Next Article in Journal
Enhancing Driving Ability in Older Adults through Health Exercises and Physical Activity: A Randomized Controlled Trial
Previous Article in Journal
Commonalities and Differences in the Experiences of Visible Minority Transnational Carer–Employees: A Qualitative Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Elucidating and Expanding the Restorative Theory Framework to Comprehend Influential Factors Supporting Ageing-in-Place: A Scoping Review

by
Anne Johanna Jacoba Grave
1,*,
Louis Neven
2 and
Masi Mohammadi
1,2
1
Smart Architectural Technologies, Department of the Built Environment, Eindhoven University of Technology, Groene Loper 3, 5612 AE Eindhoven, The Netherlands
2
Research Group Architecture in Health, HAN University of Applied Sciences, Ruitenberglaan 26, 6826 CC Arnhem, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20(18), 6801; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20186801
Submission received: 31 July 2023 / Revised: 13 September 2023 / Accepted: 19 September 2023 / Published: 21 September 2023

Abstract

:
Exposure to stress and attention fatigue resulting from changes in capabilities and residing in environments that do not align with individual needs can adversely impact older adults’ mental health and complicate ageing-in-place. Research into the psychological restoration process can help assist in alleviating these issues. Existing research on restoration perspectives has predominantly centred on university students and lacks comprehensive insights into older adults. Consequently, this study seeks to acquire a deeper understanding of the restorative theory framework within the context of ageing populations. We identified and analysed thirty-nine papers on the restoration process of older adults employing the scoping review method. Our findings indicate that adjustments to the general restorative theory framework are imperative for ageing populations. By incorporating additional features—such as being with and familiarity—the framework can more effectively support the development of age-inclusive neighbourhoods that enhance the mental health of the older population and facilitate healthy ageing-in-place. While more in-depth research is required on the restoration process of older adults, this research marks the initial in adapting the general framework to ageing populations. Furthermore, insight is given into how the adapted framework can contribute to help address the challenges of global ageing and support ageing-in-place.

1. Introduction

Due to scientific advances, improved living conditions and medical breakthroughs, our global population is rapidly ageing. In 2050, the global population of people aged 60 and older is expected to double to nearly 2.1 billion people [1]. An ageing population affects society in many ways, including public policies, health care and social services, as well as how to design our environment [2,3]. To this end, the World Health Organization launched its Ageing and health strategy [4], which aims to support the older population through “healthy ageing”. This policy also ties in with the wish of many older adults to age in place and grow old in a familiar living environment [3,5]. Unfortunately, for many older adults, ageing-in-place becomes difficult when a misfit between their living environment and their changing capabilities arises [6,7]. As a result of the ageing process, older adults can experience changing needs and capabilities, for example, declined physical strength, self-care abilities and increased risk of developing chronic conditions and mental health issues [8,9,10,11]. These changes in the individual let people experience their living environment differently. To continue living at home, older adults must adapt to cope with the environment [6].
The literature highlights three pathways that individuals follow when adapting to changes in their needs and capabilities in response to environmental shifts: (1) the stress perspective: this pathway focuses on mitigating heavy demands; (2) the coping perspective: this approach seeks to enhance the availability of resources for adaptation; and (3) the restoration perspective: within this context, the restoration perspective aims to provide opportunities to recover resources. This includes activities like attention restoration and psychophysiological stress recovery [12,13,14]. The restoration process is defined as “the process of recovering physiological, psychological and social resources that have become diminished in efforts to meet the demands of everyday life” [15] (p. 164). In the field of research on ageing-in-place, emphasis is often put on the first two pathways. Researchers frequently investigate strategies to reduce exposure to environmental stressors, such as air pollution and noise, promote physical activity and health through the design of therapeutic or biophilic environments and foster social cohesion (e.g., [10,16,17,18,19]). However, studies focusing on the third pathway—psychological restoration in the context of the older population—remain relatively sparse [17,20]. Existing research on the restoration perspective primarily focuses on university students. While older adults do require restoration of physiological, psychological and social resources [17,20], they are more susceptible to attention fatigue and life stressors, as evidenced by previous research [21,22]. This susceptibility is not solely attributed to age-related changes in capabilities; it is also linked to a heightened likelihood of encountering stressful life events. These events may include declines in socioeconomic status, alterations in social structures and shifts in family support dynamics [11,23,24]. If older adults in their environment do not have enough opportunities to restore resources, attention fatigue and chronic stress can arise, affecting daily functioning and mental health [23,25]. It is estimated that one in five older adults without dementia experience mental health problems [8,9,10,23]. Such conditions, notably anxiety and depression, pose a significant challenge to the pursuit of healthy ageing-in-place, thereby exerting additional strain on already stretched social services and healthcare systems [2,3].
Unfortunately, even though older adults need to restore resources, the restorative pathway has, until now, not been extensively studied with this older target group [17,26,27,28]. Many experimental studies are restricted to younger study populations, such as university students (e.g., [29,30,31,32,33]). However, exploring the restoration process and its applicability and effectiveness concerning ageing populations in current ageing societies has become imperative. Therefore, in this study, we want to review the literature on older adults’ restoration process, elucidate and expand the psychological restoration theory for ageing populations and comprehend influential factors supporting ageing-in-place. Expanding knowledge on restoration theory for older populations can raise awareness about how living environments influence older adults’ mental health and that stress and attention fatigue are potential health risks for ageing populations that hinder ageing-in-place [34,35,36]. The restoration of cognitive and affective resources needs to be considered when discussing holistic and integrative approaches for developing healthy age-inclusive neighbourhoods that suit the capabilities and needs of older adults, including their need for psychological restoration. Furthermore, knowledge about the third pathway can complement the available knowledge of the other two pathways, letting us better understand older adults’ person–environment adaptation process and improving ageing-in-place strategies in policy and design.
A scoping literature review was performed to gain an overview of the existing knowledge about the restorative process for ageing populations and learn more about the influential factors of the restoration pathway. Compared with a systematic review, in a scoping review, qualitative and quantitative studies with a wide variety of methods from different research fields can be included in the review [37,38]. The scoping review method allows to review emerging studies from various fields, using varied study designs in this developing field.

The General Theory Framework of Psychological Restoration

To expand the psychological restoration theory in the context of older populations, we first need to shed light on the current state of knowledge in the field. The restoration pathway describes the process of psychological restoration: “the experience of a psychological and/or physiological recovery process that is triggered by particular environments and environmental configurations” [39] (p. 58). In the environmental psychology literature, this process is explained by two major theories, namely the Attention Restoration Theory (ART) [40] and the Stress Reduction Theory (SRT) [41,42]. Both theories propose that specifically designed environments can trigger cognitive and affective processes, allowing psychological, physiological and social resources to be restored [42,43]. The ART focuses on the capacity to direct attention, a more cognitive process [43], and the SRT focuses on reducing stress and negative moods, a more affective process [42]. Despite these differences, both theories are often used simultaneously to research the mental health effects of restorative environments [44].
Hartig [14] set up a general framework for both restorative theories (Table 1). This framework shows the resources that come into play, get depleted and need restoration, as well as which features of a person–environment transaction can permit and promote the restoration process and the outcomes of the restoration process [14]. Currently, this framework is often deployed in studies with younger target groups like university students (e.g., [29,30,31,32,33]). In this scoping review, we want to elucidate the current framework and explore the applicability and effectiveness of the framework concerning ageing populations. In addition, we focus on studying the features of person–environment transactions of the current general restoration framework that permit and promote restoration (Table 1). These features are environmental requirements of the restoration process through which the depleted resource(s) can be restored. We examine if these features are also relevant for older populations and if additional features are needed to fully describe the restoration process of older populations.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Design

The scoping review was carried out according to the five-step approach defined by Arksey and O’Malley [37] and adjusted by Levac [38]. The first step, identifying the research question, is presented in the introduction. The four sequential steps are described below. All authors discussed procedures to ensure consistent search methodology, and the PRISMA-ScR checklist was followed to ensure clarity of reporting [45].

2.2. Study Identification

A comprehensive search of the literature was conducted in January 2023 to answer the research question: “To what extent is the current framework of psychological restoration theory applicable to older adults, and how do the various elements of this framework contribute to supporting the concept of ageing-in-place?”. The databases searched to identify studies were Scopus, Pubmed, Google Scholar and Web of Science. Search terms were arranged according to the two key themes of the research question: the psychological restoration process and older populations (Table 2). The terms were used to make several search strings. Each string used at least one of the key themes’ search terms, for example: (“psychological restoration”) AND (“age differences” AND “life course”). Furthermore, we tried to find additional papers, book chapters or conference papers by scanning publication lists of well-known authors to prevent publication bias. Additionally, we looked at the backlog of essential journals in the field like the Journal of Environmental Psychology, Environment and Behavior and the International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health.

2.3. Screening and Study Selection

In total, 1083 records were identified by the first author (Figure 1). After removing duplicates, 979 papers were nominated for title selection. First, we removed non-English titles and nonoriginal works, not-peer-reviewed works, theses, and reports, keeping 698 titles for further selection. Hereafter, iterative selection cycles were performed with all three authors. In the first selection cycle, titles were selected by the first author based on two inclusion criteria: older adults and psychological restoration. Doubtful cases were discussed among all three authors. Hereafter, the abstract selection was performed. At the start of the abstract selection, the first author used a random number generator to select 25 titles. Then, inclusion/exclusion decisions were discussed and agreed upon among all three authors. After that, abstracts were selected based on two inclusion criteria: (1) The sample included older adults of 60+ years. Although in gerontology and elsewhere, there are debates about which age constitutes the start of old age, we chose the age of 60, as in several countries people start retiring at this age (for example, in China) [46]. Especially for women, it is a common retirement age worldwide, for example, in Austria, Chilli and Poland [46]. Retirement is a relevant marker of old age for this paper, as the roles people play in society and the daily activities of people are markedly different after retirement. (2) Psychological restoration needed to be discussed following the ART or SRT theory following the general restorative theory framework [14]. Therefore, other forms of restoration were excluded from the study (e.g., building, dental and nature area restoration). Eventually, 55 papers were included for full paper analyses. During the full paper analyses, participant samples were examined by the first author. Papers where all participants were 60 years or older or papers that explicitly looked at age differences within a participant sample, for example, comparing older adults with younger adults or teenagers, were included in the study. Eventually, 39 papers were included in the review (Figure 1).

2.4. Data Charting

Following the scoping literature review method [37,38], the next step was setting up a data charting table (Appendix A). Papers were grouped according to the data charting table: author, year, country, research type, theory background, research methods, psychological restoration measures, other measures, participant number and age, other sample characteristics and type of environment. These data were used for descriptive and comparative paper analyses [37,48].

2.5. Collation, Summarising and Analysis

After descriptive and comparative paper analyses using the data charting table, the next step was to upload all the selected papers in Atlas.ti for qualitative content analyses. As stated by Smit and Sherman [49], a scoping literature review is a utilised form of qualitative research and Atlas.ti is a suitable application to conduct the paper analyses in a structured way. During the analysis, 105 codes emerged. The codes were grouped following the research questions into three theme groups: (1) features of person–environment transactions that permit the restoration process of older adults, (2) features of person–environment transactions that promote the restoration process of older adults, and (3) contextualising data (e.g., type of environment, demographic data and research limitations). Finally, the code groups were analysed, and the results are presented in the next section.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Results

In this scoping review, thirty-nine peer-reviewed papers were included and analysed to elucidate and expand the restorative theory framework and comprehend influential factors that support ageing-in-place. First, the descriptive results (e.g., methods used and participant groups included) are presented to indicate the quality of the studies included in this review.
The thirty-nine peer-reviewed papers included in this study were published in twenty-six journals from various fields, like gerontology, design, landscape research and environmental psychology. These results indicate the broad distribution of knowledge about the psychological restoration process and old age in various research fields. The scoping review method allowed us to bring this scattered research together, although overall numbers remained small. This scoping review found only one paper published before 2004, written by Travis and McAuley [50] (Figure 2). From 2004 onwards, multiple authors started publishing studies focussing on the restorative experience of older adults, and a slow increase in publications over the years can be seen, indicating a growing interest in the psychological restoration process of older adults. This could possibly be related to the rising pressure of ageing populations on healthcare systems and societies and the growing need to mitigate these problems.
The majority of the analysed papers were empirical studies (N = 36). The remaining three studies of the sample were literature reviews. The topic of psychological restoration for older adults is developing, which is reflected in the wide variety of methods used (Appendix A). The studies varied between using qualitative (N = 7), quantitative (N = 25) and mixed methods (N = 7) to measure psychological restoration. Which methods were deployed depended on whether authors followed the ART or SRT and if they were more qualitative or quantitatively oriented. Most of the studies were adherent to the ART (N = 21). These studies often used attention tests (e.g., Digit Span Test) to measure restoration or validated questionnaires like the Perceived Restoration Scale (PRS) and the Restoration Outcome Scale (ROS). If the authors were qualitative-oriented, they often analysed interview or spatial data using the four ART features (extent, fascination, being away and compatibility). Studies adherent to the SRT (N = 7) looked at changes in stress levels using self-rated stress scales or measuring physiological characteristics (e.g., blood pressure and heart rate). Eleven studies used methods from both theories. As a result of these varied methods, there are also variations in the presented results. Sometimes, authors present specific features permitting or promoting psychological restoration. Other studies present restorative experiences or design solutions. Because of these variations, the results of different studies cannot always be compared easily. We took these differences between studies into account during our analyses and reported on them further in the following sections.
Although there are many differences between the methods used in the analysed studies, there is also one apparent similarity: the participant samples are predominantly comprised of healthy, relatively young (Mage = 70.9 years) individuals living independently in the community. Often, participants needed to pass a Mini-Mental State Exam and needed to be able to walk without a walking aid. Only five studies included more vulnerable older adults, including people living in an institution, sitting in a wheelchair, having dementia or recovering from a hip fracture (e.g., [50,51,52]). On the one hand, it is logical for researchers to focus on younger and fit older adults so that there are not too many differences in the study participants. On the other hand, the studies only show results from one specific group, not representing the diversity present in the older population.
The last noteworthy observation about the analysed studies is the type of environments researched. Most studies focused on traditional restorative environments like forests and urban parks [20]. However, these environments are not always easily accessible to the older population. Only seven of the thirty-nine studies researched more accessible restorative urban environments close to older adults’ homes, like streets and neighbourhood open spaces (e.g., [20,51,53,54,55]).
This review delves into a relatively underexplored area within the restoration literature: older adults’ restoration process. Despite the fact that older adults have not yet received extensive research attention, we see a slight rise in publications on the topic, indicating a growing interest in the research field. However, it is crucial to acknowledge that research on the psychological restoration of older adults is still in its developmental stages. Studies are dispersed across various disciplines. There are ongoing discussions about methodological issues, a unilateral participant group’s involvement, and there is still little variety in the type of restorative environment studied. More attention should be paid to these issues in future research to further develop the research field of psychological restoration for older populations.

3.2. The General Restorative Theory Framework

After the descriptive analysis, we explored if the ten environmental features named in the general restorative theory framework (Table 1) that permit and promote restoration are applicable and effective in the context of an ageing population.

3.2.1. Features That Permit Restoration for Older Populations

The general theory framework proposes three environmental features that permit restoration [15,40,41] (Table 1). These features allow an environment to be free of demands that cause the need for restoration. The SRT states that the absence of threat is an essential feature of a restorative environment [42]; one must feel safe before the restoration process can occur. In different qualitative studies, for example, by Jansen [56] and Finlay and colleagues [55], the absence of threat is often described by older adults as an important factor when talking about their restorative experiences (e.g., [55,56]). If older adults feel safe and comfortable in an environment, there is a higher chance that psychological restoration can occur [57,58]. Among others, Qiu and colleagues [59] and Li and colleagues [60] found in their quantitative studies evidence that if spaces can feel unsafe, for example, because of high-density vegetation, feelings of enclosure, insufficient light or too much traffic, this may increase stress and mental fatigue for older adults due to feelings of insecurity [55,59,60,61]. Furthermore, Cassarino and colleagues [62] and Lu and colleagues [63] found evidence in their experimental studies that a good balance between prospect and refuge in an environment increases a sense of security [62,63].
ART proposes that experiences of being away and compatibility permit restoration [43] (Table 1). These features include the ability to break with routines, get away from daily life and the ability of the environment to match a person’s capabilities to not further tax “already” depleted resources [14,15,44]. The feature compatibility is described in the literature as an essential feature of older adults’ restoration process [22,52,64,65] (Table 3). Scopelliti and Giuliani [64] state, for example, “a general result claims for the importance of perceived compatibility between elderly persons’ needs and environmental characteristics; when lacking, the consequence is often a dramatic decrease in the restorative potential of everyday settings” (p. 223). The importance of compatibility for older adults’ restoration process is also evident in other studies, for example, in Fumagalli and colleagues’ [22] analysis of older adults’ descriptions of restorative experiences and in the experimental study of Ottosson and Grahn [52] measuring changes in attention levels in nursing home residents. The importance of the factor compatibility is linked to older adults changing capabilities, which increases the chance of a person–environment misfit due to a lack of compatibility between the person and the environment [17,22]. A lack of compatibility decreases the restorative potential of environments for older adults. Especially in urban, manmade environments, the lack of compatibility is often the main factor negatively affecting perceived restoration [64]. A factor that can negatively impact compatibility is the accessibility of an environment. No restoration can occur if an older adult cannot access or explore the environment [56,66]. Authors like Moore [61] and Marques and colleagues [66] suggest improving accessibility and, thereby, restoration for older adults by designing restorative environments close to the homes of older adults that have toilets, seats, smooth pavement, not much traffic, tree cover, shade and water features [22,61,66].
Opinions about the importance of the permitting feature being away from older populations are divided. In different qualitative studies, older adults describe feelings of being away when describing their restorative experiences [22,55,57,67]. Furthermore, in their experimental studies, Rosenbaum and colleagues [53,54] found that environments like senior cafés or senior centres can offer escape experiences for older adults as a home away from home. However, other studies show that being away was significantly less important for older adults compared with younger age groups [26,65]. A possible explanation for this is that older adults may form strong attachments to specific, familiar environments in which they feel safe and comfortable and have strong memories connected. Therefore, they have less need to distance themselves from these environments to gain restoration [26]. Another explanation could be that older adults have less need to distance themselves from ordinary aspects of life to gain restoration [65]. More research is needed to obtain better insight into this feature’s role in permitting psychological restoration for older populations.
Table 3. Features that permit and promote the restoration process of older populations according to the general framework and additional features that can support the restoration process of ageing populations.
Table 3. Features that permit and promote the restoration process of older populations according to the general framework and additional features that can support the restoration process of ageing populations.
TheoryFeatures of P-E Transactions That Permit Restoration for the Older PopulationFeatures of P-E Transactions That Promote Restoration for the Older Population
Stress Reduction Theory (SRT)Absence of uncontrollable threat
Ijerph 20 06801 i011
An essential feature of older adults’ restoration process. If older adults feel unsafe in an environment, restoration cannot occur [51,54,55,58,59,60,61,63,64,65,66].Perception of natural contents
Ijerph 20 06801 i012
Scenes with water support feelings of calmness and relaxation due to sensory stimulation, also for the older population [22,28,55,59,61,63,64,68].
Visual stimulus attributes
Ijerph 20 06801 i013
Deflected vistas can enhance curiosity and motivate older adults to go outdoors and explore their everyday environments. There needs to be a right balance of prospect and refuge. Ability to see the environment without feeling exposed [26,50,59,60,61,62,63,65,69].
Moderate levels of complexity
Ijerph 20 06801 i014
Not named in the reviewed literature in the context of the older population.
Gross structure
Ijerph 20 06801 i015
Not named in the reviewed literature in the context of the older population.
Attention Restoration Theory (ART)Being away
Ijerph 20 06801 i016
Doubt arises in which way the component being-away is essential for older adults’ restoration process. Escape from every day routines is challenged by the need for social interaction [17,22,26,27,54,55,56,57,58,59,61,67,70,71]. Fascination
Ijerph 20 06801 i017
An important feature of older adults’ restoration process. Encourages older adults to explore their surroundings. Authors propose that fascination for older adults is not stimulated by ‘newness’ but by experiencing the familiar in a new way [17,20,22,26,52,53,54,57,58,59,61,64,65,69,70,72,73].
Compatibility
Ijerph 20 06801 i018
Due to changing capabilities, this feature becomes essential for the restoration process of the older population. Lack of compatibility between the person and the environment dramatically decreases the restorative potential for older adults. Aspects of accessibility play an important role in this feature [17,20,22,26,50,52,54,55,57,58,59,61,64,65,69,70,74]. ExtentIjerph 20 06801 i019Linked to the presence of (childhood) memories and sensory stimulation. Not named as a condition that will change for the older population [17,20,22,26,57,58,59,61,64,65].
Compatibility
Ijerph 20 06801 i020
An essential feature for the restoration process of older adults. Although their capabilities change, the environment should enable their life activities [22,54,55,58,59,61,64,65,69,70].
Outside conventional theoriesBeing with
Ijerph 20 06801 i021
Being-with others is suggested as an essential feature of the restoration process of older adults; however, individual needs need to be taken into account [17,20,22,50,51,52,54,55,56,59,60,61,63,64,65,67,71,75,76,77]. Familiarity
Ijerph 20 06801 i022
Familiarity can be an additional feature of older adults’ restoration process. Familiar environments can enhance feelings of safety and comfort, promoting the restoration process. A balance between new and familiar elements is important to prevent over or under-stimulation [20,52,55,57,62,64,65,66,74].

3.2.2. Features That Promote Restoration for Older Populations

In addition to features that can permit restoration, the general restorative theory framework also proposes seven features that promote the restoration process [15,40,41] (Table 1). These features can draw a person’s thoughts away from demand and attract and hold their attention, prolonging the restorative process [15,42,43]. Most of these features are also described when researching the restoration process of the older population.
The SRT proposes four features, of which gross structure and moderate complexity are not or hardly discussed in the context of older populations. However, this does not mean that these are not essential features, only that they have not been researched in the currently reviewed literature. The features presence of water and deflected vista are studied in the context of older populations and are confirmed as factors that play a role in their psychological restoration process (e.g., [59,75]) (Table 3). Finlay and colleagues found in their qualitative study that water enhances feelings of being connected to nature and is linked to feelings of tranquillity, calmness and spirituality [22,55]. Furthermore, water features such as fountains can enhance curiosity and stimulate the senses (e.g., sounds), promoting restoration for older populations, as found in the empirical studies of Moore [61] and Fumagalli and colleagues [22]. Similarly, Roe and Roe found in their literature review that feature-deflected vistas can enhance curiosity and motivate older adults to go outdoors and explore their everyday environments [17]. Environments with no open exposed view but deflected vistas that offer shelter and provoke exploration of the environment are the most optimal for the psychological restoration of older adults [52,59,63]. However, even though both features are important for the restoration process of older adults, no differences with other age groups are described in the analysed literature.
The ART theory proposes three features of person–environment transactions that promote restoration (Table 1). The feature extent is not named in the analysed literature as a feature that will impact the restoration process of older adults differently from other age groups. Some studies link this factor with the presence of (childhood) memories [26,50,57]. Recalling images and emotions that belong to carefree and cheerful moments from life could potentially affect feelings of connectedness (extent) and the restoration process for older adults [22]; however, more research is needed to confirm these assumptions. Furthermore, the literature names the feature fascination as an extra important feature for older adults [22,65,73] (Table 3). Jang and colleagues [73] found in their quantitative study that older adults of 60+ years focused on feature fascination (e.g., exploring the surroundings) when judging restorative environments, while younger people focused more on legality and coherence (e.g., structure and orientation), which are part of the feature extent. Others like Liao and colleagues [57] found significant evidence that elements like shade, biodiversity, colourful flowers and vegetation density can enhance fascination in an environment for older populations [17,57,61]. Lastly, feature compatibility is named as a feature for permitting and promoting restoration. In addition to allowing restoration, an environment must enable people to carry out activities they want to perform and not limit their abilities, promoting restorative experiences [14]. This is also true for ageing populations; the environment must enable older adults to carry out activities they want to perform that can offer restoration, like exploring the environment, viewing scenery and having fun [64,65]. However, whether this differs from other age groups remains to be studied.
Thus, after a thorough analysis of the literature, it becomes evident that adjustments are needed to adapt the general restorative theory framework to suit ageing populations’ needs. As seen in Table 3, most but not all features of the general restoration theory framework play a role in the context of the older population. Based on the reviewed literature, we suggest prioritising the permitting features. The features safety and compatibility become essential for older adults’ restoration process. The promoting features proposed by the general framework seem similar for older adults compared to other age groups, with the exception of feature fascination, which holds greater significance in older adults’ restoration process compared with other promoting elements outlined in the general framework. Although more research into these permitting and promoting features is needed, these results show that adjustments to the general restorative theory framework are needed to better suit ageing populations’ needs and support healthy ageing-in-place.

3.3. Additional Features for the General Restorative Theory Framework

In addition to the general restorative theory framework’s current features, we propose to extend the current framework with two additional features. The first suggestion for an addition is the feature being with others (Table 3). Chen and Yuan [75] found in their experimental study that social contact mediated restoration for older adults. Also, Lu and colleagues [63] indicated that companionship significantly impacted the Restoration Outcome Scale. This aligns with others who found that social interaction increases restorative potential, especially in urban environments [59,64,71,78]. A possible explanation is that being together enhances the features of safety and compatibility, increasing the chance that restoration can take place. Another explanation could be that being with others enhances feelings of being part of the world, distancing older adults from their more socially isolated home situation and promoting the feature of being away [17,54]. However, these explanations are not yet confirmed. Scopelliti and Giuliani [65] found in their mixed-method study interesting evidence that being with somebody significantly impacted the restoration process of teenagers and adults but not for older participants. With these varying results, it is essential to remember that social interaction does not always positively affect restoring resources. It depends on the situation and the person. For example, feelings of loneliness, as well as crowding, can negatively influence the restoration process [22,60]. The effects of the social context on restorative processes may vary for different individuals. For example, spending time with family could offer restoration, but for others, it can be mentally and physically draining [56]. Furthermore, the impact of the social component could also be different in natural environments compared with urban environments because social obligations can negatively affect the restoration process [64,65]. More research is needed on how individual differences and social contexts affect older adults’ restoration processes.
Secondly, we suggest adding the feature familiarity to the general restorative theory framework when using it in the context of ageing populations (e.g., [62,74]) (Table 3). Berto [74] found in an experimental study a correlation between restoration and familiarity for ageing populations but not for younger age groups. Ottosson and Grahn [52] found in their experiment that older adults have a greater need for familiar surroundings. Familiar environments can enhance feelings of safety and comfort and thereby promote the restoration process [52,55]. Furthermore, Roe and Roe [17] propose that fascination for older adults is not determined by the “newness” of an environment but by experiencing the familiar in a new way. However, a balance must be established between familiar and new elements [52], complementing the feature fascination. Too many new things can create feelings of insecurity, negatively influencing the restoration process, and too many familiar things can undermine the factor fascination and cause understimulation [52,55]. Older adults are more sensitive to this balance than younger people, and this needs to be considered when further studying feature familiarity and its effect on older adults’ restoration processes.
In conclusion, when elucidating the general restoration theory framework in the context of ageing populations, it becomes evident that adjustments and enhancements are needed to adapt the general restorative theory framework to better suit the needs of ageing populations. Based on the reviewed literature, we suggest prioritising safety and compatibility alongside promoting fascination because they hold greater significance in older adults’ restoration processes compared with other elements outlined in the general framework. Moreover, we propose to expand the framework with the features of being with and familiarity; adding these features could improve the framework’s applicability to ageing populations. Although more research is needed, these results must be considered when using the restorative theory framework to develop older adults’ restorative environments, which can improve older adults’ mental health and support ageing-in-place.

4. Discussion

The results presented here provide insight into the degree to which the current overarching framework of restorative theory contributes to the understanding of older adults’ psychological restoration process. The inclusion of only thirty-nine studies in this scoping review may seem relatively modest, especially considering that we cast a wide net in terms of the time period and did not select on methodological approaches. When analysing the publication dates, it becomes clear that the topic of psychological restoration among older adults has only recently gained increased attention, and interest in this research area is gradually gaining momentum. It is evident that the research field is in a state of continuous development, as demonstrated by the wide range of literature and methods employed to assess restoration. Furthermore, while attention tests and physiological measures are commonly employed, their suitability for measuring restoration in older populations is subject to debate. For instance, the measurement of heart rate variability presents challenges due to distinct patterns observed in older adults compared with their younger counterparts [79]. Moreover, the study of Cassarino and colleagues [62] found that older adults consistently exhibit lower performance on attention tasks than younger participants, thereby influencing restoration outcome levels. Consequently, we recommend that future studies adopt a mixed-method approach, integrating physiological and psychological measures with participants’ verbal accounts [29]. In a developing research area, the application of diverse methods can offer a range of valuable insights. Nonetheless, in order to improve the comparability of results, it is advisable to consider standardising methods in further studies, particularly in the context of studying psychological restoration in the older population (Table 4).
Another notable methodological issue in the reviewed studies is the homogeneous participant groups commonly employed. Most studies tend to treat older adults as a monolithic entity despite the evident diversity within ageing populations. In reality, older populations encompass a broad spectrum of characteristics and experiences, making it crucial to delve deeper into these variations and how they influence the restoration process. By gaining a nuanced understanding of these distinctions, we can design restorative environments that suit this target group’s different needs and capabilities [75]. To date, it is, for example, unclear if the restoration mechanism works similarly for older adults with cognitive impairments, such as dementia, or those facing physical constraints [57]. Additionally, factors like socioeconomic status, living situation or cultural differences could also be interesting for future research. Only three studies in the review touched on these topics, and while no significant results have emerged thus far, there are indications that these factors could potentially impact the restoration process [67,73]. Therefore, it is imperative to delve deeper into these individual differences and their effects on the restoration process. This knowledge would enable us to provide valuable guidance to researchers, policymakers and designers, allowing them to create environments that can proactively anticipate and adapt to the diverse personal needs of older adults [80] (Table 4).
As mentioned before, this paper set out to elucidate the current restorative theory framework and expand it with two additional features to better suit the restoration process of the older population. The literature showed that the features of person–environment transactions that permit restoration are of extra importance for the restoration processes of older adults, especially the features absence of threat and compatibility. This is connected to older adults changing capabilities related to the ageing process that increase the chance of a person–environment misfit due to a lack of compatibility between the person and the environment [17,22]. Constantly adapting their behaviour and activities to fit their environment can cost much attention and provoke stress. Therefore, enhancing the compatibility between older populations and their environments can offer psychological restoration benefits [22,52,64,65]. We suggest for future research that these features should be closely monitored, as they can influence the restorative experiences of older adults. Furthermore, similarities could be examined between the restoration process and the person–environment fit model of Lawton [7,81,82], where comparable person–environment transactions are important in the design of environments for older adults. Knowledge from this model may contribute to developing theories for the psychological restoration process for older populations.
Furthermore, our comprehensive literature review shows the significance of fascination as an important feature in promoting restoration among ageing populations. Fascinating elements can encourage curiosity and exploration of the living environment. However, a balance between fascinating elements is crucial to the older population. Overstimulation, feelings of unsafety and discomfort can have a negative effect on the restoration processes of older adults [52,55]. Considering this, we suggest the inclusion of the feature of familiarity in the general restoration theory framework for older populations (e.g., [63,72]). Familiar environments can lead to a sense of safety and comfort, and experiencing familiar environments in a new way can still encourage curiosity and exploration, promoting the restoration process [17,52,55]. However, it should be noted that the precise impact of the feature familiarity on older adults’ restoration processes needs further research. Theories about lifespan developmental approaches can potentially help with further developing the restorative theory framework [83,84]. These developmental theories explore how earlier life experiences shape people’s lives and reactions as they age. Such theories could potentially elucidate the importance of factors like “familiarity” and shed light on differences in the significance of other elements, such as “being away”.
Lastly, we propose the inclusion of the “being with” feature within the general restorative theory framework. Older adults experiencing stress or attention deficits often tend to isolate themselves, making them more susceptible to stress and initiating a downward spiral that negatively impacts their mental health [23]. Being with people could be essential to reduce loneliness and promote restoration for older populations [17,75]. However, too many (unknown) people can negatively influence the restoration process [56,65]. Therefore, future research should delve deeper into the impact of other people’s presence on the restoration process among older adults (Table 4). Theories about social engagement can potentially give insights into lifespan differences in the social needs of older populations compared with younger groups and could potentially expand the current restoration theory [84,85].
In conclusion, the results of our comprehensive literature review show that not all features of the general restorative theory framework are equally important for older populations. Safety, compatibility and fascination emerge as particularly important for this target group. Furthermore, based on the literature analysis, we propose the inclusion of two additional features to expand the framework: “familiarity” and “being with”. These factors could prove to be crucial determinants in the restoration process of older adults. These findings must be included in future research studies and when developing restorative environments for older populations. By putting more emphasis on these features (safety, compatibility, fascination, familiarity and being with) when developing restorative environments, environments can be created that match older adults’ needs and capabilities regarding restoring resources. Such an approach not only benefits their mental health but also supports the concept of ageing-in-place.

Strengths and Limitations of the Study

The scoping review methodology exhibits a notable strength in its ability to provide a comprehensive overview of the literature. This makes it particularly well-suited for synthesising research from diverse fields with varying research methods but centred on a common theme [38]. To bolster the credibility of this interdisciplinary study, an extensive search strategy was deployed across multiple databases without imposing date restrictions, and the study identification and selection process underwent a rigorous double review.
As previously highlighted, this review delves into a relatively underexplored area within the literature pertaining to the restorative pathway. Specifically, it addresses the restoration process among older adults, a demographic that has not received extensive research attention. Only thirty-nine studies were identified that examined the restoration process in the context of older populations. Future research should place increased emphasis on this demographic, particularly because disparities in the effectiveness of the theoretical framework were discerned between older adults and younger age groups. Furthermore, when older adults were included in studies, they predominantly comprised healthy individuals from developed nations. Future research endeavours could benefit from a more nuanced examination of individual differences and capabilities within this demographic. From the existing literature, it remains inconclusive whether the factors suggested to enhance the restoration process for older adults are similarly effective for other subgroups of older individuals, such as those dealing with dementia or physical health issues. Consequently, further research is essential to assess the generalizability of the results.
Moreover, it is worth acknowledging that this literature search was conducted exclusively in English, potentially resulting in the omission of evidence from developing countries. Another aspect for consideration is that, due to the scoping review method, no selection was made based on the research methods employed. Consequently, significant methodological variations are evident among the included studies. To enhance the comparability of findings in future research, a standardisation of methods could prove beneficial in the exploration of psychological restoration among older populations.
Lastly, it is vital to recognise that no limitations were imposed on the types of environments investigated. Nevertheless, it is noteworthy that a majority of the studies were conducted in environments that might pose challenges for older adults to access, such as forests and nature parks [10]. To design restorative spaces conducive to enhancing the mental well-being of older adults and supporting ageing-in-place, future studies could explore the restorative characteristics of (semi) public spaces in proximity to the residences of older individuals [20,33,35]. Furthermore, it has become evident that not all restorative factors hold equal significance in every environmental setting. Research by Scopelliti and Giuliani [64] revealed that compatibility and fascination were pivotal in coastal environments, while for urban parks, compatibility and being away were of greater importance to older populations. Consequently, it is conceivable that restoration may manifest through distinct processes in different settings, with varying restorative features playing pivotal roles [64]. Consequently, future research should pay heed to alterations in the restoration process owing to differing individual needs and capabilities, including those stemming from ageing, and align these with the type and design of restorative environments to optimise psychological restoration processes for older populations.
In summary, our understanding of older adults’ psychological restoration processes is steadily expanding, although further research remains imperative. Preliminary findings indicate the necessity for adaptations to the general theoretical framework underpinning restorative environments when considering older populations. This accumulating knowledge can be harnessed to inform the development of restorative environments that promote the mental well-being of older adults and facilitate ageing-in-place.

5. Conclusions

This scoping review encompasses a wide range of studies, aiming to provide a comprehensive and detailed overview of the existing knowledge regarding ageing populations’ restoration processes. Upon thorough analysis of the literature, it becomes evident that adjustments and enhancements are needed to adapt the general restorative theory framework to suit the needs of ageing populations better. Based on the reviewed literature, we suggest prioritising safety and compatibility alongside promoting fascination because they hold greater significance in older adults’ restoration processes compared with other elements outlined in the general framework. Moreover, we propose to expand the framework to include concepts like “being with” and “familiarity” to better align with the psychological restoration processes of older populations. These findings should be taken into account when designing restorative environments tailored to older adults. However, it is crucial to acknowledge that research on the psychological restoration of older adults is still in its developmental stages. Studies are dispersed across various disciplines, and there are ongoing discussions about methodological issues. Further research is imperative to fine-tune the general framework to older populations, especially considering the rapidly growing ageing demographic and its impact on healthcare systems and societies.
In conclusion, we anticipate that the insights furnished by this review will offer valuable support to researchers, policymakers and designers as they strive to create age-inclusive neighbourhoods that align with the capabilities and requirements of older adults, including their need for psychological restoration. The aim is to design environments that not only appeal to older adults but also enable them to restore their mental resources. This awareness campaign underscores the profound influence of our living environments on our mental well-being, highlighting the potential health risks posed by stress and attention fatigue in ageing populations, which can hinder the feasibility of ageing-in-place. The insights garnered from this review can serve as a guiding framework to promote the mental health of older individuals and foster healthy ageing-in-place.

Author Contributions

A.J.J.G., conceptualisation, methodology, formal analysis, visualisation, investigation and writing—original draft; L.N., conceptualisation, methodology, writing—review and editing and supervision; M.M., conceptualisation, writing—review and editing and supervision. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the housing association Wooninc. and the care organisation Oktober, located in The Netherlands.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted with the permission of the Ethical Review Board of the Eindhoven University of Technology, reference number ERB2022BE3. No humans or animals were involved in the study.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created in this literature review. Only existing sources were used for analyses; see references. Data sharing does not apply to this article.

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank the reviewers for their thorough review of our manuscript and for the excellent suggestions that we received.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the study’s design, in the collection, analyses or interpretation of data, in the writing of the manuscript or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Table A1. Data charting table. Abbreviations: PRS = Perceived Restoration Scale; PRSS = Perceived Restorativeness Soundscape Scale; ROS = Restoration Outcome Scale; RCS = Restorative Components Scale; SART = Sustained Attention Response Task; ANT-test = Attention Restoration Improvement Test; RAA = Restorative Activities Assessment; POMS = Profile of Mood states; PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect scales; BMI = Body Mass Index.
Table A1. Data charting table. Abbreviations: PRS = Perceived Restoration Scale; PRSS = Perceived Restorativeness Soundscape Scale; ROS = Restoration Outcome Scale; RCS = Restorative Components Scale; SART = Sustained Attention Response Task; ANT-test = Attention Restoration Improvement Test; RAA = Restorative Activities Assessment; POMS = Profile of Mood states; PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect scales; BMI = Body Mass Index.
AuthorCountryResearch Type and TheoryMethodMeasures
(Psychological Restoration Measures and Other Measures)
Participant Number and AgeOther Sample
Characteristics
Type of Environment Factors That
Permit Restoration for Older Adults
Factors That
Promote Restoration for Older Adults
Notes
Berto (2007) [74]ItalyEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
Lab study
Photo evaluation
Questionnaire
Italian PRS
Preference
Familiarity
N = 50
Aged between 62–93 years
M = 80 years
All living in a place for older adults. Half live in a natural setting, and half in an urban setting.Nature and urban environments
(Housing, industrial zone, city streets, hills, lakes)
CompatibilityDeflected vista
Familiarity
Experimental study comparing different age groups.
Boffi, Pola, Fumagalli, et al. (2021) [70]ItalyEmpirical study
Qualitative
ART
Lab study
Focus groups
Study 1
Descriptions of restorative environments
N = 23
Aged between 65–84 years
More femaleNature environment
(Urban community shared garden)
Compatibility
Being-away
FascinationNature experiences enhance active ageing. Interdisciplinary approach to codesign community gardens
Study 2
Analyses descriptions of restorative experiences
N = 25
Aged between 65–84 years
More female
Boffi, Grazia Pola, Fermani, et al. (2022) [34]ItalyEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
In situ study
Questionnaire
Italian PRS
Self-assessment Manikin
Activity list
N = 81
Aged 60+
(Compared within larger group N = 321)
Nature environment
(Urban community shared garden)
CompatibilityPost occupancy evaluation of virtual restorative garden with different age groups
Cassarino, Tuohy, Setti (2019) [62]IrelandEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
Lab study
Photo evaluation
Attention tasks
Questionnaire
Short PRS
SART
N = 75
Aged between 60–95 years
More males.
Healthy individuals. No cognitive deficits. Half live in a natural setting, and half in an urban setting. A total of 82% reported easy access to green space in their neighbourhood.
Nature and urban environments
(Various images are taken from the internet with no people and no water.)
FamiliarityExperimental study measuring attention restoration of older adults while viewing different scenes.
Chan, Qiu, Esposito, et al. (2021) [86]SingaporeEmpirical study
Quantitative
SRT
Lab study
Virtual reality
Physiological measures
Questionnaire
Cardiovascular activity
ECG
Self-reported stress
Positive affect
Nature connectedness
N = 26
M = 72.7 years
Nature and urban environment
(Virtual reality forest and streets)
Deflected vistaViewing nature scenes in VR can reduce stress in young and older adults.
Chen, Yuan (2020) [75]ChinaEmpirical study
Quantitative
SRT
Lab study
Photo evaluation
Questionnaire
Self-reported stress
Mental health (SF-36)
Blue space scale
Air quality index
Physical activity
Social contact
SES
N = 966
M = 69 years
Male/female was equally divided.
Low education levels.
Most were married.
Most of them retired.
Half were local residents.
Nature environment
(Blue spaces in the city)
Social context Study if blue space in the neighbourhood affects older adults’ mental health, a case study.
Elsadek, Shao, Liu (2021) [87]ChinaEmpirical study
Quantitative
SRT
Lab study
Photo evaluation
Physiological measures Brain activity (EEG)
Heart rate variability
Skin conductance
Alpha waves
Mood scales
N = 29
M = 82.9 years
Nature and urban environment
(Bamboo forest and urban scenes)
Experimental study if nature images impact older adults’ brain activity and autonomic nervous system.
Finlay, Franke, McKay, et al. (2015) [55]CanadaEmpirical study
Qualitative
ART
In situ study
Walk and Talk interviews
Analyses descriptions of restorative experiences using 4 ART components
Interpretation and interaction with the local neighbourhood context
Emotional responses
N = 27
Aged between 65–86 years
Leaving their home at least once a week. Able to walk 10 m (with mobility aid).
No sig. memory problems. Eight different self-identified racial and ethnic groups: Caucasian, aboriginal, Chinese, Southeast Asian, Japanese, Filipino, Dutch, and German.
Nature and urban environment
(Participants could indicate restorative spaces in their neighbourhood)
Safety
Being away
Compatibility
Social context
Ground Cover
Water
Familiarity
Compatibility
Qualitative study—Talking with older adults about how blue and green spaces impact their well-being.
Fumagalli, Fermani, Senes, et al. (2020) [22]ItalyLiterature review
Codesign
Mixed-method
ART
Lab study and real life (not in situ)
Focus groups
Analyses descriptions of restorative experiences using 4 ART componentsN = 23
Aged between 65–84 years
All participants were from the same neighbourhood.Nature environment
(Urban community shared garden)
Being-away
Compatibility
Social context
Deflected vista
Water
Extent (coherence)
Fascination
Compatibility
Co-design together with older adults a restorative garden, a case study.
Gamble, Howard, Howard (2014) [88]United StatesEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
Lab study
Photo evaluation
Attention tests
Questionnaire
ANT test
Backward digit span
PANAS
N = 30
Aged between 64–79
(Compared with young university students aged between 18–25 (N = 26))
All-in good health.
Mini-Mental State Examination was good.
Nature and urban environment
(Nature scenery of Nova Scotia, Ann Arbor, Detroit and Chicago)
Experimental study if viewing nature images improves attention in older adults.
Hawkins, Thirlaway, Backx, et al. (2011) [67]United KingdomEmpirical study
Quantitative
SRT
In situ study
Physiological measures
Questionnaire
Systolic and diastolic blood pressure
Perceived stress
Lung function
Body mass
BMI
Self-rated health
Physical activity level
Social activity level
Perceived social support
N = 94
Aged between 50–88 years
Members of various indoor and outdoor activity groups.
All were gardeners at private or allotment gardens.
Nature environment
(Community activity group garden)
Being away
Social context
Study if allotment gardening can reduce stress for older adults.
Husser, Roberto, Allen (2020) [26]United StatesEmpirical study
Mixed method
ART
Real-life study (not in situ)
Interviews
Analyses of descriptions of interview data using the four ART characteristics
Physical measures
Health and coping
Social support network
Quality of Life
N = 34
Aged between 71–91 years
All women lived in a small mountainous community.
More than half lived there for over 50 years.
Half lived alone.
24% lived below the poverty line.
Nature environment
(Rural area)
Being away
Compatibility
Social context
Extent
Fascination
Analysing interviews with rural living older woman and what influence nature has on their lives. Using grounded theory techniques. A connection is made between nature and coping with ageing challenges.
Jang, Jeong, Kim, et al. (2020) [73]KoreaEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
In situ study
Questionnaire
Korean PRS
PANAS
Plant cultivation activity levels
Satisfaction
Loyalty
N = 65
Aged 60+
(Compared within larger group N = 285)
Male/female was equally divided.
Educated.
Middle incomes.
Nature environment
(Shared healing garden)
FascinationExperimental study viewing images of healing gardens and testing if different age groups experienced attention restoration. Focus of the study on component fascination.
Jansen (2008) [76]United StatesEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
Real-life study (not in situ)
Questionnaire
Activity assessment
RAA
Attention function index
Geriatric depression scale
Self-rated health
N = 54
Aged between 65–87 years
Living independently in the community.
Most lived in a single home.
Half lived alone.
Half is married.
Nature and urban environments
(Participants could suggest their own restorative environments)
Social context Gathering an overview of restorative activities for community-dwelling older adults. Follow-up study.
Jansen (2005) [56]United StatesEmpirical study
Qualitative
ART
Real-life study (not in situ)
Interviews
Analyses of descriptions of interview data
Identify barriers to restorative activities
N = 30
Aged between 65–92 years.
Community dwelling.
Half married. A total of 77% lived alone. Average education 13,2 years. A total of 77% rated good/excellent health.
Nature and urban environments
(Participants could suggest their own restorative environments)
Safety
Being-away
Social context
Gathering an overview of community-dwelling older adults’ barriers to participating in restorative activities.
Jansen, von Sadovszky (2004) [58]United StatesEmpirical study
Qualitative
ART
Real-life study (not in situ)
Interviews
Analyses of descriptions of restorative experiencesN = 30
Aged between 65–92 years
Mostly women.
Community-dwelling. Half married. A total of 77% lived alone. Average education 13,2 years. A total of 77% rated good/ excellent health.
Nature and urban environments
(Participants could suggest their own restorative environments)
Safety
Being away
Compatibility
Social context
Extent
Fascination
Compatibility
Gathering an overview of restorative activities for community-dwelling older adults.
Jarosz (2022) [28]PolandEmpirical study
Qualitative
SRT
Lab study
Experience Sampling Method (telephone interviews)
Self-reported stress
Self-reported Enjoyment
N = 200
Aged 65+ years
Non-institutionalisedNature and urban environment Presence of waterThe study used a new method—experience sampling method—to collect user experiences. Results show that people in green or blue spaces had more enjoyment and less stress.
Kabisch, Püffel, Masztalerz, et al. (2021) [27]GermanyEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART and SRT
In situ study
Questionnaire
Physiological measures
Heart rate variability
ECG
Blood pressure
ROS
POMS
Green space visitation pattern
Familiarity
Medication
BMI
Short health survey
Self-perceived health
Perception of naturalness
N = 33
Aged between 55–70 years
M = 63.5 years
Male/female was equally divided.
Non-smokers with a healthy heart.
Could walk for 30 min.
Are regularly active.
Nature environment
(Urban park)
Being away
Social context
Field experiment if older adults experience physiological and psychological effects after visiting inner city areas.
Li, Liu, Yang, et al. (2021) [60]ChinaEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART and SRT
In situ study
Questionnaire
Physiological measures
Blood pressure
Heart rate
PRS
ROS
PANAS
POMS
Illumination
Temperature
Noise
Air Quality Index
N = 45
Aged between 40–71 years
Mostly women.
In good health. Absence of severe cardiovascular disease and cognitive impairment.
No problems walking
Nature and urban environment
(Urban parks, city streets, shops, high-rise buildings)
Safety
Social context
Experimental study to test if night-time walking can offer restoration to older adults.
Li, Zhai, Xiao, et al. (2019) [69]ChinaEmpirical study
Quantitative
SRT
In situ study
Questionnaire
Movement data
Self-rated stress levels
Affect
GPS measures
Pedometer data
N = 200
Aged 60+ years
Gender was equally divided.
No difficulty walking. Mostly in good health. A total of 75% were married. Half fell in the middle-income category. A total of 65% of participants visited the park almost every day.
Nature environment
(Urban park)
CompatibilityFascination
Compatibility
A study collecting self-reported psychological benefits among older adults before and after park visits.
Liao, Ou, Heng Hsieh, et al. (2020) [57]United StatesEmpirical Study
Quantitative
ART and SRT
Real-life study (not in situ)
Questionnaire
(self) rated attention abilities
(self) rated stress
Mini-Mental State Exam
ADL
Appetite
Mood
Social interaction
N = 42
Care professionals filled in the questionnaires with/for 42 senior patients
Nature environment
(Shared Garden of Geriatric care centre)
Safety
Being away
Compatibility
Extent
Fascination
Familiarity
Study to test the effect of garden visits on older people with dementia. Care professionals filed a questionnaire for the participants after the garden visit (indirect data collection).
Lu, Oh, Ooka, et al. (2022) [63]JapanEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
In situ study
Questionnaire
Japanese ROS
Climate conditions
Spatial conditions (sky view factor, green view factor, colour index, facilities (toilet, water, seats, pavement))
Vitality
N = 202
Aged 60+ years
More males than females.Nature environment
(Urban park)
Safety
Social context
WaterExperimental study to show which environmental features of SPUGS affect mental restoration of older adults (e.g., green, colour, sky factor).
Marques, McIntosh, Kershaw (2019) [66](Not
specified)
Literature Review
Medical report analyses
Mixed-Method
ART and SRT
Nature and urban environmentSafetyGround cover
Familiarity
Review on design elements in therapeutic environments that can offer psychological restoration to older adults.
Moore (2007) [61]United StatesDescriptive study
Qualitative
ART and SRT
In situ study
Environmental analyses
Spatial analyses of dementia gardens on the four ART characteristics Nature environment
(Shared therapeutic dementia garden)
Safety
Being away
Compatibility
Social context
Ground cover
Water
Extent
Fascination
Compatibility
Exploration of how the design of a restorative garden can support older people with dementia by reducing attention fatigue
Neale, Aspinall, Roe, et al. (2020) [72]United KingdomEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART and SRT
In situ study
Brain activity (EEG)
Alpha waves (relaxation)
Beta waves (attention)
N = 95
Aged between 65–92 years
Healthy adults were able to walk unassisted for at least 15 min.
No cognitive deficits (MMSE scores).
Nature and urban environment
(Urban green space, quiet urban area, busy street)
Fascination
Familiarity
Experimental study to look at brain activity in older people while walking in urban environments.
Ottosson, Grahn (2005) [52]SwedenEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART and SRT
In situ study
Attention tests
Physiological measures
Questionnaire
Necker cube pattern control test
Digit span forward
Digit span backwards
Symbol digit modalities test
Blood pressure
Heart rate
Pulse rate
Preference
N = 15
M = 85 years
Mostly women.
Four are in a wheelchair.
All need care.
Living in a care home.
Nature environment
(Shared garden of Geriatric care centre)
Safety
Compatibility
Social context
Deflected vista
Fascination
Familiarity
Experimental study to measure restoration in care home residents.
Ottosson, Grahn (2006) [51]SwedenEmpirical study
Mixed-method
ART and SRT
In situ study
Attention tests
Physiological measures
Questionnaire
Necker cube pattern control test
Digit span forward
Digit span backwards
Symbol digit modalities test
Blood pressure
Pulse rate
Degree to which they felt at home
Social climate
Activities
Mental energy
Physical condition
N = 15
Age between 67–97 years
Almost all females.
Four were in a wheelchair.
Urban environment
(Retirement home)
Safety
Social context
Experimental study to measure restoration in care home residents.
Qiu, Chen, Gao (2021) [59]ChinaEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
Lab study
Photo evaluation
Questionnaire
PRS
Perceived Sensory Dimensions (PSD)
Preferences
N = 300
Aged 60+ years
M = 69 years
Male/female was equally divided.
No cognitive or communication difficulties.
Nature environment
(Botanical garden)
Safety
Being away
Compatibility
Social context
Complexity
Ground cover
Water
Extent (coherence)
Fascination
Compatibility
Study to measure restoration of older adults when viewing nature images
Roe, Roe (2018) [17](Not
specified)
Literature review
Qualitative
ART and SRT
Nature and urban environment
(Home environment, garden, park, street, outdoor gym, water setting, adventurous environment, farmers market, dementia care centre)
Being away
Compatibility
Social context
Deflected vista
Extent
Fascination
Book chapter/review about restorative environments for older adults and how they promote activity
Rosenbaum, Sweeney, Windhorst (2009) [53]United StatesEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
In situ study
Questionnaire
Short PRS
Place attachment
Social support (SSQT)
Activities
Perceived health status
Patronising
N = 90
Age between 60–89 years
M = 70 years
Almost all were female participants.
Half were married.
Urban environment
(Matter’s More Than a Café (MMC))
Being away
Social context
FascinationStudy to get to know if senior café can offer restoration to older adults. Primarily focusing on social context.
Rosenbaum, Sweeney, Massiah (2014) [54]AustraliaEmpirical study
Mixed-method
ART
Study 1
In situ study
Interview
Analyses of descriptions of interview data using the four ART characteristics.N = 11
Age between 70–92 years
Male/female was equally divided.
Between 2–18 years visitors of the centre.
Urban environment
(Senior Centre)
Safety
Being away
Compatibility
Social context
Extent
Fascination
Compatibility
Study to analyse the restorative potential of senior activity centres—follow-up study.
Study 2
In situ study
Questionnaire
Short PRS
Fatigue (IFS instrument)
Quality of Life
Mood
Attitude
Physical strength
Mental strength
Customer behaviour
N = 85
Age between 60–92 years
60% female.
Scopelliti, Giuliani (2004) [65]ItalyEmpirical study
Mixed-method
ART
Real-life study (not in situ)
Questionnaire
Interview
Questionnaire about the restorative experience (relaxing and exciting)
Semi structured interviews about why they would feel restored in a particular situation
Moment of restorative experience: weekday, weekend, vacation
N = 22
M = 68.4 years
(Compared with larger group N = 67)
Males/females were equally divided.Nature and urban environments
(Participants could suggest their own restorative environments)
Being away
Compatibility
Social context
Extent
Fascination
Compatibility
One of the first restorative environment studies with older adults. Studying which restorative places people choose across their lifespan.
Scopelliti, Giuliani (2006) [64]ItalyEmpirical study
Mixed-method
ART
Study 1
Real-life study (not in situ)
Interviews
Analyses of descriptions of restorative experiencesN = 48
Aged between 60–85 years M = 70.54 years
Living in an urban setting.
Male/female was equally divided.
Nature and urban environments
(Participants could suggest their own restorative environments)
Safety
Being away
Compatibility
Social context
Deflected vista
Water
Extent (coherence and scope)
Fascination
Familiarity
Compatibility
Analyses of restorative experiences of older adults. Follow-up study.
Study 2
Real-life study (not in situ)
Questionnaire
Italian PRSN = 192
Aged between 63–78 years
M = 68.23 years
Male/female was equally divided. Good health. No physical or cognitive impairments. Almost all participants were married.
Tang, Brown (2006) [89]CanadaEmpirical study
Quantitative
SRT
In situ study
Physiological measures
Blood pressure
Heart rate
POMS
N = 5
Aged between 77–89 years
All females.
Lived in a retirement centre.
All completed high school.
Caucasian.
Nature and urban environment
(View from a window of the retirement home to build environment OR natural landscape)
Quasi-experiment to study the effect of viewing nature landscapes on mental health of older women.
Travis, McAuley (1998) [50]United StatesEmpirical study
Qualitative
ART
Real-life study (not in situ)
Interviews
Analyses of descriptions of restorative experiences
Preferences
N = 8
Aged 60+ years
Admitted to care facility after hip surgery.Nature and urban environments
(Participants could suggest their own restorative environments)
Compatibility
Social context
Experiment to check if restorative experience can support hip surgery rehabilitation with older adults.
Twedt, Rainey, Proffitt (2016) [90]United StatesEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
Lab study
Photo evaluation
Questionnaire
PRS
Visual appeal
Naturalness
Formal/informal garden
N = 295
Aged between 18–82 years
(Compared within age groups)
Mainly Caucasian.
More woman.
College education.
Nature environment
(Formal and informal shared gardens)
The study compares formal and informal garden designs on their restorative effect.
Weber, Trojan (2018) [20](Not
specified)
Systematic literature review
Quantitative
ART and SRT
Urban environments
(Various e.g., street, museum)
Being away
Compatibility
Social context
Extent
Fascination
Familiarity
Literature review of studies that studied urban restorative environments.
Yu, Lee, Lu, et al. (2020) [68]Taiwan Empirical study
Mixed-method
ART and SRT
Lab study
Virtual reality
Physiological measures
Attention tasks
Questionnaire
Heart rate
Systolic and diastolic blood pressure
Activity level of (para)sympathetic nervous system
RCS
SART
POMS
Descriptions of VR-experience
N = 9
Aged 65+ years
(Compared within larger sample N = 34)
Most female.
Healthy participants with no neuropsychiatric disorders, cardiovascular diseases, or cognition disorders of specific visual and hearing problems.
Nature and urban environments
(Nature reserve park, busy urban streets)
Experimental study measures restorative effects on older adults while viewing virtual nature. Compare older and middle-aged adults.
Zhao, Li, Zhu, Ge (2020) [91]ChinaEmpirical study
Quantitative
ART
In situ study
Questionnaire
PRSS
Stress level (in last month)
Comfort of soundscape
Intensity of soundscape
Preference of soundscape
N = 29
Aged 60+ years
(Compared within larger sample N = 240)
Male/female evenly distributed. Nature environment
(Urban park)
Studying the effect of birdsong soundscape on the restorative potential of urban parks.

References

  1. WHO. Ageing and Health. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/ageing-and-health (accessed on 12 July 2023).
  2. van Hees, S. The Making of Ageing-in-Place Perspectives on a Dutch Social Policy. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Maastricht, Maastricht, The Netherlands, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  3. van Helder, L.; Bos, W.; Bleijenberg, N.; van Eijck, J.; de Jager, H.; Klomp, M.; de Langen, M.; Minkman, M.; Pieterse, T.; van Rixtel, M.; et al. Oud En Zelfstandig in 2030 Een Reisadvies; De Rijksoverheid: Den Haag, The Netherlands, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  4. WHO. Global Strategy and Action Plan on Ageing and Health; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  5. Cutchin, M.P. The Process of Mediated Aging-in-Place; a Theoretically Empirically Based Model. Soc. Sci. Med. 2003, 57, 1077–1090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Iwarsson, S.; Stahl, A. Accessibility, Usability and Universal Design-Positioning and Definition of Concepts Describing Person-Environment Relationships. Disabil. Rehabil. 2003, 25, 57–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Wahl, H.-W.; Oswald, F. Environmental Perspectives on Aging. In The Sage Handbook of Social Gerontology; Dannefer, D., Phillipson, C., Eds.; Sage Publication Ltd.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2010; pp. 111–124. [Google Scholar]
  8. Yasamy, M.T.; Dua, T.; Harper, M.; Saxena, S. Mental Health of Older Adults, Addressing a Growing Concern; Academia: Lausanne, Switzerland, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  9. PAHO/WHO. Seniors and Mental Health. Available online: https://www.paho.org/hq/index.php?option=com_content&view=article&id=9877:seniors-mental-health&Itemid=40721&lang=en (accessed on 19 April 2021).
  10. Garin, N.; Olaya, B.; Miret, M.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L.; Power, M.; Bucciarelli, P.; Haro, J.M. Built Environment and Elderly Population Health: A Comprehensive Literature Review. Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Ment. Health 2014, 10, 103–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Evans, G.W. The Built Environment and Mental Health. J. Urban Health 2003, 80, 536–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Markevych, I.; Schoierer, J.; Hartig, T.; Chudnovsky, A.; Hystad, P.; Dzhambov, A.M.; de Vries, S.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Brauer, M.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J.; et al. Exploring Pathways Linking Greenspace to Health: Theoretical and Methodological Guidance. Environ. Res. 2017, 158, 301–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Hartig, T.; Mitchell, R.; De Vries, S.; Frumkin, H. Nature and Health. Annu. Rev. of Public Health 2014, 35, 207–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Hartig, T. Restoration in Nature: Beyond the Conventional Narrative. In Nature and Psychology. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation; Schutte, A.R., Torquati, J.C., Stevens, J.R., Eds.; Springer Nature: Cham, Switzerland, 2021; Volume 67, pp. 89–151. ISBN 9783030690205. [Google Scholar]
  15. Hartig, T. Three Steps to Understanding Restorative Environments as Health Resources. In Open space: People space; Ward Thompson, C., Travlou, P., Eds.; Taylor & Francis: Abingdon, UK, 2007; pp. 163–179. ISBN 9780203961827. [Google Scholar]
  16. Francis, J.; Wood, L.J.; Knuiman, M.; Giles-Corti, B. Quality or Quantity? Exploring the Relationship between Public Open Space Attributes and Mental Health in Perth, Western Australia. Soc. Sci. Med. 2012, 74, 1570–1577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Roe, J.; Roe, A. Restorative Environments and Promoting Physical Activity Among Older People. In The Palgrave Handbook of Ageing and Physical Activity Promotion; Nyman, S.R., Barker, A., Haines, T., Horton, K., Musselwhite, C., Peeters, G., Victor, C.R., Wolff, J.K., Eds.; Palgrave Mcmillan: Cham, Switserland, 2018; pp. 467–483. ISBN 9783319712918. [Google Scholar]
  18. Schmidt, T.; Kerr, J.; Schipperijn, J. Associations between Neighborhood Open Space Features and Walking and Social Interaction in Older Adults-a Mixed Methods Study. Geriatrics 2019, 4, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Zhu, W.; Wang, J.; Qin, B. Quantity or Quality? Exploring the Association between Public Open Space and Mental Health in Urban China. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2021, 213, 104–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Weber, A.M.; Trojan, J. The Restorative Value of the Urban Environment: A Systematic Review of the Existing Literature. Environ. Health Insights 2018, 12, 1178630218812805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Jansen, D.A. Attentional Demands and Restorative Activities: Do They Influence Directed Attention among the Elderly. Ph.D. Thesis, Univeristy of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  22. Fumagalli, N.; Fermani, E.; Senes, G.; Boffi, M.; Pola, L.; Inghilleri, P. Sustainable Co-Design with Older People: The Case of a Public Restorative Garden in Milan (Italy). Sustainability 2020, 12, 3166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Moore, R.C.; Straus, E.; Campbell, L.M. Stress, Mental Health, and Aging. In Handbook of Mental Health and Aging; Hantke, N., Etkin, A., O’Hara, R., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2020; pp. 37–58. ISBN 9780128001363. [Google Scholar]
  24. Fleury-Bahi, G.; Pol, E.; Navarro, O. Handbook of Environmental Psychology and Quality of Life; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  25. Collado, S.; Staats, H.; Corraliza, J.A.; Hartig, T. Restorative Environments and Health. In Handbook of Environmental Psychology and Quality of Life Research; Enric, G.F., Oscar, P., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; p. 574. ISBN 9783319314143. [Google Scholar]
  26. Husser, E.K.; Roberto, K.A.; Allen, K.R. Nature as Nurture: Rural Older Women’s Perspectives on the Natural Environment. J. Women Aging 2020, 32, 44–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Kabisch, N.; Püffel, C.; Masztalerz, O.; Hemmerling, J.; Kraemer, R. Physiological and Psychological Effects of Visits to Different Urban Green and Street Environments in Older People: A Field Experiment in a Dense Inner-City Area. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2021, 207, 103998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Jarosz, E. Direct Exposure to Green and Blue Spaces Is Associated with Greater Mental Wellbeing in Older Adults. J. Aging Environ. 2022, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bornioli, A.; Parkhurst, G.; Morgan, P.L. Psychological Wellbeing Benefits of Simulated Exposure to Five Urban Settings: An Experimental Study from the Pedestrian’s Perspective. J. Transp. Health 2018, 9, 105–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Deng, L.; Li, X.; Luo, H.; Fu, E.K.; Ma, J.; Sun, L.X.; Huang, Z.; Cai, S.Z.; Jia, Y. Empirical Study of Landscape Types, Landscape Elements and Landscape Components of the Urban Park Promoting Physiological and Psychological Restoration. Urban For. Urban Green. 2020, 48, 126488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Huang, S.; Qi, J.; Li, W.; Dong, J.; van den Bosch, C.K. The Contribution to Stress Recovery and Attention Restoration Potential of Exposure to Urban Green Spaces in Low-Density Residential Areas. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 8713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Liu, L.; Qu, H.; Ma, Y.; Wang, K.; Qu, H. Restorative Benefits of Urban Green Space: Physiological, Psychological Restoration and Eye Movement Analysis. J. Environ. Manag. 2022, 301, 113930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. San-Juan, C.; Subiza-Pérez, M.; Vozmediano, L. Restoration and the City: The Role of Public Urban Squares. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 2093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Boffi, M.; Pola, L.G.; Fermani, E.; Senes, G.; Inghilleri, P.; Piga, B.E.A.; Stancato, G.; Fumagalli, N. Visual Post-Occupancy Evaluation of a Restorative Garden Using Virtual Reality Photography: Restoration, Emotions, and Behavior in Older and Younger People. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 927688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Bornioli, A.; Subiza-Pérez, M. Restorative Urban Environments for Healthy Cities: A Theoretical Model for the Study of Restorative Experiences in Urban Built Settings. Landsc. Res. 2022, 48, 152–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Roe, J.; McCay, L. Restorative Cities. Urban Design for Mental Health and Wellbeing; Bloomsbury: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  37. Arksey, H.; O’Malley, L. Scoping Studies: Towards a Methodological Framework. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 2005, 8, 19–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Levac, D.; Colquhoun, H.; O’Brien, K.K. Scoping Studies: Advancing the Methodology. Implement. Sci. 2010, 5, 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Joye, Y.; Van Den Berg, A.E. Restorative Environments. In Environmental psychology. An introduction; Steg, L., Van der Berg, A.E., De Groot, J., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 2012; pp. 55–66. [Google Scholar]
  40. Kaplan, R.; Kaplan, S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  41. Ulrich, R.S. Aesthetic and Affective Response to Natural Environment. In Behavior and the Natural Environment; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 85–125. [Google Scholar]
  42. Ulrich, R.S.; Simonst, R.F.; Lositot, B.D.; Fioritot, E.; Milest, M.A.; Zelsont, M. Stress Recovery during Exposure to Natural and Urban Environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 201–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Kaplan, S. The Restorative Benefits of Nature: Toward an Integrative Framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 1995, 15, 169–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Staats, H. Restorative Environments. In The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology; Clayton, S.D., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  45. Tricco, A.C.; Lillie, E.; Zarin, W.; O’Brien, K.K.; Colquhoun, H.; Levac, D.; Moher, D.; Peters, M.D.J.; Horsley, T.; Weeks, L.; et al. PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMAScR): Checklist and Explanation. Ann. Intern. Med. 2018, 169, 467–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. OECD. Pensions at a Glance 2021: OECD and G20 Indicators. Available online: https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/sites/304a7302-en/index.html?itemId=/content/component/304a7302-en#:~:text=In%202020%2C%20the%20OECD%20average,%2C%20for%20men%20only%2C%20Israel (accessed on 5 September 2023).
  47. Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 Statement: An Updated Guideline for Reporting Systematic Reviews. BMJ 2021, 372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Onwuegbuzie, A.J.; Leech, N.L.; Collins, K.M.T. The Qualitative Report The Qualitative Report Qualitative Analysis Techniques for the Review of the Literature Qualitative Analysis Techniques for the Review of the Literature. Qual. Rep. 2012, 17, 7–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Smit, B.; Scherman, V. Computer-Assisted Qualitative Data Analysis Software for Scoping Reviews: A Case of ATLAS.Ti. Int. J. Qual. Methods 2021, 20, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Travis, S.S.; McAuley, W.J. Mentally Restorative Experiences Supporting Rehabilitation of High Functioning Elders Recovering from Hip Surgery. J. Adv. Nurs. 1998, 27, 977–985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Ottosson, J.; Grahn, P. Measures of Restoration in Geriatric Care Residences: The Influence of Nature on Elderly People’s Power of Concentration, Blood Pressure and Pulse Rate. J. Hous. Elder. 2006, 19, 227–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ottosson, J.; Grahn, P. A Comparison of Leisure Time Spent in a Garden with Leisure Time Spent Indoors: On Measures of Restoration in Residents in Geriatric Care. Landsc. Res. 2005, 30, 23–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Rosenbaum, M.S. Restorative Servicescapes: Restoring Directed Attention in Third Places. J. Serv. Manag. 2009, 20, 173–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Rosenbaum, M.S.; Sweeney, J.C.; Massiah, C. The Restorative Potential of Senior Centers. Manag. Serv. Qual. 2014, 24, 363–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Finlay, J.; Franke, T.; Mckay, H.; Sims-Gould, J. Health & Place Therapeutic Landscapes and Wellbeing in Later Life: Impacts of Blue and Green Spaces for Older Adults. Health Place 2015, 34, 97–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Jansen, D.A. Perceived Barriers to Participation in Mentally Restorative Activities by Community-Dwelling Elders. Act. Adapt. Aging 2005, 29, 35–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Liao, M.L.; Ou, S.J.; Heng Hsieh, C.; Li, Z.; Ko, C.C. Effects of Garden Visits on People with Dementia: A Pilot Study. Dementia 2020, 19, 1009–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Jansen, D.A.; Von Sadovszky, V. Restorative Activities of Community-Dwelling Elders. West. J. Nurs. Res. 2004, 26, 381–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Qiu, L.; Chen, Q.; Gao, T. The Effects of Urban Natural Environments on Preference and Self-Reported Psychological Restoration of the Elderly. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Li, H.; Liu, H.; Yang, Z.; Bi, S.; Cao, Y.; Zhang, G. The Effects of Green and Urban Walking in Different Time Frames on Physio-Psychological Responses of Middle-Aged and Older People in Chengdu, China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Moore, K.D. Restorative Dementia Gardens: Exploring How Design May Ameliorate Attention Fatigue. J. Hous. Elder. 2007, 21, 73–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Cassarino, M.; Tuohy, I.C.; Setti, A. Sometimes Nature Doesn’t Work: Absence of Attention Restoration in Older Adults Exposed to Environmental Scenes. Exp. Aging Res. 2019, 45, 372–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Lu, S.; Oh, W.; Ooka, R.; Wang, L. Effects of Environmental Features in Small Public Urban Green Spaces on Older Adults’ Mental Restoration: Evidence from Tokyo. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 5477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Scopelliti, M.; Giuliani, M.V. Restorative Environments in Later Life: An Approach to Well-Being from the Perspective of Environmental Psychology. J. Hous. Elder. 2006, 19, 203–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Scopelliti, M.; Giuliani, V. Choosing Restorative Environments across the Lifespan: A Matter of Place Experience. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 423–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Marques, B.; McIntosh, J.; Kershaw, C. Healing Spaces: Improving Health and Wellbeing for the Elderly through Therapeutic Landscape Design. Int. J. Arts Humanit. 2019, 3, 20–34. [Google Scholar]
  67. Hawkins, J.L.; Thirlaway, K.J.; Backx, K.; Clayton, D.A. Allotment Gardening and Other Leisure Activities for Stress Reduction and Healthy Aging. Horttechnology 2011, 21, 577–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Yu, C.P.; Lee, H.Y.; Lu, W.H.; Huang, Y.C.; Browning, M.H.E.M. Restorative Effects of Virtual Natural Settings on Middle-Aged and Elderly Adults. Urban For. Urban Green. 2020, 56, 126863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Li, D.; Zhai, Y.; Xiao, Y.; Newman, G.; Wang, D. Subtypes of Park Use and Self-Reported Psychological Benefits among Older Adults: A Multilevel Latent Class Analysis Approach. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2019, 190, 103605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Boffi, M.; Pola, L.; Fumagalli, N.; Fermani, E.; Senes, G.; Inghilleri, P. Nature Experiences of Older People for Active Ageing: An Interdisciplinary Approach to the Co-Design of Community Gardens. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 702525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Rosenbaum, M.S.; Sweeney, J.C.; Windhorst, C. The Restorative Qualities of an Activity-Based, Third Place Café. Sr. Hous Care J. 2009, 17, 39–54. [Google Scholar]
  72. Neale, C.; Aspinall, P.; Roe, J.; Tilley, S.; Mavros, P.; Cinderby, S.; Coyne, R.; Thin, N.; Ward Thompson, C. The Impact of Walking in Different Urban Environments on Brain Activity in Older People. Cities Health 2020, 4, 94–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Jang, H.S.; Jeong, S.J.; Kim, J.S.; Yoo, E. The Role of Visitor’s Positive Emotions on Satisfaction and Loyalty with the Perception of Perceived Restorative Environment of Healing Garden. J. People Plants Environ. 2020, 23, 277–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Berto, R. Assessing the Restorative Value of the Environment: A Study on the Elderly in Comparison with Young Adults and Adolescents. Int. J. Psychol. 2007, 42, 331–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Chen, Y.; Yuan, Y. The Neighborhood Effect of Exposure to Blue Space on Elderly Individuals’ Mental Health: A Case Study in Guangzhou, China. Health Place 2020, 63, 102348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Jansen, D.A. Mentally Restorative Activities and Daily Functioning Among Community-Dwelling Elders. Act. Adapt. Aging 2008, 32, 181–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Kabisch, N.; van den Bosch, M.; Lafortezza, R. The Health Benefits of Nature-Based Solutions to Urbanization Challenges for Children and the Elderly—A Systematic Review. Environ. Res. 2017, 159, 362–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Staats, H.; Hartig, T. Alone or with a Friend: A Social Context for Psychological Restoration and Environmental Preferences. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 199–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Jandackova, V.K.; Scholes, S.; Britton, A.; Steptoe, A. Are Changes in Heart Rate Variability in Middle-Aged and Older People Normative or Caused by Pathological Conditions? Findings From a Large Population-Based Longitudinal Cohort Study. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2016, 5, e002365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Moor, N.; Mohammadi, M. Grey Smart Societies: Supporting the Social Inclusion of Older Adults by Smart Spatial Design. In Data-Driven Multivalence in the Built Environment; Biloria, N., Ed.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2020; pp. 157–180. [Google Scholar]
  81. Lawton, M.P.; Nahemow, L. Ecology and the Aging Process. In The Psychology of Adult Development and Aging; Eisendorfer, C., Lawton, M.P., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1973; pp. 619–674. [Google Scholar]
  82. Wahl, H.-W.; Oswald, F. Theories of Environmental Gerontology: Old and New Avenues for Person-Environmental Views of Aging. In Handbook of Theories of Aging; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 621–641. [Google Scholar]
  83. Berg, C.A.; Smith, T.W.; Henry, N.J.M.; Pearce, G. A Developmental Approach to Psychosocial Risk Factors in Successful Aging. In Handbook of Health Psychology and Aging; Aldwin, C.M., Park, C.L., Spiro, A., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  84. Dannefer, D.; Settersten, R.A., Jr. The Study of Life Course: Implications for Social Gerontology. In The SAGE Handbook of Social Gerontology; Dannefer, D., Philipson, C., Eds.; SAGE: London, UK, 2010; pp. 3–19. [Google Scholar]
  85. Luo, M.; Ding, D.; Bauman, A.; Negin, J.; Phongsavan, P. Social Engagement Pattern, Health Behaviors and Subjective Well-Being of Older Adults: An International Perspective Using WHO-SAGE Survey Data. BMC Public Health 2020, 20, 99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Chan, S.H.M.; Qiu, L.; Esposito, G.; Mai, K.P.; Tam, K.P.; Cui, J. Nature in Virtual Reality Improves Mood and Reduces Stress: Evidence from Young Adults and Senior Citizens. Virtual Real. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Elsadek, M.; Shao, Y.; Liu, B. Benefits of Indirect Contact with Nature on the Physiopsychological Well-Being of Elderly People. Health Environ. Res. Des. J. 2021, 14, 227–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Gamble, K.R.; Howard, J.H.; Howard, D.V. Not Just Scenery: Viewing Nature Pictures Improves Executive Attention in Older Adults. Exp. Aging Res. 2014, 40, 513–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Tang, J.W.; Brown, R.D. The Effect of Viewing a Landscape on Physiological Health of Elderly Women. J. Hous. Elder. 2006, 1, 187–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Twedt, E.; Rainey, R.M.; Proffitt, D.R. Designed Natural Spaces: Informal Gardens Are Perceived to Be More Restorative than Formal Gardens. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Zhao, W.; Li, H.; Zhu, X.; Ge, T. Effect of Birdsong Soundscape on Perceived Restorativeness in an Urban Park. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flow chart of the paper selection process based on PRISMA [47] flow diagram for scoping reviews.
Figure 1. Flow chart of the paper selection process based on PRISMA [47] flow diagram for scoping reviews.
Ijerph 20 06801 g001
Figure 2. The number of publications per year.
Figure 2. The number of publications per year.
Ijerph 20 06801 g002
Table 1. A general framework for theories about restorative environments: Stress Reduction Theory and Attention Restoration Theory. The pictograms give additional information about the features of P–E transactions that permit and promote restoration according to the theories. Adapted from Hartig [14] (p. 100).
Table 1. A general framework for theories about restorative environments: Stress Reduction Theory and Attention Restoration Theory. The pictograms give additional information about the features of P–E transactions that permit and promote restoration according to the theories. Adapted from Hartig [14] (p. 100).
TheoryResource CategoryAntecedent ConditionFeatures of P-E Transactions That Permit RestorationFeatures of P-E Transactions That Promote RestorationOutcomes That Can Reflect on Restoration
Stress Reduction Theory (SRT)Ability to mobilise for actionPsychophysiological stressThe apparent absence of uncontrollable threatPerception of natural contentsModerate levels of complexityMore positive self-reported affect, lower blood pressure and cortisol levels
Ijerph 20 06801 i001Ijerph 20 06801 i002Ijerph 20 06801 i003
In view of a threatful event, feelings of safety need to be encouraged to permit the restoration process. Scenes with water enhance environmental quality.Describes the number of separated elements in an environment and the balance between structured and unstructured elements.
Gross structureOther visual stimulus attributes
Ijerph 20 06801 i004Ijerph 20 06801 i005
The environment needs to give structured information for orientation, for example, a clear focal point.The line of sight is deflected, hiding what could be lying behind this raises feelings of interest and curiosity. Impacts feelings of spaciousness
Attention Restoration Theory (ART)Ability to direct attentionDirected attention fatigueBeing awayCompatibilityFascinationExtentImproved performance on standardised tests of cognitive abilities
Ijerph 20 06801 i006Ijerph 20 06801 i007Ijerph 20 06801 i008Ijerph 20 06801 i009
Escape (physically or mentally) from everyday routine pressures and obligations.The perceived fit between the environment and the individual needs and inclinations.The environment’s capability to involuntarily catch one’s attention and not demand mental effort.Refers to properties of connectedness. The environment feels like a whole (coherence) and promises to engage one’s mind (scope).
Compatibility
Ijerph 20 06801 i010
The way that an environment enables people to experience restorative activities.
Table 2. Search items used in the search per key theme.
Table 2. Search items used in the search per key theme.
Psychological
restoration
Restoration likelihood; Restorative experiences; Restorative potential; Perceived restoration; Restorative environment; Attention restoration; Stress
Older populationElderly; Older adult; Third age; Fourth age; Life span; Life course; Old people; Elder; Age differences; Senior; Older individuals
Table 4. Critical areas of recommendation for future research.
Table 4. Critical areas of recommendation for future research.
MethodStandardisation on psychological restoration measures for older adults.
Research the compatibility of physiological measures and attention tests on older populations.
Individual and
generational differences
More research is needed with a variety of older participants, such as older old individuals, people with cognitive disabilities or different cultural backgrounds.
Features of
person-environment transaction
Further research on the permitting and promoting features proposed in the general theory and how they are applicable to older populations.
Investigation of additional features (e.g., being with and familiarity) and their influence on the psychological restoration process for older adults.
Type of environmentResearch the restorative potential of accessible environments close to older adults’ homes (for example, neighbourhood open spaces).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Grave, A.J.J.; Neven, L.; Mohammadi, M. Elucidating and Expanding the Restorative Theory Framework to Comprehend Influential Factors Supporting Ageing-in-Place: A Scoping Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 6801. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20186801

AMA Style

Grave AJJ, Neven L, Mohammadi M. Elucidating and Expanding the Restorative Theory Framework to Comprehend Influential Factors Supporting Ageing-in-Place: A Scoping Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(18):6801. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20186801

Chicago/Turabian Style

Grave, Anne Johanna Jacoba, Louis Neven, and Masi Mohammadi. 2023. "Elucidating and Expanding the Restorative Theory Framework to Comprehend Influential Factors Supporting Ageing-in-Place: A Scoping Review" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 18: 6801. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20186801

APA Style

Grave, A. J. J., Neven, L., & Mohammadi, M. (2023). Elucidating and Expanding the Restorative Theory Framework to Comprehend Influential Factors Supporting Ageing-in-Place: A Scoping Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(18), 6801. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20186801

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop