Next Article in Journal
The Effect of Alcohol Drinking on Metabolic Syndrome and Obesity in Koreans: Big Data Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Exploring Risk Factors of Recall-Associated Foodborne Disease Outbreaks in the United States, 2009–2019
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Understanding the Interaction between Regulatory Focus and Message Framing in Determining Chinese Consumers’ Attitudes toward Artificial Meat

1
School of Agricultural Economics and Rural Development, Renmin University of China, Beijing 100872, China
2
Department of Nutrition and Food Science, College of Agriculture and Natural Resources, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20740, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(9), 4948; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19094948
Submission received: 23 March 2022 / Revised: 9 April 2022 / Accepted: 13 April 2022 / Published: 19 April 2022

Abstract

:
While production and consumption of meat cast a shadow over the prospects for sustainable development, artificial meat may be the solution. However, consumer acceptability of artificial meat is a major impediment to its use as a suitable alternative. This study analyzed the relationship between regulatory focus and consumer acceptance of artificial meat using randomized controlled trial data. Results showed that promotion focus results in a higher acceptance of artificial meat products due to a higher perceived benefit and lower perceived risk, whereas prevention focus results in a lower acceptance of artificial meat products due to perceived benefit being lower and perceived risk being higher. The moderating effect of the message framing was investigated employing structural equation modeling (SEM). It was discovered that a gain-oriented message framing could greatly strengthen the association between promotion focus and perceived benefit, whereas an avoidance-oriented message framing could significantly diminish the relationship between prevention focus and perceived risk. This study has crucial implications for how policymakers and industries communicate with consumers about artificial meat.

1. Introduction

The global community is at a critical moment in its pursuit of Seventeen Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs). SDGs have directed many public initiatives, company policies, educational efforts, research activities, etc. Ensuring food security, avoiding wasting water, promote energy efficiency, and stop global warming (SDG:2;6;7;13) with regard to what we eat—less animal-based and more plant-based—and how food is produced [1,2]. The concerns about environmental degradation, public health, and animal welfare caused by the production and consumption of traditional meat have attracted extensive attention. Traditional meat consumption has a negative impact on global food security and consumes large amounts of resources. Thirty-five percent of the crops produced by agricultural systems are consumed by animals [3]. In total, 70% of global water consumption and 20% of energy consumption are directly or indirectly used in the production of animal products [4]. GHG emissions from the production of animal products exceed those emitted by all modes of transportation combined [5]. Substituting artificial meat for traditional meat is an important way to achieve the Sustainable Development Goals and has received a lot of attention from existing research. Artificial meat, including plant-based meat and cell-based meat, represents changes to current food systems that can reduce GHG emissions, water input, energy input, and ensure food security [6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13].
As a young industry, artificial meat is in constant evolution, seeking ways to overcome three key challenges: standardization, environmental sustainability, and profitability [14]. Another critical challenge is that consumers are skeptical of artificial meat [15,16]. It is therefore important to understand consumers’ perceptions of artificial meat, including those aspects they consider favorable and unfavorable.
A body of consumer insights regarding artificial meat and related novel technologies for food production already exists. In various ways, it illuminates the mix of potential advantages and disadvantages of artificial meat to consumers, including improved security in food supply, less GHG emissions, good for human health, good for animal welfare, and less antibiotic use (advantages), but premium pricing, concerns over the decline of traditional agriculture, and concerns over bad tastes (disadvantages). Stakeholder insights have also been keenly sought, often through depth interviews and/or case studies [13,16,17,18,19,20,21]. A shortcoming of this growing literature is the lack of exploration of attitudes toward and perceptions of artificial meat from different message framing settings in China. Such undertakings are pertinent considering that China is the largest consumer of meat. In this regard, consumer attitudes in China are particularly relevant because of the region’s population density and its many megacities.
This study adds to the existing body of knowledge in three ways. To begin with, the current research adds new consumer insights by conducting a randomized controlled trial investigation in which respondents were exposed to various message framing describing artificial meat in order to ascertain which aspects consumers find acceptable or unacceptable. Consumer attitudes were elicited directly in response to the researcher’s message framing of artificial meat—that is, gain-oriented message framing/avoidance-oriented message framing. The gain-oriented message emphasizes the benefits of artificial meat, while the avoidance-oriented message emphasizes the risks of artificial meat. The messages presented in the control and treatment groups are based on the companies MOSAMEAT and IMPOSSIBLEFOOD’s descriptions of artificial meat. While numerous previous studies have examined the effects of message framing and regulatory focus, the concentration of artificial meat products is relatively low. Second, this study empirically estimates and compares consumers’ acceptance across different regulatory focus and information types, and further reveals the effects of information and regulatory focus on attitude. Third, this study analyzes nationally representative data from China in order to shed light not only on consumer attitudes toward artificial meat, but also on the extent to which those attitudes are influenced by message framing, thereby filling a knowledge gap in Asia. This will help manufacturers use the information framework to target specific user groups to promote artificial meat products.

2. Literature

2.1. Message Framing and Regulatory Focus Theory

Attitudes can be thought of as affective associations that predispose consumers to evaluate a particular concept as positive or negative [22,23], and measuring attitudes is critical for comprehending consumer behavior [22]. The most frequently used method of measuring attitudes toward specific issues is to rely on explicit measures of participants’ responses to Likert-type scales [23], which may include asking participants to rate their level of agreement with a series of statements. Due to consumers’ limited knowledge of the artificial meat production process and nutrition in relation to novel food products, it is impractical to study consumer attitudes toward artificial meat without providing any message about it. However, the message framing used to describe artificial meat may have a significant impact on consumer attitudes toward novel food products [24,25], and it is critical for industry and policymakers to gain a comprehensive understanding of the message framing effects on artificial meat products.
The term “message framing” refers to a linguistic presentation strategy used to increase the persuasiveness of a particular message. The effect of message framing is theoretically based on Kahneman and Tversky’s prospect theory, which asserts that consumers’ value functions in decision-making are classified as gain or loss [26]. Consumers confronted with risky outcomes may, according to prospect theory, evaluate gains and losses differently, and their preferences may vary in terms of negative or positive framing. Previous research indicated that the way information or a message is presented has a significant impact on consumers’ purchasing decisions [24,27,28,29]. However, few studies have examined the effect of message framing on consumer perceptions of artificial meat.
Additionally, the impact of message framing on consumers may be highly varied, since individuals tend to concentrate only on the message that interests them. It has been hypothesized that the regulatory focus acts as a filter for information, causing people to react differently to the same message [30]. As a result, it is equally critical to grasp the regulatory focus. Higgins advances the regulatory focus hypothesis [31], which establishes a distinction between two systems that govern people’s judgments and actions. A promotion focus (Pro) stresses the pursuit of gains and ambitions toward an ideal, which develops an enthusiastic approach mentality. By contrast, a prevention focus (Pre) places a premium on avoiding loss and meeting responsibilities, which promotes a watchful attitude of avoidance [31]. Existing research indicates that when messages are framed in a way that corresponds to the consumer’s regulatory priorities, they are more compelling [32,33]. It has been proposed that when an individual’s regulatory focus and message framing align, his or her value judgment on information is enhanced, hence boosting the individual’s subjective appraisal of a product [34]. However, there is a dearth of studies examining consumer attitudes toward artificial meat using the regulatory focus theory, particularly in Asia.

2.2. Perceived Benefits

The term “perceived benefits” refers to “the perceived net advantages connected with the purchased items or services” [35]. Consumers assess the advantages they believe they will achieve from obtaining and utilizing the product/service against the dangers associated with doing so [35]. Frequently, this “benefit” component is derived from the product/quality of service or the purchasers’ perception of the product/quality of service [35]. The idea of perceived advantage is commonly used to explain why a person engages in a certain activity or action [36]. Both functional and non-functional motives impact the human determinants of consumption behavior [37]. Functional motivations are those that are associated with utilitarian functions such as convenience, performance, physical, and financial rewards, while non-functional reasons are those that are associated with social and emotional requirements [36,38]. Utilitarian advantages are essentially instrumental, functional, and cognitive in nature; they give clients value by serving as a means to an aim. Hedonic advantages are non-instrumental, experiential, emotional, and emotive; they are appreciated independently of their practical utility [38,39].
Similarly, customers’ benefits from artificial meat might be classified as utilitarian or hedonic. Consumers benefit from utilitarian features that increase the usefulness, efficiency, and economy of their purchases. For instance, consider the safety and nutritional value of artificial meat. In addition, the social and psychological benefits of artificial meat can be classified as hedonic, because they provide intrinsic pleasure, fun, and self-esteem. For instance, the environmental and animal welfare benefits of fake meat are a social acquisition with positive externalities, and this emotion is critical to achieving the goal of artificial meat promotion. Additionally, because regulatory focus is connected to perceived benefits and perceived benefits are related to consumer behavior [40], this study explores the function of perceived benefits as a significant intermediary variable in the relationship between regulatory focus and consumer attitudes.

2.3. Perceived Risks

The term “perceived risks” refers to a consumer’s subjective assessment of the inherent hazards associated with achieving a desired objective [41]. Consumers’ behaviors create an atmosphere of risk, as the probable adverse repercussions of their actions cannot be predicted with confidence [42]. The term “perceived risk” refers to the sum of the negative repercussions of a loss and the probability of bad outcomes [42,43,44]. In other words, perceived risk is defined as the difference between a customer’s pre-buy uncertainty about the kind and extent of anticipated loss associated with the purchase and use of a product and the actual risk associated with the purchase and use of the product [45]. In comparison to conventional meat, the function of perceived risk is more significant when it comes to artificial meat, owing to the increased uncertainty and unpredictability. Artificial meat provides very novel items and services. There is a scarcity of high-quality information on artificial meat, which increases perceived risk. As a result, artificial meat includes more variable components that cannot be checked thoroughly before purchase, making it harder to adequately assess risks.
Prior research has identified numerous characteristics of perceived risk that are significant in the context of consumption, including performance risk, financial risk, psychological risk, social risk, and physical danger [46,47]. Physical risk is concerned with the safety of commodities and the possible dangers or damages connected with their acquisition or usage [48]. Safety concerns pertain to consumers’ perceived sense of safety and security as a result of service providers emphasizing the emotional relief of customers who may be concerned about issues such as danger, injury, or loss [37]. Psychological risk is associated with the possibility of developing a negative self-image or self-concept as a result of the purchase or usage of goods [37]. Increased unpredictability in artificial meat products leads to an increased level of risk for the consumer. The relationship between perceived risk and moderated focus is often emphasized [40], and as perceived risk is closely related to consumer behavioral attitudes, this study will examine the important role of perceived risk as an intermediate variable.

2.4. Variables for Balance Check

Consumption of food is a behavior that is influenced by a variety of factors, including beliefs and habits. The primary objective of this study was to determine the effect of message framing on consumer perceptions of artificial meat. Thus, prior to conducting the study, it was necessary to ascertain that there were no significant differences in the sample’s food consumption beliefs and practices and to eliminate potential confounding variables, i.e., to conduct a balance test. By combining the study’s actual situation with the available literature, we identified three major categories of variables for the balance test: food-related lifestyle, food novelty fear, and food technology support attitude.
Food technologies generate a significant level of perceived risk since they impact important consumer concerns such as food safety, animal welfare, and the environment [49,50]. Thus, the amount of food innovation is crucial to customer approval, and consumer acceptance of new foods is influenced by both the kind of innovation and the product that incorporates it [51]. The majority of prior research on food innovation and consumption has concentrated on food neophobia [52]. Therefore, we used food neophobia as one of the variables for the balance test. Additionally, the majority of research on consumer acceptability of revolutionary food technologies has been undertaken in chosen European or North American nations, with a dearth of studies from Asian countries [53]. Consumption patterns of traditional meats by consumers are also a significant background component, which is why we include them [11,54]. In addition, consumer attitudes toward food technology may also be a variable that cannot be ignored [17].
Specifically, four questions are used to assess respondents’ level of food neophobia, which refers to an individual’s aversion to unfamiliar foods. A higher score on each of the food neophobia questions indicates that the responder is more hesitant to try unusual foods [55]. Five questions are aimed at ascertaining respondents’ level of food involvement, which relates to an individual’s efforts associated with the food they consume. A higher degree of food involvement (as measured by a score on related items) indicates that people care about the food they consume and make an effort to prepare it [56]. Three questions assess respondents’ support for food technology, while five others assess respondents’ meat eating habits [54]. Finally, eleven questions are used to assess respondents’ meat attitudes in the second part of the survey, including topics such as meat pleasure, health, the environment, and animal welfare. For instance, the term “enjoyment associated with meat belief” relates to respondents’ satisfaction with meat products, with higher scores on related questions indicating that respondents experienced greater enjoyment from eating meat.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Message Design

To examine how message framing affects consumer views about artificial meat, we randomly assigned our study participants to one of three message groups with varying message framing. The first category of information is the control group, which provides merely a neutral description of artificial meat. The other two types of information are for treatment purposes. Along with the neutral description of artificial meat used in the control group, the other two treatment groups include a gain-oriented and an avoidance-oriented message regarding artificial meat, respectively. The messages associated with each of the three message categories are provided in Table A1. Similar to the randomized controlled trial (RCT) method, we randomly allocate each participant to one of three information groups: neutral, gain-, or avoidance-oriented. As a result, study participants are randomly assigned to one of three information groups. Following the reading of the information, questionnaires concerning artificial meat are answered by the participants. We developed questions to elicit customer perceptions of two types of artificial meat technologies nowadays in practice (plant-based meat and cultured meat).

3.2. Data and Variable

All participants self-registered with an approved web panel (online survey) provider (WenJuanXing), the biggest online survey provider in China. The survey was conducted in December 2019 and concluded in January 2020. The final sample consisted of 2226 completed questionnaires. We develop surveys to infer customers’ regulatory focus. Additionally, our research gathers data on consumers’ meat beliefs, food involvement, food neophobia, attitudes toward food technology, and views toward artificial meat products. Thus, effective utilization of nationally representative data is intended to give insight on China’s broad sentiments about artificial meat and on how to engage with customers via message framing. This research abides by all ethical standards and guarantees that each participant is informed. Participants were told that their replies would be kept secret and consented to participate voluntarily. Participants received a prize of around 20 yuan (RMB) as recompense.
In accordance with regulatory focus theory [31], we employ four 1–5 scale questions to assess respondents’ promotion and preventive focus, respectively, and score their responses. For example, when respondents respond “totally disagree”, “disagree”, “neutral”, “agree”, or “strongly agree” to the question “I will care about how to achieve success”, we assign a score of 1, 2, 3, 4, or 5 to the response if the respondent selects “totally disagree”, “disagree”, “neutral”, “agree”, or “strongly agree”. A higher score indicates that respondents are more concerned with achieving success and hence more likely to suit the promotion target.

3.3. Empirical Strategy

Since this study involves latent variables, one of the mainstream approaches to deal with latent variables is to use structural equation modeling. This study employs structural equation modeling (SEM) to investigate the potential mechanism of association between regulatory focus and attitudes moderated by message framing. The structural equation model is divided into two components: the latent variable measurement model and the structural model. A model for measuring latent variables in which the relationships between the latent variables and their observable indicators are represented. A structural model that encapsulates the relationships between latent variables. To assess the goodness-of-fit of our group-level SEM estimations, we used the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) and coefficient of determination (CD). If the fit is good, then SRMR (standardized root mean squared residual) will be close to 0 and CD will be near 1.
More specifically, latent variables measurement model can be expressed as follows:
To better identify how the regulatory focus affects consumers attitude and further study the moderating effect of information frame, the following structural model is developed:
[ Pro 1 Pro 2 Pro 3 Pro 4 Pre 1 Pre 2 Pre 3 Pre 4 B 1 B 2 B 3 B 4 R 1 R 2 R 3 R 4 A 1 A 2 A 3 A 4 ] = [ 1 0 0 0 0 λ 2 , 1 0 0 0 0 λ 3 , 1 0 0 0 0 λ 4 , 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 λ 6 , 2 0 0 0 0 λ 7 , 2 0 0 0 0 λ 8 , 2 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 λ 9 , 3 0 0 0 0 λ 10 , 3 0 0 0 0 λ 11 , 3 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 λ 13 , 4 0 0 0 0 λ 14 , 4 0 0 0 0 λ 15 , 4 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 λ 17 , 5 0 0 0 0 λ 19 , 5 0 0 0 0 λ 20 , 5 ] [ Pro Pre Benefit Risk Attitude ] + [ ε 1 ε 2 ε 3 ε 4 ε 5 ε 6 ε 7 ε 8 ε 10 ε 11 ε 12 ε 13 ε 15 ε 16 ε 17 ε 18 ε 20 ε 21 ε 22 ε 23 ]
[ Attidude Benifit Risk ] = [ β 1 , 1 β 1 , 2 0 0 0 0 ] [ Benifit Risk ] + [ 0 0 β 2 , 1 β 2 , 2 β 3 , 1 β 3 , 2 ] [ Pro Pre ] + [ ε 19 ε 9 ε 14 ]
The visual representation of the study model is as follows (Figure 1):
Table A3 contains the observation groups for these latent variables. Pro1–4 denotes indicators that promote focus, while Pre1–4 denotes indicators that denote prevention focus [57]. B1–4 is the perceived benefit index, whereas R1–4 is the perceived risk index. A1–4 is a proxy for consumer attitudes toward artificial meat.

4. Results

4.1. Descriptive Statistics

Table A2 shows the descriptive statistics of the descriptive demographic characteristics of the sample. The number of participants in each of the three groups was 838 (neutral message group), 692 (gain-oriented message group), and 736 (avoidance-oriented message group). Our full sample consists of 1091 male and 1175 female respondents, suggesting a balanced number of males and females. In terms of education, respondents with bachelor’s degrees account for 73.6% of the total sample. Respondents’ ages are primarily distributed between 18–40 years old, and respondents’ monthly incomes are almost evenly distributed between 2000 and 20,000.
To obtain an initial estimate of consumer attitudes, the sum of the attitude measurement items is averaged (see Table A3). The higher the value, the more favorable consumers’ attitudes toward artificial meat products are. Figure 2 illustrates the distribution of consumer acceptance attitudes for each information group. Consumers’ attitudes toward artificial meat became more positive after information was provided (Score of plant-based meat: Neutral message group: 3.49; Gain-oriented message group: 3.65; Avoidance-oriented message group: 3.61) (Score of cultured meat: Neutral message group: 3.37; Gain-oriented message group: 3.50; Avoidance-oriented message group: 3.53).
The balance check of the sample groups entering distinct information groups is a prerequisite for the future analysis of the findings. Further study can be undertaken only if it is proven that there are no large variations in certain behaviors and beliefs regarding meat consumption between consumers in the different treatment groups before receiving the information. Certain variables are measured using multiple indicators, and their values are averaged by adding the values of the indicators used to measure them (Table A3). For example, the variable Food Technology Attitudes is measured by three indicators, and the mean score of these three measures is the value of the variable. The balance check of characteristics in Table 1 using the F test indicates that there is no significant difference between the samples of the three information groups, providing a reliable foundation for further research.

4.2. Results of Structural Equation Modeling

Table 2 and Table 3 show the SEM estimated results based on Equations (1) and (2), and all coefficients are statistically significant. Table 4 shows the degree of group-level goodness of fit of the structural equation model, and the results show that the overall fit is good. Promotion focus is positively related to perceived benefit and negatively related to perceived risk. Prevention focus and perceived benefit have a significant negative correlation, while prevention focus and perceived risk have a significant positive correlation. Perceived benefit was statistically significant and correlated with a positive attitude, whereas perceived risk was statistically significant and correlated with a negative attitude. As shown, promotion focus emphasis on benefits while ignoring risks, whereas prevention focuses emphasis on risks while ignoring benefits. Consumers who use promotion focus have a more positive attitude toward artificial meat by emphasizing higher perceived benefits and lower perceived risks. Because of lower perceived benefits and higher perceived risks, consumers who use prevention focus have a more negative attitude toward artificial meat.
When gain-oriented message framing is used, the relationship between stimulative focus and perceived benefit is significantly improved, as evidenced by a larger standardization coefficient (Table 2: Panel B Column 2; Table 3: Panel B Column 2). The association between prevention focus and perceived risk is significantly weakened when an avoidance-oriented message is used, specifically, as evidenced by a smaller standardization coefficient (Table 2: Panel C Column 3; Table 3: Panel C Column 3).
Additionally, the role of regulatory focus varies according to artificial meat product type. For plant-based meat, promotion focus has a greater impact on attitude (Table 2: Panel A and Table 3: Panel A), resulting in a more positive overall attitude toward plant meat. Thus, the gain-oriented message framing is more conducive to increasing people’s positive perceptions of plant meat, which in turn promotes acceptance. The public’s overall attitude toward cultured meat is more negative, owing to the fact that prevention focus plays a greater role among the regulatory focus influencing consumer intention. This is demonstrated by the increased standardized coefficient in the results (Table 2: Panel A and Table 3: Panel A). So, avoidance-oriented message framing has a more visible effect on weakening the relationship between prevention focus and risk perception, thereby assisting people with accepting cultured meat by assuaging their fears.

5. Conclusions

The analysis yields several key findings: Firstly, promotion focus encourages people to think positively about artificial meat by emphasizing the benefits and minimizing the concerns. The prevention focus has resulted in a higher emphasis on risks rather than benefits, decreasing the value of artificial meat. Secondly, the relationship between promotion focus and perceived benefit was strengthened by gain-oriented message framing, whereas the relationship between prevention focus and perceived risk was diminished by avoidance-oriented message framing. As a result, both sorts of the message improved people’s perceptions of artificial meat. Thirdly, for various artificial meat products, different information strategies should be used. Prevention focus has a stronger negative effect on cultured meat acceptance, and avoidance-oriented messages can weaken the link between prevention focus and perceived risk. Promotion focus has a stronger positive effect on plant meat acceptance, and gain-oriented messages can significantly strengthen the link between promotion focus and perceived gain.
The hedonic principle dominated the field of motivational psychology. It is based on the assumption that people are naturally motivated to seek pleasure and avoid pain. While the hedonic principle clearly identifies individual psychological drives or behavioral motivations, the question of how people pursue pleasure and avoid pain remains unanswered, and this is precisely what self-regulation theory is attempting to address. Regulatory focus theory, based on the hedonic principle, distinguishes the strategies and means by which individuals seek ideal states and avoid undesirable states, and specifies that different strategies and means are used for different regulatory focuses. Recent years have seen a shift toward a regulatory focus in motivational psychology research, which has been extensively used to explain individual differences in attitudes, emotions, and behaviors.
The empirical results of this study show that promotion focus has a statistically significant positive association with perceived benefits. Prevention focus has a statistically significant negative association with perceived risks. The results of this study once again validate the regulatory focus theory. According to regulatory focus theory, a gain-emergent situation activates facilitative focus, in which individuals are more sensitive to positive outcomes and actively seek to match the ideal state; a loss-emergent situation activates defensive focus, in which individuals are more sensitive to negative outcomes and actively avoid matching the undesirable state. Facilitative focus increases sensitivity to positive outcomes by utilizing a “desire” strategy approach to ensure hits against overlooked strategic tendencies and emotions associated with pleasure and frustration, whereas defensive focus increases sensitivity to negative outcomes by utilizing a “vigilance” strategy approach to ensure rejections. On the other hand, the defensive focus increases sensitivity to negative outcomes and employs a “vigilance” strategy approach to ensure rejection and avoid the strategic proclivity to do wrong, as well as emotions associated with calmness and excitement. A significant contribution of regulatory focus theory is the identification and differentiation of the various strategic means by which individuals pursue their objectives.
Further results obtained using the message framing can also be explained by the regulatory fit theory. Regulatory fit occurs when the strategic means used to accomplish a goal align with the individual’s regulatory focus, and this alignment increases the individual’s sense of value for the task at hand.

6. Policy Recommendation

While artificial meat has garnered much attention as an environmentally friendly, healthful, and safe substitute for meat, little study has been conducted on consumer attitudes toward artificial meat, particularly the types of messages utilized to communicate with consumers. Our analysis of nationally representative online survey data is thus designed to shed light not only on the consumer attitude to artificial meat but also on the extent to which the attitude is moderated by message framing.
These findings, which echo those of previous research, have important implications for policy. Above all, they underscore the urgent need to understand how to communicate with consumers. Industry and policymakers who are concerned with food marketing and health communication should employ different communication strategies depending on the type of consumer; for example, for consumers that use promotion focus, use gain-oriented messages. Different communication methods are used depending on the type of product. Avoidance-oriented messages, for example, are better for cultured meats.

Author Contributions

H.S.: Data curation, Visualization, Conceptualization, Writing—review and editing. P.M.: Data curation, Writing—review and editing. Y.Z.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis. J.S.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Investigation, Data curation, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing, Supervision. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the State Key Program of National Natural Science of China (No. 71633005) and the State Key R & D Program of China (No. 2018YFC1603300).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Renmin University of China.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Table A1. Messages presented in control and treatment information groups.
Table A1. Messages presented in control and treatment information groups.
Information GroupMessage
Control group
participants:
838
Artificial meat products are primarily made from two food technologies: plant-based meat and clean meat. Plant-based meat is an alternative for regular meat which contains ingredients such as proteins, oils, and starches. For consumers, plant-based meat is available on market. Clean meat is an alternative for regular meat which is also known as lab-grown, in vitro, or cultured meat. It is the meat that is grown in cell culture rather than an animal’s body. For consumers, the clean meat products of Mosa Meat are expected to be on market in 2021.
Treatment group: Gain-oriented Message
participants:
692
Control group message plus:
Plant-based meat is produced with natural plant-based ingredients, which is an environmentally friendly substitute for regular meat. The clean meat is produced in a natural way similar to the fermentation process of yogurt or beer production, which has been used for the last thousand years in human history. Artificial meat is an eco-friendly, sustainable, and safe food. It supplements nutrition with ingredients such as unsaturated fatty acid, which improves health. Compared to regular meat products, artificial meat products are green, healthy, and nutritious, and significantly improves animal welfares.
Treatment group: Avoidance-oriented Message
participants:
736
Control group message plus:
The traditional meat production inevitably involves large-scale animal farming and slaughter. The greenhouse gas emission from the husbandry sector is beyond the total emission from the transportation sector. Half of world’s antibiotics are used during meat production and animal plagues happen occasionally. Therefore, traditional meat production is neither eco-friendly nor free of safety problems. Compared to regular meat products, artificial meat products can avoid environmental destruction, animal slaughter, and food safety problems.
Table A2. Frequency Distribution of Respondents’ Characteristics.
Table A2. Frequency Distribution of Respondents’ Characteristics.
VariableDescriptionFrequencyPercentage
GenderFemale109148.15
Male117551.85
EducationMiddle school and below150.66
High school793.49
Some college31013.68
Bachelor’s degree166173.30
Master’s degree1898.34
Ph.D. degree or equivalent120.53
Monthly IncomeUnder 2000 RMB26611.74
2000–4000 RMB26111.52
4000–6000 RMB48821.54
6000–8000 RMB48421.36
8000–10,000 RMB38516.99
10,000–20,000 RMB31914.08
Above 20,000 RMB632.78
AgeUnder 18150.66
18–2567429.74
26–3062427.54
31–4072932.17
41–501777.81
51–60401.77
Above 6070.31
Table A3. Variable measurement.
Table A3. Variable measurement.
VariableIndicatorDescriptionMeanSD
Promotion FocusPro1I usually think about how to achieve expectations and goals4.0060.702
Pro2I will care about how to achieve success3.9180.839
Pro3I usually think good things relate to me3.5260.946
Pro4I care more about positive aspects of life3.2351.095
Prevention FocusPre1I usually think about how to avoid failure2.3730.949
Pre2I usually worry about that I didn’t try my best3.7880.857
Pre3I usually worry about things that I fear to happen2.6501.202
Pre4I tend to avoid negative aspects of life3.4021.059
Food NeophobiaFN1I always eat all kinds of novel food *3.0631.094
FN2I don’t trust novel food2.5491.000
FN3I like to try the food which I am not familiar with *2.8321.096
FN4I am afraid of the food which I have never eaten before 2.5841.196
Food InvolvementFI1I don’t spend much time thinking about the food I eat *3.2311.090
FI2I like to talk about the food I have eaten or will eat3.7740.891
FI3What to eat is not important to me comparing to other choices in life *3.2381.160
FI4I am familiar with nutrition facts of food3.0001.038
FI5My friends take my food recommendation seriously3.1451.075
Food Technology AttitudesFT1Support Genetically Modified Food technology2.6191.134
FT2Support Food additives2.3601.008
FT3Support Nano food technology3.0320.926
Meat Consumption BehaviorMCB1Consider price3.8820.874
MCB2Consider flavor4.1760.816
MCB3Consider health4.2170.939
MCB4Consider environment3.1611.038
MCB5Consider animal welfare2.4871.047
Meat Belief—EnjoymentMB1Meat is delicious4.2330.755
MB2Meals with meat yield worse flavor *3.9571.027
MB3I eat meat almost everyday3.7011.084
MB4Meals with meat yield better flavor3.9980.853
Meat Belief—HealthMB5Eating meat is not good for health *3.7560.969
MB6The nutrition facts in meat are important to health4.1680.738
Meat Belief—EnvironmentMB7Meat production is harmless to environment3.2780.956
MB8Meat production increases greenhouse gas *2.9471.005
Meat Belief—Animal welfareMB9Animal killing for food is reasonable3.2761.037
MB10Animal welfare is important *2.8990.982
attitudes toward plant-based meat A1Plant-based meat is expected to be a major trend in the future.3.6200.909
A2I am receptive to technologies based on plant-based meat.3.7080.931
A3I’m curious to see how plant-based meat tastes.3.8830.904
A4I’d like to substitute plant-based meat for regular meat.3.1101.072
attitudes toward cultured meatA1cultured meat is expected to be a major trend in the future.3.6060.934
A2I am receptive to technologies based on cultured meat.3.5700.979
A3I’m curious to see how cultured meat tastes.3.6730.982
A4I’d like to substitute cultured meat for regular meat.3.0081.131
Perceived benefit of plant-based meatB1I believe that plant-based meat can help protect animal welfare.4.0880.880
B2I believe that plant-based meat can help reduce greenhouse gas emissions.3.8690.912
B3I believe that plant meat does not require antibiotics, veterinary drugs, or hormones.3.7741.088
B4I believe that plant-based meat has the potential to improve nutrition and is healthier for the human body.3.8391.072
Perceived benefit of cultured meatB1I believe that cultured meat can help protect animal welfare.3.9770.948
B2I believe that cultured meat can help reduce greenhouse gas emissions.3.7650.967
B3I believe that cultured meat does not require antibiotics, veterinary drugs, or hormones.3.5541.148
B4I believe that cultured meat has the potential to improve nutrition and is healthier for the human body.3.4811.148
Perceived risk of plant-based meatR1I feel the color, smell and taste of the plant-based meat is bad3.0420.971
R2I believe that plant-based meat is not healthy.2.2891.035
R3I believe that plant-based meat is not safety.2.8231.246
R4I believe that plant-based meat is not natural.2.5491.097
Perceived risk of cultured meatR1I feel the color, smell and taste of the cultured meat is bad2.8791.004
R2I believe that cultured meat is not healthy.2.6761.076
R3I believe that cultured meat is not safety3.1351.232
R4I believe that cultured meat is not natural.2.8461.109
Note: The values of variables Pro1–Pro4, Pre1–Pre4, FN1–FN4, FI1–FI5, and MB1–MB10 range from 1–5 (1 = totally not agree, 2 = not agree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree); values of variables FT1–FT3 range from 1–5 (1 = never, 2 = not support, 3 = neutral, 4 = support, 5 = strongly support); values of variables MCB1-MCB5 range from 1–5 (1 = never, 2 = seldom consider, 3 = occasionally consider, 4 = usually consider, 5 = always consider). “*” denotes that such variables yield reverse score, for example, if someone totally not agree “I always eat all kinds of novel food”, the score of this question is 5 instead of 1.
Table A4. Frequency distributions of respondents’ acceptance toward artificial meat across information groups.
Table A4. Frequency distributions of respondents’ acceptance toward artificial meat across information groups.
Artificial MeatVariablesTotally Not AgreeNot AgreeNeutralAgreeStrongly Agree
Panel A Control group (Neutral information group, 1257 participants)
Plant-based meatPlant-based meat is expected to be a major trend in the future.2.1510.2632.3441.1714.08
I am receptive to technologies based on plant-based meat.2.159.1928.1643.4417.06
I’m curious to see how plant-based meat tastes.2.865.9719.9349.2821.96
I’d like to substitute plant-based meat for regular meat.8.0025.3036.63.21.728.72
Clean meatcultured meat is expected to be a major trend in the future.2.6312.2930.4339.7414.92
I am receptive to technologies based on cultured meat.2.7413.1330.5537.8315.75
I’m curious to see how cultured meat tastes.3.469.9025.6642.7218.26
I’d like to substitute cultured meat for regular meat.12.8925.0633.4119.938.71
Panel B Treatment group I (gain-oriented information group, 1064 participants)
Plant-based meatPlant-based meat is a future trend1.308.6726.1646.5317.34
I accept plant-based meat technologies1.166.7926.4544.9420.66
I would like to try plant-based meat1.165.4915.7551.7325.87
I would like to eat plant-based meat6.9417.4934.9729.6210.98
instead of regular meat
Clean meatClean meat is a future trend2.028.8228.6141.9118.64
I accept clean meat technologies2.3110.4030.2040.9016.18
I would like to try clean meat2.607.9523.2744.5121.68
I would like to eat clean meat6.9423.4132.6625.4311.56
instead of regular meat
Panel C Treatment group II (avoidance-oriented information group, 1120 participants)
Plant-based meatPlant-based meat is a future trend1.229.9227.8547.0113.99
I accept plant-based meat technologies1.639.2425.2742.6621.20
I would like to try plant-based meat1.495.4321.2046.3325.54
I would like to eat plant-based meat6.5218.4836.9626.3611.68
instead of regular meat
Clean meatClean meat is a future trend1.497.0730.8446.0614.54
I accept clean meat technologies2.0410.8727.9940.2218.89
I would like to try clean meat2.729.1025.8244.2918.07
I would like to eat clean meat8.8322.5533.0223.9111.68
instead of regular meat
Note: Figures in the table above are percentage values.

References

  1. McCarthy, W.J.; Li, Z. Healthy Diets and Sustainable Food Systems. Lancet 2019, 394, 214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Clark, M.; Tilman, D. Comparative Analysis of Environmental Impacts of Agricultural Production Systems, Agricultural Input Efficiency, and Food Choice. Environ. Res. Lett. 2017, 12, 064016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bellarby, J.; Tirado, R.; Leip, A.; Weiss, F.; Lesschen, J.P.; Smith, P. Livestock Greenhouse Gas Emissions and Mitigation Potential in Europe. Glob. Chang. Biol. 2012, 19, 3–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Fox, J.L. Test Tube Meat on the Menu? Nat. Biotechnol. 2009, 27, 873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Orzechowski, A. Artificial Meat? Feasible Approach Based on the Experience from Cell Culture Studies. J. Integr. Agric. 2015, 14, 217–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bonny, S.P.F.; Gardner, G.E.; Pethick, D.W.; Hocquette, J.-F. Artificial Meat and the Future of the Meat Industry. Anim. Prod. Sci. 2017, 57, 2216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Stephens, N.; Di Silvio, L.; Dunsford, I.; Ellis, M.; Glencross, A.; Sexton, A. Bringing Cultured Meat to Market: Technical, Socio-Political, and Regulatory Challenges in Cellular Agriculture. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2018, 78, 155–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bhat, Z.F.; Morton, J.D.; Mason, S.L.; Bekhit, A.E.A.; Bhat, H.F. Technological, Regulatory, and Ethical Aspects of In Vitro Meat: A Future Slaughter-Free Harvest. Compr. Rev. Food Sci. Food Saf. 2019, 18, 1192–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. van Vliet, S.; Kronberg, S.L.; Provenza, F.D. Plant-Based Meats, Human Health, and Climate Change. Front. Sustain. Food Syst. 2020, 4, 128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bonnet, C.; Bouamra-Mechemache, Z.; Réquillart, V.; Treich, N. Viewpoint: Regulating Meat Consumption to Improve Health, the Environment and Animal Welfare. Food Policy 2020, 97, 101847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Munteanu, C.; Mireşan, V.; Răducu, C.; Ihuţ, A.; Uiuiu, P.; Pop, D.; Neacşu, A.; Cenariu, M.; Groza, I. Can Cultured Meat Be an Alternative to Farm Animal Production for a Sustainable and Healthier Lifestyle? Front. Nutr. 2021, 8, 749298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Van Eenennaam, A.L.; Werth, S.J. Animal Board Invited Review: Animal Agriculture and Alternative Meats—Learning from Past Science Communication Failures. Animal 2021, 15, 100360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Asioli, D.; Bazzani, C.; Nayga, R.M. Are Consumers Willing to Pay for In-vitro Meat? An Investigation of Naming Effects. J. Agric. Econ. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Choudhury, D.; Singh, S.; Seah, J.S.H.; Yeo, D.C.L.; Tan, L.P. Commercialization of Plant-Based Meat Alternatives. Trends Plant Sci. 2020, 25, 1055–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mancini, M.C.; Antonioli, F. Exploring Consumers’ Attitude towards Cultured Meat in Italy. Meat Sci. 2019, 150, 101–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bryant, C.J.; Barnett, J.C. What’s in a Name? Consumer Perceptions of in Vitro Meat under Different Names. Appetite 2019, 137, 104–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Bekker, G.A.; Fischer, A.R.H.; Tobi, H.; van Trijp, H.C.M. Explicit and Implicit Attitude toward an Emerging Food Technology: The Case of Cultured Meat. Appetite 2017, 108, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hocquette, A.; Lambert, C.; Sinquin, C.; Peterolff, L.; Wagner, Z.; Bonny, S.P.F.; Lebert, A.; Hocquette, J.-F. Educated Consumers Don’t Believe Artificial Meat Is the Solution to the Problems with the Meat Industry. J. Integr. Agric. 2015, 14, 273–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Slade, P. If You Build It, Will They Eat It? Consumer Preferences for Plant-Based and Cultured Meat Burgers. Appetite 2018, 125, 428–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Palmieri, N.; Perito, M.A.; Lupi, C. Consumer Acceptance of Cultured Meat: Some Hints from Italy. Br. Food J. 2020, 123, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Recuero-Virto, N.; Valilla-Arróspide, C. Forecasting the next Revolution: Food Technology’s Impact on Consumers’ Acceptance and Satisfaction. Br. Food J. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Conrey, F.R.; Smith, E.R. Attitude Representation: Attitudes as Patterns in a Distributed, Connectionist Representational System. Soc. Cogn. 2007, 25, 718–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Fazio, R.H. Attitudes as Object–Evaluation Associations of Varying Strength. Soc. Cogn. 2007, 25, 603–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Britwum, K.; Yiannaka, A. Shaping Food Safety Perceptions: The Influence of Informational Nudges. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 2019, 81, 139–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Britwum, K.; Yiannaka, A. Consumer Willingness to Pay for Food Safety Interventions: The Role of Message Framing and Issue Involvement. Food Policy 2019, 86, 101726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica 1979, 47, 263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Pichierri, M.; Pino, G.; Peluso, A.M.; Guido, G. The Interplay between Health Claim Type and Individual Regulatory Focus in Determining Consumers’ Intentions toward Extra-Virgin Olive Oil. Food Res. Int. 2020, 136, 109467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Mesías, F.J.; Martín, A.; Hernández, A. Consumers’ Growing Appetite for Natural Foods: Perceptions towards the Use of Natural Preservatives in Fresh Fruit. Food Res. Int. 2021, 150, 110749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Demsar, V.; Sands, S.; Rosengren, S.; Campbell, C. Ad Creativity in a Negative Context: How a Thanking Message Frame Enhances Purchase Intention in Times of Crisis. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2022, 64, 102825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Wang, J.; Lee, A.Y. The Role of Regulatory Focus in Preference Construction. J. Mark. Res. 2006, 43, 28–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Higgins, E.T. Beyond Pleasure and Pain. Am. Psychol. 1997, 52, 1280–1300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Lee, A.Y.; Aaker, J.L. Bringing the Frame Into Focus: The Influence of Regulatory Fit on Processing Fluency and Persuasion. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 86, 205–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Cesario, J.; Grant, H.; Higgins, E.T. Regulatory Fit and Persuasion: Transfer from “Feeling Right”. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 86, 388–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Zhou, R.; Pham, M.T. Promotion and Prevention across Mental Accounts: When Financial Products Dictate Consumers& Investment Goals. J. Consum. Res. 2004, 31, 125–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Grewal, D.; Monroe, K.B.; Krishnan, R. The Effects of Price-Comparison Advertising on Buyers’ Perceptions of Acquisition Value, Transaction Value, and Behavioral Intentions. J. Mark. 1998, 62, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Forsythe, S.; Liu, C.; Shannon, D.; Gardner, L.C. Development of a Scale to Measure the Perceived Benefits and Risks of Online Shopping. J. Interact. Mark. 2006, 20, 55–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lee, S.H. New Measuring Stick on Sharing Accommodation: Guest-Perceived Benefits and Risks. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2020, 87, 102471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Menon, S.; Kahn, B. Cross-Category Effects of Induced Arousal and Pleasure on the Internet Shopping Experience. J. Retail. 2002, 78, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Childers, T.L.; Carr, C.L.; Peck, J.; Carson, S. Hedonic and Utilitarian Motivations for Online Retail Shopping Behavior. J. Retail. 2001, 77, 511–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Zhang, X.; Bai, X.; Shang, J. Is Subsidized Electric Vehicles Adoption Sustainable: Consumers’ Perceptions and Motivation toward Incentive Policies, Environmental Benefits, and Risks. J. Clean. Prod. 2018, 192, 71–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Venkatraman, M.P. The Impact of Innovativeness and Innovation Type on Adoption. J. Retail. 1991, 67, 51–67. [Google Scholar]
  42. Bettman, J.R. Perceived Risk and Its Components: A Model and Empirical Test. J. Mark. Res. 1973, 10, 184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Taylor, J.W. The Role of Risk in Consumer Behavior: A Comprehensive and Operational Theory of Risk Taking in Consumer Behavior. J. Mark. 1974, 38, 54–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Kaplan, L.B.; Szybillo, G.J.; Jacoby, J. Components of Perceived Risk in Product Purchase: A Cross-Validation. J. Appl. Psychol. 1974, 59, 287–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Cox, D.F. Risk Taking and Information Handling in Consumer Behavior; Division of Research, Graduate School of Business Administration, Harvard University: Cambridge, MA, USA; Reed Business Education: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1967. [Google Scholar]
  46. Bart, Y.; Shankar, V.; Sultan, F.; Urban, G.L. Are the Drivers and Role of Online Trust the Same for All Web Sites and Consumers? A Large-Scale Exploratory Empirical Study. J. Mark. 2005, 69, 133–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Laroche, M.; McDougall, G.H.G.; Bergeron, J.; Yang, Z. Exploring How Intangibility Affects Perceived Risk. J. Serv. Res. 2004, 6, 373–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Murray, K.B.; Schlacter, J.L. The Impact of Services versus Goods on Consumers’ Assessment of Perceived Risk and Variability. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 1990, 18, 51–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Nazzaro, C.; Lerro, M.; Stanco, M.; Marotta, G. Do Consumers like Food Product Innovation? An Analysis of Willingness to Pay for Innovative Food Attributes. Br. Food J. 2019, 121, 1413–1427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Miles, S.; Frewer, L.J. Investigating Specific Concerns about Different Food Hazards. Food Qual. Prefer. 2001, 12, 47–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Guerrero, L.; Guàrdia, M.D.; Xicola, J.; Verbeke, W.; Vanhonacker, F.; Zakowska-Biemans, S.; Sajdakowska, M.; Sulmont-Rossé, C.; Issanchou, S.; Contel, M.; et al. Consumer-Driven Definition of Traditional Food Products and Innovation in Traditional Foods. A Qualitative Cross-Cultural Study. Appetite 2009, 52, 345–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Cui, H.-J.; Fam, K.-S.; Zhao, T.-Y. Regulatory Focus and Message Framing’s Effects on Intention to Consume Ethnic Food in China. Br. Food J. 2019, 122, 1969–1982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Siegrist, M.; Hartmann, C. Consumer Acceptance of Novel Food Technologies. Nat. Food 2020, 1, 343–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Choi, J. Who Cares for Nutrition Information at a Restaurant? Food-Related Lifestyles and Their Association to Nutrition Information Conscious Behaviors. Br. Food J. 2016, 118, 1625–1640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Song, X.; Bredahl, L.; Diaz Navarro, M.; Pendenza, P.; Stojacic, I.; Mincione, S.; Pellegrini, G.; Schlüter, O.K.; Torrieri, E.; Di Monaco, R.; et al. Factors Affecting Consumer Choice of Novel Non-Thermally Processed Fruit and Vegetables Products: Evidence from a 4-Country Study in Europe. Food Res. Int. 2022, 153, 110975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Jaeger, S.R.; Lee, P.-Y.; Ares, G. Product Involvement and Consumer Food-Elicited Emotional Associations: Insights from Emoji Questionnaires. Food Res. Int. 2018, 106, 999–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Zhao, X.; Namasivayam, K. The Relationship of Chronic Regulatory Focus to Work–Family Conflict and Job Satisfaction. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2012, 31, 458–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The Structural equation modeling.
Figure 1. The Structural equation modeling.
Ijerph 19 04948 g001
Figure 2. Distribution of consumer acceptance attitudes for each information group.
Figure 2. Distribution of consumer acceptance attitudes for each information group.
Ijerph 19 04948 g002
Table 1. Balance check.
Table 1. Balance check.
CharacteristicNeutral Message Group (838)Gain-Oriented Message Group (692)Avoidance-Oriented Message Group (736) F
Meat belief—health3.980
(0.684)
3.950
(0.692)
3.953
(0.713)
0.43
Meat belief—animal welfare3.060
(0.761)
3.103
(0.764)
3.105
(0.786)
0.88
Meat belief—environment3.113
(0.605)
3.097
(0.638)
3.127
(0.644)
0.41
Meat belief—enjoyment3.968
(0.694)
3.985
(0.686)
3.981
(0.669)
0.13
Consider price3.862
(0.878)
3.902
(0.848)
3.887
(0.894)
0.42
Consider flavor4.195
(0.807)
4.197
(0.822)
4.136
(0.819)
1.33
Consider health4.229
(0.960)
4.233
(0.939)
4.189
(0.915)
0.50
Consider environment3.187
(1.009)
3.182
(1.069)
3.110
(1.041)
1.30
Consider animal welfare2.479
(1.036)
2.539
(1.040)
2.447
(1.065)
1.42
Food involvement3.273
(0.654)
3.253
(0.666)
3.307
(0.627)
1.26
Food tech attitude2.642
(0.754)
2.674
(0.756)
2.697
(0.741)
1.07
Food neophobia2.771
(0.818)
2.757
(0.794)
2.742
(0.819)
0.25
Standard errors in parentheses.
Table 2. The structural model of plant-based meat (standardized coefficients).
Table 2. The structural model of plant-based meat (standardized coefficients).
BenefitRiskAttitude
Panel A: Neutral message group
Pro0.279 ***−0.239 ***
(0.0292)(0.0357)
Pre−0.123 ***0.147 ***
(0.0253)(0.0162)
Benefit 0.588 ***
(0.0347)
Risk −0.426 ***
(0.0313)
Panel B: Gain-oriented message group
Pro0.561 ***−0.475 ***
(0.0203)(0.0198)
Pre−0.113 ***0.108 ***
(0.0107)(0.0113)
Benefit 0.421 ***
(0.0407)
Risk −0.518 ***
(0.0337)
Panel C: Avoidance-oriented message group
Pro0.286 ***−0.263 ***
(0.0399)(0.0401)
Pre−0.0924 ***−0.0843 ***
(0.0211)(0.0227)
Benefit 0.430 ***
(0.0383)
Risk −0.503 ***
(0.0331)
Standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01.
Table 3. The structural model of cultured meat (standardized coefficients).
Table 3. The structural model of cultured meat (standardized coefficients).
BenefitRiskAttitude
Panel A: Neutral message group
Pro0.163 ***−0.145 ***
(0.00908)(0.0145)
Pre−0.289 ***0.255 ***
(0.0509)(0.0423)
Benefit 0.583 ***
(0.0284)
Risk −0.493 ***
(0.0268)
Panel B: Gain-oriented message group
Pro0.211 ***−0.218 ***
(0.0152)(0.0356)
Pre−0.231 ***0.233 ***
(0.0587)(0.0590)
Benefit 0.576 ***
(0.0321)
Risk −0.549 ***
(0.029)
Panel C: Avoidance-oriented message group
Pro0.141 ***−0.103 ***
(0.0318)(0.0218)
Pre−0.0611 ***0.0739 ***
(0.0237)(0.0216)
Benefit 0.404 ***
(0.0346)
Risk −0.541 ***
(0.0294)
Standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01.
Table 4. Group-level goodness of fit.
Table 4. Group-level goodness of fit.
Plant-Based MeatCultured Meat
SRMR0.0910.093
CD0.9400.963
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Shi, H.; Ma, P.; Zeng, Y.; Sheng, J. Understanding the Interaction between Regulatory Focus and Message Framing in Determining Chinese Consumers’ Attitudes toward Artificial Meat. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 4948. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19094948

AMA Style

Shi H, Ma P, Zeng Y, Sheng J. Understanding the Interaction between Regulatory Focus and Message Framing in Determining Chinese Consumers’ Attitudes toward Artificial Meat. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(9):4948. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19094948

Chicago/Turabian Style

Shi, Hongxu, Peihua Ma, Yinchu Zeng, and Jiping Sheng. 2022. "Understanding the Interaction between Regulatory Focus and Message Framing in Determining Chinese Consumers’ Attitudes toward Artificial Meat" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 9: 4948. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19094948

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop