Next Article in Journal
Institutional Drivers of Crowdfunding Volumes
Next Article in Special Issue
The Evolution of Arrears among US Households 1995–2013
Previous Article in Journal
The Effect and Impact of Signals on Investing Decisions in Reward-Based Crowdfunding: A Comparative Study of China and the United Kingdom
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How to Measure Financial Literacy?

by
Marc Oliver Rieger
Department IV, University of Trier, 54286 Trier, Germany
J. Risk Financial Manag. 2020, 13(12), 324; https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm13120324
Submission received: 15 November 2020 / Revised: 7 December 2020 / Accepted: 15 December 2020 / Published: 19 December 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Household Finance)

Abstract

:
Financial (il-)literacy and its effects have been studied extensively in recent years. The measurement of this concept is, however, tricky and numerous measurement instruments exist. In this paper, we study the connection between these measures empirically. We find that these measures are often only slightly related and that this is a so-far overlooked empirical problem in this field. As a result of our analysis, we suggest the combination of two measures as the best potential alternative to the existing measures. Finally, we analyze the predictive power of this suggested measure for stock investment decisions.

1. Introduction

Financial literacy is defined as the “person’s competency for managing money”, according to Remund (2010), or, according to a more comprehensive definition, as “a combination of awareness, knowledge, skill, attitude and behaviour necessary to make sound financial decisions and ultimately achieve individual financial well-being” (INFE 2011). It is a necessary precondition for reasonable financial decisions and, thus, for financial well-being. Lack of financial literacy and its causes have been discussed by academics and practitioners alike, see, e.g., Reference Bucher-Koenen and Lusardi (2011) and Burke and Manz (2014). Furthermore, the effects of financial illiteracy have also been investigated in numerous studies (Bianchi 2018; Fernandes et al. 2014; Gathergood and Weber 2017; Lusardi and Mitchell 2014; Van Ooijen and van Rooij 2016; Van Rooij et al. 2011). In all these studies, the measurement of financial literacy is of central importance. This is a non-trivial task (Marcolin and Abraham 2006), for the completion of which various scales have been applied. It has been criticized that, in many studies, the concept of financial literacy was not adequately defined (Huston 2010). The underlying assumption seems to be that financial literacy can be measured in essentially every possible way and would still lead to the same general conclusions. In this paper, we aim to investigate the following question: Do different scales of financial literacy correlate or do they measure entirely different variables that are not much related?
Hung et al. (2009) have already studied this issue for a small set of measures, but we want to consider more (and also newer) measures and take a step forward to illustrate which of these scales are able to best represent the overall effect and whether the appropriate combination of scales can improve this. Knoll and Houts (2012) used the Item Response Theory (IRT) as a tool for selecting the relevant items from a list of existing questions of a larger survey.
Our approach differs from the previous ones in that we start with a number of scales that have been applied in literature to measure financial literacy and conduct a survey that includes these financial literacy scales. None of these scales has been developed by the author of this article, so the analysis can be conducted freely and without any danger of conflicting interests.
We will see that the relationship between these different measures is weaker than one might expect, and this raises certain concerns with regard to their application. As a result of the conducted analysis, we will derive a general recommendation for the measurement of financial literacy. Then, we will test whether the proposed scale can be used as a predictive instrument for investment decisions and the question of whether they yield long-term returns. This scale will also be compared to a scale of self-assessed financial literacy which did not prove to be a sufficiently appropriate instrument for measuring financial literacy nor predicting outcomes of financial decisions, while the suggested scale performs very well in this context.
The novelty of our study is that we measure financial literacy using multiple scales within one subject population (which, so far, has been done only in a small number of studies by Hung et al. 2009). This allows to test the existing various scales against each other. Moreover, we derive practical recommendations from this analysis and test the ability of the resulting scale to predict actual financial decisions.
The remainder of this paper is structured as follows: In Section 2, we present the methodology of our research, in particular the survey design and the subject pool. In Section 3, we discuss the results, describing the financial literacy questions by topic and then following the research agenda outlined above. Conclusions and recommendations are presented in Section 4.

2. Methodology

2.1. Survey Design

Our survey consists of three parts:
The survey was programmed in Unipark and administered online. As announced in the advertisement for the survey, one of the subjects was randomly selected to receive a monetary prize following the expiration of the survey. The exact amount was determined based on their answer given to the (risky) investment decision and chance, as specified below.

2.2. Subjects

The survey was conducted with the participation of two subject samples, with the first sample containing 129 participants and the second sample containing 149 participants. The only difference between the samples was that the questionnaire for the second sample included several additional questions. For the purposes of this paper, we will pool both groups.
The survey was advertised at a university, and this is the reason why the majority of the participants (89%) are students. However, there were also subjects (21%) that were already employed (at least part-time). In the advertisement, we did not reveal the fact that the survey was about financial literacy. Instead, we mentioned that the questions included “knowledge questions” but also made it clear that the chances of winning were not influenced by the given answers in order to reduce self-selection effects. The percentage of women in the survey (62%) slightly exceeded that of men. Very few subjects had work experience in the financial sector (7%), and only a fifth studied economics (or a related subject), so it was no surprise that the self-assessed financial literacy level was rather low (around 3 on a scale from 1 to 7); see Table 1 for details.

2.3. Analysis of Financial Literacy Questions

To get a better understanding of the financial literacy scales proposed in literature, we first classified the questions into five categories: specific financial knowledge, financial mathematics, inflation rates, mathematics, and cognitive reflection.1
These topics were not evenly distributed, and none of the scales covered all of these topics; see Table 2 for an overview.
The table makes it clear that the scales might be too narrow to cover every aspect of financial literacy. On the other hand, there is the possibility that these topics are so closely intertwined that measuring one of them will already be enough to capture the full spectrum. And of course, our characterization is not comprehensive, and the items of one scale might be broader than our overview suggests.

3. Results

3.1. Comparison of Measurements

A good scale should be able to distinguish between different levels of financial literacy. A scale where all participants get all (or no) questions right would not be very useful. Table 3 shows, however, that this is not a major issue—probably, with the exception of the scale proposed by Lusardi and Mitchell (2011), which might have been too easy for our subjects. A comparison of the proportion of correct answers in our sample with other studies would be of interest, but, given the inherent differences between subject groups, we will refrain from it within the framework of this paper.
Another point to examine is the validity of the proposed scales. To this end, we calculated Cronbach’s Alpha for all eight scales. Generally, high values are desirable, with 0.5 typically being considered as the smallest acceptable value. Yet, given that some of the scales comprised very few items, we cannot expect Cronbach’s Alpha to be too high, so that even a value of 0.4 could be considered sufficient in this case. We found, however, that some of the scales failed to reach this low threshold, which suggests that they might measure other concepts along with financial literacy (compare Table 4). Accordingly, the scale proposed by Ćumurović and Hyll (2019), with or without CRT, was most reliable, while we consider the scale proposed by Lusardi and Mitchell (2011) still to be acceptable, given that it consists of only three items.
Reliability, however, is not sufficient for a good scale: A scale can be highly reliable, but, if it measures something entirely different, it cannot successfully serve our purposes. Therefore, we tested the correlations between the various financial literacy measures (see Table 5). The good news is that most measures do significantly correlate with each other. Thus, despite the fact that the questions included in the scales usually focus on different topics, the scales are still related with each other. The correlation coefficients, however, are often relatively small and sometimes not statistically significant. This means that it matters which scale we use.
Next, we calculated the correlations between the scales and the scores in the five categories mentioned above (specific financial knowledge, financial mathematics, inflation rates, mathematics, and cognitive reflection). In most cases, the correlations were statistically significant (see Table 6). There were, however, considerable differences between the correlation coefficients (see Table A1 for a robustness check).
To illustrate the relationship between the existing measures and to find out which of them are the most representative ones, we used multidimensional scaling (PROXimity. SCALing (PROXSCAL)), which allows to depict these relationships graphically (Figure 1). The graph suggests that the scales proposed by Lusardi and Mitchell (2011) and Ćumurović and Hyll (2019) might be most representative for the various financial literacy scales.

3.2. Suggested New Measure

In the previous sections, we came to the conclusion that the scales proposed by Lusardi and Mitchell (2011) and Ćumurović and Hyll (2019) are the ones best suited for measuring financial literacy. We also found that the removal of CRT questions from Ćumurović and Hyll (2019) does not impair the overall performance of the scale (compare Table 4 and Table 5). Thus, we decided to combine the non-CRT part of the scale proposed by Ćumurović and Hyll (2019) with the scale proposed by Lusardi and Mitchell (2011). The resulting scale consists of six items, which is a reasonable quantity of items that enables easy measurements. The reliability of the scale is good with Cronbach’s Alpha being 0.62.
The correlation of the suggested scale with other scales is also good, as shown in Table 7. We also find that the correlation of the suggested scale with the total financial literacy score, which is calculated as the total of the questions included in the eight scales, is excellent (above 80%, see Table 7). Multidimensional scaling with these additional variables shows that the combined measure gives an even better representation of financial literacy than the scales individually (Figure 2).
The percentages of correct answers for the individual items in the combined scale are between 60% and 90% (see Table 8).

3.3. Dependence of Measurement on Other Factors

In the following, we will study several characteristics of the new combined Cumurovic-Lusardi measure (“CL” for short). In particular, we want to find out whether self-assessed financial literacy is related to CL and how demographics (age, gender, education) and cognitive reflection affect CL. Finally, we will also test whether the attitude of individuals towards money is correlated with this financial literacy measure.
As seen in Table 9, CL is higher for males, students, working people, and those having a university degree.2 There is no significant correlation between the CL-scale and age. Given that the sample is very homogeneous in terms of age, it is no surprise that we do not find age effects. Thus, this finding might be attributable to the composition of our subject pool and should be tested on a more heterogeneous sample.
We also find that CL is not higher for students majoring in economics, but it is higher for students having a university degree. This might appear surprising, but a possible explanation to this is that our undergraduate programs in Business and Economics do not include a finance class in the first year of studies. This means that many students that took our survey had not taken finance classes before. Moreover, the finance class in the second year of studies concentrates mostly on corporate finance and, therefore, might not considerably promote the enhancement of general financial literacy. Subjects with a Bachelor’s degree, on the other hand, do exhibit a higher level of financial literacy.
In addition, further analysis reveals a seemingly surprising result: there is no significant correlation between CL and self-assessed financial literacy (with a Pearson correlation coefficient of 6%). Does this signal a potential error of the new scale?
To find this out, we should take a look at other financial literacy measures for comparison: Burke’s and Anderson’s scales show modest significant correlations with self-assessed financial literacy (21% and 18%, respectively), but all other measures show no significant correlation. It appears that self-assessed financial literacy and measured financial literacy are barely related.
A possible explanation for this interesting observation could be that people tend to compare their knowledge level with that of their peers with similar backgrounds, and, as a result, they report a “relative” level of knowledge. This can also explain why there is no effect of education on self-assessed financial literacy, while we have already observed such an effect for the measured financial literacy. To sum up, one should be cautious about trusting self-assessed financial literacy in surveys.
How is financial literacy related to the attitudes towards money (Fünfgeld and Wang 2009)? Of the five dimensions (anxiety, need for saving, interest in financial issues, free-spending, and intuitive decisions), only interest in financial issues, anxiety and intuitive decision-making should be a priori related to financial literacy. (Anxiety about financial issues could discourage subjects from familiarizing themselves with this topic, which is needed to increase financial literacy. Intuitive decision-making would be indicative of a less thoughtful approach towards financial affairs, which would again prevent people from making more effort to enhance their financial literacy.) Indeed, in our correlation analysis, we find that these three factors are significantly related to CL, while the same cannot be said about the other two factors (see Table 10). With the exception of one scale (Burke), none of the other financial literacy measures correlate significantly with these three scales.

3.4. Predictive Power of the New Scale: Stock Investment and Belief in Stock Returns

Within the framework of this study, we have defined a more reliable financial literacy scale that seems to perform well. But we have not discussed its predictive power for actual behavior and attitudes.
To test this, we included two relevant items into our survey regarding stock market investment. The first one was an incentivized question on asset allocation where the subjects could select stocks, and the second one was a question, where the subjects were asked to state to what extent they agreed with the statement that stocks yielded good returns in the long-run. Table 11 shows that financial literacy—as measured with the CL scale—does, indeed, have an impact on these survey items. This is the case even after controlling for a number of other items, including CRT, demographics or the self-assessed financial literacy. In fact, CL is the only variable that has a strong significant impact on all of these items.
When comparing the external validity of CL with the external validity of other financial literacy measures (see Table 12 and Table 13), we see that some of them seem to have no significant external validity with regard to stock investments or the fact of viewing stocks as good long-term investments. In fact, only three scales (Burke, Lusardi, Ooijen) perform as well as CL. We do not claim that this alone proves the superiority of the CL scale because other external validity tests could be conducted that might produce different results. The lack of significance for some of the measures, however, may be indicative of crucial limitations.

4. Conclusions

We have seen that typical financial literacy measures are mostly significantly, but not always, strongly correlated. We also found that some of the scales proposed in literature have a low reliability. By putting together parts of the two measures that were most representative of the financial literacy questions in our study, we have proposed a short (six-item) and powerful scale that has a significant predictive power for actual financial behavior and attitudes.
For now, we would recommend using this combined scale (“CL scale”) to measure general financial literacy.
There are obvious limitations to our study: in particular, we studied “only” eight measures proposed in literature, even though there are many other measures. There are, however, limitations to the feasibility of a study where every subject has to answer many questions regarding financial literacy. Of course, it would be of great value to have direct connections to real-life financial decisions in the data, rather than only hypothetical ones. However, it seems difficult to simultaneously elicit a lengthy questionnaire and obtain field data on financial decision-making. Nevertheless, this approach might be promising for future work and could help to identify or propose measures that have a higher predictive power for actual financial decision-making.
Future studies could test whether the CL scale can be improved, e.g., by adjusting different weights to its items or by reducing the number of items. We refrained from doing this, since we did not aim to provide an optimal scale but, rather, to point out the general problem of weak correlation between certain scales and to suggest a reasonable alternative, which would be as close as possible to the existing frequently-applied scales.
Another line of future research may study the question of how financial literacy measures vary among different demographic groups, and especially which tests have a higher discriminatory power for which group, e.g., a certain measure might be too easy for more knowledgeable individuals, and other measures might be too difficult for less knowledgeable individuals. Our results point out such differences, but further studies are needed.
We hope that our work is a step in the right direction and that we have been able to illustrate the limitations of currently applied measures and give some practical advice on what scale should be used to measure financial literacy in a more reliable way.

Funding

This research was partially funded by the State of Rhineland-Palatinate through the research initiative at the University of Trier.

Acknowledgments

I thank Rico Dering and Pascal Langer for the data collection on this project and Marco Korngiebel and Karine Nanyan for their help with the preparation of this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

Appendix A. Survey Questions

In this appendix, we want to summarize the questions that were not simply taken from the literature cited in Section 2.1:
Demographic questions were standard items eliciting gender, age, education, status as student, having a job (at least half-time), previous work experience in the financial sector, and whether the subject studies or has ever studied an economic-related subject.
Self-assessed financial literacy was elicited with the question: “On a scale of 1 to 7, where 1 means very low and 7 very high, how would you rate your evaluate financial knowledge?”
The hypothetical investment question was formulated as follows:
“Suppose you can use a time machine to travel back in time to a randomly selected time between 2002 and 2012, your year of arrival. You will receive 50 EUR as starting capital. You can deposit the money for five years. The final amount you receive is the value your machine would have had after the year of arrival. (for example: If you happen to land in 2008, the 50 EUR will be invested on 1 January 2008 as you have indicated. Your payout then corresponds to the value of your investment on 1 January 2013, exactly five years later).
How would you divide your 50 EUR among the following investments?”
The subjects had to split the amount between four different assets: a saving account, a government bond with 5 years maturity, the German stock market index (DAX), the S&P 500. The share of the two stock market investments was added to arrive at the variable “stock investment”.
One of the subjects was randomly selected at the end and obtained the payoff that his chosen investment would have gotten in the scenario described above, where the starting year was decided by chance, and the payoff computed according to historic data. In this way, the question was incentivized.
The general ideas about long-term investment were computed based on the following two items:
“In the long run, shares have a good return.” and “Investments in shares are only something for gamblers.”3
The subjects could indicate to what extent they agreed or disagreed with each of these statements using a 5-point Likert scale from “Strong approval” to “Strongly dismissive”. The difference of these two answers was then coded as “stocks having good long-term investment”.

Appendix B. Education and Financial Literacy Measures

Figure A1. Effects of education on financial literacy measures. For most (but not all) measures in our sample, a university qualification (in Germany, this corresponds usually to an “Abitur”) increases financial literacy, but a university degree does not.
Figure A1. Effects of education on financial literacy measures. For most (but not all) measures in our sample, a university qualification (in Germany, this corresponds usually to an “Abitur”) increases financial literacy, but a university degree does not.
Jrfm 13 00324 g0a1

Appendix C. Robustness Test

Table A1. Robustness check: partial correlations of the financial literacy measures with the four subtopics and cognitive reflection test (CRT), controlling for the survey wave.
Table A1. Robustness check: partial correlations of the financial literacy measures with the four subtopics and cognitive reflection test (CRT), controlling for the survey wave.
Inflation KnowledgeFinancial MathFinancial KnowledgeMath AbilityCRT
Burke0.939 **0.281 **0.271 **0.0440.209 **
<0.001<0.001<0.0010.469<0.001
Lusardi0.282 **0.591 **0.497 **0.134*0.19 **
<0.001<0.001<0.0010.0260.001
Anderson0.0810.363 **0.34 **0.27 **0.149*
0.179<0.001<0.001<0.0010.013
Bianchi0.1710.408 **0.789 **0.249 **0.372 **
0.004<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
Gathergood0.1950.783 **0.288 **0.1710.293 **
0.001<0.001<0.0010.004<0.001
Ooijen0.213 **0.681 **0.26 **0.1810.282 **
<0.001<0.001<0.0010.003<0.001
Alexeev0.423 **0.42 **0.382 **0.1130.524 **
<0.001<0.001<0.0010.06<0.001
Cumurovic0.305 **0.447 **0.691 **0.475 **0.832 **
<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
CL0.339 **0.547 **0.829 **0.399 **0.314 **
<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
** Correlation is significant at the 1% level, * correlation is significant at the 5% level.

References

  1. Alexeev, Natalia, Brenda J. Cude, and Irina A. Kunovskaya. 2014. Evaluation of a financial literacy test using classical test theory and item response theory. Journal of Family and Economic Issues 35: 516–31. [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson, Anders, Forest Baker, and David T. Robinson. 2017. Precautionary savings, retirement planning and misperceptions of financial literacy. Journal of Financial Economics 126: 383–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Bianchi, Milo. 2018. Financial literacy and portfolio dynamics. The Journal of Finance 73: 831–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Bucher-Koenen, Tabea, and Annamaria Lusardi. 2011. Financial literacy and retirement planning in germany. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance 10: 565–84. [Google Scholar]
  5. Burke, Mary A., and Michael Manz. 2014. Economic literacy and inflation expectations: evidence from a laboratory experiment. Journal of Money, Credit and Banking 46: 1421–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Ćumurović, Aida, and Walter Hyll. 2019. Financial literacy and self-employment. Journal of Consumer Affairs 53: 455–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Fernandes, Daniel, John G. Lynch Jr., and Richard G. Netemeyer. 2014. Financial literacy, financial education, and downstream financial behaviors. Management Science 60: 1861–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Frederick, Shane. 2005. Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives 19: 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Fünfgeld, Brigitte, and Mei Wang. 2009. Attitudes and behaviour in everyday finance: Evidence from switzerland. International Journal of Bank Marketing 27: 108–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Gathergood, John, and Jörg Weber. 2017. Financial literacy, present bias and alternative mortgage products. Journal of Banking & Finance 78: 58–83. [Google Scholar]
  11. Hung, Angela, Andrew M. Parker, and Joanne Yoong. Defining and Measuring Financial Literacy. RAND Corporation Working Paper WR-708. Santa Monica, CA, USA: RAND Corporation.
  12. Huston, Sandra J. 2010. Measuring financial literacy. Journal of Consumer Affairs 44: 296–316. [Google Scholar]
  13. INFE. 2011. Measuring financial literacy: Questionnaire and guidance notes for conducting an internationally comparable survey of financial literacy. In Periodical Measuring Financial Literacy: Questionnaire and Guidance Notes for conducting an Internationally Comparable Survey of Financial Literacy. Paris: OECD. [Google Scholar]
  14. Knoll, Melissa A. Z., and Carrie R. Houts. 2012. The financial knowledge scale: An application of item response theory to the assessment of financial literacy. Journal of Consumer Affairs 46: 381–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2011. Financial literacy around the world: an overview. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance 10: 497–508. [Google Scholar]
  16. Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2014. The economic importance of financial literacy: Theory and evidence. Journal of Economic Literature 52: 5–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Marcolin, Sonia, and Anne Abraham. 2006. Financial Literacy Research: Current Literature and Future Opportunities. Paper presented at Conference Proceedings: 3rd International Conference on Contemporary Business, Blue Mountains, NSW, Australia, 21–22 September. [Google Scholar]
  18. Remund, David L. 2010. Financial literacy explicated: The case for a clearer definition in an increasingly complex economy. Journal of Consumer Affairs 44: 276–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Van Ooijen, Raun, and Maarten C. J. van Rooij. 2016. Mortgage risks, debt literacy and financial advice. Journal of Banking & Finance 72: 201–17. [Google Scholar]
  20. Van Rooij, Maarten, Annamaria Lusardi, and Rob Alessie. 2011. Financial literacy and stock market participation. Journal of Financial Economics 101: 449–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
1.
We were, indeed, surprised to find questions that have nothing to do with financial literacy as such, for example questions that are pure math problems or the classical cognitive reflection test (CRT) from Frederick (2005). In fact, the use of CRT in the scale proposed by Ćumurović and Hyll (2019) seemed to us so out of place in the given context, that we also used a modified version of this scale which did not include CRT.
2.
The effects of education on the other financial literacy measures are shown in Figure A1 in the Appendix A.
3.
In the original German version, “gamblers” was translated as “Zocker” which has a much more negative connotation than the English word.
Figure 1. Multidimensional scaling of the financial literacy measures shows that the scales by Lusardi and Cumurovic are most representative.
Figure 1. Multidimensional scaling of the financial literacy measures shows that the scales by Lusardi and Cumurovic are most representative.
Jrfm 13 00324 g001
Figure 2. Multidimensional scaling of the different financial literacy measures shows that the joint scale by Lusardi and Cumurovic (without CRT) is the most representative scale for the total financial literacy (Finlit). We use PROXSCAL (PROXimity. SCALing), minimizing normalized raw stress, with ratio proximity transformations.
Figure 2. Multidimensional scaling of the different financial literacy measures shows that the joint scale by Lusardi and Cumurovic (without CRT) is the most representative scale for the total financial literacy (Finlit). We use PROXSCAL (PROXimity. SCALing), minimizing normalized raw stress, with ratio proximity transformations.
Jrfm 13 00324 g002
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Proportion/AverageN
Female62%278
Male38%278
Age (years)27.5149
Students89%149
Working (at least 50%)21%149
Highest degree:
  No degree0%278
  High school, practical level3%278
  High school, medium level10%278
  High school, highest level37%278
  University degree49%278
Working experience in finance7%149
Studying economics (or a similar subject)20%149
Subjective financial knowledge, scale2.95149
  from 1 (very low) to 7 (very high)
Table 2. Content of financial literacy measurements.
Table 2. Content of financial literacy measurements.
MeasurementTopics
StudyInflationFinancialFinancialMath.Cognitive
(First Author)KnowledgeMath.KnowledgeSkillReflection
Burkex
Lusardi xx
Anderson xx
Bianchi xx
Gathergood x
Ooijen x
Alexeevxx x
Cumurovic x x
Table 3. Proportions of correct answers for various measures of financial literacy.
Table 3. Proportions of correct answers for various measures of financial literacy.
MeasurementCorrect Answers
(First Author)MeanMedianStd. Dev.
Burke49%50%28%
Lusardi83%100%26%
Anderson44%50%34%
Bianchi68%80%24%
Gathergood53%50%29%
Ooijen35%33%25%
Alexeev71%67%29%
Cumurovic63%67%28%
Cumurovic w/o CRT71%67%33%
CRT56%67%37%
Table 4. Reliability of financial literacy measurements and cognitive reflection (CRT). Values of Cronbach’s Alpha above 0.4 are in italics; values above 0.6 are in bold.
Table 4. Reliability of financial literacy measurements and cognitive reflection (CRT). Values of Cronbach’s Alpha above 0.4 are in italics; values above 0.6 are in bold.
Cronbach’s AlphaNumber of Items
Burke0.2924
Lusardi0.4343
Anderson0.1442
Bianchi0.3645
Gathergood0.4684
Ooijen0.1793
Alexeev0.2733
Cumurovic0.6556
Cumurovic w/o CRT0.6173
CRT0.6013
Table 5. Correlations between different financial literacy measures.
Table 5. Correlations between different financial literacy measures.
BurkeLusardiAnd.BianchiGath.OoijenAlexeevCum.Cum. w/o CRT
Burke10.255 **0.0820.141*0.171 **0.234 **0.238 **0.273 **0.244 **
<0.0010.170.0190.004<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
278278278278278278278278278
Lusardi0.255 **1.138*0.374 **0.404 **0.329 **0.294 **0.319 **0.322 **
<0.001 0.021<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
278278278278278278278278278
Anderson0.0820.138 *10.180 **0.147 *0.0570.123 *0.195 **0.171 **
0.170.021 0.0030.0140.340.0410.0010.004
278278278278278278278278278
Bianchi0.141 *0.374 **0.180 **10.303 **0.260 **0.191 **0.521 **0.474 **
0.019<0.0010.003 <0.001<0.0010.001<0.001<0.001
278278278278278278278278278
Gathergood0.171 **0.404 **0.147 *0.303 **10.415 **0.1170.345 **0.255 **
0.004<0.0010.014<0.001 <0.0010.052<0.001<0.001
278278278278278278278278278
Ooijen0.234 **0.329 **0.0570.260 **0.415 **10.136 *0.338 **0.258 **
<0.001<0.0010.34<0.001<0.001 0.023<0.001<0.001
278278278278278278278278278
Alexeev0.238 **0.294 **0.123 *0.191 **0.1170.136 *10.460 **0.255 **
<0.001<0.0010.0410.0010.0520.023 <0.001<0.001
278278278278278278278278278
Cumurovic0.273 **0.319 **0.195 **0.521 **0.345 **0.338 **0.460 **10.786 **
<0.001<0.0010.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001 <0.001
278278278278278278278278278
Cumurovic0.244 **0.322 **0.171 **0.474 **0.255 **0.258 **0.255 **0.786 **1
w/o CRT<0.001<0.0010.004<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
278278278278278278278278278
Total fin. lit.0.538 **0.657 **0.359 **0.669 **0.637 **0.564 **0.490 **0.714 **0.661 **
(=all items)<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
278278278278278278278278278
** Correlation is significant at the 1% level, * correlation is significant at the 5% level.
Table 6. Correlations of the financial literature measures with question categories.
Table 6. Correlations of the financial literature measures with question categories.
InflationFin. MathFin. KnowledgeMathCRT
Burke0.939 **0.242 **0.252 **0.0240.202 **
<0.001<0.001<0.0010.6910.001
Lusardi0.257 **0.610 **0.515 **0.176 **0.201 **
<0.001<0.001<0.0010.0030.001
Anderson0.0830.335 **0.329 **0.255 **0.146*
0.168<0.001<0.001<0.0010.015
Bianchi0.128*0.474 **0.793 **0.313 **0.376 **
0.033<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
Gathergood0.155 **0.804 **0.327 **0.236 **0.302 **
0.009<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
Ooijen0.199 **0.678 **0.276 **0.204 **0.289 **
0.001<0.001<0.0010.001<0.001
Alexeev0.431 **.289 **0.307 **0.0320.479 **
<0.001<0.001<0.0010.59<0.001
Cumurovic0.285 **0.468 **0.699 **0.494 **0.832 **
<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
CL0.308 **0.577 **0.836 **0.434 **0.322 **
<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
Table 7. Correlations of the Cumurovic-Lusardi scale (CL) with the existing scales, with CRT, as well as with the total financial literacy score. The correlation with the total financial literacy score is high at 80.7% and is indicative of the high potential of the new scale.
Table 7. Correlations of the Cumurovic-Lusardi scale (CL) with the existing scales, with CRT, as well as with the total financial literacy score. The correlation with the total financial literacy score is high at 80.7% and is indicative of the high potential of the new scale.
CL Fin.Lit. CL Fin.Lit.
Burke0.305 ** Alexeev0.333 **
<0.001 <0.001
Lusardi0.757 ** Cumurovic0.713 **
<0.001 <0.001
Anderson0.192 ** Cumurovic0.863 **
0.001 w/o CRT<0.001
Bianchi0.527 **
<0.001
Gathergood0.392 ** CRT0.322 **
<0.001 <0.001
Ooijen0.354 ** Total0.807 **
<0.001 fin. literacy<0.001
** Correlation is significant at the 1% level, * correlation is significant at the 5% level.
Table 8. The six items of the CL scale and the percentage of correct answers for these items.
Table 8. The six items of the CL scale and the percentage of correct answers for these items.
ItemCorrect Answers
Buying a single share is safer than buying an equity fund. True or false?76.3%
You have 100EUR on your savings account with 2% interest per year. How much will you have after 5 years if you let your money grow?90.6%
Your savings account earns 1% interest per year, and inflation amounts to 2% per year. How much can you buy after one year with the money in your savings account?
More than today/The same as today/Less than today.81.3%
Which investment normally has the largest fluctuations?
Savings account/Fixed-interest securities/Shares87.8%
Which of the following statements best describes the main task of the stock market?
The stock market predicts stock profits./The stock market leads to an increase in stock prices./The stock market brings together potential buyers and sellers./None of the 3 statements.62.2%
Which of the following statements is correct?
Once you have invested in a mutual fund, you cannot withdraw the money in the first year /Investment funds can invest in several assets, e.g., shares and bonds /Investment funds pay a guaranteed return, which depends on the past performance /None of the 3 statements.61.9%
Table 9. Demographic characteristics and financial literacy as measured by the new Cumurovic-Lusardi scale (CL).
Table 9. Demographic characteristics and financial literacy as measured by the new Cumurovic-Lusardi scale (CL).
CL Fin.Lit. (%)Np-Value
male811722]*0.021 *
female74106
student811322]*0.007 **
non-student9017
working92322]*<0.001 **
non-working79117
economics student82292]*0.848
others81115
university degree801372]*0.039 *
no university degree74141
** Correlation is significant at the 1% level, * correlation is significant at the 5% level.
Table 10. Correlation of CL financial literacy with attitudes towards money (Fünfgeld and Wang 2009).
Table 10. Correlation of CL financial literacy with attitudes towards money (Fünfgeld and Wang 2009).
AnxietyInterest in FinanceIntuitive Decision MakingNeed for SavingNeed for Spending
Burke−0.303 **0.385 **−0.236 **0.010.013
<0.001<0.0010.0040.9080.87
Lusardi−0.222 **0.118−0.1180.067−0.074
0.0070.150.1510.4160.368
Anderson−0.198 *0.119−0.129−0.066−0.057
0.0160.1490.1160.4270.492
Bianchi−0.0180.15−0.115−0.037−0.062
0.8290.0690.1630.6510.45
Gathergood−0.1420.13−0.0290.0170.022
0.0850.1130.7290.8360.786
Ooijen−0.243 **0.233 **−0.14−0.167 *0.023
0.0030.0040.0890.0420.78
Alexeev−0.259 **0.071−0.127−0.037−0.07
0.0010.3890.1230.6530.396
Cumurovic−0.346 **0.218 **−0.113−0.0780.01
<0.0010.0070.1680.3430.901
CL−0.311 **0.234 **−0.194 *0.037−0.006
<0.0010.0040.0180.6580.945
** Correlation is significant at the 1% level, * correlation is significant at the 5% level.
Table 11. CL can explain stock market investment decisions and general attitudes towards long-term returns. Self-assessed financial knowledge has an explanatory power for the latter, while gender affects only the investment decisions. The effect is robust when controlling for other demographics and for attitudes towards money.
Table 11. CL can explain stock market investment decisions and general attitudes towards long-term returns. Self-assessed financial knowledge has an explanatory power for the latter, while gender affects only the investment decisions. The effect is robust when controlling for other demographics and for attitudes towards money.
Stock Investment (%)Stocks as a Good Long-Term Investment
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 4Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 4
CL0.24 *0.17 *0.18 *0.192 *0.29 **0.28 **0.31 **0.235 **
(2.89)(2.01)(2.15)(2.274)(3.66)(3.38)(3.83)(2.875)
Male 0.30 **0.28 **0.256 ** 0.090.01−0.019
(3.60)(3.11)(2.915) (1.13)(0.14)(−0.219)
CRT −0.06−0.023 −0.07−0.125
(−0.70)(−0.285) (−0.83)(−1.586)
Self-assessed 0.120.043 0.35 **0.249 *
financial knowledge (1.21)(0.416) (3.58)(2.484)
Anxiety 0.132 −0.08
(1.432) (−0.898)
Interest in finance 0.279 ** 0.155
(2.848) (1.631)
Intuitive decision 0.076 −0.084
making (0.885) (−1.014)
Need for saving −0.044 −0.077
(−0.542) (−0.976)
Need for spending −0.003 0.096
(−0.033) (1.263)
Other demographicsnoyesyesyesnoyesyesyes
N143143143149143143143149
R 2 5.5%17.0%18.1%21.2%8.6%17.5%24.9%26.2%
** Correlation is significant at the 1% level, * correlation is significant at the 5% level.
Table 12. External validity of the original financial literacy measures regarding stock investment in the stock market task.
Table 12. External validity of the original financial literacy measures regarding stock investment in the stock market task.
Stock Investment (%)
Burke0.169 *
(2.07)
Lusardi 0.165 *
(2.103)
Anderson −0.059
(−0.745)
Bianchi 0.146
(1.765)
Gathergood 0.085
(1.054)
Ooijen 0.261 **
(3.361)
Alexeev −0.05
(−0.568)
Cumunovic 0.263
(1.918)
Male0.306 **0.319 **0.355 **0.341 **0.352 **0.336 **0.359 **0.364 **
(3.543)(3.757)(4.194)(4.05)(4.168)(4.106)(4.21)(4.343)
CRT0.004−0.0010.032−0.029−0.001−0.0440.047−0.196
(0.055)(−0.013)(0.402)(−0.341)(−0.006)(−0.558)(0.536)(−1.406)
Self-assessed0.0490.0750.0740.0820.0670.0470.0640.055
financial knowledge(0.6)(0.923)(0.891)(0.993)(0.816)(0.591)(0.779)(0.668)
N149149149149149149149149
R 2 15.3%15.4%13.1%14.6%13.4%19.1%12.9%14.9%
** Correlation is significant at the 1% level, * correlation is significant at the 5% level.
Table 13. External validity of the original financial literacy measures regarding the view of stocks as a good long-term investment.
Table 13. External validity of the original financial literacy measures regarding the view of stocks as a good long-term investment.
Stocks as a Good Long-Term Investment
Burke0.279 **
(3.405)
Lusardi 0.176 *
(2.186)
Anderson 0.087
(1.077)
Bianchi 0.151
(1.783)
Gathergood 0.158
(1.934)
Ooijen 0.189*
(2.314)
Alexeev 0.108
(1.202)
Cumunovic 0.391 **
(2.817)
Male0.0310.0720.1050.0950.1060.0950.0940.125
(0.357)(0.825)(1.216)(1.101)(1.229)(1.113)(1.08)(1.466)
CRT−0.029−0.023−0.006−0.051−0.043−0.045−0.044−0.323 *
(−0.371)(−0.284)(−0.072)(−0.59)(−0.51)(−0.546)(−0.486)(−2.295)
Self-assessed0.239 **0.276 **0.252 **0.282 **0.269 **0.252 **0.267 **0.249 **
financial knowledge(2.909)(3.298)(2.945)(3.341)(3.208)(3.019)(3.163)(3.013)
N149149149149149149149149
R 2 14.7%10.8%8.6%9.8%10.2%11.2%8.8%12.7%
** Correlation is significant at the 1% level, * correlation is significant at the 5% level.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Rieger, M.O. How to Measure Financial Literacy? J. Risk Financial Manag. 2020, 13, 324. https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm13120324

AMA Style

Rieger MO. How to Measure Financial Literacy? Journal of Risk and Financial Management. 2020; 13(12):324. https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm13120324

Chicago/Turabian Style

Rieger, Marc Oliver. 2020. "How to Measure Financial Literacy?" Journal of Risk and Financial Management 13, no. 12: 324. https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm13120324

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop