on three key elements, namely: the credibility or trustworthiness of the speaker (the source), power of the argument (the message) and the receptiveness of the audience (the recipient) [24]. In the context of environmental problems, the model assumes that the actors’ current behaviour is due to some lack of awareness/knowledge regarding the right strategies to mitigate environmental problems; consequently, this model has more recently been termed the ‘information deficit model’ [38]. It is also assumed that attitude and behavioural change can only happen when individuals access the required information and assimilate it. Based on these assumptions, the model prescribes information provision as the solution, contending that information provision results in awareness, which in turn leads to a change in attitude and behaviour.
The model has been criticised for being deterministic as it assumes that information provision is a guarantee for realising the desired behaviour [21,38]. This deterministic view (if one becomes aware, one behaves desirably), overlooks the role of other multiple intervening factors (e.g., economic, cultural, social pressure) that determine outcomes of interventions, amidst information provision [21,38,39,40]. Behaviour and behaviour change are complex, therefore a strategy based only on ‘information provision’ will struggle to induce behavioural change. The linear perspective of the model is also problematic. Though the information-attitude-behaviour pathway appears reasonable, individuals can learn without necessarily assimilating the ‘persuasive message’, and behavioural change can occur without attitudinal influences [25].
Despite its flaws, the model has made contributions in influencing pro-environmental behaviours since its key elements are still relevant in behaviour, particularly for land manager behaviour. For example, research shows that for farmers, the source of information (e.g., the farm advisor) and the persuasiveness of the message are indeed crucial (albeit not necessarily sufficient) in determining whether the farmer acts upon the advice, particularly where the message is consistent, well-placed and positive [6,41]. Using MASEM, Bamberg and Moser [22] showed that awareness affects attitudes towards pro-environmental behaviours, and this in turn affect behaviours (through intentions to act).

2.2. Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA) and the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB)

Proponents of the TRA reason that two factors directly affect people’s intentions to act: attitude and subjective norms (referred to as an individual’s perception regarding whether people who are important to him or her think if he should perform or not perform the behaviour at hand) and this in turn leads to the performance of the behaviour in question [26]. Closely related to the TRA is the TPB, which integrates the actor’s perceived control over the behaviour [27]. TPB looks at ‘intentions to behave’, and not necessarily actual actions of individuals, and argues that attitudes, subjective norms and perceived behavioural control affect intentions to act and these in turn predict behaviour.
Proponents of these theories acknowledge that the link between belief and behaviour is not straightforward; there are other elements that influence the relationship between the two. However, these models fail to provide clear understanding on the cognitive, normative and affective facets of human behaviour [21]. Additionally, even a positive intention does not necessarily result in pro-environmental behaviour, as this depends on situational factors such as financial ability, time, and available resources, among others [38]. These complex factors make it difficult to predict behaviours relying solely on intentions. However, Bamberg and Moser [22] found strong evidence in support of this model, after their MASEM revealed that indeed attitudes, perceived behavioural control and subjective norms predicted intentions, and intentions in turn predicted behaviours.

2.3. Norm-Activation Model

This model was developed to help explain the determinants of moral behaviours: pro-social and altruistic behaviours among people. It is premised on the idea that awareness of the consequences of one’s behaviour and ascription of responsibility to self are the main factors that determine personal norm, and this in turn determines whether individuals behave altruistically or not [29,42]. Personal norms, i.e., feelings of moral obligations to carry out specific behaviours, are therefore viewed as direct antecedents of prosocial behaviours and Schwartz rejects the idea of intentions mediating the relationship between personal norm and behaviour. He argues that when people are aware of the consequences of (or not) engaging in specific prosocial behaviours and also ascribe such responsibilities to themselves, they are more likely to act positively. This is probably because personal norms serve as a benchmark for what an individual considers to be proper or improper and may in turn govern the person’s behaviour. Subsequently, it is hypothesised that awareness of consequences and ascription of responsibility are not just viewed as fundamental independent variables, but also, moderators of the relationship between personal norm and behaviour [21]. This model however fails to provide insights into how personal norms are derived from the larger social fabric and how socially established measures could drive personal norms and behaviours.

2.4. Value-Belief-Norm Theory

The Value-Belief-Norm Theory was proposed as an attempt to modify the Norm Activation Model to reflect the complex connection between values, beliefs, attitudes and norms [30,31,32]. This theory suggests that values affect beliefs and this in turn affects personal norms, a direct antecedent of pro-environmental behaviour. In this model, Stern argues that endorsement of the new environmental paradigm (NEP) directly predicts awareness of consequences and the extent to which an individual accepts the NEP may reflect a high level of environmental value [30,31,32,33]. Subsequently, three major categories of pro-environmental behaviours are identified: environmental citizenship, support for policy and private sphere behaviours.
The Value-Belief-Norm Theory has made a substantial contribution to understanding environmental behaviour, particularly in understanding the key role of values, beliefs and norms in environmentally significant behaviours. Nonetheless, this theory has a major limitation as it overlooks the role of situational factors in actual behaviours [21]. The influence of situational factors (e.g., institutional support or constraint, availability or lack of facilities) in behaviour is key and perhaps explains why values explained less than 35% of variance in general environmental behaviour (and less than 20% in private sphere behaviours) in Stern et al.’s study [30]. Aside situational factors, it is also possible that environmental values and beliefs may perform better as moderators as opposed to mediators or direct independent variables in explaining behaviour [13,14,20,43].

2.5. Attitude-Behaviour-Context Theory

The Attitude-Behaviour-Context (ABC) models behaviour (B) as a complex interactive outcome of intrinsic or ‘internal’ attitudinal (A) variables and extrinsic or ‘external’ circumstantial elements (C) [32]. The strength of this model lies in the acknowledgement of the complex structural interaction or dynamics concerning the influence of attitudes (labelled as internal factors) and situational factors (also known as external or contextual factors). It is argued that where situational factors (e.g., monetary incentives and costs, institutional and legal support or constrains, time), play a weak role, the attitude-behaviour link is strong, however, the link is non-existent or weak at best, when situational factors exert a strong influence [21]. This suggests that situational factors moderate the relationship between attitude and behaviour hence, pro-environmental behaviour will be dynamic as situational factors change overtime and in different locations i.e., pro-environmental behaviour may be predisposed to temporal and spatial dynamics.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Research Design and Data Collection

The aim of the present study is to assess the empirical evidence base of the determinants of pro-environmental behaviour predicted by the theories described in Section 2. For this purpose, we adopted a meta-analytic structural equation modelling (MASEM) technique. The first step involved an online search of the literature focussing on the determinants of pro-environmental behaviour (see Figure 2 for an overview of the research design used in this study). We restricted our search to papers written in English and published after 1994, when application of theories to understand pro-environmental behaviour gained considerable momentum [22]. Keywords such as pro-environmental behaviour, pro-ecological behaviour, etc. were applied in multiple search queries to retrieve data from Google Scholar and other academic databases such as Web of Science, Scopus, Business Source Premier, PROQUEST’s ABI/INFORM Collection, the Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts (ASSIA) and Sociological Abstracts (see Figure 3 for the document selection process and Appendix A for keyword groupings). After applying a follow-up snowball search and removing duplicates, we obtained 387 papers.
Next, we scanned the titles, abstracts and skimmed through the methods sections of the 387 papers with the aim of keeping only papers that contained outputs of “empirical research” as opposed to “reviews”, “process-based modelling” or “conceptual papers”. After this, there was an intensive reading of methods and result sections of these papers with the aim of retaining only papers that analysed at least one of the relationships of interest e.g., attitude-behaviour link, attitude-intention link, intention-behaviour link (see Figure 1, see also Table 1 for definitions of the variables of interest) and also reported the correlation coefficients and sample sizes as these were needed for the meta-analysis. After this step, 109 papers were retained and used for the meta-analysis (see Appendix B for references of these papers). From Appendix B, it could be observed that the papers analysed cover a wide disciplinary spectrum and are published in a variety of journals. This wide disciplinary spectrum is a strength of the present study and a step towards advancing interdisciplinary approaches to understanding the complexity of pro-environmental behaviour.
As mentioned in Section 1, this study covers a broader range of areas in which pro-environmental behaviour has been studied, beyond just land management. All 109 studies were then grouped under these (four) environmental policy areas based on the focus of the paper: recycling, land management, sustainable consumer behaviour and general ecological behaviour (see Table 2).

3.2. Implementing the MASEM

Meta-analysis is a widely accepted technique for the synthesis of evidence [46]. It is appropriate for this study, since our goal is to establish the quantitative empirical evidence base of the relationships of interest from studies sharing similar hypotheses [46,47,48,49]. In this case, it involved pooling the correlation coefficients and samples from the primary studies. Using Hedges and Olkin [50] technique, we calculated the pooled mean correlations and associated p-values of the relationships of interest e.g., the attitude-behaviour link, attitude-intention link, intention-behaviour link. This was done through the application of the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (Version 2) software. Publication bias was assessed using funnel plots [51] which indicated a general absence of publication bias, except for the relationship between intention and environmental concern, which might be biased by not reporting small trials or studies that show non-significant results. We interpreted our results under the random-effects assumption, as heterogeneity test results indicated strong between-study heterogeneity of the pooled correlation matrix2 [51]. The random-effect model is an extension of the fixed-effect model where variations in the mean effect across studies are attributed to factors such as differences in location, experimental conditions, and variable type, among others [49,50,52,53].
In an analysis such as this, where the evidence is pooled from multiple sources, it is important to assess the extent to which the statistical significance of the relationships are affected by moderators, i.e., by an additional factor that might contingently impact the statistical significance, direction and/or magnitude of the relationship [54]. Because we used a broad evidence base from different types of environmental behaviours, we tested for the moderating effects of type of environmental behaviour or focus of the primary research through sub-group analysis. Technically, applying sub-group analysis as a meta-regression has advantages as it focusses on differences between multiple groups as appropriate, as opposed to effects in each sub-group separately [55]. A detailed description of how sub-group analysis is applied is presented in [56]. The pooled mean correlation matrix was then fitted onto structural equation models (using AMOS 24) to test the behavioural theories. The structural equation models help to determine the regression weights and associated p-values of all the paths hypothesised in the respective theories (see Figure 1).

4. Results

4.1. Overview of the Evidence

Of the 109 papers studied, only seven focussed on environmentally responsible behaviours related to land management while the most, 52, focussed on sustainable consumer behaviour (Table 3). Table 4 presents the number of retrieved independent primary bivariate correlation coefficients and the respective combined total sample size on which these coefficients are based. This information shows that while some relationships have been commonly studied empirically (e.g., attitude–intention, self-efficacy–intention), others have received little attention (e.g., knowledge–emotion, social norm–ascription of responsibility) and some not studied at all (e.g., habit–behavioural willingness). Table 4 also shows that the awareness–habit link had the smallest (pooled) total sample size (N = 319) while the attitude–self-efficacy link had the largest sample size (N = 22,390).

4.2. Pooled Mean Correlation Analysis

Pooled mean correlations of the relationships analysed are presented in Table 5. The strongest association was found between awareness of problem and/or action strategies and perceived consequences of individual’s actions/inactions (r = 0.67; p < 0.01; N = 1479; 6 studies). This indicates that people who are aware of an environmental problem (e.g., diffuse pollution) and action strategies (e.g., best land management practices) are more likely to perceive associated consequences of (not) engaging in that practice. The second strongest relationship was found between moral norm and environmental attitude (r = 0.62; p < 0.01; N = 5415; 9 studies), indicating that individuals who feel a moral obligation to behave in an environmentally responsible manner are more likely to report a positive environmental attitude. On the contrary, the weakest (but still statistically significant) relationships were found between habit and environmental awareness (r = 0.02; p < 0.01; N = 319; 1 study) and between habit and attitude (r = 0.03; p < 0.01; N = 1597; 4 studies). There were a few associations (e.g., emotions-attitude) for which results indicated no significant relationship.
Results also indicate a strong between-study heterogeneity (Appendix D), which is partly attributable to the different environmental policy areas that the studies address (Table 6). For instance, while an overall correlation of r = 0.21 was recorded for the relationship between environmental awareness and behaviour, results varied between different environmental policy areas: it was slightly stronger (r = 0.23; p < 0.05) in sustainable consumer behaviour, slightly weaker (r = 0.20; p < 0.05) in general ecological behaviour, but it was non-significant (p > 0.05) in recycling and land management behaviours.

4.3. Meta-analytic Structural Equation Modelling

Results of the MASEM that were used to test the predicted paths of the theories under consideration (see Figure 4) are presented in Table 7. First, we evaluated the fit of the models using a combination of recommended fit indices. When model fit indices such as the Normed Fit Index and the Comparative Fit Index3 are greater than 0.95, it shows that the model fits the data very well [57,58]. Based on this criterion, all theories fitted the data with the exception of the Value-Belief-Norm Theory and Norm Activation Model. Results of the paths hypothesised by the behavioural theories for which we obtained acceptable fits (Table 7) are presented in Table 8. They indicate that almost all predictions of the theories are statistically significant with the exception of the knowledge-awareness pathway for Persuasion Theory and the subjective norm-intention link for Theories of Reasoned Action and Planned Behaviour.
Results in Table 8 further indicate that the evidence supports the postulation that awareness affects attitude (56% of the variance explained) and attitude in turn affects pro-environmental behaviour (49% of the variance explained) in line with the Persuasion Theory. Also, the results support the hypotheses of the Theory of Reasoned Action: we found that attitude predicts intention (78% of variance explained) and intentions in turn predict behaviour (74% of variance explained). Furthermore, our results confirm the postulations of the Theory of Planned Behaviour as intentions mediate the relationship between factors such as attitude, subjective norms and perceived behaviour control (referred to as self-efficacy in the present study) and pro-environmental behaviour (76% of variance explained). Overall, there is strong evidence from the MASEM supporting the majority of the theoretical frameworks under consideration.

5. Discussion

The results of our meta-analysis indicate that almost all the pooled correlations between variables were significant but the strength of the relationships are mainly weak to moderate. The strongest association was found between awareness (of problem and/or action strategies) and perceived consequences of individual’s actions. This finding is unsurprising, as people who are conscious of an environmental problem (e.g., diffuse water pollution from agriculture) and best management practices are more likely to be concerned with the consequences of engaging in practices that could cause or induce such environmental problems [9,14,20]. Results from the MASEM show that most of the paths hypothesised in the Theories of Planned Behaviour and Reasoned Action, Attitude-Behaviour-Context Model, and the Persuasion Theory are supported by evidence from the literature. For instance, the link between attitudes and intentions, and intentions and behaviours (in the Theory of Planned Behaviour) are supported. This suggests that people with strong pro-environmental attitudes are more likely to have positive intentions to engage in pro-environmental behaviour, and this in turn can result in uptake of best management practices.
Although generally the evidence supports the theoretical frameworks, there were a few cases where results indicated non-significant relationships. For instance, the knowledge-awareness link predicted in the Persuasion Theory was non-significant. This implies that having access to knowledge or environmental management information does not automatically guarantee environmental awareness as this may depend on factors such as environmental concern, emotions, experience, information source, message framing and delivery [6,7,37]. Indeed, proponents of the Persuasion Theory acknowledge that the link between knowledge and awareness depends on some of these factors; however, in the present study, we were unable to confirm this due to a lack of quantitative data or limited data on these variables in the literature. Nonetheless, qualitative evidence from land management studies (e.g., [4,6,9,59]) has indeed indicated that experience, knowledge networks, advice source and nature plays a key role in land managers’ awareness and uptake of best management practices (specifically diffuse pollution mitigation measures).
The MASEM suggests that the Value-Belief-Norm Theory and the Norm Activation Model were not supported by the data. The lack of evidence supporting their framing may be due to the neglect of situational factors (which this study was unable to confirm due to data limitation) in these two theories and their reductionist approach (i.e., the emphasis placed on altruistic factors). Pro-environmental behaviour is complex, as many decisions and actions are influenced by self-interest and situational factors. For instance, in land management, while a farmer may be willing to engage in an environmentally sustainable practice, cost of compliance, income forgone and ‘goodness of fit’ of agri-environment schemes are key players of the ultimate decision and action [7,18,37,40,60]. Evidence suggests that for those policies that achieved significant successes (e.g., the National Landcare Programme, Monitor Farms Programme, Diffuse Pollution Management Strategy), success depended greatly on contextual factors such as the financial incentives given to farmers, identifying trusted sources which exert the greatest social pressure on farmers (e.g., friends, advisors, family), presenting optimistic messages and highlighting benefits to farmers [9,14]. Perhaps this could explain why policies (e.g., the Vietnamese Government Forest Conservation Policy) that place little emphasis on such situational factors are less likely to succeed in triggering positive behaviours [61] and also why we did not find statistical evidence in the present study to support some of these models. Additionally, situational factors are predisposed to temporal and spatial dynamics [32], hence, future research in land management could examine these models considering the peculiarities of different spatial and temporal factors (such as how much time is needed for farmers to adopt different best land management practices and how much does one’s location influence such decisions). Further, future research could focus on unpacking the complex interaction between the variables explored in this study and other potential factors such as land managers’ demographic characteristics, and how that influence their behaviours. A deeper understanding of these interactions could further contribute to designing effective policies to encourage positive behavioural change amongst farmers.
Another important finding is that there is a strong between-study heterogeneity, which is partly attributable to the type of environmental behaviour which the different studies address [22,55,62]. That is, results for the same model varied in terms of statistical significance and strength of association for different environmental behaviours. For instance, results for the relationship between environmental awareness and behaviour indicated that this relationship was slightly stronger in sustainable consumer behaviour and slightly weaker in general ecological behaviour, but it was non-significant in recycling and land management behaviours. This is an important finding because it would preclude the validity of directly transferring recommendations across sectors (i.e., what is valid for recycling in terms of promoting behaviours might not be successful in trying to promote uptake of best land management practices). Therefore, policy recommendations should be tailored to the specific environmental issues. This is because models of pro-environmental behaviour will yield varying levels of predictive capacity or effectiveness for different environmental behaviours; hence, a critical evaluation should be undertaken in order to select which models are more suitable for understanding a particular environmental behaviour (e.g., best land management practices) while paying attention to the different conditions in which a particular model is the most useful [38].
We now turn to discuss potential limitations of our study. It must be noted that most of the primary studies used a self-reported approach in measuring the variables under study (e.g., attitudes and behaviour). Self-reported studies are usually impacted by social desirability bias, memory bias and variation in survey participants’ environmental knowledge and beliefs [63,64]. This can potentially introduce estimation errors in the results of the primary studies, and by extension, the results of this meta-analysis. Additionally, we are aware of the fact that meta-analysis of relationships involves some form of data aggregation (e.g., lots of different actions which may be affected by other variables are being pooled together and generically referred to as ‘behaviour’) which could be faced with issues of ecological confounding [55,64]. As a result, we treat non-significant relationships as tentative cases requiring further investigation in primary research rather than as an absence of true relationships.
Another limitation lies in the fact that the MASEM used a correlation matrix instead of a covariance matrix as input. This could potentially result in unreliable/incorrect Chi square statistics and standard errors [22,56]. Furthermore, the MASEM was based on correlations, not covariance, which could result in overestimation of some parameters due to problems of multicollinearity (as can be observed from the results for the intention-behaviour link of the TPB model). Finally, the subgroup analysis relied on correlation coefficients and not regression coefficients, and also used a dichotomous method. This may result in the loss of valuable information, as it fails to take account of the complex nature of data from the different primary studies [56]. We entreat readers to consider these limitations when interpreting our results, while encouraging future studies to take account of study-level variables as continuous covariates in the application of MASEM.
The final limitation stems from the variation in the number of papers (from the different environmental policy areas) and the unequal sample sizes. For instance, only seven papers were drawn from the land management sector (probably due to the restriction to only papers that reported correlations and sample sizes). This could have impacted the statistical analysis. Also, removing qualitative studies and papers that do not report correlations and sample sizes could limit our understanding of the ‘true’ relationships between variables reported across the different environmental sectors. It is also important to note that although we attempted to reduce the possibility of missing relevant papers by using the snowball technique, it is possible that we still missed a few key papers. Nonetheless, the results provide insights into the significant variation in relationships studied across the different environmental policy areas.

6. Concluding Remarks

Land management policies are increasingly emphasising behavioural change as a means to improve environmental management outcomes. This is based, implicitly or explicitly, on theories that suggest that pro-environmental behaviour can be predicted and altered based on certain factors (referred to as determinants of pro-environmental behaviour), and that behavioural changes, in turn, can be expected to lead to improved environmental outcomes (such as water quality, soil health, biodiversity loss) through the adoption of mitigation measures or reduction of impactful activities. This assumes the existence of a straightforward pathway linking the determinants of behaviour to pro-environmental behaviour and then to environmental outcomes. However, studies examining the determinants of pro-environmental behaviour have found mixed evidence. Establishing whether existing evidence supports the postulations of such theories (in aggregate terms) could help provide answers that may trigger new policy directions. It is therefore important that these theories are revisited to assess if the evidence supports their postulations so that a more robust knowledge base can be established to inform land management policies. In this study, we conducted such analysis. By using meta-analytic structural equation modelling (MASEM), we expanded on previous reviews because the method allows us to test the mechanisms through which these different variables affect one another, which previous studies relying on correlations and multiple regressions were unable to reveal. Moreover, although we focus on land management, the evidence base that we explore goes beyond that of land management only, including related areas and general ecological behaviours more broadly.
Our findings suggest a strong evidence base for the Persuasion Theory, Theory of Reasoned Action, Theory of Planned Behaviour and the Attitude-Behaviour-Context (ABC) Model. In contrast, evidence supporting the Value-Belief-Norm Theory and the Norm Activation Model is less established. The lack of evidence supporting the Value-Belief-Norm Theory and the Norm Activation Model suggests the need to include situational factors in models of pro-environmental behaviour (and by extension, environmental policies that are implicitly or explicitly based on such models). Including such variables could improve our understanding of the complex determinants of pro-environmental behaviour, and this may in turn, help adjust policies to address various motivations, capabilities and circumstances of stakeholders (e.g., farmers). From a research perspective, qualitative methods could be used to gather rich data that provides a deeper understanding of the determinants of pro-environmental behaviour and how best to influence stakeholders’ behaviour.
The results also indicate that type of environmental policy area (e.g., recycling, land management) moderates the relationships between the different variables and therefore the underlying assumptions might not be shared across fields. This has key policy implications as while lessons can be learnt from other environmental areas, land management policies aimed at influencing behaviours will need to be tailored to the specific context rather than simply ‘imported’ from other areas. Therefore, models upon which policies may (implicitly/explicitly) be based need to consider context-specific nuances. Such context-specific policies may encourage uptake of pro-environmental behaviours. This does not pre-empt, however, the need for the assessment of the full pathway, i.e., including also the assessment of the effectiveness of changes in behaviour on environmental outcomes [14]. This study has attempted to investigate the first component of the pathway: the path linking psycho-social factors to pro-environmental behaviour, which is a contribution towards understanding the effects of policies on uptake of pro-environmental behaviours. We encourage future research to explore the full pathway drawing both on psychosocial and biophysical data.

Author Contributions

This research was conceived by M.O., with the guidance of J.M.-O., P.N. and P.J.C. Data extraction, formal analysis, interpretation of results, and preparation of original draft were carried out by M.O. with inputs from J.M.-O., P.N. and P.J.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the University of Leeds International Doctoral Scholarship (LIDS) (2017–2020 and the RePhoKUs project (The role of phosphorus in the sustainability and resilience of the UK food system) funded by BBSRC, ESRC, NERC and the Scottish Government under the UK Global Food Security research programme (Grant No. BB/R005842/1).

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to Ata Senior Yeboah, Amponsah Abraham, Jefim Vogel and John K.B. Kwatia for their advice and support of this work.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A. Search Terms and Groupings

As terms such as “pro-environmental behaviour” and “theories” are sometimes expressed differently, we used substitute words to help capture the fullness of the terms as well as wildcard symbols (e.g., asterisks) to capture the variations of the words (see samples below):
Search 1. Variations of Pro-environmental behaviour and the theories.
  • Level 1: one of the following: pro-environment*   pro ecolog*   sustain* environment* friend*   environment*   good environmental   positive environmental
  • Environmentally significant   environmentally responsible
  • PLUS one of the following
  • Level 2: behavio*   attitud*   act*   habit*   practice*   measure*   way*   effort*
  • PLUS one of the following
  • Norm Activation Model   Value-Belief-Norm Theory   Theory of Reasoned Action   Theory of Planned Behaviour   Persuasion Theory Attitude-Behaviour-Context Theory Social Learning Theory
Search 2. Substitutions made include:
  • For environmental behaviour:
  • Climat* adapt*   mitigat* preparedness   waste manag*   conservation green   behavior   green consum*   land manag*   forest manag*   antipollution
  • The terms theory or model were also substituted with:
  • Framework, concept*, proposition

Appendix B

Table A1. Final Papers Included in the meta-analysis.
Table A1. Final Papers Included in the meta-analysis.
StudyTitleIssue AreaPolicy
[65]When do recycling attitudes predict recycling? An investigation of self-reported versus observed behaviour1x
[66]What influences water conservation and towel reuse practices of hotel guests?3x
[67]Using the Theory of Planned Behaviour to investigate the determinants of recycling behaviour: a case study from Brixworth, UK1EU Landfill Directive
[68]Understanding and facilitating sustainable agricultural practice: A comprehensive analysis of adoption behaviour among Malaysian paddy farmers2Sustainable Agricultural Practices
[69]Two challenges to a moral extension of the theory of planned behaviour: moral norms and just world beliefs in conservationism4x
[70]Travellers’ pro-environmental behaviours in a green lodging context: Converging value-belief-norm theory and the theory of planned behaviour3Eco-friendly guidelines
[71]Transcendental values and the valuation and management of ecosystem services2x
[72]Time Perspective and Sustainable Behaviour: Evidence for the Distinction Between Consideration of Immediate and Future Consequences4x
[73]Sustainable attitudes and behaviours amongst a sample of non-academic staff: A case study from an Information Services Department, Griffith University, Brisbane3Institutional Sustainability Initiative
[74]Socio-Cognitive Determinants of Consumers’ Support for the Fair Trade Movement3Fair trade principles
[75] Smallholder farmers’ motivations for using Conservation Agriculture and the roles of yield, labour and soil fertility in decision making2Conservation Agricultural Policy
[76]The role of perceived socio-spatial distance in adolescents’ willingness to engage in pro-environmental behaviour4x
[77]The Relative Importance of Social and Personal Norms in Explaining Intentions to Choose Eco-Friendly Travel Options3x
[78]Recycling: Planned and self-expressive behaviour1x
[79]Psychosocial and Demographic Variables Associated with Consumer Intention to Purchase Sustainably Produced Foods as Defined by the Midwest Food Alliance3x
[80]A proposed structural model for housewives’ recycling behaviour: A case study from Turkey1Waste Management Policies
[81]Pro-environmental behaviour in the workplace and the role of managers and organisation4x
[82]Predictors of Pro-Environmental Behaviours of American and Korean Students: The Application of the Theory of Reasoned Action and Protection Motivation Theory3x
[83]Predictors of individuals’ intention to conserve water in a lodging context: the application of an extended Theory of Reasoned Action3General sustainability policies
[84]Predicting residents’ pro-environmental behaviours at tourist sites: The role of awareness of disaster’s consequences, values, and place attachment3x
[85]Predicting pro-environmental agricultural practices: The social, psychological and contextual influences on land management2General best land management practices
[86]Predicting consumer intentions to purchase energy-efficient products3x
[87]The norm activation model and theory-broadening: Individuals’ decision-making on environmentally-responsible convention attendance4x
[88]A moral extension of the theory of planned behaviour: Norms and anticipated feelings of regret in conservationism4x
[89]Moderating effects of social value orientation on determinants of Pro-environmental behaviour intention4x
[90]The Moderating Effect of Perceived Lack of Facilities on Consumers’ Recycling Intentions1The Compulsory Classification of Refuse program
[91]Mechanism of environmental concern on intention to pay more for renewable energy: Application to a developing country3Anti-pollution laws
[92]Linking green scepticism to green purchase behaviour3x
[93]Investigating willingness to save energy and communication about energy use in the American workplace with the attitude-behaviour-context model3x
[94]Investigating pro-Environmental Behaviours of Lithuanian University Students3x
[95]Intention to buy eco-friendly packaged products among young consumers of India: A study on developing nation3Eco-friendly Strategies
[96]Influences on Student Intention and Behaviour Toward Environmental Sustainability3x
[97]Impact of fear appeals on pro-environmental behaviour and crucial determinants4x
[98]The impact of direct and indirect experiences on the development of environmental knowledge, attitudes, and behaviour4x
Filho [99]Heterogeneity in Intention to Adopt Organic Strawberry Production Practices Among Producers in the Federal District, Brazil3x
[100]Guests’ pro-environmental decision-making process: Broadening the norm activation framework in a lodging context3x
[101]Fostering customers’ pro-environmental behaviour at a museum3x
[102]Factors influencing green purchasing behaviour: Empirical evidence from the Lebanese consumers3x
[103]Extending the theory of planned behaviour model to explain people’s energy savings and carbon reduction behavioural intentions to mitigate climate change in Taiwane- moral obligation matters3 The 25 G8 energy efficiency recommendations
[104]Extending the Theory of Planned Behaviour in the context of recycling: The role of moral norms and of demographic predictors1x
[105]An extended theory of planned behaviour model of the psychological factors affecting commuters’ transport mode use3x
[106]Explaining the environmentally sustainable consumer behaviour: a social capital perspective3x
[107]Explaining pro-environment consumer behaviour in air travel3x
[108]Explaining consumers’ willingness to be environmentally friendly3x
[109]Explaining consumer use of renewable energy: determinants and gender and age moderator effects3x
[110]An examination of the value-belief-norm theory model in predicting pro-environmental behaviour in Taiwan4x
[111]Environmental action and student environmental leaders: exploring the influence of environmental attitudes, locus of control, and sense of personal responsibility4x
[112]Engaging Great Barrier Reef Stakeholders: Mediation Analyses of Barriers Among the Antecedents of Pro-Environmental Behaviour4x
[113]Encouraging sustainability in the workplace: a survey on the pro-environmental behaviour of university employees4x
[114]Enacting Ecological Sustainability in the MNC: A Test of an Adapted Value-Belief-Norm Framework4ISO 14031
[115]Effects of values, problem awareness, and personal norm on willingness to reduce personal car use3x
[116]Effecting Durable Change: A Team Approach to Improve Environmental Behaviour in the Household4Eco Team Program
[117]Differentiation of determinants of low-cost and high-cost recycling1x
[118]Developing an extended Theory of Planned Behaviour model to predict consumers’ intention to visit green hotels1x
[119]Determinants of Water Conservation Intention in Blagoevgrad, Bulgaria3x
[120]Determinants of Southern Italian households’ intention to adopt energy efficiency measures in residential buildings3Energy Efficiency Policies
[121]Cruise travellers’ environmentally responsible decision-making: An integrative framework of goal-directed behaviour and norm activation process3x
[122]A comprehensive action determination model: Toward a broader understanding of ecological behaviour using the example of travel mode choice3x
[123]Cognitive, affective, normative, and moral triggers of sustainable intentions among convention-goers3x
[124]Behavioural intentions toward afforestation and carbon reduction by the Taiwanese public3Climate change Policies
[125]Attitudes, efficacy beliefs, and willingness to pay for environmental protection when travelling3x
[126]Analysis of the ecological conservation behaviour of farmers in payment for ecosystem service programs in eco-environmentally fragile areas using social psychology models2x
[127]An analysis of intentions to recycle household waste: The roles of past behaviour, perceived habit, and perceived lack of facilities1x
[128]Ambivalence and conservation behaviour: An exploratory study on the recycling of metal cans1x
[129]Adolescents’ Perspectives and Food Choice Behaviours in Terms of the Environmental Impacts of Food Production Practices: Application of a Psychosocial Model3x
[130]Adolescent Environmental Behaviours: Can Knowledge, Attitudes, and Self-Efficacy Make a Difference?4x
[131]Responsibility And Environment: Ecological Norm Orientation and External Factors in the Domain of Travel Mode Choice Behaviour3x
[132]Influences on attitude-behaviour relationships: a natural experiment with curbside recycling 1x
[133]Influence Of Socio-demographics And Environmental Attitudes on General Responsible Environmental Behaviour Among Recreational Boaters4x
[134]Motivations to Participate in Riparian Improvement Programs: Applying the Theory of Planned Behaviour2x
[135]Promoting Sustainable Consumption: Determinants of Green Purchases by Swiss Consumers3x
[136]Measuring Responsible Environmental Behaviour: Self-Reported and Other-Reported Measures and Their Differences in Testing a Behavioural Model4x
[137]Measuring implementation intentions in the context of the theory of planned behaviour1x
[138]Understanding air force members’ intentions to participate in pro-environmental behaviours: an application of the theory of planned behaviour4x
[139]The Proposition of a General Version of the Theory of Planned Behaviour: Predicting Ecological Behaviour4x
[140]Predicting antipollution behaviour: The role of molar behavioural intentions, past behaviour and locus of control 4x
[141]The Impact of Norms And Assumed Consequences On Recycling Behaviour1x
[142]Beyond the intention–behaviour mythology An integrated model of recycling1x
[143]Understanding the Relationship Between Christian Orthodoxy and Environmentalism: The Mediating Role of Perceived Environmental Consequences4x
[144]Integrating social value orientation and the consideration of future consequences within the extended norm activation model of pro-environmental behaviour4x
[145]Responsibility As A Predictor of Ecological Behaviour4x
[146]Environmental Attitude and ecological Behaviour4x
[147]The Effects of Environmental Concern on Environmentally Friendly Consumer Behaviour: An Exploratory Study3x
[148]Travel behaviour and environmental concern3x
[149]Constraints On Environmental Behaviour4x
[150]The theory of planned behaviour: Self-identity, social identity and group norms4x
[151]Action speaks louder than words: The effect of personal attitudes and family norms on adolescents’ pro-environmental behaviour4x
[152]What matters to promote consumers’ intention to patronize sustainable business-and-industry (B&I) food services?3x
[153]Motivating energy conservation in the workplace: An evaluation of the use of group-level feedback and peer education3x
[154]Environmentally friendly behaviour and green purchase in Austria and Lithuania3x
[2]Sustainable transportation in Argentina: Values, beliefs, norms and car use reduction3Transport pricing policies
[155]A study of goal frames shaping pro-environmental behaviour in university students4x
[156]The role of Islamic values on green purchase intention3x
[157]Physical Outdoor Activity versus Indoor Activity: Their Influence on Environmental Behaviours4x
[158]A Mediation Model of Air Travellers’ Voluntary Climate Action3x
[159]Is the intention to travel in a pro-environmental manner and the intention to use the car determined by different factors?3x
[160]Exploring people’s willingness to bike using a combination of the theory of planned behavioural and the trans-theoretical model3x
[161]Exploring Additional Determinants of Environmentally Responsible Behaviour: The Influence of Environmental Literature and Environmental Attitudes4x
[162]Assessing the intention-behaviour gap in electronic waste recycling: the case of Brazil1x
[163]Acceptability of travel demand management measures: The importance of problem awareness, personal norm, freedom, and fairness3x
[164]Environmental locus of control, sympathy, and pro-environmental behaviour: A Test of Geller’s Actively Caring Hypothesis4x
[165]Repeated Behaviour and Environmental Psychology: The Role of Personal Involvement and Habit Formation in Explaining Water Consumption3x
[166]Conservation technology adoption decisions and the theory of planned behaviour2Water saving irrigation technology
[167]Motivations and behaviours that support recycling1x
[168]Effects of increased awareness on choice of travel mode 3x
[169]Social context, personal norms and the use of public transportation: Two field studies3x
[170]Accounting for Sustained Volunteering by Young People: An Expanded TPB4x
[171]The Dynamic Interaction of Personal Norms and Environment-Friendly Buying Behaviour: A Panel Study3x
Note: 1 = recycling; 2 = land management; 3 = sustainable consumer behaviour; 4 = general ecological behaviour; X = No policy highlighted.

Appendix C

Table A2. A 95%-confidence interval of the correlations calculated under the random effect assumptions (model).
Table A2. A 95%-confidence interval of the correlations calculated under the random effect assumptions (model).
SN Construct 123456789101112131415161718
1knowledge--
2beliefs LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.21
0.73
-
3Awareness or consciousness LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.03 NS
0.49
−0.15NS
0.45
-
4Attitude LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.04
0.61
0.34
0.66
0.17
0.44
-
5Habit LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.12a
0.32
−0.09a
0.13
−0.24 NS
0.30
-
6IntentionLCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.06
0.66
0.05
0.69
0.28
0.53
0.43
0.61
−0.37NS
0.55
-
7Behavioural willingness LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.15
0.38
0.18a
0.28
0.29
0.48
0.00
0.38
00.08
0.68
-
8Behaviour LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.17
0.39
0.17
0.30
0.13
0.31
0.27
0.46
0.09
0.45
0.35
0.52
0.19–0.48-
9Social norm LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.04
0.39
−0.03
0.56
0.27
0.46
0.34
0.50
0.31
0.51
-
10Moral norm LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.13
0.32
0.32
0.50
0.36
0.79
−0.22
0.68
0.15
0.54
-
11Subjective norm LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.01
0.86
0.27–0.510.38–0.52 0.37–0.48 0.14–0.35 -
12Emotions LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.35a
0.53
0.06
0.40
−0.05NS
0.56
0.13
0.58
−024NS
0.43
−0.39NS
0.65
0.16
0.44
-
13Self-efficacy LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.03
0.32
0.17
0.46
0.32
0.46
0.12

0.43
0.40
0.57
0.27
0.50
0.19
0.35
0.17
0.43
0.24
0.65
0.46
0.51
-
14Perceived consequences LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.05
0.38
0.47
0.81
0.27
0.55
0.25
0.51
0.09
0.22
0.12
0.23
0.38
0.58
-
15Ascription of responsibility LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.19
0.58
0.14
0.56
0.42
0.60
0.15
0.31
0.16
0.52
0.38
0.56
-
16Situational factors LCI 95%
UCI 95%
−0.19NS
0.15
−0.18NS
0.05
-
17Environmental value LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.31
0.49
0.18
0.60
0.26
0.51
-
18Environmental concern LCI 95%
UCI 95%
0.15
0.44
0.16
0.33
0.23
0.37
-
Note: a = no pooled correlation (i.e., only one correlation was used but we calculated the 95% CI), NS = non-significant relationship (all other r values are significant at the p ≤ 0.05); LCI = Lower Confidence interval; UCI = upper Confidence interval.

Appendix D

Table A3. Results of test of heterogeneity.
Table A3. Results of test of heterogeneity.
SN VARIABLE 123456789101112131415161718
1knowledge-
2beliefs Q(158.23)
df(3)
P(0.00)
I(98.10)
-
3Awareness or consciousness Q(10.41)
Df(1)
P(0.00)
I(90.39)
Q(93.29)
Df(2)
P(0.00)
I(97.86)
-
4Attitude Q(1218.16)
Df(9)
P(0.00)
I(99.26)
Q(264.47)
Df(6)
P(0.00)
I(97.73)
Q(155.59)
Df(9)
P(0.00)
I(94.22)
-
5Habit Q(0.00)
Df(0)
P(1.00)
I(0.00)ns
Q(0.00)
Df(0)
P(1.00)
I(0.00)ns
Q(80.56)
Df(3)
P(0.00)
I(96.28)
-
6IntentionQ(915.77)
Df(7)
P(0.00)
I(99.24)
Q(231.94)
Df(3)
P(0.00)
I(98.71)
Q(327.09)
Df(12)
P(0.00)
I(96.33)
Q(3460.70)
Df(45)
P(0.00)
I(98.70)
Q(328.51)
Df(4)
P(0.00)
I(98.78)
-
7Behavioural willingness Q(18.96)
Df(4)
P(0.00)
I(78.90)
Q(0.00)
Df(0)
P(1.00)
I(0.00)ns
Q(35.28)
Df(5)
P(0.00)
I(85.83)
Q(32.72)
Df(3)
P(0.00)
I(90.83)
0Q(99.81)
Df(3)
P(0.00)
I(96.99)
-
8Behaviour Q(106.74)
Df(8)
P(0.00)
I(92.50)
Q(49.43)
Df(6)
P(0.00)
I(87.86)
Q(273.26)
Df(17)
P(0.00)
I(93.78)
Q(1350.24)
Df(29)
P(0.00)
I(97.85)
Q(36.33)
Df(3)
P(0.00)
I(91.74)
Q(842.73)
Df(28)
P(0.00)
I(98.68)
Q(39.26)
Df(4)
P(0.00)
I(89.81)
-
9Social norm Q(4.91)
Df(1)
P(0.03)
I(79.65)
Q(20.90)
Df(1)
P(0.00)
I(95.22)
Q(65.54)
Df(5)
P(0.00)
I(92.37)
Q(108.01)
Df(8)
P(0.00)
I(92.59)
Q(82.66)
DF(8)
P(0.00)
I(90.32)
-
10Moral norm Q(6.31)
Df(2)
P(0.04)
I(68.30)
Q(281.63)
Df(13)
P(0.00)
I(95.38)
Q(4785.79)
Df(17)
P(0.00)
I(99.64)
Q(1505.70)
Df(12)
P(0.00)
I(99.20)
Q(2185.43)
DF(18)
P(0.00)
I(99.18)
-
11Subjective norm Q(77.03)
Df(1)
P(0.00)
I(98.70)
Q(47.32)
Df(5)
P(0.00)
I(89.43)
Q(1563.64)
Df(41)
P(0.00)
I(97.38)
Q(596.75)
Df(32)
P(0.00)
I(94.64)
Q(513.02)
DF(19)
P(0.00)
I(96.30)
-
12Emotions e.g., guilt Q(0.00)
Df(0)
P(1.00)
I(0.00)ns
Q(289.17)
DF(9)
P(0.00)
I(96.89)
Q(345.39)
Df(5)
P(0.00)
I(98.55)
Q(1106.91)
Df(12)
P(0.00)
I(98.92)
Q(230.76)
DF(4)
P(0.00)
I(98.27)
-
13Self-efficacy Q(42.99)
Df(3)
P(0.00)
I(93.02)
Q(69.99)
DF(5)
P(0.00)
I(92.86)
Q(1304.70)
Df(36)
P(0.00)
I(97.24)
Q(5.73)
Df(1)
P(0.02)
I(82.55)
Q(2218.73)
Df(38)
P(0.00)
I(98.29)
Q(39.95)
Df(5)
P(0.00)
I(87.48)
Q(409.37)
DF(22)
P(0.00)
I(94.63)
Q(22.87)
DF(3)
P(0.00)
I(86.88)
Q(202.82)
DF(4)
P(0.00)
I(98.03)
Q(325.26)
DF(8)
P(0.00)
I(97.54)
-
14Perceived consequences Q(15.21)
Df(2)
P(0.00)
I(86.85)
Q(233.26)
Df(5)
P(0.00)
I(97.86)
Q(214.84)
Df(9)
P(0.00)
I(95.81)
Q(670.23)
Df(18)
P(0.00)
I(97.31)
Q(51.40)
Df(11)
P(0.00)
I(78.60)
Q(1.07)
DF(2)
P(0.59)
I(0.00)
Q(315.72)
DF(11)
P(0.00)
I(96.52)
-
15Ascription of responsibility Q(14.16)
Df(2)
P(0.00)
I(85.88)
Q(82.92)
Df(4)
P(0.00)
I(95.18)
Q(70.85)
Df(7)
P(0.00)
I(90.12)
Q(15.87)
Df(5)
P(0.01)
I(68.50)
Q(7.49)
DF(1)
P(0.01)
I(86.65)
Q(6.95)
DF(2)
P(0.03)
I(71.22)
-
16Situational factors Q(185.50)
Df(8)
P(0.00)
I(95.68)
Q(159.18)
DF(10)
P(0.00)
I(93.72)
-
17Environmental value Q(2.36)
Df(1)
P(0.12)
I(57.66)ns
Q(10.16)
DF(1)
P(0.00)
I(90.1)6
Q(78.41)
Df(6)
I(0.00)
I(92.35)
-
18Environmental concern Q(22.14)
Df(3)
P(0.00)
I(86.45)
Q(30.64)
Df(6)
P(0.00)
I(80.42)
Q(59.53)
Df(10)
P(0.00)
I(82.20)
-
Q = Q statistic; Df = Degrees of freedom; p = p-value; I = I squared statistic.

References

  1. Chen, C.-F.; Xu, X.; Frey, S. Who wants solar water heaters and alternative fuel vehicles? Assessing social–psychological predictors of adoption intention and policy support in China. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 2016, 15, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Jakovcevic, A.; Steg, L. Sustainable transportation in Argentina: Values, beliefs, norms and car use reduction. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2013, 20, 70–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Kesternich, M.; Reif, C.; Rübbelke, D. Recent Trends in Behavioral Environmental Economics. Environ. Resour. Econ. 2017, 67, 403–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Blackstock, K.; Ingram, J.; Burton, R.; Brown, K.; Slee, B. Understanding and influencing behaviour change by farmers to improve water quality. Sci. Total. Environ. 2010, 408, 5631–5638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Prager, K. Understanding Behaviour Change: How to Apply Theories of Behaviour Change to Seweb and Related Public Engagement Activities; Scottish Government/Scottish Environment Protection Agency (SEPA): Aberdeen, UK; Edinburg, TX, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  6. Vrain, E.; Lovett, A. The roles of farm advisors in the uptake of measures for the mitigation of diffuse water pollution. Land Use Policy 2016, 54, 413–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Vrain, E.; Lovett, A.A.; Noble, L.; Grant, F. Farmer Attitudes towards Diffuse Pollution Mitigation Measures in England; Department for Environment, Food & Rural Affairs: London, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  8. Lockie, S.; Higgins, V. Roll-out neoliberalism and hybrid practices of regulation in Australian agri-environmental governance. J. Rural. Stud. 2007, 23, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Dwyer, J.; Mills, J.; Ingram, J.; Taylor, J.; Burton, R. Understanding and Influencing Positive Behaviour Change in Farmers and Land Managers; CCRI: Warwick, RI, USA; Macaulay Institute: Aberdeen, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  10. Environment Agency. Catchment Sensitive Farming—ECSFDI Phase 1 & 2 Evaluation Report; Environment Agency Bristol: Bristol, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  11. Environment Agency. Catchment Sensitive Farming: A Clear Solution for Farmers; Environment Agency: Bristol, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  12. DPMAG. Strategy to Reduce Diffuse Pollution; Scottish Environment Protection Agency: Edinburgh, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  13. Okumah, M.; Martin-Ortega, J.; Novo, P. Effects of awareness on farmers’ compliance with diffuse pollution mitigation measures: A conditional process modelling. Land Use Policy 2018, 76, 36–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Okumah, M.; Chapman, P.; Martin-Ortega, J.; Novo, P. Mitigating Agricultural Diffuse Pollution: Uncovering the Evidence Base of the Awareness–Behaviour–Water Quality Pathway. Water 2019, 11, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Okumah, M.; Yeboah, A.S.; Nkiaka, E.; Azerigyik, R.A. What Determines Behaviours towards Water Resources Management in a Rural Context? Results of a Quantitative Study. Resources 2019, 8, 109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Baumgart-Getz, A.; Prokopy, L.S.; Floress, K. Why farmers adopt best management practice in the United States: A meta-analysis of the adoption literature. J. Environ. Manag. 2012, 96, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. MacGregor, C.; Warren, C.R. Adopting sustainable farm management practices within a Nitrate Vulnerable Zone in Scotland: The view from the farm. Agric. Ecosyst. Environ. 2006, 113, 108–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Barnes, A.; Willock, J.; Hall, C.; Toma, L. Farmer perspectives and practices regarding water pollution control programmes in Scotland. Agric. Water Manag. 2009, 96, 1715–1722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Inman, A.; Winter, M.; Wheeler, R.; Vrain, E.; Lovett, A.; Collins, A.L.; Jones, J.I.; Johnes, P.; Cleasby, W. An exploration of individual, social and material factors influencing water pollution mitigation behaviours within the farming community. Land Use Policy 2018, 70, 16–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Okumah, M.; Ankomah-Hackman, P. Applying conditional process modelling to investigate factors influencing the adoption of water pollution mitigation behaviours. Sustain. Water Resour. Manag. 2020, 6, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Jackson, T. Motivating Sustainable Consumption: A Review of Evidence on Consumer Behaviour and Behavioural Change; Sustainable Development Research Network; University of Surrey: Guildford, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  22. Bamberg, S.; Möser, G. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 14–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Cheung, M.W.-L.; Hong, R.Y. Applications of meta-analytic structural equation modelling in health psychology: Examples, issues, and recommendations. Heal. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 11, 265–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Hovland, C.I.; Janis, I.L.; Kelley, H.H. Communication and Persuasion; Psychological Studies of Opinion Change; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 1953. [Google Scholar]
  25. Petty, R.; Cacioppo, J. Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change; Springer-Verlag: New York, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  26. Ajzen, I.; Fishbein, M. Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behaviour; Prentice-Hall Inc.: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  27. Ajzen, I. The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Schwartz, S.H. Awareness of Consequences and the Influence of Moral Norms on Interpersonal Behavior. Sociometry 1968, 31, 355–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Schwartz, S.H. Elicitation of moral obligation and self-sacrificing behavior: An experimental study of volunteering to be a bone marrow donor. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1970, 15, 283–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T.; Abel, T.; Guagnano, G.A.; Kalof, A. A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: The case of environmentalism. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 1999, 6, 81–97. [Google Scholar]
  31. Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T. The Value Basis of Environmental Concern. J. Soc. Issues 1994, 50, 65–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Stern, P.C. New Environmental Theories: Toward a Coherent Theory of Environmentally Significant Behavior. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 407–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T.; Black, J.S. Support for environmental protection: The role of moral norms. Popul. Environ. 1985, 8, 204–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Boiral, O. Tacit Knowledge and Environmental Management. Long Range Plan. 2002, 35, 291–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Bandura, A. Social Learning Theory; Prentice H.: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  36. Michie, S.; Johnston, M.; Francis, J.; Hardeman, W.; Eccles, M. From Theory to Intervention: Mapping Theoretically Derived Behavioural Determinants to Behaviour Change Techniques. Appl. Psychol. 2008, 57, 660–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Daxini, A.; Ryan, M.; O’Donoghue, C.; Barnes, A.P. Understanding farmers’ intentions to follow a nutrient management plan using the theory of planned behaviour. Land Use Policy 2019, 85, 428–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Kollmuss, A.; Agyeman, J. Mind the Gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 2002, 8, 239–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Steg, L.; Vlek, C. Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: An integrative review and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 309–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Okumah, M.; Yeboah, A.S.; Amponsah, O. Stakeholders’ willingness and motivations to support sustainable water resources management: Insights from a Ghanaian study. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 2020, 2, 170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Juntti, M.; Potter, C. Interpreting and Reinterpreting Agri-Environmental Policy: Communication, Trust and Knowledge in the Implementation Process. Sociol. Rural. 2002, 42, 216–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Schwartz, S.H. Normative influences on altruism. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Academic Press Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1977; pp. 221–279. [Google Scholar]
  43. Floress, K.; De Jalón, S.G.; Church, S.P.; Babin, N.; Ulrich-Schad, J.D.; Prokopy, L.S. Toward a theory of farmer conservation attitudes: Dual interests and willingness to take action to protect water quality. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017, 53, 73–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Gibbons, F.X.; Gerrard, M.; Lane, D.J. A social reaction model of adolescent health risk. In Social Psychological Foundations of Health and Illness; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2003; pp. 107–136. [Google Scholar]
  45. Farrow, K.; Grolleau, G.; Ibanez, L. Social Norms and Pro-environmental Behavior: A Review of the Evidence. Ecol. Econ. 2017, 140, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Koricheva, J.; Gurevitch, J. Place of Meta-Analysis Among Other Methods of Research Synthesis, in Handbook of Meta-Analysis in Ecology and Evolution; Koricheva, J., Gurevitch, J., Mengersen, K., Eds.; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA; Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  47. Glass, V.G.; McGraw, B.; Smith, M.L. Meta-Analysis in Social Research; SAGE: London, UK, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  48. Hunter, J.E.; Schmidt, F.L. Fixed Effects vs. Random Effects Meta-Analysis Models: Implications for Cumulative Research Knowledge. Int. J. Sel. Assess. 2000, 8, 275–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Hedges, L.V.; Vevea, J.L. Fixed- and Random-Effects Models in Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Methods 1998, 3, 486–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Hedges, L.V.; Olkin, I. Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis; Academic Press Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  51. Jennions, M.D. Publication and Related Biases. In Handbook of Meta-analysis in Ecology and Evolution; Koricheva, J., Gurevitch, J., Mengersen, K., Eds.; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA; Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  52. Hunter, J.E.; Schmidt, F.L. Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings, 2nd ed.; Sage: London, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  53. Rosenberg, M.S.; Rothstein, H.R.; Gurevitch, J. Effect Sizes: Conventional Choices and Calculations, in Handbook of Meta-Analysis in Ecology and Evolution; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA; Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  54. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation and Conditional Process Analysis; The Guildford Press: New York, NY, USA; London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  55. Thompson, S.; Higgins, J.P. How should meta-regression analyses be undertaken and interpreted? Stat. Med. 2002, 21, 1559–1573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Jak, S.; Cheung, M.W.-L. Testing moderator hypotheses in meta-analytic structural equation modeling using subgroup analysis. Behav. Res. Methods 2018, 50, 1359–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Hooper, D.; Coughlan, J.; Mullen, M.R. Structural Equation Modelling: Guidelines for Determining Model Fit. J. Bus. Res. 2008, 6, 53–60. [Google Scholar]
  58. Hu, L.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Blackstock, K. Negotiating Change: The importance of Knowledge networks in mitigating diffuse pollution. In Proceedings of the CAIWA 2007, International Conference on Adaptive and Integrated Water Management, Basel, Switzerland, 12–15 September 2007. [Google Scholar]
  60. Morris, J.; Mills, J.; Crawford, I. Promoting farmer uptake of agri-environment schemes: The Countryside Stewardship Arable Options Scheme. Land Use Policy 2000, 17, 241–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Phuc, T.X. Why did the forest conservation policy fail in the Vietnamese uplands? Forest conflicts in Ba Vi National Park in Northern Region. Int. J. Environ. Stud. 2009, 66, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Hines, J.M.; Hungerford, H.R.; Tomera, A.N. Analysis & Synthesis of Research on Responsible Environmental Responsible Behavior: A Meta-Analysis. J. Environ. Educ. 1986, 18, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  63. Kormos, C.E.; Gifford, R. The validity of self-report measures of pro-environmental behavior: A meta-analytic review. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 359–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Warriner, G.; Mcdougall, G.; Claxton, J. Any data or none at all? Living with inaccuracies in self-reports of residential energy consumption. Environ. Behav. 1984, 503–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Huffman, A.H.; Van Der Werff, B.R.; Henning, J.B.; Watrous-Rodriguez, K. When do recycling attitudes predict recycling? An investigation of self-reported versus observed behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 262–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Han, H.; Hyun, S.S. What influences water conservation and towel reuse practices of hotel guests? Tour. Manag. 2018, 64, 87–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Tonglet, M.; Phillips, P.; Read, A.D. Using the Theory of Planned Behaviour to investigate the determinants of recycling behaviour: A case study from Brixworth, UK. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2004, 41, 191–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Adnan, N.; Nordin, S.M.; Bin Abu Bakar, Z. Understanding and facilitating sustainable agricultural practice: A comprehensive analysis of adoption behaviour among Malaysian paddy farmers. Land Use Policy 2017, 68, 372–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Kaiser, F.G.; Scheuthle, H. Two challenges to a moral extension of the theory of planned behavior: Moral norms and just world beliefs in conservationism. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2003, 35, 1033–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Han, H. Travelers’ pro-environmental behavior in a green lodging context: Converging value-belief-norm theory and the theory of planned behavior. Tour. Manag. 2015, 47, 164–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Raymond, C.M.; Kenter, J.O. Transcendental values and the valuation and management of ecosystem services. Ecosyst. Serv. 2016, 21, 241–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Arnocky, S.; Nicol, J.R.; Milfont, T.L. Time Perspective and Sustainable Behavior. Environ. Behav. 2013, 46, 556–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Davis, G.; O’Callaghan, F.; Knox, K. Sustainable attitudes and behaviours amongst a sample of non-academic staff. Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 2009, 10, 136–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Chatzidakis, A.; Kastanakis, M.; Stathopoulou, A. Socio-Cognitive Determinants of Consumers’ Support for the Fair Trade Movement. J. Bus. Ethics 2014, 133, 95–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Lalani, B.; Dorward, P.; Holloway, G.; Wauters, E. Smallholder farmers’ motivations for using Conservation Agriculture and the roles of yield, labour and soil fertility in decision making. Agric. Syst. 2016, 146, 80–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Busse, M.; Menzel, S. The role of perceived socio-spatial distance in adolescents’ willingness to engage in pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 412–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Doran, R.; Larsen, S. The Relative Importance of Social and Personal Norms in Explaining Intentions to Choose Eco-Friendly Travel Options. Int. J. Tour. Res. 2016, 18, 159–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Mannetti, L.; Pierro, A.; Livi, S. Recycling: Planned and self-expressive behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 227–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Robinson, R.; Smith, C. Psychosocial and demographic variables associated with consumer intention to purchase sustainably produced foods as defined by the Midwest Food Alliance. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2003, 34, 316–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Ari, E.; Yılmaz, V. A proposed structural model for housewives’ recycling behavior: A case study from Turkey. Ecol. Econ. 2016, 129, 132–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Wesselink, R.; Blok, V.; Ringersma, J. Pro-environmental behaviour in the workplace and the role of managers and organisation. J. Clean. Prod. 2017, 168, 1679–1687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Kim, S.; Jeong, S.-H.; Hwang, Y. Predictors of Pro-Environmental Behaviors of American and Korean Students. Sci. Commun. 2012, 35, 168–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Untaru, E.-N.; Ispas, A.; Candrea, A.N.; Luca, M.; Epuran, G. Predictors of individuals’ intention to conserve water in a lodging context: The application of an extended Theory of Reasoned Action. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2016, 59, 50–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Zhang, Y.; Zhang, H.; Zhang, J.; Cheng, S. Predicting residents’ pro-environmental behaviors at tourist sites: The role of awareness of disaster’s consequences, values, and place attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 131–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Price, J.; Leviston, Z. Predicting pro-environmental agricultural practices: The social, psychological and contextual influences on land management. J. Rural. Stud. 2014, 34, 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Ha, H.-Y.; Janda, S. Predicting consumer intentions to purchase energy-efficient products. J. Consum. Mark. 2012, 29, 461–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Han, H. The norm activation model and theory-broadening: Individuals’ decision-making on environmentally-responsible convention attendance. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 462–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Kaiser, F.G. A moral extension of the theory of planned behavior: Norms and anticipated feelings of regret in conservationism. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2006, 41, 71–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Gärling, T.; Fujii, S.; Gärling, A.; Jakobsson, C. Moderating effects of social value orientation on determinants of proenvironmental behavior intention. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Chen, M.-F.; Tung, P.-J. The Moderating Effect of Perceived Lack of Facilities on Consumers’ Recycling Intentions. Environ. Behav. 2009, 42, 824–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Lin, C.-Y.; Syrgabayeva, D. Mechanism of environmental concern on intention to pay more for renewable energy: Application to a developing country. Asia Pac. Manag. Rev. 2016, 21, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Goh, S.K.; Balaji, M. Linking green skepticism to green purchase behavior. J. Clean. Prod. 2016, 131, 629–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Xu, X.; Maki, A.; Chen, C.-F.; Dong, B.; Day, J.K. Investigating willingness to save energy and communication about energy use in the American workplace with the attitude-behavior-context model. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 2017, 32, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Poškus, M.S. Investigating pro-Environmental Behaviors of Lithuanian University Students. Curr. Psychol. 2016, 37, 225–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Prakash, G.; Pathak, P. Intention to buy eco-friendly packaged products among young consumers of India: A study on developing nation. J. Clean. Prod. 2017, 141, 385–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Swaim, J.; Maloni, M.; Napshin, S.A.; Henley, A.B. Influences on Student Intention and Behavior Toward Environmental Sustainability. J. Bus. Ethics 2013, 124, 465–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Chen, M.-F. Impact of fear appeals on pro-environmental behavior and crucial determinants. Int. J. Advert. 2015, 35, 74–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Duerden, M.D.; Witt, P.A. The impact of direct and indirect experiences on the development of environmental knowledge, attitudes, and behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 379–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Andow, D.; Filho, M.R.; Carneiro, R.; Lorena, D.; Sujii, E.; Alves, R. Heterogeneity in Intention to Adopt Organic Strawberry Production Practices Among Producers in the Federal District, Brazil. Ecol. Econ. 2017, 140, 177–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Han, H.; Hwang, J.; Kim, J.; Jung, H. Guests’ pro-environmental decision-making process: Broadening the norm activation framework in a lodging context. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2015, 47, 96–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Han, H.; Hyun, S.S. Fostering customers’ pro-environmental behavior at a museum. J. Sustain. Tour. 2016, 25, 1240–1256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Dagher, G.K.; Itani, O.S. Factors influencing green purchasing behaviour: Empirical evidence from the Lebanese consumers. J. Consum. Behav. 2014, 13, 188–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Chen, M.-F. Extending the theory of planned behavior model to explain people’s energy savings and carbon reduction behavioral intentions to mitigate climate change in Taiwan–moral obligation matters. J. Clean. Prod. 2016, 112, 1746–1753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Botetzagias, I.; Dima, A.-F.; Malesios, C. Extending the Theory of Planned Behavior in the context of recycling: The role of moral norms and of demographic predictors. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2015, 95, 58–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Donald, I.; Cooper, S.; Conchie, S. An extended theory of planned behaviour model of the psychological factors affecting commuters’ transport mode use. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Castaneda, M.G.; Martinez, C.P.; Marte, R.; Roxas, B. Explaining the environmentally-sustainable consumer behavior: A social capital perspective. Soc. Responsib. J. 2015, 11, 658–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Van Birgelen, M.; Semeijn, J.; Behrens, P. Explaining pro-environment consumer behavior in air travel. J. Air Transp. Manag. 2011, 17, 125–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  108. Abdul-Muhmin, A.G. Explaining consumers? willingness to be environmentally friendly. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2007, 31, 237–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Gonçalves, H.; Viegas, A. Explaining consumer use of renewable energy: Determinants and gender and age moderator effects. J. Glob. Sch. Mark. Sci. 2015, 25, 198–215. [Google Scholar]
  110. Chen, M.-F. An examination of the value-belief-norm theory model in predicting pro-environmental behaviour in Taiwan. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 18, 145–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Ernst, J.; Blood, N.; Beery, T. Environmental action and student environmental leaders: Exploring the influence of environmental attitudes, locus of control, and sense of personal responsibility. Environ. Educ. Res. 2015, 23, 149–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Wynveen, C.; Sutton, S.G. Engaging Great Barrier Reef Stakeholders: Mediation Analyses of Barriers Among the Antecedents of Pro-Environmental Behavior. Hum. Dimens. Wildl. 2016, 22, 126–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Blok, V.; Wesselink, R.; Studynka, O.; Kemp, R. Encouraging sustainability in the workplace: A survey on the pro-environmental behaviour of university employees. J. Clean. Prod. 2015, 106, 55–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Andersson, L.; Shivarajan, S.; Blau, G. Enacting Ecological Sustainability in the MNC: A Test of an Adapted Value-Belief-Norm Framework. J. Bus. Ethic 2005, 59, 295–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Nordlund, A.M.; Garvill, J. Effects of values, problem awareness, and personal norm on willingness to reduce personal car use. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 339–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Staats, H.; Harland, P.; Wilke, H.A.M. Effecting Durable Change. Environ. Behav. 2016, 36, 341–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  117. Andersson, M.; Von Borgstede, C. Differentiation of determinants of low-cost and high-cost recycling. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 402–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Chen, M.-F.; Tung, P.-J. Developing an extended Theory of Planned Behavior model to predict consumers’ intention to visit green hotels. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2014, 36, 221–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Clark, W.A.; Finley, J.C. Determinants of Water Conservation Intention in Blagoevgrad, Bulgaria. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2007, 20, 613–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Prete, M.I.; Piper, L.; Rizzo, C.; Pino, G.; Capestro, M.; Mileti, A.; Pichierri, M.; Amatulli, C.; Peluso, A.M.; Guido, G. Determinants of Southern Italian households’ intention to adopt energy efficiency measures in residential buildings. J. Clean. Prod. 2017, 153, 83–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Han, H.; Jae, M.; Hwang, J. Cruise travelers’ environmentally responsible decision-making: An integrative framework of goal-directed behavior and norm activation process. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2016, 53, 94–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Klöckner, C.A.; Blöbaum, A. A comprehensive action determination model: Toward a broader understanding of ecological behaviour using the example of travel mode choice. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 574–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  123. Han, H.; Hwang, J.; Lee, S. Cognitive, affective, normative, and moral triggers of sustainable intentions among convention-goers. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017, 51, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Lin, J.-C.; Wu, C.-S.; Liu, W.-Y.; Lee, C.-C. Behavioral intentions toward afforestation and carbon reduction by the Taiwanese public. For. Policy Econ. 2012, 14, 119–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Doran, R.; Hanss, D.; Larsen, S. Attitudes, efficacy beliefs, and willingness to pay for environmental protection when travelling. Tour. Hosp. Res. 2015, 15, 281–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  126. Deng, J.; Sun, P.; Zhao, F.; Han, X.; Yang, G.; Feng, Y. Analysis of the ecological conservation behavior of farmers in payment for ecosystem service programs in eco-environmentally fragile areas using social psychology models. Sci. Total. Environ. 2016, 550, 382–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Knussen, C.; Yule, F.; MacKenzie, J.; Wells, M. An analysis of intentions to recycle household waste: The roles of past behaviour, perceived habit, and perceived lack of facilities. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 237–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Castro, P.; Garrido, M.; Reis, E.; Menezes, J. Ambivalence and conservation behaviour: An exploratory study on the recycling of metal cans. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 24–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Bissonnette, M.M.; Contento, I.R. Adolescents’ perspectives and food choice behaviors in terms of the environmental impacts of food production practices: Application of a psychosocial model. J. Nutr. Educ. 2001, 33, 72–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Meinhold, J.L.; Malkus, A.J. Adolescent Environmental Behaviors. Environ. Behav. 2016, 37, 511–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Hunecke, M.; Blöbaum, A.; Matthies, E.; Höger, R. Responsibility and Environment. Environ. Behav. 2001, 33, 830–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Guagnano, G.A.; Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T. Influences on Attitude-Behavior Relationships. Environ. Behav. 1995, 27, 699–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Cottrell, S.P. Influence of Sociodemographics and Environmental Attitudes on General Responsible Environmental Behavior among Recreational Boaters. Environ. Behav. 2003, 35, 347–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Corbett, J.B. Motivations to Participate in Riparian Improvement Programs. Sci. Commun. 2002, 23, 243–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Tanner, C.; Kast, S.W. Promoting sustainable consumption: Determinants of green purchases by Swiss consumers. Psychol. Mark. 2003, 20, 883–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Chao, Y.-L.; Lam, S.-P. Measuring Responsible Environmental Behavior: Self-Reported and Other-Reported Measures and Their Differences in Testing a Behavioral Model. Environ. Behav. 2011, 43, 53–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Rise, J.; Thompson, M.; Verplanken, B. Measuring implementation intentions in the context of the theory of planned behavior. Scand. J. Psychol. 2003, 44, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Laudenslager, M.S.; Holt, D.T. Understanding Air Force Members’ Intentions to Participate In Pro-Environmental Behaviors: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2004, 98, 1162–1170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Kaiser, F.G.; Gutscher, H. The Proposition of a General Version of the Theory of Planned Behavior: Predicting Ecological Behavior1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 33, 586–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Hamid, P.N.; Cheng, S.-T. Predicting Antipollution Behavior. Environ. Behav. 1995, 27, 679–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Bratt, C. Do Norms and Assumed Consequences Influence Environmental Behavior? Environ. Behav. 1999, 1, 630–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Davies, J.; Foxall, G.R.; Pallister, J. Beyond the Intention–Behaviour Mythology. Mark. Theory 2002, 2, 29–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Truelove, H.B.; Joireman, J. Understanding the Relationship Between Christian Orthodoxy and Environmentalism. Environ. Behav. 2009, 41, 806–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Joireman, J.A.; Lasane, T.P.; Bennett, J.; Richards, D.; Solaimani, S. Integrating social value orientation and the consideration of future consequences within the extended norm activation model of pro-environmental behaviour. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 40, 133–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  145. Kaiser, F.G.; Shimoda, T.A. Responsibility as a Predictor of Ecological Behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 1999, 19, 243–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. Kaiser, F.G.; Wölfing, S.; Fuhrer, U. Environmental Attitude and Ecological Behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 1999, 19, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  147. Minton, A.P.; Rose, R.L. The Effects of Environmental Concern on Environmentally Friendly Consumer Behavior: An Exploratory Study. J. Bus. Res. 1997, 40, 37–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Nilsson, M.; Kuller, R. Travel behaviour and environmental concern. Transp. Res. Part D: Transp. Environ. 2000, 5, 211–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Tanner, C. Constraints on Environmental Behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 1999, 19, 145–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Terry, D.J.; Hogg, M.A.; White, K.M. The theory of planned behaviour: Self-identity, social identity and group norms. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 38, 225–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Grønhøj, A.; Thøgersen, J. Action speaks louder than words: The effect of personal attitudes and family norms on adolescents’ pro-environmental behaviour. J. Econ. Psychol. 2012, 33, 292–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. Kim, S.-Y.; Yoon, J.; Choi, I. What matters to promote consumers’ intention to patronize sustainable business-and-industry (B&I) food services? Br. Food J. 2016, 118, 2710–2731. [Google Scholar]
  153. Carrico, A.; Riemer, M. Motivating energy conservation in the workplace: An evaluation of the use of group-level feedback and peer education. J. Environ. Psychol. 2011, 31, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Liobikienė, G.; Grincevičienė, Š.; Bernatonienė, J. Environmentally friendly behaviour and green purchase in Austria and Lithuania. J. Clean. Prod. 2017, 142, 3789–3797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Chakraborty, A.; Singh, M.P.; Roy, M.; Filho, W.L.; Sima, M. A study of goal frames shaping pro-environmental behaviour in university students. Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 2017, 18, 1291–1310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  156. Hassan, S.H. The role of Islamic values on green purchase intention. J. Islam. Mark. 2014, 5, 379–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Fang, W.-T.; Ng, E.; Chang, M.-C. Physical Outdoor Activity versus Indoor Activity: Their Influence on Environmental Behaviors. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Heal. 2017, 14, 797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  158. Choi, A.; Ritchie, B.W.; Fielding, K.S. A Mediation Model of Air Travelers’ Voluntary Climate Action. J. Travel Res. 2015, 55, 709–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  159. Eriksson, L.; Forward, S.E. Is the intention to travel in a pro-environmental manner and the intention to use the car determined by different factors? Transp. Res. Part D: Transp. Environ. 2011, 16, 372–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  160. Forward, S. Exploring people’s willingness to bike using a combination of the theory of planned behavioural and the transtheoretical model. Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 2014, 64, 151–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  161. Mobley, C.; Vagias, W.M.; DeWard, S.L. Exploring Additional Determinants of Environmentally Responsible Behavior: The Influence of Environmental Literature and Environmental Attitudes. Environ. Behav. 2009, 42, 420–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  162. Echegaray, F.; Hansstein, F. Assessing the intention-behavior gap in electronic waste recycling: The case of Brazil. J. Clean. Prod. 2017, 142, 180–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  163. Eriksson, L.; Garvill, J.; Nordlund, A.M. Acceptability of travel demand management measures: The importance of problem awareness, personal norm, freedom, and fairness. J. Environ. Psychol. 2006, 26, 15–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  164. Allen, J.B.; Ferrand, J.L. Environmental Locus of Control, Sympathy, And Pro-environmental Behavior: A Test of Geller’s Actively Caring Hypothesis. Environ. Behav. 1999, 31, 338–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  165. Gregory, G.D.; di Leo, M. Repeated behaviour and environmental psychology: The role of personal involvement and habit formation in explaining water consumption. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 33, 1261–1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  166. Lynne, G.D.; Casey, C.F.; Hodges, A.; Rahmani, M. Conservation technology adoption decisions and the theory of planned behavior. J. Econ. Psychol. 1995, 16, 581–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  167. Werner, C.M.; Mäkelä, E. Motivations and Behaviors that Support Recycling. J. Environ. Psychol. 1998, 18, 373–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  168. Jorgen, G.; Agneta, M.; Annika, N. Effects of increased awareness on choice of travel mode. Transportation 2003, 30, 63–79. [Google Scholar]
  169. Bamberg, S.; Hunecke, M.; Blöbaum, A. Social context, personal norms and the use of public transportation: Two field studies. J. Environ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 190–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  170. Veludo-De-Oliveira, T.; Pallister, J.G.; Foxall, G.R. Accounting for Sustained Volunteering by Young People: An Expanded TPB. Volunt. Int. J. Volunt. Nonprofit Organ. 2012, 24, 1180–1198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  171. Thøgersen, J.; Ölander, F. The Dynamic Interaction of Personal Norms and Environment-Friendly Buying Behavior: A Panel Study 1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 36, 1758–1780. [Google Scholar]
1
The Social learning theory has also been applied to understand individual and group pro-environmental behaviours related to networking and the development of tacit knowledge [34] [This theory regards behavioural change as a learning process where the individual moulds his/her behaviour by observing and learning from their environment mainly through interactions and personal communication [35]. This theory was not tested in the present study due to a lack of data for variables that explicitly measure experiential learning and how knowledge is acquired through networking.
2
The p-value associated with the Q-statistic and I-squared statistic were significant (p < 0.001) with the I2 statistic mostly greater than 80 (Appendix D, see also Appendix C for the 95% confidence intervals).
3
Although the chi square appears to be a traditional measure for assessing overall model fit [57], we do not rely on it. This is because it is susceptible to sample size, thus, unlikely to accurately differentiate between an acceptable fitting model and a poor one. We therefore rely on the Normed Fit Index (NFI) and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI). This combination helps us to overcome problems of sample size susceptibility, as the CFI is least affected by the sample size [57].
Figure 1. Integrated Framework of the behavioural theories to aid understanding of pro-environmental behaviour. Note: We use letters to represent the different pathways (e.g., knowledge-awareness link, awareness-behaviour link, knowledge-behaviour link) hypothesised in each of the theories.
Figure 1. Integrated Framework of the behavioural theories to aid understanding of pro-environmental behaviour. Note: We use letters to represent the different pathways (e.g., knowledge-awareness link, awareness-behaviour link, knowledge-behaviour link) hypothesised in each of the theories.
Land 09 00135 g001
Figure 2. Research design used in this study.
Figure 2. Research design used in this study.
Land 09 00135 g002
Figure 3. Document Selection Process.
Figure 3. Document Selection Process.
Land 09 00135 g003
Figure 4. Integrated Framework of paths tested under the different theories. Note: We use letters to represent the different pathways (e.g., knowledge-awareness link, awareness-behaviour link, knowledge-behaviour link) hypothesised in each of the theories.
Figure 4. Integrated Framework of paths tested under the different theories. Note: We use letters to represent the different pathways (e.g., knowledge-awareness link, awareness-behaviour link, knowledge-behaviour link) hypothesised in each of the theories.
Land 09 00135 g004
Table 1. Definitions of study variables.
Table 1. Definitions of study variables.
Variable Definition
KnowledgeFactual information regarding an environmental problem, action strategy or standard.
Beliefs Perceived knowledge of an environmental problem or action strategy.
Awareness A profound understanding or consciousness of an environmental problem or action strategy.
Attitude A relatively stable feeling about the environment, a problem or an action strategy.
Habit A way of behaving that has become a routine and a relatively stable behavioural pattern, often occurring without pronounced deliberate effort.
IntentionAn intent or resolve (not) to effect environmentally significant behaviours.
Behavioural Willingness “An openness to risk opportunity—what an individual would be willing to do under various circumstances” [44]
Behaviour Actions, acts or measures that support or result in environmental management gains or at worst causes no harm to the environment.
Social Norm Social or group understandings of what individuals ought to do.
Moral Norm A feeling of moral obligation as opposed to what he perceives that others think [45].
Subjective Norm Perceptions regarding what others think of him/her concerning a behaviour in question i.e., perceived social pressure.
Emotions Emotions are reactions to an object or a process, and they include both sentimental and cognitive aspects.
Self-EfficacySelf-efficacy is a personal judgement of how well an individual can perform courses of action necessary to deal with prospective situations.
Perceived Consequences Views regarding the outcomes of certain actions or inactions, or an environmental issue.
Ascription of Responsibility Claiming or apportioning responsibility for environmental problems and solutions to self, other individuals or institutions/society.
Situational Factors Extrinsic factors that contingently facilitate or constrain pro-environmental behaviour. These may include institutional/regulatory support or constraints, time, farm size, farm type, land tenure system, income, cost of materials, etc.
Environmental Value Refers to a desirable trans-situational goal regarding the environment, ecology, ecosystems or nature that serve as a guiding principle or influence individuals’ environmental behaviours.
Environmental Concern An assessment of one’s own behaviour, or others’ actions with consequences for ecological systems or nature.
Table 2. Classification of Papers based on Different Environmental Policy Areas.
Table 2. Classification of Papers based on Different Environmental Policy Areas.
NumberIssue Area/Broad Focus Items Covered
1Recycling Psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviours related to recycling, public support for recycling support, etc.
2Land management The main issues here include sustainable agriculture/conservation, biodiversity, diffuse pollution water quality issues
3Sustainable consumer behaviourGreen behaviour, sustainable transport modes, renewable and efficient energy use, etc.
4General ecological behaviour Cross-cutting issues e.g., climate change or paper discusses issues that cut across two or three of the specific areas above
Table 3. Summary of papers based on different environmental policy areas.
Table 3. Summary of papers based on different environmental policy areas.
NumberEnvironmental Policy AreaNumber of Papers% of Total
1Recycling 1614.7
2Land management 76.4
3Sustainable consumer behaviour5247.7
4General ecological behaviour 3431.2
Total109100.0
Table 4. Number of independent primary bivariate correlation coefficients and pooled total sample size.
Table 4. Number of independent primary bivariate correlation coefficients and pooled total sample size.
VARIABLE 123456789101112131415161718
1Knowledge-
2BeliefsK(4)
N(2739)
-
3Awareness or consciousness K(2)
N(523)
K(3)
N(2244
-
4Attitude k(10)
N(5462)
K(7)
N(4455)
K(10)
N(3080)
-
5Habit K(1)
N(352)
K(1)
N(319)
K(4)
N(1597)
-
6IntentionK(8)
N(4401)
K(4)
N(2889)
K(13)
N(4746)
K(46)
N(21,886)
K(5)
N(1563)
-
7Behavioural willingness K(5)
N(1222)
K(1)
N(1467)
K(6)
N(2353)
K(4)
N(1060)
0K(4)
N(911)
-
8Behaviour K(9)
N(4106)
K(7)
N(12,828)
K(17)
N(7694)
K(30)
N(18,078)
K(4)
N(1493)
K(29)
N(10,805)
K(5)
N(1498)
-
9Social norm K(2)
N(627)
K(2)
N(751)
K(6)
N(4621)
K(9)
N(6557)
K(9)
N(2658)
-
10Moral norm K(1)
N(352)
K(1)
N(316)
K(9)
N(5415)
K(7)
N(3132)
K(3)
N(1462)
-
11Subjective norm K(2)
N(2202)
K(6)
N(1944)
K(42)
N(22,242)
K(33)
N(15,355)
K(20)
N(8676)
-
12Emotions K(1)
N(321)
K(10)
N(3653)
K(6)
N(2286)
K(13)
N(5323)
K(5)
N(1777)
K(6)
N(2420)
-
13Self-efficacy K(4)
N(3377)
K(6)
N(2158)
K(37)
N(22,390)
K(2)
N(1079)
K(39)
N(19,528)
K(6)
N(1809)
K23)
N(9822)
K(4)
N(1733)
K(5)
N(3472)
K(9)
N(8757)
-
14Perceived consequences K(3)
N(971)
K(6)
N(1479)
K(10)
N(3346)
K(19)
N(6402)
K(12)
N(4324)
K(3)
N(1181)
K(12)
N(6684)
-
15Ascription of responsibility K(3)
N(493)
K(5)
N(1314)
K(8)
N(2907)
K(6)
N(2006)
K(2)
N(676)
K(3)
N(974)
-
16Situational factors e.g., time, institutional support K(9)
N(3097)
K(11)
N(5194)
-
17Environmental value K(2)
N(762)
K(2)
N(590)
K(7)
N(2494)
-
18Environmental concern K(4)
N(1103)
K(7)
N(2743)
K(11)
N(4404)
-
K = Number of independent correlation coefficients obtained from reviewed papers and N = total sample size from respective K.
Table 5. Pooled correlations for all environmental policy areas.
Table 5. Pooled correlations for all environmental policy areas.
SN Construct 123456789101112131415161718
1Knowledge-
2Beliefs 0.51-
3Awareness 0.250.16-
4Attitude 0.360.520.31-
5Habit 0.22 a 0.02 a0.03-
6Intention0.400.420.410.530.12-
7Behavioural willingness 0.270.23 a0.390.2000.43-
8Behaviour 0.280.240.220.370.280.440.34-
9Social norm 0.22 0.290.37 0.42 0.42-
10Moral norm 0.23 0.410.62 0.48 0.36 -
11Subjective norm 0.57 0.400.45 0.43 0.25 -
12Emotions 0.45 a 0.240.28 NS 0.38 0.11 NS0.18 NS -
13Self-efficacy 0.18 0.320.390.280.490.390.270.310.470.31 -
14Perceived consequences 0.22 0.670.42 0.38 0.150.180.49 -
15Ascription of responsibility 0.410.37 0.51 0.230.35 0.47 -
16Situational factors −0.02 NS −0.07 NS -
17Environmental value 0.40 0.42 0.39 -
18Environmental concern 0.30 0.24 0.30 -
Note: Correlations under random-effects assumption; a = no pooled correlation (i.e., only one correlation was retrieved); NS = non-significant relationship (all other r values are significant at the p ≤ 0.05). We combined the locus of control and perceived behavioural control as self-efficacy due to limited data.
Table 6. Pooled correlations across different environmental policy areas.
Table 6. Pooled correlations across different environmental policy areas.
Group Correlation Lower Limit Upper Limit
Awareness-behaviour relationship
Recycling 0.12−0.350.55
Land management 0.25−0.220.62
Sustainable consumer behaviour 0.23 **0.070.37
General ecological behaviour 0.20 *0.000.38
Overall0.21 **0.100.32
Attitude-behaviour relationship
Recycling 0.12−0.350.55
Land management 0.25−0.220.62
Sustainable consumer behaviour 0.23 **0.070.37
General ecological behaviour 0.20 *0.000.38
Overall0.21 **0.100.32
Intention-behaviour relationship
Recycling 0.61 **0.410.76
Land management 0.07 a−0.010.15
Sustainable consumer behaviour 0.47 **0.310.60
General ecological behaviour 0.40 **0.300.49
Overall0.23 **0.180.29
Self-efficacy-behaviour relationship
Recycling 0.44 **0.270.59
Land management 0.23 *0.010.42
Sustainable consumer behaviour 0.32 **0.180.45
General ecological behaviour 0.18 **0.040.32
Overall0.28 **0.200.36
Subjective norm-behaviour relationship
Recycling 0.18 **0.050.30
Land management 0.33−0.260.74
Sustainable consumer behaviour 0.33 **0.180.46
General ecological behaviour 0.07−0.120.26
Overall0.21 **0.120.29
Notes: ** p ≤ 0.01; * p ≤ 0.05; a = just 1 correlation retrieved.
Table 7. Model fit indices.
Table 7. Model fit indices.
Theories DF NFICFIJudgement
Persuasion Theory 31.01.0Good fit
Theory of Reasoned Action31.01.0Good fit
Theory of Planned Behaviour 51.01.0Good fit
Norm Activation Model 2--Poor fit
Value-Belief-Norm Model2-0.0Poor fit
Attitude-Behaviour-Context (ABC) Model 01.01.0Good fit
DF= Degrees of freedom; NFI = Normed Fit Index; CFI = Comparative Fit Index.
Table 8. Results of regression paths.
Table 8. Results of regression paths.
Independent Variable Dependent Variable EstimateStandard Error Total Explained Variance
Persuasion Theory
Knowledge Awareness−0.10.4-
Awareness Attitude0.8 ***0.20.56
Attitude Behaviour0.7 ***0.10.49
Theory of Reasoned Action
Attitude Intention 0.9 ***0.10.78
Subjective norm Intention−0.20.3-
Intention Behaviour0.9 ***0.10.74
Theory of Planned Behaviour
Attitude Intention 0.7 ***0.10.77a
Subjective norm Intention0.30.2-
Self-efficacy Intention0.5 ***0.10.77 a
Self-efficacyBehaviour−0.4 ***0.10.76 a
Intention Behaviour1.0 ***0.10.76 a
Attitude-Behaviour-Context (ABC) Theory
Attitude Behaviour0.7 ***0.10.49
Notes: *** p < 0.001; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05; a = cumulative effect of two or more variables. Path results for Value-Belief-Norm and Norm Activation Models were excluded as data did not fit models.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Okumah, M.; Martin-Ortega, J.; Novo, P.; J. Chapman, P. Revisiting the Determinants of Pro-Environmental Behaviour to Inform Land Management Policy: A Meta-Analytic Structural Equation Model Application. Land 2020, 9, 135. https://doi.org/10.3390/land9050135

AMA Style

Okumah M, Martin-Ortega J, Novo P, J. Chapman P. Revisiting the Determinants of Pro-Environmental Behaviour to Inform Land Management Policy: A Meta-Analytic Structural Equation Model Application. Land. 2020; 9(5):135. https://doi.org/10.3390/land9050135

Chicago/Turabian Style

Okumah, Murat, Julia Martin-Ortega, Paula Novo, and Pippa J. Chapman. 2020. "Revisiting the Determinants of Pro-Environmental Behaviour to Inform Land Management Policy: A Meta-Analytic Structural Equation Model Application" Land 9, no. 5: 135. https://doi.org/10.3390/land9050135

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop