Next Article in Journal
Effects of Soil Particle Structure on the Distribution and Transport of Soil Water and Salt
Next Article in Special Issue
An Evaluation of the Humanitarian Supply Chains in the Event of Flash Flooding
Previous Article in Journal
Multi-Parameter Algorithms of Remote Sensing Reflectance, Absorption and Backscattering for Coastal Waters of the Southern Baltic Sea Applied to Pomeranian Lakes
Previous Article in Special Issue
Runoff Simulation under the Effects of the Modified Soil Water Assessment Tool (SWAT) Model in the Jiyun River Basin
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Understanding Community Resilience through the Lens of Stakeholder Participation: Empirical Evidence from the Moat System Restoration Project

Department of Landscape Architecture, Faculty of Design & Architecture, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Serdang 43400, Selangor, Malaysia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Water 2023, 15(15), 2844; https://doi.org/10.3390/w15152844
Submission received: 28 June 2023 / Revised: 30 July 2023 / Accepted: 3 August 2023 / Published: 6 August 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Flood Risk Management and Resilience Volume II)

Abstract

:
In the milieu of escalating flood occurrences, the concept of community resilience has garnered considerable attention. A series of studies recommend a river restoration approach to encourage the participation of all relevant stakeholders to manage floods to improve community resilience. However, existing studies have not sufficiently unraveled the intricate interplay of drivers shaping such participation. Therefore, using the Moat System Restoration Project in Tianchang City as the empirical conduit, this study elucidates the relationship between the drivers that drive stakeholder participation in river restoration in the context of improving community resilience. The Theory of Planned Behavior was employed to extend additional independent variables such as stakeholder attitudes, priorities, risk perception, trust in government decisions, knowledge, motivation, and intentions. A sample of 473 local residents involved in the Moat System Restoration Project was evaluated by Structural Equation Modelling. The empirical results revealed the salient influence of stakeholder attitudes, knowledge, and priorities on their behavior and risk perception where harnessing perceptions of priority has the potential to significantly improve community resilience. Notably, the study dispelled the presumed influence of trust on risk perception, a revelation that deviates from some existing literature. The findings further demonstrate a strong association between stakeholder motivation and both risk perception and intentions, a relationship underappreciated in previous studies. Theoretically, this study unearths the intricate dynamics of stakeholder participation in river restoration projects, thereby extending the Theory of Planned Behavior to the sphere of stakeholder participation, which adds a critical dimension to the understanding of flood management and community resilience. Practically, this study provides a valuable lens for practitioners as it illuminates pathways to foster stakeholder participation in river restoration initiatives, guiding the development of strategies to foster stakeholder participation in similar projects.

1. Introduction

Nowadays, the frequency of floods inevitably challenges adaptation projects, despite the great efforts that have been made to manage floods. Birkholz et al. [1] delineate that no country is immune to the effects of flooding, and floods are one of the most common natural disasters, with approximately 50–300 inland floods occurring annually worldwide [2]. Floods account for an alarming 11.7% of all natural disaster deaths [3]. In response to concerns about the consequences of the increased frequency of flooding problems, the concept of “community resilience” has received increasing attention in the scientific and policy spheres [4,5]. It refers to the ability of a community to anticipate, withstand, and bounce back from adversities, such as natural disasters, which primarily involves the participation of all community members, as well as collaboration between different sectors and levels of government. As elucidated by Ashmawy [5], resilient communities have access to the needed information and resources to prepare, respond, and recover from disasters. In 2005, the UN World Conference extolled the virtues of a bottom-up approach to community resilience and encouraged the participation of all relevant stakeholders to cater to local exigencies [6]. Indeed, involving stakeholders in the resilience-building process not only helps to increase social acceptance of flood response initiatives but also creates strong bonds between decision-makers and stakeholders working towards co-creating highly resilient communities for Sustainable Development Goals [7]. Consequently, a myriad of countries are proactively fostering the integration of stakeholder and community resilience into risk policies and interventions [8]. In terms of the specific implementation, a series of studies recommend a ‘river restoration’ approach to encourage the participation of all relevant stakeholders managing floods to improve community resilience [9,10]. The Society for Ecological Restoration [11] defines restoration as “a process that assists the recovery of degraded, damaged, or destroyed ecosystems”, and many international organizations have made declarative commitments to engage in restoration as a means of addressing global environmental change [12]. JMR Benayas et al. [13] utilized meta-analysis to confirm that ecological restoration can provide ecological services, restore river socio-ecological functions, improve flood management capabilities, and emphasize the indispensable role of stakeholder participation [14,15].
However, Conniff [10,15] reported that due to insufficient stakeholder participation, 75% of the implemented river restoration projects did not reach their minimum targets. Whereas less resilient community residents tend to be older, marginalized groups who may lack the necessary resources and lag behind other communities in the restoration process [16]. Therefore, investigating the drivers that drive stakeholder participation in river restoration projects has both theoretical and practical implications for building resilience in local communities.
According to Turcanu et al. [17], setting restoration goals is a value-laden event as it involves people’s attitudes, risk perceptions, knowledge, trust in the government, and ultimately their behavior or willingness to participate. Several scholars have examined the relationship between stakeholders and river restoration projects from different perspectives. For example, Alam [18] analyzed the relationship between attitudes and river restoration from the perspective of local attachment, and Schaich [19] measured people’s perceptions of the floodplain and their attitudes towards the restoration measures taken. Phalen [20] explained people’s reactions to restoration projects through human behavior, motivation, and cognitive theory. Aggestam [21] investigated how people’s values influence the restoration process, and Hong et al. [22] showed the relative importance and priority of the values of each stakeholder in a river restoration project. Nevertheless, this study found that few studies have attempted to examine stakeholder knowledge, priorities, and behaviors towards participating in river restoration projects. Follett et al. [23] observed that only 3% of papers addressed participants’ priorities in the context of improving community resilience. To fill this distance, using the medium of the Moat System Restoration Project (MSRP), this study explores the relationship between the drivers that drive stakeholder participation in river restoration in the context of improving community resilience. The findings of this study contribute to shaping local community resilience and help decision-makers know how to motivate stakeholders to participate in environmental decision-making, especially in flood management.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Stakeholders and Theory of Planned Behavior

According to Cascetta et al. [24], stakeholders are defined as individuals and organizations that have a stake in a particular issue, even if they have no formal role in the decision-making process. Yosie and Herbst [25] distinguished between different categories of stakeholders. These include (1) those who are directly affected by the decision and take action; (2) people who have an interest in the project and want to participate and offer input; (3) those who are interested in participating in seeking information; and (4) those affected by the consequences of the decision but unaware of participatory processes. This study follows Freeman’s [26] definition of stakeholders as those who live or work close to the moat system, including local authorities, academics, and relevant institutions. Because their lives will be affected by the MSRP, they have the ability to participate in environmental decision-making.
In fact, stakeholder participation in environmental decision-making has been widely discussed in previous studies. Both the Water Framework Directive [27] and the EU Flood Assessment and Management Directive [28] require stakeholder participation in environmental decision-making and emphasize the need for coordination of efforts between different stakeholders, as simply following the people-centered principle itself may lead to mismatches of needs and delays in responsibilities [29,30]. According to the Asian Development Bank [31], engaging people outside of academia and politics in decision-making supports good governance, citizenship, and accountability and helps local authorities use available evidence to make decisions that address stakeholder priorities. Moreover, bringing together multiple stakeholders facilitates mutual learning to integrate priorities not often considered by decision-makers [32]. However, Blicharska and Rönnbäck [33] highlighted that relevant stakeholder willingness to act is required if a specific project or policy is to be implemented. Previous studies have shown that a theoretical model frequently used to identify stakeholders’ attitudinal factors related to behavior is the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) [34]. Thus, this study extended TPB theory to consider the situation of MSRP by adding additional independent variables to understand the relationships between the factors driving stakeholder participation in such river restoration projects in the context of improving community resilience.
TPB has been applied in the field of participation and has proved to be effective in explaining relevant behavioral intentions [35]. The TPB assumes that an individual’s participation behavior is the result of his or her intentions, and behavioral intentions are affected by three distinct factors: attitude toward behavior, subjective norms, and perceived behavior control [36]. Specifically, attitudes refer to people’s evaluation of particular objects in a like or dislike manner, and subjective norms refer to the perceived social pressure to perform or not perform behavior. Perceived behavioral control is people’s perception of the ease or difficulty of performing the behavior. If a person has a positive assessment of a particular behavior, believes that people who are important to them want them to perform the behavior, and perceives it to be easy, they will have a strong intention to perform the behavior. For example, stakeholders may feel that their participation can influence the outcome of a restoration project, which falls within the realm of perceived behavioral control.
Considering the situation of MSRP, the term ‘subjective norms’ is replaced by stakeholder behaviors. The concept of ‘perceived behavioral control’ is replaced by stakeholder knowledge. More specifically, ‘attitude’ in this study can be interpreted as stakeholders’ response to MSRP, while ‘behavior’ refers to stakeholder’s participation behavior. The concept of ‘motivation’ refers to the driving factors that make stakeholders voluntarily participate in decision-making, while ‘intention’ represents the goals that stakeholders expect to achieve through their participation behavior. The term ‘risk perception’ refers to stakeholders’ perceived level of risk to the moat system, while ‘knowledge’ reflects stakeholders’ familiarity with participating in MSRP decision-making. The concept of ‘trust’ indicates the degree of trust stakeholder confidence in local government or government decisions.

2.2. Research Hypotheses and Conceptual Framework

2.2.1. Stakeholder Attitudes and Priorities

It has been acknowledged that the effectiveness of river restoration projects depends mainly on stakeholders’ attitudes [37]. Kelly [38] established that attitude is the most important predictor of people’s participation. Stakeholders are more likely to participate if they have a positive attitude toward a river restoration project. Deffner and Haase [39] indicated that most people have a positive attitude toward river restoration and benefit from restored river sections. Tunstall et al. [40] emphasized the importance of understanding stakeholder perceptions of restoration goals and incorporating them into river restoration decisions. Risk decision priorities have been shown to correlate with risk perception [41]. In the literature, prioritization means ranking objectives in order of importance [42], which can reflect the ideal river restoration goals of stakeholders and enhance the fairness of decision-making [43]. For example, Weitzner and Deutsch [44] explored the relationship between stakeholder priorities and motivation and demonstrated that individuals’ priorities are linked to their attitudes and risk perceptions [45]. Gallego-Ayala and Juízo [46] successfully identified stakeholders’ priorities by applying the analytic hierarchy process method. Based on the above discussion, this study proposes the following hypotheses:
H1. 
Stakeholder attitudes have a positive impact on stakeholder behavior towards participation in river restoration decision-making.
H2. 
Stakeholder attitudes have a positive impact on stakeholder priorities.
H3. 
Stakeholder priorities have a positive impact on stakeholder behavior.

2.2.2. Risk Perception and Trust in Government Decisions

Risk perception plays an important role in decision-making [47], as it can lead decision-makers to make decisions and take action to reduce risk [48]. Paul Slovic [49] defines the concept of “risk perception” as people’s subjective assessment or judgment of potential risks. Moreover, risk perception has been previously incorporated into the TPB framework [50], and sufficient evidence shows that risk perception can affect people’s participation behavior [51] and behavioral intention [52]. There is a correlation between risk perception and attitudes [53,54]. Likewise, Moen [55] observed that risk perception has a positive effect on priorities. When the perceived risk is high, the intention to choose safer alternatives is seen as prioritizing safety. However, Swapan’s [56] empirical study found that a lack of trust in the government was the cause of stakeholder indifference to participation. A correlation exists between trust and participation [57]. Low trust in government reduces participation, while low participation reduces trust [58]. The higher perceived risk reduces people’s confidence in the governing institutions [59].
Rousseau et al. [60] defined ‘trust’ as a state of mind comprising the intention to accept vulnerability based upon positive expectations of the intentions or behavior of another that allows individuals to determine whether they are willing to let others influence their decisions and behavior [61]. According to previous studies, risk perception, participation, and trust in local government are closely related [62]. Carnevale and Wechsler [63] found that trust and attitudes are related to some extent. Trust in local government can affect citizens’ motivation to participate in decision-making [64]. When laypeople do not have specific knowledge, they will place more trust in authorities when judging risks [65]. Thus, this study proposes the following hypotheses:
H4. 
Stakeholder priorities have a positive impact on stakeholder risk perception.
H5. 
Stakeholder risk perception has a positive impact on stakeholder behavior.
H6. 
Stakeholders’ trust in government decisions has a positive impact on stakeholder behavior.
H7. 
Stakeholders’ trust in government decisions has a positive impact on stakeholder risk perception.

2.2.3. Stakeholder Knowledge

According to Ajzen [36], the concept of knowledge refers to situational factors that influence attitudes toward certain behaviors. In the existing literature, knowledge is often considered a prerequisite for one’s behavior [66]. Some scholars have studied how knowledge can predict behavior by applying TPB [67]. Dreyer [68] argued that people’s willingness to participate is influenced by trust and knowledge factors. Stakeholders who have the required knowledge are more likely to participate in decision-making [69]. Rather, some of these stakeholders are unwilling to participate because they do not know the behavior to be performed [70]. In addition, Venkataramanan [71] found that knowledge helps shape people’s attitudes and predict their intentions and motivations for behavioral change. Knowledge also helps determine people’s priorities [72]. The following hypotheses were proposed:
H8. 
Stakeholder knowledge has a positive impact on stakeholder behavior.
H9. 
Stakeholder knowledge has a positive impact on stakeholder priorities.

2.2.4. Participation, Motivation, Intention, and Behavior

Participation is one of the outcomes of motivation [73]. Gul [74] define the term ‘motivation’ as ‘directing a person to behave in a certain way’. Understanding stakeholder motivation could stimulate their desire to participate in and improve the quality of decision-making. The concept of motivation includes both extrinsic and intrinsic motivation [75]. Following Deci [76], one is said to be intrinsically motivated to perform a behavior when one receives no apparent reward except the behavior itself. Shirk et al.’s [77] and Ryan et al.’s [78] surveys found that people’s motivation for participating in environmental stewardship is to help the environment and to learn. Instead, extrinsic motivation is just a tool to achieve a certain desired outcome [79]. TPB states that the most proximate determinant of an individual’s behavior is his or her intention to engage in the behavior [36]. Expected value theory suggests that human behavioral intentions are based on the expectation that a particular behavior will produce a particular outcome [80,81]. However, Gollwitzer [82] argues that a key factor leading to intention formation and behavior is personal motivation to achieve the ultimate goal. Behavior is guided by intentions [83]. People’s beliefs and desires causally combine to determine their intentions, which in turn control their behavior [84]. Accordingly, the following hypotheses were proposed:
H10. 
Stakeholder motivation has a positive impact on stakeholder risk perception.
H11. 
Stakeholder motivation has a positive impact on stakeholder intentions.
H12. 
Stakeholder intentions have a positive impact on stakeholder behavior.
According to the description and analysis above, the conceptual framework has been developed, which summarizes the hypothesized relationships, as shown in Figure 1.

3. Methods

3.1. Study Area

The study was undertaken in Tianchang City, Anhui Province, China, which is the nearest city to the mouth of the Yangtze River in this province, as it is seriously affected by floods almost every year, causing huge economic losses and negative impacts. In detail, Tianchang is a sub-prefecture-level city with a total area of 1770 square kilometers. The local population is about 628,900. Its average temperature is between 14 and 17 °C, and its annual precipitation is between 750 and 1700 mm. During the summer, Tianchang has frequent and intense rainfall, leading to more vulnerability to flooding.
The moat system is a famous landmark of Tianchang City, with the unique functions of cultural heritage and ecological optimization. In 2017, some scholars discovered a section of city wall ruins in the moat system. The city wall is about 17.65 m in length. Some of the bricks still retain old symbols. The moat system originated in the Ming and Qing dynasties in ancient China and was mainly used for flood control and protection of the city. However, Zheng [85] argues that the function of the moat is more for drainage purposes than its original defensive function. When the city suffers from floods, the moat can play the role of flood discharge, thereby reducing the risk of flooding. Figure 2 shows the location of the moat system in Tianchang City.
The moat system has experienced frequent flooding problems due to frequent and concentrated rainfall. One of the worst floods in Anhui during the last three years affected 1.608 million people, resulting in four deaths and direct economic losses of 6.55 billion yuan, as reported by China’s Ministry of Emergency Management. Communities that settled near the moat system suffered a series of negative impacts due to flooding in the moat system. To address this serious issue, the Tianchang government and the Moat System Management Team initiated the MSRP to increase the resilience of the local communities and inspire stakeholders to participate in it.
The success of river restoration depends largely on the stakeholders who may support or oppose restoration decisions [86]. Conversely, even though the Tianchang government has recognized the urgency of taking measures to mitigate frequent flooding, previous response decisions have not always been accepted by the population, which indicates that the frequent flooding problem is apparent to local authorities, but the stakeholders may not be as conscious, thus restricting the progress of river restoration and the development of community resilience. Stakeholder participation is an important step in ensuring that the MSRP considers stakeholder needs, priorities, and interests. Thus, employing the MSRP in Tianchang City as the study area offers a compelling case for an investigation of the relationship between the drivers that drive stakeholder participation in river restoration projects in the context of improving community resilience.

3.2. Measures

This study utilized the questionnaire as the survey instrument. The questionnaire contained two sections. The first section is designed to collect the demographic characteristics of the respondents, including questions on respondents’ knowledge of participation in MSRP and whether they favored flood management as a priority. The second section consists of a series of measurement items for identified variables and the measurement items were adapted from the scales in the existing literature. Specifically, the stakeholder knowledge (SE) measurement item was adapted from [87,88]. Stakeholder trust in government decisions (STD) was used in Mah’s [89] scale with a minor modification. Stakeholder Risk Perception (SRP) referenced the scale developed by Su [53] and Whitmarsh [90]. We adopted the scale from Beechie [91] and Berander [92] to measure stakeholder priorities (SP). To measure the participation motivation (SM) of stakeholders, we adopted the 5-item scale of van Riper [93] and Corbett [94]. Stakeholders’ participation intention (SI) was measured using the scale from Woosnam [95] and Venkataramanan [71]. Stakeholder attitudes (SA) towards MSRP were captured by adapting the items from Schaich [19] and Tunstall [96]. Stakeholder Participation Behavior (SB) uses the scale from Wang [97] and Sarvilinna [98], with slight modifications. All items in the questionnaire were measured on a 5-point scale, from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”.

3.3. Data Collection

Data were collected from 1 February to mid-March 2023. Respondents were drawn from residents living or working in communities near the Tianchang Moat System. All participation was voluntary and anonymous. Most questionnaires were completed by scanning WeChat QR codes. Those unfamiliar with online surveys were collected on-site. The study received a total of 510 questionnaires, 368 online and 142 offline. In the process of data screening, 37 invalid questionnaires were eliminated. Therefore, 473 valid questionnaires were obtained for analysis.

3.4. Data Analysis

SPSS 25.0 was employed for descriptive statistical analysis of the collected data. Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was used to test the hypotheses proposed in this study with the assistance of AMOS 26.0 software [99]. SEM is an advanced multivariate statistical technique employed to incorporate multiple dependent and independent variables into a single model and identify the relationships between various latent constructions or variables [100]. Thus, SEM was considered an appropriate analytical tool in this study.
SEM consists of a measurement model and a structural model [101]. The measurement model focuses on analyzing the causal relationships between measurement items and latent variables, while the structural model tends to explore the associations between latent variables. In specific analysis, first, Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) assessed the validity and model fit of the measurement model. Second, path analysis was carried out to explore the structural model, i.e., testing H1–H12.

4. Results

4.1. Sample Description

The demographic profiles of the respondents are shown in Table 1. Males and females accounted for 50.7% and 49.3%, respectively. It was found that 36.6% of the respondents were aged between 26 and 35 years old, and about 22.2% were aged between 18 and 25. The respondents who were aged between 46 and 55 made up 18.2%, while 17.1% were aged between 36 and 45. The results show that most of the respondents had high educational qualifications. It was found that 37.6% of respondents had a bachelor’s degree as their highest degree; 18% had a master’s degree and 4.2% had a doctorate. Primary and secondary education levels collectively accounted for 40.2%. In terms of employment status, civil servants were 39.7%, students 18.4%, retirees 1.7%, and self-employed 18.0%. Farmers or workers make up 22.2%. Moreover, 22.0% of the respondents have lived around the moat system for more than 20 years. Participants who had lived in this area for 8–13 and 14–19 years accounted for 18.6% and 18.4%, respectively, and 21.1% of the people had lived in the area for 2–7 years. Only 19.9% of respondents had lived near the moat system for less than a year. On the other hand, 20.5% and 20.3% of the participants were extremely familiar and very familiar with the knowledge of participation in MSRP, respectively. The proportion of moderately familiar participants was 19.5. The proportion of slightly familiar participants was 22.2. Only 17.5% of the respondents were not at all familiar with the knowledge of participation in MSRP. Regarding the priority target, 23.9% of respondents strongly approved of “flood protection” as a priority, while 22.2% of respondents strongly disapproved. The proportion of respondents disapproving was 19.2%, while 18.2% approved. It was found that 16.5% of the participants were neutral.

4.2. Measurement Model and CFA

A measurement model encompassing all scale items was tested through CFA. Specifically, the reliability of constructs was first examined by Cronbach’s α value and Composite Reliability (CR). As shown in Table 2, Cronbach’s α value of each construct is between 0.811 and 0.905, exceeding the recommended value of 0.70 [102], while CR coefficients of the variables are between 0.841 and 0.902, above the standard of 0.70 [103]. These indicate that the constructs have good internal consistency and reliability. The convergent validity was secondly checked through factor loadings and Average Variance Extracted (AVE). The results show that the factor loadings are all above 0.7, exceeding the suggested threshold of 0.50, while the AVE values were above 0.6, exceeding the recommended threshold value of 0.50 [102], indicating good convergent validity. Finally, discriminant validity was verified by assessing the correlations between the constructs and the square root of AVE for each construct. The square root of AVE for each construct was more significant than the correlation coefficient between other constructs, indicating adequate convergent validity. As shown in Table 3, the square roots of AVE values were higher than the correlation coefficients. Therefore, the discriminant validity of the constructs was supported.
The fit of the model was tested by chi-square statistics, the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), the approximate root mean square error (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). Table 4 shows that the proposed models are acceptable overall fit, that is measurement model (Chi-square = 762.402; df = 674; Chi-square/df = 1.131; SRMR = 0.031; RMSEA = 0.017; TLI = 0.991; CFI = 0.992) and structural model (Chi-square = 989.358; df = 684; Chi-square/df = 1.446; SRMR = 0.065; RMSEA = 0.031; TLI = 0.970; CFI = 0.972).

4.3. Structural Model and Hypothesis Testing

The measurement model was turned into a structural model by adding hypothesized paths between the constructs. Table 5 shows the results of the hypothesis testing.
The results indicate that the standardized path coefficient from SA to SB was 0.201 (p < 0.001), suggesting that stakeholder attitudes significantly affected stakeholder behavior, thereby supporting H1. The standardized path coefficient from SA to SP was 0.212 (p < 0.001), and the path was significant, suggesting that stakeholder attitudes positively affected stakeholder priorities, thereby supporting H2. Moreover, SP could positively influence both SB (β = 0.175, p < 0.001) and SRP (β = 0.244, p < 0.001), which indicates that stakeholder priorities could positively influence both stakeholder behavior and stakeholder risk perception. Therefore, H3 and H4 were accepted. The standardized path coefficient from SRP to SB was 0.237 (p < 0.001), and the path was significant, suggesting that stakeholder risk perception positively impacted stakeholder behavior. Therefore, H5 is accepted. STD could positively influence SB (β = 0.395, p < 0.001), but STD did not affect SRP (β = 0.013, p > 0.05), which indicates that stakeholder trust in government decisions is significantly correlated with stakeholder behavior. By contrast, stakeholder trust in government decisions did not affect stakeholder risk perception as it failed to reach statistical significance. As a result, H6 was accepted while H7 was rejected. Furthermore, SK could positively influence both SB (β = 0.266, p < 0.001) and SP (β = 0.284, p < 0.001), which indicates that stakeholder knowledge has significant positive effects on stakeholder behavior and priority, respectively. Accordingly, H8 and H9 are supported. Similarly, SM could positively influence both SRP (β = 0.162, p < 0.001) and SI (β = 0.498, p < 0.001), which indicates that stakeholder motivation could positively influence stakeholder risk perception and stakeholder intentions, supporting H10 and H11. Finally, the standardized path coefficient from SI to SB was 0.418 (p < 0.001), suggesting that stakeholder behavior was positively impacted by stakeholder intentions, thereby supporting H12. Figure 3 presents the standardized regression weights of the hypothesis paths.

5. Discussion

Using the MSRP in Tianchang City as the medium, this study explores the relationship between the drivers that drive stakeholder participation in river restoration in the context of improving community resilience. The results indicate that out of 12, only one hypothesis was rejected while the remaining were accepted.
The results reveal the positive influence of stakeholder attitudes on both stakeholder behavior and stakeholder priorities that attitudes serve as a significant determinant of behavior. This is in line with the assertions of Ajzen [104] and buttresses the connection drawn in previous studies between attitudes and participation in environmental management [105,106]. These findings suggest that individuals who harbor favorable perceptions of MSRP are more likely to participate actively, while the perception of river restoration projects is an underpinning driver for stakeholder prioritization. Concurrently, this study broadens the existing discourse, that is attitudes held toward flood protection measures bear a significant sway over stakeholder’s proactivity. Consequently, the potential utility of harnessing favorable stakeholder attitudes and targeted knowledge dissemination as catalysts for proactive stakeholder participation, thereby contributing to the effective execution of river restoration projects and the fortification of community resilience against flooding.
Moreover, stakeholder priorities also demonstrated a substantial effect on stakeholder behavior and risk perception, indicating that these priorities shape behavioral tendencies and perceptions of risk. This, in essence, elucidates the theory that more significant emphasis placed on a particular area, such as flood protection in this study, tends to influence stakeholders’ subsequent behavior and risk perception, complementing prior studies in the realm of environmental risk perception [107]. This revelation has subtle implications for stakeholders’ risk communication and management strategies, suggesting that perceptions of priority can be harnessed to bolster community resilience. More specifically, the nuanced understanding of the relationship between stakeholder priorities and risk perception could inform risk communication strategies, enhancing the efficiency and effectiveness of resilience-building efforts.
The hypothesis that stakeholder risk perception positively impacts stakeholder behavior was affirmed, reaffirming the notion that apprehended risks, in this instance, towards flooding, stimulate the adaptation of congruent behavioral measures, aligning with the findings of scholars such as Leppin and Aro [108]. It is noteworthy, however, that the inability to substantiate Hypothesis 7 implies that trust does not, in fact, influence risk perception, juxtaposing with some literature that argues the vital role of trust in shaping risk perceptions [65], where trust is suggested as a buffer for perceived risks. While intuitively counterintuitive, this finding may suggest that stakeholders independently assess risks, notwithstanding their trust levels in government decisions. This discrepancy may be attributable to context-specific factors or differing conceptualizations of trust, which reveals an intriguing facet of stakeholder psychology, warranting further exploration.
Stakeholder knowledge, we found, had significant positive effects on stakeholder behavior and priorities, demonstrating its centrality in shaping environmental behavior, an insight that resonates with Kollmuss and Agyeman’s [109] affirmation of the vital role knowledge plays in ecological behavior. Stakeholder motivation similarly revealed positive influence on stakeholder risk perception and stakeholder intentions, which underscores its importance as an intrinsic driver of pro-environmental behavior, in line with scholars such as De Groot and Steg [110]. The relationship between stakeholder knowledge, priorities, and behavior elucidates the importance of knowledge in shaping both priorities and behaviors, a significant contribution to our understanding of stakeholder involvement in river restoration projects. Previous studies have emphasized the role of knowledge in stakeholder behavior [111]; our findings further extend this narrative by showcasing its influence on stakeholder priorities. This correlation posits that enhancing stakeholder knowledge could be instrumental in prioritizing river restoration projects more effectively towards improving community resilience.
Of note, a point of divergence with earlier research lies in the influence of stakeholder motivation on stakeholder risk perception and stakeholder intentions, i.e., H10 (β = 0.162, p < 0.001) and H11 (β = 0.498, p < 0.001). While earlier research may suggest a muted relationship between these variables [112], our study provided robust empirical evidence for a strong association, an indication of the evolving nature of stakeholder dynamics in river restoration and improving community resilience contexts.

6. Conclusions

This study utilized the MSRP in Tianchang City as an empirical conduit and endeavored to delineate the complex interplay of factors shaping stakeholder participation in river restoration efforts, contributing to the ongoing discourse on community resilience and flood management.
Theoretically, this study substantiates the applicability of TPB in the realm of flood management and community resilience. It confirms the centrality of attitudes, knowledge, risk perception, and motivations as determinants of stakeholder behavior and priorities. Our findings enrich the existing theoretical landscape by explicating the complex and interdependent nature of these drivers, particularly emphasizing the roles of stakeholder knowledge and priorities—areas hitherto not explored extensively. Noteworthy is our findings’ divergence from earlier scholarship concerning the negligible influence of stakeholder trust on risk perception [65] and the amplified influence of stakeholder motivation on risk perception and intentions [112], reflecting the dynamic nature of stakeholder attitudes and behavior in different contexts.
From the practical perspective, the insights generated here equip practitioners (e.g., policymakers, project managers, and community leaders) with an informed understanding of stakeholder behavior, as these insights illuminate the pathways to foster stakeholder participation in river restoration initiatives, thus enabling the development of efficacious strategies to stimulate active participation in river restoration projects, with the potential to enhance community resilience significantly. The nuanced relationships uncovered between stakeholder priorities, risk perception, and behavior suggest the need for well-crafted communication strategies that take these factors into account to optimize stakeholder participation and, thus, the project outcomes.
However, these findings are not without their constraints. This study concentrated solely on Tianchang City’s MSRP, while the unique cultural, social, and environmental contexts could influence stakeholder behavior and participation, which may differ in other settings. Consequently, future investigations employing a longitudinal design across diverse geographical contexts would augment the external validity and inferential power of our findings. Additionally, the counterintuitive finding concerning stakeholder trust’s negligible influence on risk perception reveals an intriguing facet of stakeholder psychology, inciting further exploration. How stakeholders perceive, assess, and react to risks in the absence of trust merits a comprehensive investigation, potentially uncovering novel dynamics between trust, risk perception, and behavior.

Author Contributions

Methodology, S.Y.; Validation, R.B.I.; Investigation, S.Y.; Writing—original draft, S.Y.; Writing—review & editing, R.B.I. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Data Availability Statement

Data can be made available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Birkholz, S.; Muro, M.; Jeffrey, P.; Smith, H.M. Rethinking the Relationship between Flood Risk Perception and Flood Management. Sci. Total Environ. 2014, 478, 12–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. World Meteorological Organization. Social Aspects and Stakeholder Involvement in Integrated Flood Management; World Meteorological Organization (WMO): Geneva, Switzerland, 2006; ISBN 92-63-11008-5. [Google Scholar]
  3. Xie, Z.; Du, Y.; Zeng, Y.; Miao, Q. Classification of Yearly Extreme Precipitation Events and Associated Flood Risk in the Yangtze-Huaihe River Valley. Sci. China Earth Sci. 2018, 61, 1341–1356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Burnside-Lawry, J.; Carvalho, L. A Stakeholder Approach to Building Community Resilience: Awareness to Implementation. Int. J. Disaster Resil. Built Environ. 2016, 7, 4–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Ashmawy, I.K.I.M. Stakeholder Involvement in Community Resilience: Evidence from Egypt. Environ. Dev. Sustain. 2021, 23, 7996–8011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Moreno, J.; Lara, A.; Torres, M. Community Resilience in Response to the 2010 Tsunami in Chile: The Survival of a Small-Scale Fishing Community. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2019, 33, 376–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Blair, M.J.; Mabee, W.E. Evaluation of Technology, Economics and Emissions Impacts of Community-Scale Bioenergy Systems for a Forest-Based Community in Ontario. Renew. Energy 2020, 151, 715–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. CCA. Building a Resilient Canada: The Expert Panel on Disaster Resilience in a Changing Climate; Council of Canadian Academies: Ottawa, ON, Canada, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  9. Junker, B.; Buchecker, M.; Müller-Böker, U. Objectives of Public Participation: Which Actors Should Be Involved in the Decision Making for River Restorations? Water Resour. Res. 2007, 43, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Conniff, R. Rebuilding the Natural World: A Shift in Ecological Restoration. The Yale School of the Environment. 2014. Available online: https://e360.yale.edu/features/rebuilding_the_natural_world_a_shift_in_ecological_restoration#:~:text=From%20forests%20in%20Queens%20to,nature%20and%20what%20does%20not (accessed on 20 December 2015).
  11. SER. The SER International Primer on Ecological Restoration. In Society for Ecological Restoration, Washington; Society for Ecological Restoration International: Washington, DC, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  12. Baker, S.; Eckerberg, K.; Zachrisson, A. Political Science and Ecological Restoration. Environ. Polit. 2014, 23, 509–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Rey Benayas, J.M.; Newton, A.C.; Diaz, A.; Bullock, J.M. Enhancement of Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services by Ecological Restoration: A Meta-Analysis. Science. 2009, 325, 1121–1124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Buijs, A.E. Public Support for River Restoration. A Mixed-Method Study into Local Residents’ Support for and Framing of River Management and Ecological Restoration in the Dutch Floodplains. J. Environ. Manag. 2009, 90, 2680–2689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Luyet, V.; Schlaepfer, R.; Parlange, M.B.; Buttler, A. A Framework to Implement Stakeholder Participation in Environmental Projects. J. Environ. Manag. 2012, 111, 213–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Masterson, J.H.; Peacock, W.G.; Van Zandt, S.S.; Grover, H.; Schwarz, L.F.; Cooper, J.T. Planning for Community Resilience: A Handbook for Reducing Vulnerability to Disasters; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2014; ISBN 978-1610915854. [Google Scholar]
  17. Turcanu, C.; Perko, T.; Laes, E. Public Participation Processes Related to Nuclear Research Installations: What Are the Driving Factors behind Participation Intention? Public Underst. Sci. 2014, 23, 331–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Alam, K. Public Attitudes toward Restoration of Impaired River Ecosystems: Does Residents’ Attachment to Place Matter? Urban Ecosyst. 2011, 14, 635–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Schaich, H. Local Residents’ Perceptions of Floodplain Restoration Measures in Luxembourg’s Syr Valley. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2009, 93, 20–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Phalen, K.B. An Invitation for Public Participation in Ecological Restoration: The Reasonable Person Model. Ecol. Restor. 2009, 27, 178–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Aggestam, F. Wetland Restoration and the Involvement of Stakeholders: An Analysis Based on Value-Perspectives. Landsc. Res. 2014, 39, 680–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hong, C.Y.; Chung, E.S.; Chang, H. The Right to Urban Streams: Quantitative Comparisons of Stakeholder Perceptions in Defining Adaptive Stream Restoration. Sustain. 2020, 12, 9500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Follett, R.; Strezov, V. An Analysis of Citizen Science Based Research: Usage and Publication Patterns. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0143687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Cascetta, E.; Cartenì, A.; Pagliara, F.; Montanino, M. A New Look at Planning and Designing Transportation Systems: A Decision-Making Model Based on Cognitive Rationality, Stakeholder Engagement and Quantitative Methods. Transp. Policy 2015, 38, 27–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Yosie, T.F.; Herbst, T.D. Using Stakeholder Processes in Environmental Decisionmaking An Evaluation of Lessons Learned, Key Issues, and Future Challenges; The Global Development Research Center: Kobe, Japan, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  26. Freeman, R.E. Strategic Management: A Stakeholder Approach; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  27. European Commission Environment. Common Implementation Strategy for the Water Framework Directive (2000/60/EC); European Commission Environment: Luxembourg, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  28. Mysiak, J.; Testella, F.; Bonaiuto, M.; Carrus, G.; De Dominicis, S.; Cancellieri, U.G.; Firus, K.; Grifoni, P. Flood Risk Management in Italy: Challenges and Opportunities for the Implementation of the EU Floods Directive (2007/60/EC). Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. 2013, 13, 2883–2890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ride, A.B.D. Community Resilience in Natural Disasters; Palgrave Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 2011; ISBN 978-1-349-29585-2. [Google Scholar]
  30. Scolobig, A.; Prior, T.; Schröter, D.; Jörin, J.; Patt, A. Towards People-Centred Approaches for Effective Disaster Risk Management: Balancing Rhetoric with Reality. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2015, 12, 202–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Asian Development Bank. Strengthening Participation for Development Results: An Asian Development Bank Guide to Participation; Asian Development Bank: Mandaluyong, Philippines, 2012; ISBN 9789290925262. [Google Scholar]
  32. Duea, S.R.; Zimmerman, E.B.; Vaughn, L.M.; Dias, S.; Harris, J. A Guide to Selecting Participatory Research Methods Based on Project and Partnership Goals. J. Particip. Res. Methods 2022, 3, 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Blicharska, M.; Rönnbäck, P. What Factors Enable or Hinder Engagement of Civil Society in Ecosystem Management ? The Case of ‘ Pike Factories ’ and Wetland Restoration in Sweden. J. Environ. Plan. Manag. 2018, 61, 950–969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. World Meteorological Organization. Community-Based Flood Management; World Meteorological Organization (WMO): Geneva, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  35. Luo, Z.; Li, J.; Wu, Z.; Li, S.; Bi, G. Investigating the Driving Factors of Public Participation in Public-Private Partnership (PPP) Projects—A Case Study of China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 5192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Ajzen, I. The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Santoro, S.; Pluchinotta, I.; Pagano, A.; Pengal, P.; Cokan, B.; Giordano, R. Assessing Stakeholders’ Risk Perception to Promote Nature Based Solutions as Flood Protection Strategies: The Case of the Glinščica River (Slovenia). Sci. Total Environ. 2019, 655, 188–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Kelly, C.; Breinlinger, S. Attitudes, Intentions, and Behavior: A Study of Women’s Participation in Collective Action. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 25, 1430–1445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Deffner, J.; Haase, P. The Societal Relevance of River Restoration. Ecol. Soc. 2018, 23, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Tunstall, S.M.; Tapsell, S.M.; Eden, S. How Stable Are Public Responses to Changing Local Environments? A “before” and “after” Case Study of River Restoration. J. Environ. Plan. Manag. 1999, 42, 527–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Slovic, P.; Weber, E.U. Perception of Risk Posed by Extreme Events. In Regulation of Toxic Substances and Hazardous Waste, 2nd ed.; Foundation Press: Santa Barbara, CA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  42. Koehn, J.D.; Brierley, G.J.; Cant, B.L.; Lucas, A.M. River Restoration Framework; Land & Water Australia: Wayville, Australia, 2001; ISBN 0 642 76056 X. [Google Scholar]
  43. Maluka, S.O. Strengthening Fairness, Transparency and Accountability in Health Care Priority Setting at District Level in Tanzania. Glob. Health Action 2011, 4, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Weitzner, D.; Deutsch, Y. Understanding Motivation and Social Influence in Stakeholder Prioritization. Organ. Stud. 2015, 36, 1337–1360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Jennings, T.E.; Jean-Philippe, S.R.; Willcox, A.; Zobel, J.M.; Poudyal, N.C.; Simpson, T. The Influence of Attitudes and Perception of Tree Benefits on Park Management Priorities. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2016, 153, 122–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Gallego-Ayala, J.; Juízo, D. Integrating Stakeholders’ Preferences into Water Resources Management Planning in the Incomati River Basin. Water Resour. Manag. 2014, 28, 527–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Williams, J.; Damine, J.M.N. How Does Our Perception of Risk Influence Decision-Making? Implications for the Design of Risk Information. Theor. Issues Ergon. Sci. 2007, 8, 1–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Savadori, L.; Savio, S.; Nicotra, E.; Rumiati, R.; Finucane, M.; Slovic, P. Expert and Public Perception of Risk from Biotechnology. Risk Anal. 2004, 24, 1289–1299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Slovic, P. The Perception of Risk. In London and Sterling; Earthscan: Oxford, UK, 2000; ISBN 9781315661773. [Google Scholar]
  50. Lobb, A.E.; Mazzocchi, M.; Traill, W.B. Modelling Risk Perception and Trust in Food Safety Information within the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Food Qual. Prefer. 2007, 18, 384–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Zeng, J.; Wei, J.; Zhu, W.; Zhao, D.; Lin, X. Residents’ Behavioural Intentions to Resist the Nuclear Power Plants in the Vicinity: An Application of the Protective Action Decision Model. J. Risk Res. 2017, 22, 382–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Yüksel, A.; Yüksel, F. Shopping Risk Perceptions: Effects on Tourists’ Emotions, Satisfaction and Expressed Loyalty Intentions. Tour. Manag. 2007, 28, 703–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Su, Y.; Sun, X.P.; Zhao, F. Trust and Its Effects on the Public’s Perception of Flood Risk: A Social Science Investigation of the Middle and Lower Reaches of the Yangtze River. J. Flood Risk Manag. 2017, 10, 487–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Leiserowitz, A. Climate Change Risk Perception and Policy Preferences: The Role of Affect, Imagery, and Values. Clim. Chang. 2006, 77, 45–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Moen, E. Risk Perception, Priority of Safety, and Demand for Risk Mitigation in Transport; Norwegian University of Science and Technology: Trondheim, Norway, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  56. Swapan, M.S.H. Who Participates and Who Doesn’t? Adapting Community Participation Model for Developing Countries. Cities 2016, 53, 70–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kang, M. Understanding Public Engagement: Conceptualizing and Measuring Its Influence on Supportive Behavioral Intentions. J. Public Relat. Res. 2014, 26, 399–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Payton, M.A.; Fulton, D.C.; Anderson, D.H. Influence of Place Attachment and Trust on Civic Action: A Study at Sherburne National Wildlife Refuge. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2005, 18, 511–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Stern, M.J.; Coleman, K.J. The Multidimensionality of Trust: Applications in Collaborative Natural Resource Management. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2015, 28, 117–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Rousseau, D.M.; Sitkin, S.B.; Burt, R.S.; Camerer, C.; Rousseau, D.M.; Burt, R.S. Not so Different after All: A Cross-Discipline View of Trust. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1998, 23, 393–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Boss, R.W. Trust and Managerial Problem Solving Revisited. Gr. Organ. Stud. 1978, 3, 331–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Liu, H.; Zhu, G.; Li, Y. Research on the Impact of Environmental Risk Perception and Public Participation on Evaluation of Local Government Environmental Regulation Implementation Behavior. Environ. Chall. 2021, 5, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Carnevale, D.G.; Wechsler, B. Trust in the Public Sector: Individual and Organizational Determinants. Adm. Soc. 1992, 23, 471–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Antonini, M.; Barbieri, B.; Giacomantonio, M.; Mannetti, L. Trust and Expected Costs as Antecedents of Citizens’ Motivation to Participate in Public Policymaking. Cesk. Psychol. 2015, 59, 26–35. [Google Scholar]
  65. Siegrist, M.; Cvetkovich, G. Perception of Hazards: The Role of Social Trust and Knowledge. Risk Anal. 2000, 20, 713–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Frick, J.; Kaiser, F.G.; Wilson, M. Environmental Knowledge and Conservation Behavior: Exploring Prevalence and Structure in a Representative Sample. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2004, 37, 1597–1613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Ajzen, I.; Joyce, N.; Sheikh, S.; Cote, N.G. Knowledge and the Prediction of Behavior: The Role of Information Accuracy in the Theory of Planned Behavior. Basic Appl. Soc. Psych. 2011, 33, 101–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Dreyer, M.; Kosow, H.; Bauer, A. Public Engagement with Research: Citizens’ Views on Motivations, Barriers and Support. Res. All 2021, 5, 302–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Xing, H.G.; Que, T.; Wu, Y.X.; Hu, S.Y.; Li, H.B.; Li, H.; Skitmore, M.; Talebian, N. Public Intention to Participate in Sustainable Geohazard Mitigation: An Empirical Study Based on an Extended Theory of Planned Behavior. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. 2023, 23, 1529–1547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Sutton, S.G.; Tobin, R.C. Constraints on Community Engagement with Great Barrier Reef Climate Change Reduction and Mitigation. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2011, 21, 894–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Venkataramanan, V.; Lopez, D.; McCuskey, D.J.; Kiefus, D.; McDonald, R.I.; Miller, W.M.; Packman, A.I.; Young, S.L. Knowledge, Attitudes, Intentions, and Behavior Related to Green Infrastructure for Flood Management: A Systematic Literature Review. Sci. Total Environ. 2020, 720, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Garpenby, P. The Priority Setting Process: A Macro Perspective; National Centre for Priority Setting in Health Care: Linköping, Sweden, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  73. Iwasaki, Y.; Havitz, M.E. Examining Relationships between Leisure Involvement, Psychological Commitment and Loyalty to a Recreation Agency. J. Leis. Res. 2004, 36, 45–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Gul, F.; Sattar, S.; Kazmi, I.B.; Ayub, F.; Arshad, S. Analysis of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivational Factors Affecting Sports Participation of University Level Athletes. PalArch’s J. Archaeol. Egypt/Egyptol. 2021, 18, 1583–1592. [Google Scholar]
  75. Deci, E.L.; Ryan, R.M. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. In Perspectives in Social Psychology; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1985; ISBN 978-1-4899-2273-1. [Google Scholar]
  76. Deci, E.L. Effects of Externally Mediated Rewards on Intrinsic Motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1971, 18, 105–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Shirk, J.L.; Ballard, H.L.; Wilderman, C.C.; Phillips, T.; Wiggins, A.; Jordan, R.; McCallie, E.; Minarchek, M.; Lewenstein, B.V.; Krasny, M.E.; et al. Public Participation in Scientific Research: A Framework for Deliberate Design. Ecol. Soc. 2012, 17, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Ryan, R.L.; Kaplan, R.; Grese, R.E. Predicting Volunteer Commitment in Environmental Stewardship Programmes. J. Environ. Plan. Manag. 2001, 44, 629–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Gagné, M.; Deci, E.L. Self-Determination Theory and Work Motivation. J. Organ. Behav. 2005, 26, 331–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Gollwitzer, P.M.; Delius, J.D.; Oettingen, G. Motivation. In The International Handbook of Psychology; Sage Publications: London, UK, 2000; pp. 191–206. [Google Scholar]
  81. Hankins, M.; French, D.; Horne, R. Statistical Guidelines for Studies of the Theory of Reasoned Action and the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Psychol. Heal. 2000, 15, 151–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Gollwitzer, P.M.; Bargh, J.A. Planning and Coordinating Action. In The Psychology of Action: Linking Cognition and Motivation to Behaviour; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 283–312. [Google Scholar]
  83. Van Lange, P.A.M.; Kruglanski, A.W.; Higgins, E.T. Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology: Volume 1; SAGE: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2012; Volume 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Kashima, Y.; McKintyre, A.; Clifford, P. The Category of the Mind: Folk Psychology of Belief, Desire, and Intention. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 1, 289–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Zheng, X.Y. The Ancient Urban Water System Construction of China: The Lessons from History for a Sustainable Future. Int. J. Glob. Environ. Issues 2015, 14, 187–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Che, Y.; Li, W.; Shang, Z.; Liu, C.; Yang, K. Residential Preferences for River Network Improvement: An Exploration of Choice Experiments in Zhujiajiao, Shanghai, China. Environ. Manag. 2014, 54, 517–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Buchecker, M.; Menzel, S.; Home, R. How Much Does Participatory Flood Management Contribute to Stakeholders’ Social Capacity Building? Empirical Findings Based on a Triangulation of Three Evaluation Approaches. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. 2013, 13, 1427–1444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Buchecker, M.; Meier, C.; Hunziker, M. Measuring the Effects of Consensusbuilding Processes with Methods of Intervention Research. Eur. Plan. Stud. 2010, 18, 259–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Mah, D.N.; Hills, P.; Tao, J. Risk Perception, Trust and Public Engagement in Nuclear Decision-Making in Hong Kong. Energy Policy 2014, 73, 368–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Whitmarsh, L. Are Flood Victims More Concerned about Climate Change than Other People? The Role of Direct Experience in Risk Perception and Behavioural Response. J. Risk Res. 2008, 11, 351–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Beechie, T.; Pess, G.; Roni, P.; Giannico, G. Setting River Restoration Priorities: A Review of Approaches and a General Protocol for Identifying and Prioritizing Actions. North Am. J. Fish. Manag. 2008, 28, 891–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Berander, P.; Andrews, A. Requirements Prioritization. In Engineering and Managing Software Requirements; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2005; pp. 69–94. [Google Scholar]
  93. van Riper, C.J.; Lum, C.; Kyle, G.T.; Wallen, K.E.; Absher, J.; Landon, A.C. Values, Motivations, and Intentions to Engage in Proenvironmental Behavior. Environ. Behav. 2018, 52, 437–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Corbett, J.B. Motivations to Participate in Riparian Improvement Programs: Applying the Theory of Planned Behavior. Sci. Commun. 2002, 23, 243–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Woosnam, K.M.; Strzelecka, M.; Nisbett, G.S.; Keith, S.J. Examining Millennials’ Global Citizenship Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions to Engage in Environmental Volunteering. Sustain. 2019, 11, 2324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  96. Tunstall, S.M.; Penning-Rowsell, E.C.; Tapsell, S.M.; Eden, S.E. River Restoration: Public Attitudes and Expectations. Water Environ. J. 2000, 14, 363–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Wang, Y.; Cai, J.; Zuo, J.; Bartsch, K.; Huang, M. Conflict or Consensus? Stakeholders’ Willingness to Participate in China’s Sponge City Program. Sci. Total Environ. 2021, 769, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Sarvilinna, A. Saving Our Streams: Public Willingness to Participate in Stream Restoration in Finland; JYU Dissertations: Jyväskylä, Finland, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  99. MacCallum, R.C.; Austin, J.T. Applications of Structural Equation Modeling in Psychological Research. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2000, 51, 201–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Lei, P.W.; Wu, Q. Introduction to Structural Equation Modeling: Issues and Practical Considerations. Educ. Meas. Issues Pract. 2007, 26, 33–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Anderson, J.C.; Gerbing, D.W. Structural Equation Modeling in Practice: A Review and Recommended Two-Step Approach. Psychol. Bull. 1988, 103, 411–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Fornell, C.; Larcker, D.F. Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error: Algebra and Statistics. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 382–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Chin, W.W. Commentary: Issues and Opinion on Structural Equation Modeling. MIS Q. 1998, 22, 7–16. [Google Scholar]
  104. Ajzen, I. From Intentions to Actions: A Theory of Planned Behavior. In Action Control: From Cognition to Behavior; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1985; pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
  105. Dietz, T.; Stern, P.C.; Guagnano, G.A. Social Structural and Social Psychological Bases of Environmental Concern. Environ. Behav. 1998, 30, 450–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  106. Kaiser, F.G.; Gutscher, H. The Proposition of a General Version of the Theory of Planned Behavior: Predicting Ecological Behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 33, 586–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Slovic, P. Perception of Risk. Science 1987, 236, 280–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Leppin, A.; Aro, A.R. Risk Perceptions Related to SARS and Avian Influenza: Theoretical Foundations of Current Empirical Research. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2009, 16, 7–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Kollmuss, A.; Agyeman, J. Mind the Gap: Why Do People Act Environmentally and What Are the Barriers to pro-Environmental Behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 2002, 8, 239–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. de Groot, J.I.M.; Steg, L. Value Orientations to Explain Beliefs Related to Environmental Significant Behavior: How to Measure Egoistic, Altruistic, and Biospheric Value Orientations. Environ. Behav. 2008, 40, 330–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Stern, P.C. New Environmental Theories: Toward a Coherent Theory of Environmentally Significant Behavior. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 407–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Lorenzoni, I.; Nicholson-Cole, S.; Whitmarsh, L. Barriers Perceived to Engaging with Climate Change among the UK Public and Their Policy Implications. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2007, 17, 445–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Conceptual framework.
Figure 1. Conceptual framework.
Water 15 02844 g001
Figure 2. Location of the moat system in Tianchang City.
Figure 2. Location of the moat system in Tianchang City.
Water 15 02844 g002
Figure 3. The results of the hypothesis paths. Note: Only solid lines represent significant paths; * p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001.
Figure 3. The results of the hypothesis paths. Note: Only solid lines represent significant paths; * p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001.
Water 15 02844 g003
Table 1. A sociodemographic overview of the sample (n = 473).
Table 1. A sociodemographic overview of the sample (n = 473).
AttributesDescriptionFrequencyPercentage (%)
GenderMale
Female
240
233
50.7
49.3
Age18–25
26–35
36–45
46–55
55+
105
173
81
86
28
22.2
36.6
17.1
18.2
5.9
Education LevelPrimary School
Secondary School
Undergraduate
Master’s degree
PhD+
95
95
178
85
20
20.1
20.1
37.6
18.0
4.2
Employment
Status
Civil servant
Self-employed
Retired
Student
Farmer or worker
188
85
8
87
105
39.7
18.0
1.7
18.4
22.2
YearsLess than 1 year
2–7 years
8–13 years
14–19 years
More than 20 years
94
100
88
87
104
19.9
21.1
18.6
18.4
22.0
Familiarity with Participation in MSRPNot at all familiar
Slightly familiar
Moderately familiar
Very familiar
Extremely familiar
83
105
92
96
97
17.5
22.2
19.5
20.3
20.5
Flood Management as a Restoration PriorityApprove
Strongly Approve
Neutral
Disapprove
Strongly Disapprove
86
113
78
91
105
18.2
23.9
16.5
19.2
22.2
Table 2. Reliability and convergent validity.
Table 2. Reliability and convergent validity.
ConstructItemLoadingCRAVECronbach’s α
SASA1. I am interested in Moat System Restoration Projects.0.7800.8920.6230.811
SA2. I am concerned with Moat System Restoration Projects.0.798
SA3. I have only a little information on the Moat System Restoration Project.0.767
SA4. I think the restoration project is necessary for the moat system.0.802
SA5. I believe the Moat System Restoration Project can help solve real problems.0.797
SBSB1. I am willing to participate in decision-making.0.7810.8890.6150.870
SB2. I am open to sharing knowledge.0.781
SB3. I am willing to be trained to participate in decision-making.0.764
SB4. I am glad to communicate with people in different roles.0.803
SB5. I am able to encourage others to participate in decision-making.0.791
STD6. I think local government decisions can address people’s concerns.0.759
SPSP1. Flooding Management.0.8230.9010.6470.902
SP2. Social welfare0.819
SP3. Strengthen legislation.0.803
SP4. Ensure health and safety.0.777
SP5. Economic development0.798
SP3. Strengthen legislation.0.803
SP4. Ensure health and safety.0.777
SP5. Economic development0.798
SRPSRP1. I think solving the flooding problem of the moat system is very urgent.0.8260.8410.6380.823
SRP2. I think the flooding issue of the moat system is threatening my life.0.816
SRP3. I agree with flood management as a priority for Moat System Restoration Projects.0.753
STDSTD1. I think the decision of the government is trustworthy.0.8070.9020.6050.901
STD2. I have confidence in the competence of decision-makers.0.775
STD3. I am satisfied with the current decision-making process.0.764
STD4. I believe the decision-making process is fair in Tianchang City.0.801
STD5. I think the local government’s decision is accepted.0.758
STD6. I think local government decisions can address people’s concerns.0.759
SKSK1. I have received all the information necessary to participate in decision-making.0.7870.9010.6470.905
SK2. I have the requisite knowledge to participate in decision-making.0.793
SK3. I understand how to participate in decision-making.0.806
SK4. I am aware of my role in the decision-making process.0.802
SK5. I know the benefits of participating in decision-making.0.832
SMSM1. I have the right to participate in decision-making.0.8120.8980.6370.884
SM2. I have the ability to participate in decision-making.0.802
SM3. I feel confident when participating in decision-making.0.780
SM4. I think my participation can influence decisions.0.801
SM5. I have a sense of responsibility to participate in decision-making.0.797
SISI1. Gives me the opportunity to learn new skills.0.7680.8880.6120.883
SI2. Network with academics related to my major.0.792
SI3. Let decision-makers know and consider my thoughts.0.777
SI4. Make more like-minded friends.0.774
SI5. To preserve the ecology of the place where I live.0.800
Table 3. Discriminant validity for the measurement model.
Table 3. Discriminant validity for the measurement model.
ConstructSKSTDSRPSPSMSISASB
SK0.804
STD0.0560.778
SRP0.3400.0590.799
SP0.3100.1240.2150.804
SM0.1070.1190.1230.0470.798
SI0.1280.1200.0800.0640.4020.782
SA0.2780.4140.2540.2630.1400.3620.789
SB0.4660.5060.4070.3940.5120.5050.6140.784
Note: The square root of AVE is on a diagonal.
Table 4. Goodness-of-fit of the model.
Table 4. Goodness-of-fit of the model.
Chi-SquaredfChi-Square/dfSRMRRMSEATLICFI
Recommended value//<3<0.10<0.05>0.9>0.9
Measurement model762.4026741.1310.0310.0170.9910.992
Structural model989.3586841.4460.0650.0310.9700.972
Table 5. Results of the hypothesis testing.
Table 5. Results of the hypothesis testing.
PathPath
Direction
Non-Standard CoefficientSEZ
(CR Value)
pStandardized CoefficientResult
H1SA→SB0.1850.0345.3930.0000.201Accepted
H2SA→SP0.2290.0564.1160.0000.212Accepted
H3SP→SB0.1490.0295.2300.0000.175Accepted
H4SP→SRP0.2390.0514.6540.0000.244Accepted
H5SRP→SB0.2060.0297.2110.0000.237Accepted
H6STD→SB0.3610.03510.3290.0000.395Accepted
H7STD→SRP0.0140.0550.2550.7990.013Rejected
H8SK→SB0.2390.0317.7160.0000.266Accepted
H9SK→SP0.2990.0555.4430.0000.284Accepted
H10SM→SRP0.1650.0533.1090.0020.162Accepted
H11SM→SI0.4550.0489.4760.0000.498Accepted
H12SI→SB0.4040.03511.4750.0000.418Accepted
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yan, S.; Ibrahim, R.B. Understanding Community Resilience through the Lens of Stakeholder Participation: Empirical Evidence from the Moat System Restoration Project. Water 2023, 15, 2844. https://doi.org/10.3390/w15152844

AMA Style

Yan S, Ibrahim RB. Understanding Community Resilience through the Lens of Stakeholder Participation: Empirical Evidence from the Moat System Restoration Project. Water. 2023; 15(15):2844. https://doi.org/10.3390/w15152844

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yan, Shihua, and Roziya Binti Ibrahim. 2023. "Understanding Community Resilience through the Lens of Stakeholder Participation: Empirical Evidence from the Moat System Restoration Project" Water 15, no. 15: 2844. https://doi.org/10.3390/w15152844

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop