Next Article in Journal
Heritage Interpretation and Sustainable Development: A Systematic Literature Review
Previous Article in Journal
New Mobility Paradigm and Indigenous Construction of Places: Physical and Symbolic Mobility of Aymara Groups in the Urbanization Process, Chile
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Evaluating the Effect of Framing Energy Consumption in Terms of Losses versus Gains on Air-Conditioner Use: A Field Experiment in a Student Dormitory in Japan

1
Department of Environmental Science, Fukuoka Women’s University, Fukuoka 813-8529, Japan
2
Marketing Department, Rennes School of Business, 35065 Rennes, France
3
Behavioral Economics and Engineering Group, KU Leuven, 3000 Leuven, Belgium
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2021, 13(8), 4380; https://doi.org/10.3390/su13084380
Submission received: 12 March 2021 / Revised: 5 April 2021 / Accepted: 12 April 2021 / Published: 14 April 2021

Abstract

:
Promoting energy conservation in university dormitories is challenging because student residents are typically charged a flat utility fee. One possibility to curb excessive energy use in the absence of monetary incentives is to highlight the environmental consequences of energy use. However, it is still largely unknown how these consequences should be communicated to effectively change people’s behavior. In the present study, we analyzed the effect of framing the environmental consequences of energy use in terms of losses versus gains on the air-conditioner use of student residents of a Japanese university dormitory. A total of 330 students were provided with stickers to attach to the air-conditioner remote control in their dormitory room during the winter term. The stickers conveyed that increasing the temperature will hurt the environment (loss frame), that reducing the temperature will protect the environment (gain frame), or that changing the temperature will affect the environment (neutral frame). Day-to-day variations in objective air-conditioner use data were analyzed as a function of experimental condition to examine the effect of message framing. The change in air-conditioner use from pre-intervention to intervention period did not differ between experimental groups and neither did the change from pre-intervention period to a period after the intervention.

1. Introduction

Rapid and far-reaching transitions in energy use are necessary to mitigate climate change [1]. This requires a thorough understanding of how people can be motivated to moderate their energy use behavior. Promoting energy conservation is a particular challenge in residential halls and university dormitories because student residents are typically charged a flat utility fee. As a consequence, student residents (like hotel guests or office workers) do not have financial incentives to save energy [2,3,4,5,6]. Several attempts have been made to address this lack of monetary incentives by linking energy use to social or material rewards [2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9]. A possible limitation of this approach is that the effects of such rewards may be relatively short-lived and that energy use may return to baseline once the intervention is discontinued ([10,11,12], but see [13]). Here, we adopt an alternative approach by informing people about a potentially valued natural consequence of their energy use: its impact on the environment.
Previous evaluations of environmental impact messages (often also referred to as biospheric appeals) have found mixed results. Some studies have found positive effects on conservation behavior or its proposed psychological antecedents [14,15,16,17], whereas others found no such effects [18,19,20].
Critically, the effectiveness of environmental impact messages may depend on how the consequences of people’s behavior are framed [21]. For example, messages may be more effective when the consequences of environmentally relevant behaviors are framed in terms of losses rather than gains [22,23,24]. In other words, attempts to promote energy conservation may be more successful when they convey that energy-intensive behavior will hurt the environment rather than that energy-saving behavior will help the environment. Such superiority of loss versus gain framing may be due to people finding the current state of the environment satisfactory (i.e., good enough), so that the prevention of negative change would be more motivating than the promotion of positive change. However, these findings need to be taken with a grain of salt as they are largely based on self-report studies examining attitudes and intentions rather than actual energy use behavior (see [25], for a discussion of the validity issues related to such self-report proxies). In fact, a recent review of 61 framing studies in the environmental domain [24] included only seven studies on actual behavior, only one of which [26] included a measure of energy use. Moreover, the one study that examined the effect of loss framing on actual energy use behavior [26] confounded the framing intervention with additional interventions, which makes it impossible to attribute the intervention effect to the loss framing. As a result, it is still largely unclear whether framing the environmental impact of energy use behavior in terms of losses versus gains can promote actual energy conservation.
In the present study, we analyzed the effect of differentially framed environmental impact messages on air-conditioner use of student residents of a Japanese university dormitory. We provided residents with stickers to attach to the air-conditioner remote control in their dormitory room. These stickers highlighted the impact of air-conditioner use in terms of losses, gains, or neutral effects. By comparing energy use before, during, and after this intervention, we examined which of the three message frames was most effective in reducing air-conditioner use.

2. Methodology

2.1. Study Site

The study took place in a female student dormitory at Fukuoka Women’s University, Japan. All new local and foreign exchange students who enter the university are required to live in the dormitory for almost one year from April to February. They live together in groups of up to four (three local and one foreign student) in one of 84 units distributed across three buildings. Each unit consists of four private single rooms (each including a balcony) as well as a shared dining kitchen, bathroom, and lavatory. At the time of the study (November 2018 to January 2019), 330 student residents lived in such four-person dormitory units and were included in our study. Dormitory residents pay 6300 Japanese yen per month (approximately USD 60, EUR 50 at the time of the study) as an all-inclusive utility fee that covers electricity, gas, and water use. That is, they do not have a monetary incentive for energy conservation.
Air-conditioners are installed in the private single rooms and the shared dining kitchen, but we focused only on the air-conditioner data from private single rooms. These air-conditioners are used for cooling in the summer and for heating in the winter. Our decision to focus on air-conditioner data was motivated partly by practical concerns and partly by the earlier finding that air-conditioner use accounts for the largest share of electricity use in the dormitory [27].

2.2. Procedure

We created three types of environmental impact messages (loss frame, gain frame, and neutral frame) to communicate the environmental consequences of air-conditioner use (see Figure 1). The loss-frame sticker indicated that choosing higher temperatures (i.e., temperatures that deviate more from the low outside temperatures during the time of the study) would hurt the environment. It used a color gradient from grey to red, with the red pole (i.e., 24 °C) denoting the level that would hurt the environment the most. The gain-frame sticker indicated that choosing lower temperatures (i.e., temperatures that deviate less from outside temperatures) would protect the environment. It used a color gradient from green to grey, with the green pole (i.e., 18 °C) denoting the level that would protect the environment the most. The neutral frame sticker depicted an exclusively grey color scale and did not use positively or negatively valenced terms to describe the effect of temperature settings on the environment. We used English messages on the stickers because the dormitory residents included foreign students and the Japanese students were expected to understand English.
The dormitory students living in the unit buildings described above were divided into the three experimental groups (loss frame: n = 111, gain frame: n = 110, neutral frame: n = 109). Each member of a unit (consisting of three or four students) received the same sticker. The stickers were distributed on 3 December 2018 by means of letters sent to the leaders of each unit. This letter included information about the general aims of the study and instructions for attaching the sticker to the air-conditioner remote control in students’ private rooms (see Figure 2). Unit leaders were asked to pass on these instructions and the stickers to the other students living in the unit. On 21 December 2018, students received a text message on messaging application LINE requesting them to remove the sticker before the end of the winter break (7 January 2019).

2.3. Data and Analysis

From the administrative office of Fukuoka Women’s University, we obtained data on the energy used by the air conditioners in 330 rooms, grouped into 84 units, on 92 days, beginning on Thursday 11 January 2018 and ending on Thursday 31 January 2019. As each room within a unit was in the same experimental condition (28 units in each condition) and because air conditioner use within units is not independent, we analyzed the data at the level of the unit. The unit of analysis is the average energy used by an air conditioner within a unit, measured in kilowatt-hours (we averaged because the number of occupied rooms was different across the units: 78 units had 4 occupied rooms and 6 had 3 occupied rooms). This results in a fully balanced data set of 7728 observations.
The intervention started on 4 December 2018 and ended on 21 January 2018. To test whether the change in energy consumption from pre-intervention to intervention and post-intervention period was smaller in the loss or gain frame conditions than in the control condition, we conducted a panel analysis with condition (loss frame vs. gain frame vs. neutral frame) and period (post is one day after the intervention or later, pre is one day before the intervention or earlier, during otherwise) as independent variables.
In the statistical model, we included some fixed effects to control for differences between units and time periods that may affect air-conditioner use. Building, floor, and the interaction between building and floor controlled for differences in the spatial orientation of the rooms. Weekday (Monday, Tuesday, etc.) and a term named holiday, indicating whether the students had class on a particular day, controlled for differences between the days. We obtained meteorological information for Fukuoka for the days of the experiment [28]: temperature measured in Celsius, air pressure measured in millibars, humidity percentage, and wind speed measured in kilometers per hour.
The data were not normally distributed because there were many days with no air-conditioner use. Therefore, we took the double logarithm of the daily energy use, which rendered the residuals more normal. Together with the independent variables, we got the following model:
l o g l o g Y i t + 1 + 1 = b u i l d i n g i + f l o o r i + b u i l d i n g i × f l o o r i + w e e k d a y t + h o l i d a y t + t e m p e r a t u r e t + a i r p r e s s u r e t + h u m i d i t y t + w i n d s p e e d t + c o n d i t i o n i + p e r i o d t + c o n d i t i o n i × p e r i o d t + ε i
where i refers to unit and t refers to period.

