Next Article in Journal
Global Sustainability Crossroads: A Participatory Simulation Game to Educate in the Energy and Sustainability Challenges of the 21st Century
Next Article in Special Issue
A System Dynamics Model of Online Stores’ Sales: Positive and Negative E-WOM and Promotion Perspective
Previous Article in Journal
Low-Carbon Behaviour Performance of Scenic Spots in a World Heritage Site
Previous Article in Special Issue
Consumer Purchase Intention toward Crowdfunding Products/Services: A Cost–Benefit Perspective
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Who Clicks on Online Donation? Understanding the Characteristics of SNS Users during Participation in Online Campaigns

1
Institute for Business Research & Education, Korea University, 145, Anam-ro, Seongbuk-gu, Seoul 02841, Korea
2
Department of Business Administration, College of Economics and Business Administration, Daegu University, 201, Daegudae-ro, Gyeongsan-si, Gyeongsangbuk-do 38453, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2019, 11(13), 3674; https://doi.org/10.3390/su11133674
Submission received: 23 May 2019 / Revised: 26 June 2019 / Accepted: 2 July 2019 / Published: 4 July 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Digital Markets and the Consumer)

Abstract

:
The purpose of this study is to explore the characteristics of SNS (Social Network Service) users as potential donors for the sustainability of online donation campaigns. For this purpose, we examined the relationship between SNS users’ characteristics, social interaction and donation intention. Based on self-construal level theory, we assume that SNS users’ characteristics (independent-self and interdependent-self) affect their social interaction and in turn their online donation behaviors. Specifically, we suggest that people with an interdependent disposition tend to have more social interaction, which in turn has a positive effect on participation in donation through the mediation of empathy for the beneficiaries of the donation described in the advertisement. In order to verify the hypotheses, we use structural equation modeling to analyze data collected from 116 participants through online surveys. The findings of this study are as follows. First, individuals with an interdependent disposition have more online social interaction, whereas an independent disposition does not have a significant effect on social interaction online. Second, SNS users’ social interaction has a positive effect on empathy for the beneficiaries of donation. Third, empathy for beneficiaries has a positive effect on participation in online donations. These findings confirm the psychological mechanism between online social interaction and donation behavior and contribute useful guidance for non-profit organizations that design and implement online donation campaigns.

1. Introduction

Companies and people are interested not only in economic benefits and profits but also in social sustainability. In order to build a good corporate image and enhance customer loyalty in the long run, companies are strengthening their corporate social responsibility (CSR) activities related to the industries they belong to as well as to the socially underprivileged and the environment [1]. Individuals are also making efforts to promote social sustainability in a variety of ways, from changing small lifestyle habits such as buying environmentally-friendly products and making sure to recycle used products [2] to more active acts of donating money and time to the areas of their own interests such as culture, art, the environment, education and women’s, children’s and poverty issues. Considering that private donations in the U.S. (in the amount of US $286.65 billion) accounted for approximately 70% of the total U.S. donation amount of US $410.02 billion as of the end of 2017 [3], individuals’ donation activity is an important aspect of social sustainability [4]. Donation campaigns that lead to individual donations are generally planned and promoted through various non-governmental organizations (NGOs, non-profit organizations, e.g., UNICEF). At this time, Most NGOs seek ways to improve communication strategies and donation participation methods based on a deep understanding of potential donors in order to increase campaign performance. This situation is similar to that of a commercial enterprise trying to establish an effective marketing strategy for consumers in general consumption situations.
In this study, we focus on using digital means (e.g., online media) as a method to increase the number of opportunities individuals have to donate. Digital markets are growing rapidly in the marketplace of general offerings (goods and services), and this trend is the same in the donation market. This donation trend gives non-profit organizations and individuals that seek to raise funds access to more potential donors without the constraints of time and physical distance and allows individual donors to search efficiently for information on donation opportunities, promoting participation in donations. According to consumption data from Mastercard users [5], personal online donation behavior in the U.S. has grown steadily from 4.8% to 11.4% annually during the period 2010 through 2017. This is about twice as much as in-person donations as of 2017. In China, which has the fourth-largest number of donors in the world, global companies such as Tencent and Alibaba are also paying attention to the growth potential of online donations and investing in building online donation tools [6]. In sum, understanding and researching donation activities via online platforms is a vital topic for both researchers and practitioners who design donation marketing. Therefore, this study mainly aims to investigate the mechanism which determines the donor’s willingness to donate in the online context.
According to a 2018 report on giving [7], 54% of donors across the world prefer making donations through credit or debit card followed by 9% through PayPal. Donors tend to make fewer donations through mobile apps/wallets (4%) and text messages (1%). See Figure 1.
As seen in Figure 2, 29% of donors across the world say that they were inspired the most by social media to make donations. Among the donors inspired by social media, 56% replied that they were the most inspired by Facebook, 20% said Instagram and 13% said Twitter. Furthermore, 18% of donors across the world made donations to charitable organizations directly through Facebook, and 88% of these donors said they prefer to give through Facebook again. Facebook stands on top among all social media across the world. Its new tools for fundraising will likely transform giving on a global scale. This report [7] tells us that online fundraising is driven by peer-to-peer platforms which solicit donations through individual networks such as SNS.
The advent of social media has changed peer-to-peer fundraising in that it reduces costs for the fundraiser and, at the same time, makes it easier for potential donors to participate. Ultimately, it can be concluded that the relationship between SNS users and their social networks affects donation behavior. For example, the Ice Bucket Challenge became a global sensation that attracted donators worldwide in a short time without spending much money using the method of early participants attracting the next participants through human networks on SNS.
This study looks into online campaigns, a very effective donation method in this age of ever-increasing interest in social sustainability, and aims to find out the characteristics that influence individuals’ responses to these campaigns. Specifically, this study focuses on the social networks of SNS users who are potential donors and regards these networks as an important factor in online charity-giving. In other words, we expect that participation in online-based donation campaigns differs depending on the level of SNS users’ social interaction. The findings can be used to suggest effective digital marketing strategies to improve the performance of an NGO’s online campaign.
Prior studies report a positive relationship between the use of SNS and social networks [8]. That is, forming relationships through SNS is easy and does not incur costs [8], and increasing the use of SNS enables users to form networks with various people [9] and further to make their relationship more intimate [10]. Surprisingly, however, little is known about what effects SNS users’ social networks have on online charity-giving behavior. We expect that this study can make up for the shortcomings of prior studies by positing the level of social interaction and empathy as the mechanisms of donation behaviors. For example, generally, the more interactions with other people a person maintains, the better and more opportunities that person has to understand and care about others’ situations, and this experience stirs feelings or empathy toward people in need. Thus, we can expect that the more and wider social networks the SNS user has, the more actively he or she will invite friends to participate in donation and the more donor participation will result. Here, empathy refers to a fundamental emotional factor which generates pro-social behaviors [11,12,13]. When a person with empathy sees other people in difficult situations or in need, the person understands their situation and becomes sympathetic to their feelings, which arouses the willingness and behavior to help those in need. Therefore, we expect that SNS users’ social interaction has a positive effect on donation behavior, and this relationship is operated by the psychological mechanism of empathy.
In addition, we use self-construal level theory as a lens to look into individual characteristics (independent self and interdependent self) as an antecedent variable affecting SNS users’ social interactions. Many prior studies using this theory have demonstrated that the interdependent self, which values interaction with family, friends and people around the self, better maintains social relationships than the independent self [14,15]. Therefore, SNS users with an interdependent disposition are expected to have broader interpersonal relationships online as they are good at social interactions, which results in increased donation behavior. On the other hand, an independent disposition is not expected to have a positive effect on interpersonal relationships online.
In the next section, we review the literature on SNS users’ social interactions, explore antecedent factors which affect interpersonal relationships based on self-construal level theory and develop a research model by predicting relationships among social interaction, empathy and online donation behavior. We also use structural equation modeling to analyze data collected through online surveys to verify the hypotheses and the research model. Finally, we summarize the study findings and discuss the implications and limitations of this study.

2. Theory and Hypothesis

Prior studies on offline-based donations have investigated the effect of various determinants (e.g., image and brand power of charity organization, demographic characteristics such as age, income, gender, and occupation, internal factors such as the individual’s sense of justice and compassion, previous experience of donation) on donation behaviors [16]. On the other hand, prior studies on online donation conceptually explain the interaction between the companies hosting online campaigns and the participants [17], and rarely conduct empirical studies on the factors affecting donation intention. The empirical studies that have been conducted mainly verify that marketing input factors of the organizations which plan and perform online donations can affect donation behaviors. For example, the aesthetics of an online campaign site or the quantity and quality of information obtained through the website have a positive effect on donation intentions [18], and an online system with quick and easy (efficient) delivery of information can stimulate donation intentions since potential online donors have a high level of need for information about the use of their donations [19]. However, empirical studies on the relationship between the characteristics of participants in online campaigns and their donation behaviors are difficult to find. This study focuses specifically on participants’ characteristics related to online media (online social relationships).

2.1. Online Social Interaction

Early internet studies suggested that interpersonal relationships built through the medium of the internet are insincere, impersonal and even hostile [20,21]. However, these early studies mainly explored how interpersonal relationships were formed through the Internet in laboratory settings and showed that face-to-face situations was more effective for problem-solving tasks than the internet situations [22]. Many of these studies concluded that it is difficult to form deep and serious interpersonal relationships on the internet [23,24,25].
More recent studies suggest a different view on the formation of interpersonal relationships through the internet; although desirable relationships are not formed by a short-term experiment, it is possible to form a meaningful relationship if an online relationship is maintained in the long run. That is, it is possible to form a good human relationship through the Internet in the long term although the formation of the relationship is delayed. Meta-analyses of studies on computer-mediated communication demonstrate that socio-emotional communication is found more frequently in conversations over the Internet than in face-to-face conversations in the long term [26]. In addition, it has been found that interpersonal relationships can be formed on the internet which share intimacy and emotional sympathy although the internet cannot help people to meet directly [27,28,29]. Online game users say they have deeper relationships with the people they play with online than with people they meet in real life, and members of a church mentioned that they know people who interact on the church’s website better than their closest friends [30]. In addition, more dialogues are conducted with people and more friendships are formed by the use of the internet [31]. There is also a view that the internet helps people to build new and meaningful interpersonal relationships beyond the geographical limitations of physical distance [32].
Many studies on internet activities and formation of online relationships have reported that the level of internet activity has a positive effect on interpersonal relationships [33]. As the duration of activity in an online community and the level of participation in the community increase, emotional attachment in online interpersonal relationships also increases [33]. In addition, the more communication one has through online messaging systems, the greater the emotional bond and the more intimate feeling one has with the other party. These findings suggest that people who are active on the internet for a long time and interact with different people can form deep and varied interpersonal relationships on the internet.

2.2. Self-Construal Level Theory and Social Interaction

Interpersonal relationships develop based on deep and intimate social interaction [34], but these relationships differ depending on the level of interdependence [33].
“Self-construal level is a collective concept of an individual’s thoughts, emotions and behaviors in such a way that one individual is considered as independent or as associated with other people" [19]. Self-construal may be independent, which regards oneself as an independent being separate from others, or interdependent, which regards oneself as a connected entity rather than being separate from others [35]. People with independent self-construal seek independence and separation from others and emphasize individuality and uniqueness. Because they seek to achieve uniqueness and self-realization, they value individual thoughts, feelings and behaviors over anything else and are less responsive to social or interpersonal settings. On the other hand, a person with interdependent self-construal seeks to harmonize or assimilate with others and emphasizes connections and relationships. These people put parents, friends, colleagues or close friends before themselves [14,19].
Extending self-construal level theory to interpersonal relationships in the online context, individuals with independent self-construal maintain distance from specific groups and have low levels of social interaction in online networks due to their characteristic of pursuing a more independent and unique personality. By contrast, individuals with interdependent self-construal are expected to have positive and active social interactions in online networks due to their characteristic of valuing assimilation and bonding with others and desiring to show their orientation toward the group. Thus, we predict the following:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Interdependent self-construal has a positive effect on social interaction in online networks.
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
Independent self-construal has a negative effect on social interaction in online networks.

2.3. Social Interaction and Empathy

Positive emotions such as emotional support, empathy and sharing of pleasant experiences are vital in the process of forming and maintaining interpersonal relationships. Communication skills that enable one to actively deliver and accept what one wants to communicate are the beginning of interpersonal relationships and understanding toward others empowers one to form desirable interpersonal relationships [36]. Barnes [37] demonstrated that understanding and sharing empathy with others can be possible based on positive interpersonal interactions, which implies that empathy toward and understanding of the other party are the basic emotions which form and maintain social interaction.
A sizable body of research on the association between interpersonal relationships and empathy has verified a positive relationship between the two [12,38,39]. Empathy is defined as an emotional bond which enables one to identify with what the other party feels and thinks. Without empathy, it is impossible to convey one’s mind to the other party and it is also difficult to understand the thoughts and actions of the other party [40]. In particular, empathy effectively maintains interpersonal relationships and includes the ability to think, feel and communicate in an appropriate manner [41].
Online social interaction also has an important effect on emotion. That is, online interpersonal relationships affect an individual’s positive emotion and help the individual maintain a positive emotional state [42]. Thus, we can predict that online social interaction has a positive effect on empathy, which enables one to feel the emotions of the other party:
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
Social interaction in online networks has a positive effect on empathy.

2.4. Empathy and Prosocial Behavior

Many studies show that empathy is related with helping behavior [38,43,44,45]. According to the empathy-altruism hypothesis (EAH), empathy induces altruistic motivation, and its ultimate goal is to protect or enhance the welfare of the other person with whom one feels empathy [43]. Prior studies on helping behavior refer to empathy and sympathy as the most frequent motivators for donors [43,46]. Most people extend a helping hand more quickly when they feel a stronger emotional pain toward the needy [38,47]. This pain decreases more quickly and they feel better when they help than when they do not. Hence, in order for the donation campaigns to be more effective, it is important to draw out a high level of empathy from the donors for the beneficiaries [12,48]. A recent study on SNS users confirmed that the simple act of writing with empathy for the other party improves prosocial behaviors. Therefore, we expect that empathy positively affects donation intention and serves as a psychological mechanism to explain the relationship between social interaction and donation intention. The hypothesis is presented as follows:
Hypothesis 4 (H4).
Empathy has a positive effect on online donation.

3. Methods and Analysis

3.1. Stimuli and Measures

Our study assumes that online social interaction differs by self-construal level and that online social interaction affects donation intention through empathy (see Figure 3).
To verify the research model, we developed a donation advertisement used as the stimulus in this study. We utilized an online advertisement which could be seen on both a personal computer (PC) and mobile tablet or cellular phone. Specifically, in accordance with the guidelines of Facebook to produce an effective advertisement, the text of the message was less than 500 characters, and the image was set to a resolution of 1200 × 900 and similar to images used in existing online donation campaigns. The advertisement presented images of sick African children receiving treatment along with a solicitation to donate. The message of the donation advertisement under the image is “HELP! You could save a child’s life” (see Figure 4). At the bottom of the ad message, there was a button the user could click to make donations. We did not show a name of the organizer of the donation campaigns (e.g., UNICEF or Save the Children), as these can affect donation behavior depending on the survey participants’ biases. The text was in Korean.
The research questionnaire was translated into Korean and all items except for donation amount used a 7-point Likert scale (strongly disagree-strongly agree). Detailed questionnaire items are shown in Table 1 below. The instrument used to measure the independent variable self-construal was composed of 16 items [19], with 8 items each for independent self-construal and interdependent self-construal [49]. Social interaction as a mediating variable was measured with the scale of Zhang, Matsuoka and Huang [50], modified to fit the online environment and composed of three items: level (number) of relationships with other people, level of interaction with people and level of variety of relationships. Empathy was measured by two items of feeling sympathy and compassion for victims [51]. Finally, donation behavior as a dependent variable was measured with a single item for donation intention, which is a predictor of donation behavior commonly used in studies on marketing and consumers [52,53].
For additional analysis, we asked participants to name a donation amount, which is the resulting behavior of actual respondents in an online donation environment. Most prior studies of individuals’ donation behavior have selected either choice (whether to make donation or not) or amount (level of donation amount) as the result of donation marketing and assumed that the result would be the same if they had chosen the other variable [54,55]. However, researchers have recently understood the outcome of donation marketing in a multi-dimensional manner and point out the problem that these two consequent variables (donation intention and amount of donation) may have different causal relationships with various antecedents [56]. Therefore, in the additional analysis of this study, we examine the problem raised by the prior studies by analyzing the study model through the application of donation amount as a consequent variable.

3.2. Data Collection

A questionnaire survey using the online survey platform Qualtrics was administered to students at a college of business administration in June 2015. They viewed the donation advertisement and were asked to respond to questions about their donation intention, donation amount, empathy toward the subjects of the donation advertisement, level of online social interaction, self-construal level and demographic characteristics.
After administering the questionnaire survey to a total of 147 students, we deleted 31 questionnaires with careless answers, leaving 116 questionnaires for analysis. In terms of their demographic characteristics, 57 respondents were males (49.1%), 59 were females (50.9%) and 90% of respondents were 20 years old. Of the respondents, 61 (52.5%) had an interdependent self-construal while 55 (47.5%) had an independent one. A total of KRW 164,400 was collected through donations from 116 donors. Donation amounts ranged from 0 to 50,000 KRW and 41% of donors donated 10,000 KRW. Looking into donation amount based on self-construal level, donation amount was 15,066 KRW by those with interdependent tendency and 13,182 KRW by those with independent tendency (p > 0.05). The average donation amount by men was 11,649 KRW and that by women was 16,610 KRW. There was no significant difference between male and female groups (Table 2).
To confirm the alternative explanation of prior studies that self-construal can differ depending on culture, respondents were asked whether they have experience of living in foreign countries [14]. The results of t-tests (Table 3) to examine the difference in donation intention between groups depending on the experience of living abroad showed no statistically significant difference between people with experience of living abroad (M = 4.65) and those without (M = 4.4, p = 0.56). In addition, the mean donation amount had no statistically significant difference between the two groups (Mabroad = 13.00 million KRW vs. Mwithout = 14.37 million KRW, p = 0.67).

3.3. Data Analysis

As seen in Figure 1, the research model of this study assumes a three-level causal relationship. While causality or causal relationship between two concepts has traditionally been verified through correlation analysis or regression analysis, these analyses are unable to compare the effect of each path and do not show the causal relationship of two or more mediating variables clearly, which is considered a disadvantage. Thus, for analysis, we used structural equation modeling (using AMOS 17.0), which can identify the causal relationships of all paths for the overall model and demonstrate the relative effects of these causal relationships.
Prior to testing our hypotheses using structural equation modeling, we have to confirm the goodness-of-fit of our measurement model and the reliability and validity of each variable. First, we use confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to validate our model. To demonstrate an acceptable level of fit, the overall fit indexes of the measurement model should be as follows: (1) χ2/df should be smaller than 3~5 [57]; (2) the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) should be under 0.08 [58]; (3) the goodness-of-fit-index (GFI), comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) should be 0.90 or greater [59,60,61]; and (4) the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) should be smaller than 0.10 [62]. Next, we compute Cronbach’s alpha to verify the reliability of the variables; this must be bigger than 0.70 [63]. Though Cronbach’s alpha is not provided by AMOS, it can be calculated using SPSS 25. To confirm the validity of our measures, we identify convergent validity and discriminant validity as those are demanded for structural equation modeling. Convergent validity was assessed using three criteria: (1) average variance extracted (AVE) should be over 0.50; (2) composite reliability (CR) of each construct should be 0.70 or greater; and (3) factor loadings should be greater than 0.70 [64]. Discriminant validity is supported by verifying that the AVE of the latent construct is greater than the square of its correlation with other variables [65].
The next section presents the result of CFA and verifies the hypotheses of this study by analyzing the research model.

4. Results

4.1. Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), the overall fit index of the measurement model is χ2 = 26.72, df = 13, χ2/df = 2.05, p = 0.01, SRMR = 0.06, GFI = 0.95, CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.90, and RMSEA = 0.09, which is an acceptable level. The reliability and convergent validity of the social interaction and empathy variables except for donation intention (which was measured by a single item) and the independent self-construal and interdependent self-construal variables (which were measured by a second-order structure) are presented in Table 4. Discriminant validity is shown in Table 5.

4.2. Hypothesis Analysis

The fit index of the research model indicates a good fit as χ2 = 34.53, df = 18, χ2/df = 1.98, p = 0.01, SRMR = 0.07, GFI = 0.94, CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.91, and RMSEA = 0.09. Table 6 displays the results of the hypothesis analysis.
For Hypothesis H1, which assumes negative causality between independent self-construal level and social interaction, standardized β was 0.71 (p > 0.05), demonstrating no statistically significant interaction. The result of the analysis of H2 that interdependent self-construal has a more positive effect on social interaction was a standardized β of 0.44 (p = 0.00), supporting the hypothesis. For H3, we confirmed that social interaction promotes empathy at a statistically significant level (standardized β = 0.38 (p = 0.00) and for H4, that empathy strengthens donation intention (standardized β = 0.64 (p = 0.00)).
Additional analysis used the donation amount as a consequent variable (Table 7). The fit of the research model shows χ2 = 27.94, df = 18, χ2/df = 1.55, p = 0.06, SRMR = 0.07, GFI = 0.95, CFI = 0.96, TLI = 0.94 and RMSEA = 0.07, indicating appropriateness for analysis. The results of the analysis are shown in Table 6 and are similar to those acquired when donation intention is set as the consequent variable for donation. The hypothesis of the negative relationship between independent self-construal level and social interaction was rejected (H1, standardized β = 0.07 (p > 0.05)), while a positive causal relationship was confirmed between interdependent self-construal level and social interaction (H2, standardized β = 0.44 (p = 0.00)). Next, it was confirmed that social interaction promotes empathy (H3, standardized β = 0.33 (p = 0.00) and empathy enhances donation intention (H4, standardized β = 0.28 (p < 0.01)).
This additional analysis shows that the results for the hypotheses when donation intention was used as a consequent variable were the same as those when donation amount was used as consequent variable, increasing the reliability of this research model.

5. Discussion

5.1. Summary of Findings and Implications

As mentioned in the introduction, the rapidly growing digital market has a great impact on NGO donation campaigns. This study, based on various previous studies on consumer behavior in digital markets, assumes that consumers’ construal levels are expressed in the digital market (social interaction online) and that these consumers’ characteristics affect individuals’ donation intent. In other words, this study makes the first theoretical contribution to expanding the research spectrum of digital markets from general offerings (products and services that are provided from commercial enterprises) to products emphasizing social sustainability through donations.
This study investigated the types of people who are willing to make donations in an online donation environment and what characteristics they have. The results show that donation participation increases with increased online social interaction and that the relationship between social interaction and donation intention is mediated by the psychological mechanism of empathy. Although social interaction, empathy and donation intention have been studied in various research areas, little research on these topics has been conducted in the online setting. That is, the results of SNS users’ donation behavior demonstrated in this study are very new findings and are expected to broaden the range of online donation research. These findings have the additional theoretical contribution of broadening the spectrum of diverse research on sustainable societies. In particular, the findings of this study offer the following theoretical implications:
First, we investigated donors’ characteristics to confirm the characteristics of people who have more social interactions. Based on the self-construal level theory, we classified users as having an interdependent self and independent self and investigated the effect of each characteristic on online social interaction. The interdependent self values relationships with people and tries to form and maintain diverse relationships with various people. This characteristic plays a positive role in online social interactions as well as in face-to-face relationships. However, our results also reject the hypothesis of a negative effect of the independent self on social interactions. A possible explanation for this is cultural influences. Many Eastern countries such as China, Japan and Korea represent highly collectivistic cultures, where an interdependent self-view prevails. In such cultures, harmony and relatedness are more important than individual uniqueness [66]. In other words, although we measured survey participants’ self-construal tendency and divided it into an independent self and interdependent self for analysis, we did not rule out the influence of collectivist culture. This collectivist influence combined with the effect of interdependent self may render the effect on online social interaction insignificant.
Second, through additional analysis, this study settles the problem of using consequent variables raised by a recent study on donation behavior [56]. The research model of this study has even more significant meaning as a donation behavior decision model of online donors as it confirmed that the results of the study when donation intention is used as the consequent variable are not different from those when the donation amount is used as the consequent variable.
Next, empirically, this study shows the need for NGOs to understand potential donors as a sort of consumer and approach them strategically in order to develop a successful online campaign. Specifically, the findings of this study offer the following empirical implications:
First, this study investigated the characteristics of potential donors participating in online donation campaigns. We identified behavioral characteristics of potential donors in online fundraising activities by examining whether those who are active in online social interactions such as SNS activities or online communities participate more in online donations. We expect that users who interact with many people online are leaders of communities or power users who exert significant influence through SNS. As mentioned above, online fundraising campaigns are conducted through peer-to-peer channels such as SNS. Therefore, if practitioners and nonprofit organizations involved in fundraising activities target online influencers in an appropriate manner, these people can be expected to invite their friends to more actively participate, resulting in more donations.
Second, this study investigated the psychological mechanisms of donors who participate in online fundraising. Prior studies have demonstrated that prosocial behaviors are caused by emotions such as empathy and sympathy for beneficiaries. We confirmed that donor empathy works as a psychological mechanism to increase donation participation in the online environment as well. Empathy is one of the factors that best explains altruism as the donor’s emotional response to the beneficiaries. People are more likely to engage in prosocial behaviors such as donations when they are highly empathetic with other people [43,49,52]. Hence, in order to promote participation in donation campaigns, it is vital to draw out a high level of donors’ empathy with beneficiaries.

5.2. Limitations and Suggestions

This study has several limitations that suggest directions for future research. First, the findings of this study rejected the hypothesis of a negative effect between independent self-construal and social interaction. This may be due to the dominant collectivist tendency emphasizing relations in the Eastern culture to which the survey participants belong. Future researchers should distinguish and verify individual tendencies and cultural differences more precisely.
Second, academic studies on the relationship between age and donation have contradictory results; some have found that age has a positive effect on donation experience or donation levels [67,68,69], while others found an inconsistent effect on donations [70,71,72]. Thus, we suppose that it is difficult to apply the results of this study to all age groups. In particular, the Millennial generation has different preferences for making donations than the Baby Boomer generation. While 39% of Millennials prefer to make donation via social media, only 19% of Baby Boomers do [7]. In addition, prior studies on donations have demonstrated that while donor characteristics such as income, occupation, and education level can have an effect on donation behaviors, their results are not consistent. Thus, studies on the relationship between various demographic variables such as donors’ age, gender, income, occupation, and education level and online donation are deemed to be an important follow-up task in the future.
Third, while donors show differential donation behavior based on demographic characteristics such as age, income and religion, they also engage in donation behavior spurred by external motivation such as tax benefits or solicitations or requests from people around them as well as by internal motivations such as empathy and sympathy [43]. Although this study is limited to a psychological mechanism, it is also possible that external motivations such as recommendations from others and social evaluation due to the nature of online donation will have an effect. Future researchers could provide practical implications if they take into account the multifarious aspects of online donation motivation.
Finally, this study used a survey method in a strictly controlled environment to clarify the causal relationship. Although the online donation advertisement used as the stimulus in this study was similar to a real advertisement, caution is necessary in concluding whether survey participants in a virtual environment would make the same donation decisions in a real online donation campaign. In a real-world setting, we may not obtain the same results depending on participants’ available spending budget, past donation experience and so on. To overcome these limitations, future researchers should consider the influence of various variables using actual field data.

Author Contributions

E.Y.P. suggested the initial research idea, performed the literature review, designed and executed the survey. J.H.R. developed the research model and hypotheses, analyzed the data and will be the primary party to handle the review process. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Porter, M.E.; Kramer, M.R. Strategy and society: the link between competitive advantage and corporate social responsibility. Harv. Bus. Rev. 2006, 84, 78–92. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  2. Lee, Y.K. A Comparative Study of Green Purchase Intention between Korean and Chinese Consumers: The Moderating Role of Collectivism. Sustainability 2017, 9, 1930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Givingusa.org. See the Numbers—Giving USA 2018 Infographic. 2017. Available online: https://givingusa.org/see-the-numbers-giving-usa-2018-infographic/ (accessed on 9 January 2019).
  4. Park, K.; Ryu, G. The Effect of Regulatory Focus on Individuals’ Donation Behavior. Sustainability 2018, 10, 760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Nccs.urban.org. Mastercard Donation Insight. 2018. Available online: file:///C:/Users/USER/Downloads/Donation_Insights_June_2018.pdf (accessed on 9 January 2019).
  6. Stanford Social Innovation Review. Available online: https://ssir.org/articles/entry/using_online_donations_to_strengthen_philanthropy_in_china (accessed on 9 January 2019).
  7. The Global Trends in Giving Report. Available online: https://givingreport.ngo/ (accessed on 23 June 2019).
  8. Dońath, J.; Boyd, D. Public Displays of Connection. BT Technol. J. 2004, 22, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Steinfield, C.; Ellison, N.B.; Lampe, C. Social capital, self-esteem, and use of online social network sites: A longitudinal analysis. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 29, 434–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Vitak, J.; Ellison, N.B.; Steinfield, C. The Ties That Bond: Re-Examining the Relationship between Facebook Use and Bonding Social Capital. In Proceedings of the 2011 44th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (HICSS 2011), Kauai, HI, USA, 4–7 January 2011; pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar]
  11. Batson, C.D.; Sager, K.; Garst, E.; Kang, M.; Al, E. Is empathy-induced helping due to self-other merging? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 73, 495–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Batson, C.D. Empathy-induced altruistic motivation. In Prosocial Motives, Emotions, and Behavior: The Better Angels of Our Nature; American Psychological Association (APA): Washington, DC, USA, 2010; pp. 15–34. [Google Scholar]
  13. Batson, C.D.; Lishner, D.A.; Cook, J.; Sawyer, S. Similarity and Nurturance: Two Possible Sources of Empathy for Strangers. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 27, 15–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Markus, H.R.; Kitayama, S. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 1991, 98, 224–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Singelis, T.M. The Measurement of Independent and Interdependent Self-Construals. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1994, 20, 580–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Sargeant, A. Charitable Giving: Towards a Model of Donor Behaviour. J. Mark. Manag. 1999, 15, 215–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Olsen, M.; Keevers, M.L.; Paul, J.; Covington, S. E-relationship development strategy for the nonprofit fundraising professional. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark. 2001, 6, 364–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Shatnawi, T.; Algharabat, R.S. Investigating antecedents and their consequences of usability in online donations: the case of university students’ community services programs. Int. J. Technol. Policy Manag. 2018, 18, 125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Boeder, P.; Hohn, B. Fundraising on the Internet: On-Line Strategies for Nonprofit Organizations. Encyclopedia of Library and Information Science; Marcel Dekker Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  20. Beninger, J.R. Personalization of mass media and the growth of pseudo- community. Commun. Res. 1987, 14, 352–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Stoll, C. The internet? bah. Newsweek 1995, 27 February 1995; 41. [Google Scholar]
  22. Garton, L.; Wellman, B. Social Impacts of Electronic Mail in Organizations: A Review of the Research Literature. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 1995, 18, 434–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Kiesler, S.; Sproull, L. Group decision making and communication technology. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1992, 52, 96–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Dubrovsky, V.; Kiesler, S.; Sethna, B. The Equalization Phenomenon: Status Effects in Computer-Mediated and Face-to-Face Decision-Making Groups. Human–Computer Interact. 1991, 6, 119–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Kiesler, S.; Siegel, J.; McGuire, T.W. Social psychological aspects of computer-mediated communication. Am. Psychol. 1984, 39, 1123–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Walther, J.B.; Anderson, J.F.; Park, D.W. Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction: A meta-analysis of social and antisocial communication. Commun. Res. 1994, 21, 460–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kim, M.S. Social Desirability and Social Contextual Cue in the Formation of Interpersonal Relationship through Computer Mediated Communication. Korean J. Journal. Commun. Stud. 2005, 49, 191–217. [Google Scholar]
  28. Brennan, P.F.; Moore, S.M.; Smyth, K.A. Alzheimer’s Disease Caregivers’ Uses of a Computer Network. West. J. Nurs. Res. 1992, 14, 662–673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Wilkins, H. Computer Talk: Long-Distance Conversations by Computer. Writ. Commun. 1991, 8, 56–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Best, S.J.; Krueger, B.S. Online interactions and social capital: Distinguishing between new and existing ties. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 2006, 24, 395–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Dicken-Gracia, H. The internet and continuing historical discourse. Journal. Mass Commun. Q. 1998, 75, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Rheingold, H. The Virtual Community: Finding Commection in a Computerized World; Addison-Wesley Longman Publishing Co., Inc.: Boston, MA, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  33. Floyd, K.; Parks, M.R. Making Friends in Cyberspace. J. Commun. 1996, 46, 80–97. [Google Scholar]
  34. Kelley, H.H. Attribution in social interaction. In Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of Behavior; Jones, E.E., Kanouse, D.E., Kelley, H.H., Nisbett, R.E., Valins, S., Weiner, B., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Hillsdale, NJ, US, 1987; pp. 1–26. [Google Scholar]
  35. Williams, P.; Aaker, J.L. Empathy Versus Pride: the Influence of Emotional Appeals Across Cultures. J. Consum. Res. 1998, 25, 241–261. [Google Scholar]
  36. Schlein, A.; Guerney, B.G.; Stover, L.; Gurney, B.G. Relationship Enhancement; Josey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
  37. Barnes, S.J. Wireless digital advertising: nature and implications. Int. J. Advert. 2002, 21, 399–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Eisenberg, N.; Miller, P.A. The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychol. Bull. 1987, 101, 91–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Zhou, Q.; Valiente, C.; Eisenberg, N. Empathy and its measurement. In Positive Psychological Assessment: A Handbook of Models and Measures; American Psychological Association (APA): Washington, DC, USA, 2003; pp. 269–284. [Google Scholar]
  40. Smith, A. Cognitive Empathy and Emotional Empathy in Human Behavior and Evolution. Psychol. Rec. 2006, 56, 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Egan, G. Interpersonal Living: A Skills-contract Approach to Human-relations Training in Groups; Brooks Cole: Pacific Groove, CA, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
  42. McIntyre, C.W.; Watson, D.; Cunningham, A.C. The effects of social interaction, exercise, and test stress on positive and negative affect. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 1990, 28, 141–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Batson, C.D.; Shaw, L.L. Evidence for Altruism: Toward a Pluralism of Prosocial Motives. Psychol. Inq. 1991, 2, 107–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Dovidio, J.F.; Piliavin, J.A.; Schroeder, D.A.; Penner, L.A. The Social Psychology of Prosocial Behavior, 1st ed.; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  45. Penner, L.A.; Dovidio, J.F.; Piliavin, J.A.; Schroeder, D.A. Prosocial Behavior: Multilevel Perspectives. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2005, 56, 365–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. A Small, D.; Verrochi, N.M. The Face of Need: Facial Emotion Expression on Charity Advertisements. J. Mark. Res. 2009, 46, 777–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Hoffman, M.L. Empathy and prosocial behavior. In Handbook of Emotions, 3rd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  48. Bekkers, R.; Wiepking, P. A literature review of empirical studies of philanthropy: Eight mechanisms that drive charitable giving. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Q. 2011, 40, 924–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Cross, S.E.; Bacon, P.L.; Morris, M.L. The relational-interdependent self-construal and relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 78, 791–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Zhang, H.; Matsuoka, R.H.; Huang, Y.-J. How Do Community Planning Features Affect the Place Relationship of Residents? An Investigation of Place Attachment, Social Interaction, and Community Participation. Sustainability 2018, 10, 2726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Kogut, T.; Ritov, I. The singularity effect of identified victims in separate and joint evaluations. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2005, 97, 106–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lee, S.; Winterich, K.P.; Ross, W.T., Jr. I’m moral, but I won’t help you: The distinct roles of empathy and justice in donations. J. Consum. Res. 2014, 41, 678–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Kashif, M.; Sarifuddin, S.; Hassan, A. Charity donation: intentions and behaviour. Mark. Intell. Plan. 2015, 33, 90–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Shang, J.; Reed, A.; Croson, R. Identity Congruency Effects on Donations. J. Mark. Res. 2008, 45, 351–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Winterich, K.P.; Mittal, V.; Aquino, K. When Does Recognition Increase Charitable Behavior? Toward a Moral Identity-Based Model. J. Mark. 2013, 77, 121–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Fajardo, T.M.; Townsend, C.; Bolander, W. Toward an Optimal Donation Solicitation: Evidence from the Field of the Differential Influence of Donor-Related and Organization-Related Information on Donation Choice and Amount. J. Mark. 2018, 82, 142–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Carmines, E.; Mclver, J. Analyzing Models with Unobserved Variables: Analysis of Covariance Structures. In Social Measurement: Current Issues; Bohrnstedt, G., Borgatta, E., Eds.; Sage: Beverly Hills CA, USA, 1981; pp. 51–102. [Google Scholar]
  58. Joreskog, K.G.; Sorbom, D. LISREL 7: A Guide to the Program and Applications; SPSS Publications: Chicago, IL, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  59. Herting, J.R.; Costner, H.L. Respecification in multiple indicator models. In Casual Models in the Social Science; Blalock, H.M., Ed.; Aldine Transaction: New York, NY, USA, 1985; pp. 321–393. [Google Scholar]
  60. Bentler, P.M. Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 1990, 107, 238–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Tucker, L.R.; Lewis, C. A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika 1973, 38, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Steiger, J.H. Structural Model Evaluation and Modification: An Interval Estimation Approach. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1990, 25, 173–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Nunnally, J.C. Psychometric Methods; McGraw Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1978; pp. 87–95. [Google Scholar]
  64. Hair, R.J.; Black, W.C.; Babin, B.J.; Anderson, R.E.; Tatham, R.L. Multivariate Data Analysis, 6th ed.; Practice-Hall International: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2006; pp. 71–76. [Google Scholar]
  65. Larcker, D.F.; Fornell, C. Evaluating Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 39. [Google Scholar]
  66. Yoon, S.O.; Suk, K.; Lee, S.M.; Park, E.Y. To seek variety or uniformity: The role of culture in consumers’ choice in a group setting. Mark. Lett. 2011, 22, 49–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Mathur, A. Older adults’ motivations for gift giving to charitable organizations: An exchange theory perspective. Psychol. Mark. 1996, 13, 107–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Midlarsky, E.; Hannah, M.E. The generous elderly: Naturalistic studies of donations across the life span. Psychol. Aging 1989, 4, 346–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Nichols, J.E. Targeting older America. Fund Rasing Manag. 1992, 23, 38–41. [Google Scholar]
  70. Danko, W.D.; Stanley, T.J. Identifying and reaching the donation prone individual: A nationwide assessment. J. Prof. Serv. Mark. 1986, 2, 117–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Edmundson, B. Who gives to charity. Am. Demogr. 1986, 8, 45–49. [Google Scholar]
  72. Schlegelmilch, B.B.; Tynan, A.C.; Tynan, C. The scope for market segmentation within the charity market: An empirical analysis. Manag. Decis. Econ. 1989, 10, 127–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Preferred methods of giving (Source: The Global Trends in Giving Report).
Figure 1. Preferred methods of giving (Source: The Global Trends in Giving Report).
Sustainability 11 03674 g001
Figure 2. Communication tool that most inspires giving and social media that most inspires giving. (Source: The Global Trends in Giving Report).
Figure 2. Communication tool that most inspires giving and social media that most inspires giving. (Source: The Global Trends in Giving Report).
Sustainability 11 03674 g002
Figure 3. Research model.
Figure 3. Research model.
Sustainability 11 03674 g003
Figure 4. Charitable advertisements.
Figure 4. Charitable advertisements.
Sustainability 11 03674 g004
Table 1. Variables and items.
Table 1. Variables and items.
VariablesMeasurement Items
Independent-self1. I enjoy being unique and different from others in many respects.
2. I do my own thing, regardless of what others think.
3. I feel it is important for me to act as an independent person.
4. I’d rather say “No” directly than risk being misunderstood.
5. Speaking up during a class (or a meeting) is not a problem for me.
6. I try to do what is best for me, regardless of how that might affect others.
7. Being able to take care of myself is a primary concern for me.
8. My personal identity, independent of others, is very important to me.
Interdependent- self1. I will sacrifice my self-interest for the benefit of the group I am in.
2. I should take into consideration my parents’ advice when making education/career plans.
3. I feel my fate is intertwined with the fate of those around me.
4. I feel good when I cooperate with others.
5. If my brother or sister fails, I feel responsible.
6. I often have the feeling that my relationships with others are more important than my own accomplishments.
7. My happiness depends on the happiness of those around me.
8. It is important for me to maintain harmony within my group.
Social interaction1. I have a lot of friends in my online community.
2. I like to socialize with my friends online.
3. I know various people in my online community.
Empathy1. I feel sympathy toward the sick children
2. I feel compassion toward the sick children
Table 2. Donation amount1 by respondents’ characteristics.
Table 2. Donation amount1 by respondents’ characteristics.
GenderSelf-ConstrualMeanS.DN
MaleInterdependent11.5658.09523
Independent11.70512.20434
Subtotal11.64910.65457
FemaleInterdependent17.18414.02938
Independent15.57115.62921
Subtotal16.61014.50459
TotalInterdependent15.06612.36661
Independent13.18213.60455
Subtotal14.17212.946116
1 standard unit: millions KRW (1 USD = 1121 KRW as of 27 January 2019). S.D: standard deviation.
Table 3. Results of t-tests.
Table 3. Results of t-tests.
Group VariableNDonation IntentionAmount of Donation1
MeanS.D.tpMeanS.D.tp
Have experience of living abroad?Yes 174.651.320.590.5613.0011.80−0.440.67
No 994.431.6214.3713.18
1 standard unit: millions KRW (1 USD = 1121 KRW as of 27 January 2019).
Table 4. Reliability and convergent validity.
Table 4. Reliability and convergent validity.
VariablesCronbach AlphaFactor LoadingsAVE1CR2
Social interaction0.820.74–0.870.510.76
Empathy0.800.78–0.860.560.71
1 average variance extracted. 2 composite reliability.
Table 5. Root of AVE, correlations matrix.
Table 5. Root of AVE, correlations matrix.
Variables A V E 12345
1. Independent self-construalN.A.1
2. Interdependent self-construal N.A.0.101
3. Social interaction0.720.140.40 *1
4. Empathy0.750.040.180.29 *1
5. Donation intentionN.A.0.080.29 *0.41 *0.561
* p < 0.01.
Table 6. Results of hypotheses.
Table 6. Results of hypotheses.
Hypothesisβ1Standardized βS.E.C.R.
H1. Independent self-construal → Social interaction0.090.710.120.78Reject
H2. Interdependent self-construal → Social interaction0.61 ***0.44 ***0.134.67Support
H3. Social interaction → Empathy0.45 ***0.38 ***0.133.55Support
H4. Empathy → Donation intention0.82 ***0.64 ***0.136.34Support
*** p = 0.00.
Table 7. Results of additional analysis.
Table 7. Results of additional analysis.
Hypothesisβ1Standardized βS.E.C.R.
H1. Independent self-construal→ Social interaction0.090.070.12.81Reject
H2. Interdependent self-construal → Social interaction0.60 ***0.44 ***0.134.63Support
H3. Social interaction →Empathy0.48 ***0.33 ***0.133.60Support
H4. Empathy→ Donation amount0.33 **0.28 **0.132.53Support
*** p = 0.00, ** p < 0.01.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Park, E.Y.; Rhee, J.H. Who Clicks on Online Donation? Understanding the Characteristics of SNS Users during Participation in Online Campaigns. Sustainability 2019, 11, 3674. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11133674

AMA Style

Park EY, Rhee JH. Who Clicks on Online Donation? Understanding the Characteristics of SNS Users during Participation in Online Campaigns. Sustainability. 2019; 11(13):3674. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11133674

Chicago/Turabian Style

Park, Eun Young, and Jin Hwa Rhee. 2019. "Who Clicks on Online Donation? Understanding the Characteristics of SNS Users during Participation in Online Campaigns" Sustainability 11, no. 13: 3674. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11133674

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop