1. Introduction
Previous research links a broad range of financial behaviors, including savings behaviors and portfolio choices, with financial literacy (see for example, [
1,
2]). This connection presumes, however, that households can and will use financial knowledge to make rational choices. Most of this research focuses on saving and investment behaviors, with almost no attention to credit behaviors. In addition, the role of personality as an influence on financial behaviors has recently attracted the attention of researchers as an important factor that explains financial behaviors [
3].
The current paper examined the influence of both objective financial knowledge and personality traits on risky borrowing behaviors, defined in this research as borrowing relatively large amounts, using rent-to-own, and using payday loans. While rent-to-own is not technically credit, it mimics credit with its requirement of regular payments and costs greater than paying with cash.
The data were from a unique and proprietary sample of Iranian households. Iran is an Islamic country, and, as such, usury-based trading, which includes loans, is prohibited [
4]. However, Islamic alternatives, such as various forms of deferred contracts of exchange in which the borrower pays extra as a profit margin for the seller, effectively provide credit options to Iranian households [
4]. This makes Iran an interesting environment in which to pursue the research question.
2. Literature Review
For decades, researchers have documented inadequacies among individuals in their economic and financial knowledge [
5]. A lack of financial knowledge (or financial illiteracy) affects financial well-being, as it is linked to borrowing, saving, and spending patterns. Mitchell and Lusardi [
6] used three basic knowledge questions to demonstrate that financial literacy is low in many countries, including those with well-developed financial markets. Financial knowledge is especially low in some demographic groups, including women and the less educated. Bucher-Koenen et al. [
7] argued that financial knowledge is perhaps more important for women than for men, but reported that, in their research, men are more financially knowledgeable. Lusardi [
8] analyzed Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) data, which show an alarming lack of financial literacy among the 15-year-old students in the 18 countries and economies where the instrument was administered.
Only limited research has linked financial literacy to risky borrowing behaviors. Lusardi and Mitchell [
9] reported that greater financial literacy is associated with better debt outcomes in the data that they analyzed. In previous research [
10,
11,
12], those with lower financial literacy (or debt literacy in the Lusardi and Tufano [
12] study) were more likely to engage in high-cost borrowing behaviors, including the young adults in the de Bassa Scheresberg [
10] study.
Research about the role of personality traits and financial behaviors has produced important insights. Personality traits are defined in a variety of ways, but a common approach is to identify the Big Five personality traits (extroversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism (vs. emotional stability), and openness to experience (or intellect)) [
13]. Researchers have established a relationship between the Big Five personality traits and financial well-being in general [
14], as well as with saving and borrowing behaviors [
3].
Several researchers have found a relationship between the personality trait extraversion and a positive attitude toward debt and/or risky borrowing behaviors. Brown and Taylor [
15] reported that, among the Big Five personality traits, extraversion and openness to experience have larger (and positive) influences on household finances, in terms of holding unsecured debt and asset holdings, than conscientiousness and neuroticism. In Harrison and Chudry’s [
16] study among university students, extraversion was associated with borrowing behaviors, while neuroticism was not. Schaefer et al. [
17] examined the relationship between the Big Five personality traits and overconfidence in general knowledge among undergraduate university students in an experiment. They found that Extraversion significantly predicted overconfidence, a relationship that the authors suggested could “turn their poor judgments into costly decisions” (p. 298), which could include risky borrowing behavior. Although their measure of personality was whether an individual is Type A, Jalilvand et al. [
18] demonstrated that uninformed Iranian investors were more influenced by personality than informed investors were.
Previous literature suggested two important control variables in addition to selected demographic characteristics. One was financial help-seeking behaviors; Grable and Joo’s [
19] framework guided much of the research in this area. In general, previous research suggested that those more likely to seek help were younger and had fewer resources, including financial knowledge. However, financial help-seeking behaviors vary by population and the type of financial advice sought; both Britt et al. [
20] and Lim et al. [
21] described the unique characteristics of college students who seek financial counseling, a type of help seeking.
van Rooij et al. [
22] demonstrated that sources of financial advice vary by financial literacy. Although 20% of the most financially literate respondents in their sample sought help from parents or friends, in general, those with the least financial knowledge were most likely to seek advice from informal sources. Those with more financial knowledge were more likely to seek advice from professional sources. Grable and Joo [
23] indicated that individuals who chose not to seek financial advice or sought it from non-professionals (family and friends) may be more likely to engage in risky borrowing behaviors than their counterparts.
A second control variable is one’s investment planning horizon. Veld-Merkoulova [
24] reported that those with a longer investment planning horizon also held a greater share of risky financial investments. The tendency to take risks if one has a longer planning horizon also may apply to credit behaviors.
Finally, gender, age, marital status, education, monthly income, and employment were the demographic characteristics used as control variables in this research. Previous research to explain financial behaviors used some or all of these variables (see, for example, [
25,
26]).
Credit in Iranian Culture
As mentioned earlier, Islamic law prohibits usury-based trading. However, Islamic alternatives provide credit options to Iranian households through formal, informal, and semiformal intermediaries [
27]. The Central Bank of Iran sets “profit rates” for lending and borrowing. In 2017, that rate was 18% per year; from 1973 to 2017, the average profit rate was 14.82% [
28].
An alternative to a loan from a bank for Iranian households is using rent-to-own companies. In 2017, the Central Bank of Iran announced that rent-to-own companies would be permitted to operate in the housing market with a profit rate cap of 21% [
29]. Prior to the announcement, Iranian rent-to-own leasing companies were either bank leasing companies or offered services in the auto industry.
A final borrowing option for Iranian households is an illegal loan against one’s next payday. Although referred to as a payday loan in this article, the source for an Iranian payday loan is a relative, friend, or someone in the neighborhood known to offer such loans—not a commercial entity as in the U.S. Because they are illegal, the profit rate is unregulated. Thus, this borrowing behavior may be perceived by financial experts as the riskiest of the three. However, because they are loans from known individuals, Iranian borrowers may not perceive payday loans as risky.
Johan [
30,
31,
32,
33,
34] and others [
35,
36,
37] have investigated Islamic credit card use, use of Islamic personal financing through banks [
38,
39,
40], and even Islamic pawnbroking [
41] in a variety of countries. However, we found no academic work about rent-to-own or payday lending in Islamic culture.
4. Results
The demographic characteristics of the sample are described in
Table 2. The majority of the sample sought financial advice from parents and friends (62.1%). Overall, they were least likely to report a mid-term investment planning horizon (24.8%). The sample included more males (63%) than females, were 45 years old or younger (79.5%), were married (61.1%), well-educated (86.5% had at least a bachelor’s degree), earned incomes of
$600 USD or less (59.4%), and were employed in the public sector (42.6%) or students (20.8%).
Just more than one half of the sample reported that they had borrowed more than $1500 USD; 43.6% and 45.3%, respectively, of the sample used rent-to-own and payday loans. Each of the three risky borrowing behaviors were reported by more than 55% of those who relied on parents and friends for financial advice. A greater proportion of men than women and married than single respondents engaged in each of three types of risky borrowing behaviors. A greater proportion of respondents who earned a monthly income of $300 USD to $600 USD participated in each of the three types of borrowing behaviors compared with the other income groups.
The descriptions for the personality traits are in
Table 3. The results show that, on a scale of 0 to 66, the mean score for each of the five personality traits was relatively equal (35.61 for neuroticism to 37.34 for extraversion). The range in standard deviations was from 5.80 (agreeableness) to 7.06 (extraversion).
The binary correlations between personality traits and borrowing behaviors are shown in
Table 4. There were significant correlations between Openness and engaging in each of the three types of borrowing behaviors. Rent-to-own participation was strongly correlated with each of the five personality traits, suggesting that different consumers may have different motivations for its use.
The descriptive statistics for the responses to the five financial knowledge items are in
Table 5. More than one half of the sample answered three (numeracy, inflation, and money illusion) of the five basic financial knowledge questions correctly (Shafir et al. [
46] explained money illusion as “a tendency to think in terms of nominal rather than real monetary values” (p. 341)). Only 11.1% correctly answered the compound interest question and just 19.1% correctly answered the time value of money question. These results indicate that Iranian individuals are far less financially literate than the respondents in other studies that used these measures. Lusardi and Mitchell [
2] reported mean scores of 77.2% or greater across the five questions in a sample of U.S. adults. Lusardi and Mitchell [
2] also reported that the mean response to the compound interest question in the U.S. was 67.1%, and ranged from 35.2% in Sweden to 86.0% in New Zealand among the 12 countries in which these questions have been asked. Iranians’ low scores on both the compound interest and the time value of money questions may be attributed to the limitations on financial experiences created by the country’s ban on usury-based lending.
The descriptive statistics for the responses to the five financial knowledge items are in
Table 5. More than one-half of the sample answered three (numeracy, inflation, and money illusion) of the five basic financial knowledge questions correctly (Shafir et al. [
46] explained money illusion as “a tendency to think in terms of nominal rather than real monetary values” (p. 341)). Only 11.1% correctly answered the compound interest question, and just 19.1% correctly answered the time value of money question. These results indicate that Iranian individuals are far less financially literate than the respondents in other studies that used these measures. Lusardi and Mitchell [
2] reported mean scores of 77.2% or greater across the five questions in a sample of U.S. adults. Lusardi and Mitchell [
2] also reported the mean response to the compound interest question in the U.S. was 67.1%, and ranged from 35.2% in Sweden to 86.0% in New Zealand, among the 12 countries in which these questions have been asked. Iranians’ low scores on both the compound interest and the time value of money questions may be attributed to the limitations on financial experiences created by the country’s ban on usury-based lending.
In contrast, Iranians were more knowledgeable about inflation than some other populations. The mean response to the inflation question in the U.S. was 75.2%, and ranged from 31.8% in Romania to 81.0% in New Zealand. Inflation rates in Iran have been as high as 50% in recent years, and even higher (85.3% in 2019) for food, drink, and tobacco [
47]. Thus, Iranians should be very aware of what inflation is.
4.1. Determinants of Risky Borrowing Behaviors
The logistic regression results are reported in
Table 6. The pseudo R
2s ranged from 0.237 to 0.263 in the three logistic regressions. Several financial literacy variables were significant in the logistic regressions for borrowing more than
$1500 USD and using rent-to-own. Those who answered the numeracy question correctly were 50% less likely to borrow more than
$1500 USD (odds = 0.501;
p < 0.05) and 36% less likely to use rent-to-own (odds = 0.644;
p < 0.10). Knowledge of compound interest was also negatively associated with use of rent-own (odds = 0.771;
p < 0.10). Knowledge of the time value of money was associated with significantly lower odds of borrowing more than
$1500 USD (odds = 0.521;
p < 0.05) and using rent-to-own (odds = 0.431;
p < 0.05). Knowledge of money illusion was negatively associated with use of rent-to-own (odds = 0.542;
p < 0.10).
The personality trait conscientiousness was significantly and negatively related with borrowing more than $1500 USD (odds = 0.971; p < 0.05) and using rent-to-own (odds = 0.978; p < 0.1). Neuroticism was significantly and negatively related to payday lending.
Seeking information from newspapers, magazines, and other media sources, relative to seeking advice from parents and friends, was associated with reduced odds of borrowing more than $1500 USD (odds = 0.323; p < 0.1) and using payday loans (odds = 0.467; p < 0.05). Additionally, seeking the advice of professional financial advisors was also negatively associated with accessing payday loans (odds = 0.305; p < 0.05). Having a mid-term investment horizon relative to a short-term horizon was associated with reduced odds of borrowing more than $1500 USD, as well as reduced odds of using rent-to-own.
Compared to the reference group of respondents aged 61 or older, those who were between 46 and 60 years old were 79% less likely to borrow more than $1500 USD (odds=0.213; p < 0.1). Compared to those respondents who had monthly incomes of less than $300 USD, those who earned more than $1500 USD were more likely to engage in each of the three borrowing behaviors than those with the lowest incomes. Being employed in the private sector (relative to being unemployed) was associated with increased odds for each of the three borrowing behaviors; perhaps at least in part because lenders may view those individuals as better credit risks.
4.2. Interaction of Income and Financial Knowledge and Risky Borrowing Behaviors
A linear probability model was used to further explore the influence of financial knowledge. Specifically, variables were created to interact each financial knowledge variable with each of the three income categories (less than $300 USD annually was the omitted category). Significant interactions between a financial knowledge variable and the lowest income category would be one indicator of the importance of knowledge of that concept as an influence on financial behavior, as individuals with lower incomes have fewer degrees of freedom to apply their knowledge.
Coefficients, robust standard errors, and odds ratios are reported in
Table 7. Personality traits and the same control variables as used in the previous regression were included in the analysis; however, those results are not reported in
Table 7. The R-squares are similar to those in the earlier regression.
Numeracy and compound interest were the only two financial knowledge variables that were significant influences on risky borrowing behaviors, when those variables interacted with the $301–600 USD annual US income category. Respondents in that income category who correctly answered the numeracy question were less likely to borrow more than $1500 USD and to use payday loans than individuals who earned $300 USD or less a year. Those who understood compound interest and earned $301–600 USD annually were less likely to borrow more than $1500 USD relative to those in the lowest income category.
Knowledge of compound interest and money illusion were significant and negatively influenced use of payday loans and use of rent-to-own, respectively, when interacting with the $601–1500 USD income category. Finally, several of the financial knowledge variables were significant when they interacted with the highest income category. Those earning at least $1500 USD a year, who correctly answered the numeracy, inflation, and time value of money questions were less likely to borrow more than $1500 USD and use payday loans than those who earned less than $300 USD annually. Those who understood the money illusion concept or time value of money and were in the highest income category were less likely to use rent-to-own than those in the lowest income group.
Thus, overall, it appears that numeracy and knowledge of compound interest were the most significant influences on risky borrowing behaviors for lower-income individuals. More income may have given individuals with financial knowledge a greater ability to apply that knowledge to avoid risky borrowing behaviors.
4.3. Interaction of Personality Traits and Financial Literacy on Risky Borrowing Behaviors
As a robustness check, coefficients for interactions between an index of financial knowledge and personality traits were estimated using LPM. The index was constructed based on the number of financial knowledge questions answered correctly. Financial help-seeking sources, investment horizon, and the demographic characteristics included in the previous regressions were also in this regression. Results are reported in
Table 8 only for the primary variables.
The results in
Table 8 show that financial knowledge was negatively associated with all three types of borrowing behaviors. Among the personality traits, neuroticism was negatively associated with borrowing more than
$1500 USD, while openness was positively associated with borrowing larger amounts. The interaction of neuroticism and financial knowledge also was negatively associated with borrowing more than
$1500 USD, as well as with using a payday loan. In contrast to the results for openness alone, openness, when it interacted with financial knowledge, was negatively associated with borrowing more than
$1500 USD. Conscientiousness as well as the interaction of financial knowledge with conscientiousness were negatively associated with using rent-to-own.
The results suggest that financial knowledge may reinforce the influence of personality traits that tend to be associated with positive financial behaviors (such as conscientiousness). However, financial knowledge may not be a strong influence on behaviors among individuals whose dominant personality trait is one associated with risky financial behaviors (such as extraversion).
5. Discussion
A primary goal of this research was to better understand the influence of financial knowledge and personality traits on risky borrowing behaviors. The results indicate that, as in previous studies about household financial decisions and behaviors [
5,
8,
12], objective financial knowledge is associated with the borrowing behaviors of Iranian households. Numeracy and knowledge of the time value of money were associated with lower odds of borrowing more than
$1500 USD and using rent-to-own. Similarly, knowledge of compound interest and money illusion were negatively associated with use of rent-to-own. Respondents who had a better understanding of the concept of time value of money, compound interest, and money illusion, each of which also requires some numerical ability, may be better able to assess the long-term costs of borrowing.
Conscientiousness was associated with reduced odds of borrowing more than
$1500 USD and using rent-to-own. Conscientiousness has been negatively associated with debt holding in previous literature [
15]. Individuals exhibiting conscientiousness often are described as having greater self-control and being less impulsive in their decision-making [
48]. Perhaps individuals exhibiting conscientiousness personality traits are averse to borrowing more than
$1500 USD and the use of rent-to-own, due to the risky nature of these transactions as well as the ambiguity of ownership in rent-to-own arrangements. Rent-to-own for housing also is a relatively recent option in Iran, perhaps creating more discomfort among those exhibiting conscientiousness. In addition, Neuroticism was negatively associated with the use of payday loans. In previous literature [
15], Neuroticism was not associated with having unsecured debt. Thus, more research is needed to understand whether neuroticism is associated with risky borrowing behavior globally, or whether this association is significant only in the Iranian context, where the market for payday loans is mostly informal.
Among other interesting findings from this study, the results indicated that seeking financial information from newspapers, magazines, or other media and receiving professional financial advice from experts were negatively associated with seeking payday loans. Seeking financial information from newspapers, magazines, or other media also was negatively associated with borrowing more than $1500 USD. Compared to those who sought advice from their parents and friends, perhaps those who sought information about debt-related products through more credible and professional sources such as professional financial advisors, newspapers, media, and magazines have gained a better understanding of the higher costs associated with these types of credit. If so, they may be less likely to engage in this type of borrowing behavior. It also is possible that because an Iranian payday loan is an informal loan, seeking advice from parents and friends and asking for a loan is a single act.
Among the demographic characteristics, only income and employment had any consistent influence on borrowing behaviors. Incomes greater than
$1500 USD and private sector employment were positively associated with seeking all three types of risky borrowing. Although, more research is needed to understand this association, respondents earning less than
$1500 USD may not meet the income threshold to qualify for some loans. Further analyses of interactions between income and financial knowledge (
Table 7) suggested that financial knowledge can moderate the likelihood of risky borrowing behaviors, especially among individuals with higher incomes.
The interactions of financial knowledge with personality traits also show that financial knowledge may reinforce the influence of personality traits when the trait is associated with positive financial behaviors. However, financial knowledge may have less influence on individuals whose behaviors are influenced by traits associated with risky behaviors. Additional research is needed to better understand the associations between financial knowledge, personality traits, and financial behaviors.
As with all research, this study has limitations. The most obvious is the convenience sample drawn from Iranian investors. Because they are investors, it seems likely that this sample would have greater financial knowledge than the general population. On the other hand, one fifth of the sample was students and nearly a quarter were in the “other” category, likely unemployed or self-employed. These are not the characteristics typically ascribed to investors. Iran has been described as having an “over-education” problem [
49], as 7.4% of the country’s adult population is enrolled in higher education and unemployment among youth was 26% in 2016.
In this research, payday loans, or loans from a relative, friend, or someone in the household, were classified as risky because they are illegal. However, those seeking these loans may not agree, because the source is a known individual whom they presumably trust. In addition, a payday loan in Iran that is a private money transfer may be motivated by altruism or expectations of future services [
50] and perhaps unique from the other borrowing behaviors studied in this research.
The negative association between financial knowledge and risky borrowing behaviors among Iranian households suggests a silver lining in the context of Iran, where the inclusion of financial education into the academic curriculum could improve the financial knowledge and hence the financial capability of Iranian households. Jalilvand et al. [
18] called for a serious effort to improve financial literacy among individual investors in Iran, citing statistics indicating that only 20% of Iranians are financially literate. The results also suggest, however, that there are limitations to the potential impact of financial education, especially among those with lower incomes and with personality traits often associated with riskier financial behaviors.
The Danns and Danns’ [
51] framework would be useful to design a national financial education strategy. Economic education, financial education, and financial inclusion make up the three prongs of their framework. The Danns [
51] also noted that the Central Bank provides leadership for financial education programs in most developing countries.
Garcia et al.’s [
52] recommendations also would be useful to create a national strategy. The recommendations include providing efficient structures for cooperation between the public and private sectors and considering the needs of the financially excluded and rural populations, which may be different from those of urban populations. However, it also is worth noting that previous research (for example, [
53,
54]) has demonstrated that consumer education and/or disclosure may not impact consumer use of high-cost borrowing behaviors such as payday loans and rent-to-own if those are the consumers’ “best-worst” options.
To evaluate a nation’s financial consumer protection framework, the guidelines and principles from the OECD and G20 and the World Bank [
55,
56] are useful. Ahmed and Ibrahim [
57] organized the key features of a financial consumer protection regime as legislative empowerment and supervisory framework, information disclosure and protection, fair treatment of the financial consumer, complaints and redress, and financial literacy. However, Mak and Braspenning [
58] cautioned that, while both information disclosure and financial literacy are key features of the framework, consumers, especially the most vulnerable groups, often lack the financial knowledge to use disclosures for self-protection. They urge balance with regulation that requires companies to warn and, in certain situations, even prevent high-risk consumers from entering certain risky transactions. Thus, a valid, reliable, and culturally appropriate assessment of a nation’s financial literacy is key information to have before designing a financial consumer protection framework.
Future research and additional data from a more representative sample are needed to better understand the financial knowledge of the Iranian people. Specifically, while measuring financial knowledge by using questions such as those developed by Lusardi and Mitchell [
2] allows for comparisons across countries, this approach may not produce valid results in Islamic countries. Future research in Islamic countries should consider using questions specific to Islamic finance, such as those developed by Antara et al. [
59], Erb and Mutlu [
60], and Ahmad et al. [
61].