3. Results

Figure 3 shows the daily energy use, averaged per condition, across time. Figure 4 shows a violin plot that depicts the distribution of the average energy use per unit, per period. Figure A1 shows the residuals of the model with only the covariates. As the figures suggest, there was no difference between the loss frame condition and the neutral condition in the increase from pre-intervention to intervention period (in the following, all estimates are on the double log scale: estimate = 0.003, t(7627) = 0.26, p = 0.791) or the increase from pre-intervention to post-intervention period (estimate = 0.009, t(7627) = 0.92, p = 0.358). The same holds for the comparison between the gain frame condition and the neutral condition: no difference in the increase from pre-intervention to intervention period (estimate = 0.003, t(7627) = 0.2, p = 0.839) or the increase from pre-intervention to post-intervention period (estimate = 0.011, t(7627) = 1.1, p = 0.271). Finally, also when comparing the loss frame condition and the gain frame condition, there was no difference in the increase from pre-intervention to intervention period (estimate = −0.001, t(7627) = −0.06, p = 0.951) or the increase from pre-intervention to post-intervention period (estimate = 0.002, t(7627) = 0.18, p = 0.855). Appendix A reports the full regression table. The interactions between condition and period remained nonsignificant when we tested them without controlling for covariates. The results do not change if we drop the assumption that air conditioner use within units is related and analyze the data at the level of the individual air conditioner.

4. Discussion

Using objective energy use data with high temporal resolution, the present study investigated the effect of differentially framed environmental impact messages on the air-conditioner use of student residents of a Japanese university dormitory. We did not find these messages to be more effective when students’ impact on the environment was framed in terms of losses rather than gains. Moreover, the energy use of students receiving loss-framed or gain-framed messages did not differ from energy use of students in a neutrally framed control condition.
In line with several recent studies (e.g., [18,19,20]), the present study did not find evidence for the effect of environmental impact messages on environmentally relevant behavior. However, it must be noted that each of our three experimental conditions (even the neutral control condition) involved some kind of information about the environmental consequences of participants’ air-conditioner use. This information was arguably more vague in the control condition, but the differential effect between the conditions might have been too small to be detectable in our study. The same applies to the critical comparison between the loss-frame condition and the gain-frame condition. We took several measures to ensure that the stickers used in these conditions were as similar as possible on all dimensions except the framing of the message. Because of that, the difference between the two conditions may have been too subtle to exert significant effects in a field experiment. In comparison to laboratory studies, field experiments on environmentally relevant behavior are often more noisy and less controlled [29]. This high level of noise might critically attenuate the effect of behavioral interventions. For example, in the present study, we were not able to ensure that every participant was similarly exposed to the experimental manipulation, because we could not check whether they attached the sticker according to our instructions and paid attention to the sticker when making temperature setting decisions in everyday life. Future field experiments testing the effect of gain vs. loss frame messages on energy use may benefit from implementing less subtle interventions, for example, highly attention-grabbing posters pre-installed in the shared parts of dormitories. In addition, a larger sample size will likely be necessary to detect the effects of such interventions. Pooling data from multiple study sites (e.g., through using the Psychological Science Accelerator [30]) may be one strategy to meet these sample-size demands. Such multisite studies may also increase the heterogeneity of the recruited samples. The present study exclusively included female students studying at the same Japanese institution of higher education. Further research in different populations is necessary to examine the generality of message framing effects on energy use.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.I., S.F., S.D. and F.L.; methodology, S.I., S.F., S.D. and F.L.; data curation, S.I.; data analysis, S.F. and S.I.; writing—draft, S.I., S.F. and F.L.; writing—review and editing, S.I., S.F., S.D. and F.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by the Fukuoka Women’s University Epidemiology and Ethics, etc. Research Review Committee (No. 2018-26).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study using an opt out approach.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Mina Nakagawa for supporting the field intervention. Florian Lange was supported by a FWO postdoctoral fellowship (No 12U1221N).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Table A1. Full regression table.
Table A1. Full regression table.
TermEstimateStandard_ErrorTDfP
(Intercept)0.42340.04299.879680.0000
temperature−0.04410.0010−43.63976270.0000
airpressure−0.00640.0007−8.80476270.0000
humidity−0.00010.0003−0.24476270.8069
windspeed−0.01330.0020−6.55976270.0000
weekdayThursday−0.00730.0068−1.07476270.2827
weekdayFriday−0.01690.0070−2.42076270.0155
weekdaySaturday0.06050.00837.29376270.0000
weekdaySunday0.07140.00838.56476270.0000
weekdayTuesday0.00280.00690.40576270.6854
weekdayWednesday−0.00750.0070−1.07476270.2826
holidaylecture_day−0.15390.0062−24.71676270.0000
periodintervention0.07780.00987.96576270.0000
periodpost_intervention0.05260.00945.59676270.0000
buildingB−0.07810.0607−1.286680.2027
buildingC0.07250.05551.306680.1961
floor3−0.01010.0555−0.183680.8557
floor40.04090.05550.737680.4636
floor50.03160.05540.571680.5699
floor60.05400.05550.972680.3343
conditiongain_frame0.00870.02790.311680.7564
conditionloss_frame−0.01540.0281−0.546680.5870
conditiongain_frame:periodintervention0.00250.01250.20376270.8392
conditiongain_frame:periodpost_intervention0.01100.01001.10276270.2705
conditionloss_frame:periodintervention0.00330.01250.26476270.7915
conditionloss_frame:periodpost_intervention0.00920.01000.91976270.3581
buildingB:floor30.14810.08581.726680.0889
buildingB:floor40.02890.08620.335680.7388
buildingB:floor50.04850.08580.566680.5735
buildingB:floor60.18580.08902.089680.0405
buildingC:floor3−0.04930.0784−0.629680.5315
buildingC:floor4−0.00350.0802−0.043680.9658
buildingC:floor5−0.03650.0822−0.444680.6584
Figure A1. Residuals, averaged per day, of the model with only covariates, per condition.
Figure A1. Residuals, averaged per day, of the model with only covariates, per condition.
Sustainability 13 04380 g0a1

References

  1. IPCC. Summary for Policymakers. In Global Warming of 1.5 °C. An IPCC Special Report on the Impacts of Global Warming of 1.5 °C above Pre-Industrial Levels and Related Global Greenhouse Gas Emission Pathways, In the Context of Strengthening the Global Response to the Threat of Climate Change, Sustainable Development, and Efforts to Eradicate Poverty. 2018. Available online: https://www.ipcc.ch/site/assets/uploads/sites/2/2019/05/SR15_SPM_version_report_LR.pdf (accessed on 10 March 2021).
  2. Brewer, R.S.; Lee, G.E.; Johnson, P.M. The Kukui Cup: A Dorm Energy Competition Focused on Sustainable Behavior Change and Energy Literacy. In Proceedings of the 2011 44th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences, Kauai, HI, USA, 4–7 January 2011; Available online: https://www.computer.org/csdl/pds/api/csdl/proceedings/download-article/12OmNzmLxLl/pdf (accessed on 10 March 2021).
  3. Petersen, J.E.; Frantz, C.M.; Shammin, M.R.; Yanisch, T.M.; Tincknell, E.; Myers, N. Electricity and water conservation on college and university campuses in response to national competitions among dormitories: Quantifying relationships between behavior, conservation strategies and psychological metrics. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0144070. [Google Scholar]
  4. Petersen, J.E.; Shunturov, V.; Janda, K.; Platt, G.; Weinberger, K. Does providing dormitory residents with feedback on energy and water use lead to reduced consumption. In Proceedings of the Greening the Campus VI, Muncie, IN, USA, 15–17 September 2005; Available online: </named-content>https://www.researchgate.net/publication/228634568_Dormitory_Residents_Reduce_Electricity_Consumption_When_Exposed_to_Real-Time_Visual_Feedback_and_Incentives (accessed on 10 March 2021).
  5. Petersen, J.E.; Shunturov, V.; Janda, K.; Platt, G.; Weinberger, K. Dormitory residents reduce electricity consumption when exposed to real-time visual feedback and incentives. Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 2007, 8, 16–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Romanowicz, J.; Jennings, N.; Kaklauskas, A.; Laskari, M.; Tsekeri, E.; Charalambous, C.; Lambrou, C.; Sjolin, E.; Bull, R.; Everitt, D.; et al. Students Achieving Valuable Energy Savings: Project Report 2014-17. 2017. Available online: https://s3-eu-west-1.amazonaws.com/nusdigital/document/documents/34529/2a0cb6a7d13f6081a4125321e1af88a6/20170328_publishable_report_-_final_draft.pdf (accessed on 10 March 2021).
  7. Bekker, M.J.; Cumming, T.D.; Osborne, N.K.; Bruining, A.M.; McClean, J.I.; Leland, L.S., Jr. Encouraging electricity savings in a university residential hall through a combination of feedback, visual prompts, and incentives. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2010, 43, 327–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Jain, M.; Chhabra, D.; Singh, A. Comparing Energy Feedback Techniques for Dormitory Students in India. BuildSys’15. In Proceedings of the 2nd ACM International Conference on Embedded Systems for Energy-Efficient Built, Seoul, South Korea, 4–5 November 2015; Available online: http://www.dgp.toronto.edu/~mjain/BuildSys-2015.pdf (accessed on 10 March 2021).
  9. Odom, W.; Pierce, J.; Roedl, D. Social incentive & eco-visualization displays: Toward persuading greater change in dormitory communities. In Proceedings of the Public and Situated Displays to Support Communities Workshop, OZCHI, Cairns, QLD, Australia, 9 December 2008; Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/228492450_Social_Incentive_Eco-Visualization_Displays_Toward_Persuading_Greater_Change_in_Dormitory_Communities (accessed on 10 March 2021).
  10. Abrahamse, W.; Steg, L.; Vlek, C.; Rothengatter, T. A review of intervention studies aimed at household energy conservation. J. Environ. Psychol. 2005, 25, 273–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kaiser, F.G.; Henn, L.; Marschke, B. Financial rewards for long-term environmental protection. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 68, 101411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Lehman, P.K.; Geller, E.S. Behavior analysis and environmental protection: Accomplishments and potential for more. Behav. Soc. Issues 2004, 13, 13–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Maki, A.; Burns, R.J.; Ha, L.; Rothman, A.J. Paying people to protect the environment: A meta-analysis of financial incentive interventions to promote proenvironmental behaviors. J. Environ. Psychol. 2016, 47, 242–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bolderdijk, J.W.; Steg, L.; Geller, E.S.; Lehman, P.K.; Postmes, T. Comparing the effectiveness of monetary versus moral motives in environmental campaigning. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2013, 3, 413–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Dogan, E.; Bolderdijk, J.W.; Steg, L. Making small numbers count: Environmental and financial feedback in promoting eco-driving behaviours. J. Consum. Policy 2014, 37, 413–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Spence, A.; Leygue, C.; Bedwell, B.; O’malley, C. Engaging with energy reduction: Does a climate change frame have the potential for achieving broader sustainable behaviour? J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 17–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Steinhorst, J.; Klöckner, C.A. Effects of monetary versus environmental information framing: Implications for long-term pro-environmental behavior and intrinsic motivation. Environ. Behav. 2018, 50, 997–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Herziger, A.; Berkessel, J.B.; Steinnes, K.K. Wean off green: On the (in) effectiveness of biospheric appeals for consumption curtailment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 69, 101415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Kesenheimer, J.S.; Greitemeyer, T. Ego or Eco? Neither Ecological nor Egoistic Appeals of Persuasive Climate Change Messages Impacted Pro-Environmental Behavior. Sustainability 2020, 12, 10064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sloot, D.; Jans, L.; Steg, L. Is an Appeal Enough? The Limited Impact of Financial, Environmental, and Communal Appeals in Promoting Involvement in Community Environmental Initiatives. Sustainability 2021, 13, 1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. White, K.; Habib, R.; Hardisty, D.J. How to SHIFT consumer behaviors to be more sustainable: A literature review and guiding framework. J. Mark. 2019, 83, 22–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Cheng, T.; Woon, D.K.; Lynes, J.K. The use of message framing in the promotion of environmentally sustainable behaviors. Soc. Mark. Q. 2011, 17, 48–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Frederiks, E.R.; Stenner, K.; Hobman, E.V. Household energy use: Applying behavioural economics to understand consumer decision-making and behaviour. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 2015, 41, 1385–1394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Homar, A.R.; Cvelbar, L.K. The effects of framing on environmental decisions: A systematic literature review. Ecol. Econ. 2021, 183, 106950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Lange, F.; Dewitte, S. Measuring pro-environmental behavior: Review and recommendations. J. Environ. Psychol. 2019, 63, 92–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Gonzales, M.H.; Aronson, E.; Costanzo, M.A. Using Social Cognition and Persuasion to Promote Energy Conservation: A Quasi-Experiment 1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 18, 1049–1066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Shinohara, H. Ryousei No Denryoku Shouhiryou No Zittai Oyobi Shouene Potential No Kanousei Nikansuru Kenkyu: Kokusaigakuyuuryou Nadeshiko No Eakon Shiyou Wo Zireini. Bachelor’s Thesis, Fukuoka Women’s University, Fukuoka, Japan, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  28. Japan Meteorological Agency. Kako No Kishou Data Kensaku. 2021. Available online: http://www.data.jma.go.jp/obd/stats/etrn/index.php (accessed on 10 March 2021).
  29. Lange, F.; Steinke, A.; Dewitte, S. The Pro-Environmental Behavior Task: A laboratory measure of actual pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 56, 46–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Moshontz, H.; Campbell, L.; Ebersole, C.R.; IJzerman, H.; Urry, H.L.; Forscher, P.S.; Gragem, H.E.; McCarthy, R.J.; Musser, E.D.; Antfolk, J.; et al. The Psychological Science Accelerator: Advancing psychology through a distributed collaborative network. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 1, 501–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. The stickers used in the experiment. Left: loss-frame condition, middle: gain-frame condition, right: neutral frame condition.
Figure 1. The stickers used in the experiment. Left: loss-frame condition, middle: gain-frame condition, right: neutral frame condition.
Sustainability 13 04380 g001
Figure 2. The stickers applied on the air-conditioner remote control.
Figure 2. The stickers applied on the air-conditioner remote control.
Sustainability 13 04380 g002
Figure 3. Average daily energy use per experimental condition in kilowatt-hours.
Figure 3. Average daily energy use per experimental condition in kilowatt-hours.
Sustainability 13 04380 g003
Figure 4. Violin plot of the average energy use per unit, per period. Blue dots represent medians.
Figure 4. Violin plot of the average energy use per unit, per period. Blue dots represent medians.
Sustainability 13 04380 g004
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Iwasaki, S.; Franssens, S.; Dewitte, S.; Lange, F. Evaluating the Effect of Framing Energy Consumption in Terms of Losses versus Gains on Air-Conditioner Use: A Field Experiment in a Student Dormitory in Japan. Sustainability 2021, 13, 4380. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13084380

AMA Style

Iwasaki S, Franssens S, Dewitte S, Lange F. Evaluating the Effect of Framing Energy Consumption in Terms of Losses versus Gains on Air-Conditioner Use: A Field Experiment in a Student Dormitory in Japan. Sustainability. 2021; 13(8):4380. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13084380

Chicago/Turabian Style

Iwasaki, Shimpei, Samuel Franssens, Siegfried Dewitte, and Florian Lange. 2021. "Evaluating the Effect of Framing Energy Consumption in Terms of Losses versus Gains on Air-Conditioner Use: A Field Experiment in a Student Dormitory in Japan" Sustainability 13, no. 8: 4380. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13084380

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop