Next Article in Journal
Recovery-Oriented Cross-Sectoral Network Meetings between Mental Health Hospital Professionals and Community Mental Health Professionals: A Critical Discourse Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Risk Analysis of Viral Diseases in Infected Pig Farms during the Lockdown Period in China, January to May 2020
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Happiness and Economic Status among Older Adults in Two Myanmar Regions

1
Department of International Health and Collaboration, National Institute of Public Health, Wako City 351-0197, Japan
2
Graduate School of Medical and Dental Sciences, Niigata University, Niigata City 951-8510, Japan
3
Bureau of International Health Cooperation, National Center for Global Health and Medicine, Tokyo 162-8655, Japan
4
Department of Health and Social Behavior, Graduate School of Medicine, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo 113-0033, Japan
5
Department of Family Medicine, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo 113-8510, Japan
6
Department of Global Health and Population, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, MA 02115, USA
7
Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, University of Medicine 1, Yangon 245, Myanmar
8
National Institute of Public Health, Wako City 351-0197, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(6), 3216; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063216
Submission received: 4 January 2022 / Revised: 4 March 2022 / Accepted: 7 March 2022 / Published: 9 March 2022

Abstract

:
Few studies have examined whether objective or subjective economic status (ES) has a greater association with the happiness of older adults, despite concerns regarding the growing economic cost of morbidity and their functional dependence in developing countries with aging populations. Thus, this study examined whether objective/subjective ES was associated with happiness in older adults in two Myanmar regions. A multistage random sampling procedure and face-to-face interviews were conducted in the urban and rural areas of Myanmar. The happiness of 1200 participants aged >60 years was evaluated using a single happiness score ranging from 0 (very unhappy) to 10 (very happy). The wealth index, used as an objective ES, was calculated from 17 household asset items, such as radio, washing machines, and television. Subjective ES was assessed by asking “Which of the following best describes your current financial situation in light of general economic conditions?” Responses ranged from “very difficult” to “very comfortable”. Both low objective and subjective ES were negatively associated with happiness, after adjusting for confounding variables and stratification by region (urban and rural areas). Although objective and subjective ES had similar associations with happiness in urban areas, subjective ES had a stronger association in rural areas.

1. Introduction

There is a growing interest in the role of well-being in improving a population’s health. The European Commission argued that current measures of economic performance, such as gross domestic product, are insufficient indicators of a society’s progress and that personal well-being should also be considered [1,2]. Furthermore, it has been argued that psychological well-being should be addressed in measures of health valuation and considered in healthcare resource allocation [1,3].
Happiness, defined as “the overall appreciation of one’s life-as-a-whole,” is a key marker of psychological well-being [4] and is closely related to health [1]. Many studies have reported that happiness is associated with better health outcomes such as lower mortality, reduced morbidity, and functional independence in both community-dwelling and clinical populations [5,6,7,8,9,10,11]. The association between happiness and health may become more important with age, as the prevalence of chronic illnesses increases with it [1].
Several physiological mechanisms may explain these findings [12]. Happiness appears to be inversely related to perceived stress [13] and may protect against illness by improving immune responses [4]. Happy people typically enjoy better health outcomes because they demonstrate more successful adaptation; better problem-solving skills and coping strategies; more creative, imaginative, and integrative thinking; greater resilience; and a stronger ability to deal with adversity [8,14,15].
Identifying the factors of happiness is not only important for improving personal well-being, but also for gaining insight into the factors associated with health and longevity. The recognition of these factors may lead to an elevated health status, increased life expectancy, and lower medical expenses [16]. Literature suggests that happiness is associated with having high self-perceived health [15,17], health insurance coverage [18,19], social support [15], living partners [15,20,21,22] (or not living alone) [23], lower stress [21], male sex [15,24], longevity [1,12,24], absence of depression [23], and smoking history [24].
Objective economic status (ES) is also correlated with happiness. For example, people with high ES, as measured by objective indicators such as household income, are more likely to be happier than people with low ES [15,16,17,20,21,22,25,26,27]. The results of a systematic narrative review of 71 studies demonstrated that poorer well-being was associated with objective ES, which included income, wealth, and financial assets [28]. In contrast, income was strongly associated with satisfaction, but its association with happiness was weaker in 15 countries [29]. Subjective ES (i.e., perceptions of having a higher ES or better financial status) may also be important in this context [26,30,31,32]. Studies that have examined the role of subjective socioeconomic status (SES) have provided valuable comprehensive knowledge on SES–wellbeing/health connection [33]. For example, a study in five sub-Saharan countries showed that people who were moderately to completely satisfied with their financial situation were significantly more likely to report richer health and happiness than those who were completely dissatisfied [31]. Another study demonstrated that subjective SES, compared to objective SES, is a stronger predictor of psychological functioning indicators among healthy white women [34].
In Myanmar, the proportion of the population aged ≥60 years will almost double to 15% between 2010 and 2030 and is projected to reach 25% of the overall population by 2050 [35]. The implications of the increasing rate of functional dependence in Myanmar are expected to be significant, as effective medical care systems are still in their developmental stages. In addition, depression and anxiety, which are negatively associated with psychological well-being, account for 5% of disability-adjusted life years and are among the top 10 contributors to disability in Myanmar [36,37]. However, no public health organizations in Myanmar have implemented psychological health care policies, to the best of our knowledge. Consequently, psychological health services are not prioritized in primary health care, thus preventing thousands of people from accessing the services they need [36,38]. Several health organizations provide community-based healthcare services, even in remote areas, and seek to coordinate health service provision with the central healthcare system [39]. Religious organizations are also involved in service provision, and their role is gaining importance with the increasing need for collaborative action in health care [40]. However, effective medical care systems, including mental health services, remain underdeveloped. Myanmar has been under military control for several years, and national health investment has been extremely low during this period [41,42]; it resumed in 2021.
There are few published studies on the mental health of older adults in Myanmar, owing to the country’s international isolation. Although we have investigated the association between depressive symptoms and objective/subjective ES in a previous study [38], happiness and depressive symptoms are not completely contradictory concepts: happiness does not mean exhibiting non-depressive symptoms [43]. Therefore, it is crucial to examine the factors associated with happiness among older adults in Myanmar, as happiness can buffer psychological and morbidity risks and functional dependence. Furthermore, to the best of our knowledge, few studies have examined whether objective or subjective ES has a greater association with the happiness of older adults, despite concerns about the growing economic cost of morbidity and functional dependence of older adults in developing countries with ageing populations [44].
This study aimed to examine (1) the level of happiness, (2) whether objective/subjective ES is associated with happiness, and (3) differences in this association between rural and urban areas among older adults in two regions of Myanmar.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Participants

This was a baseline survey of the 2018 longitudinal study on healthy and active aging in Myanmar, which examined the predictors of physical and psychological health in community-dwelling Myanmar adults aged ≥60 years. A follow-up survey is currently ongoing.
The field sites were the Yangon and Bago regions, located 91 km northeast of Yangon. Multistage random sampling was conducted in both regions. There are 45 townships in the Yangon region and 28 in the Bago region. First, six townships were randomly selected from each region via proportional population sampling based on the population of each township. Subsequently, in Yangon, 10 wards were randomly selected from each township, while in Bago, 10 village tracts were selected from each township based on the population of each township/village tract. Finally, 10 people were randomly selected from each extracted ward/village tract using the ledger lists of residents aged ≥60 years. In rural areas, multiple villages exist within a single village tract. In such cases, one village was randomly selected from the village tract.
The differences between the wards and village tracts are related to the degree of urbanization: urban areas were defined as wards, and rural areas were defined as village tracts or villages. Wards and village tracts sometimes coexist within townships. In the survey, only wards from townships in the Yangon region and only village tracts from those in the Bago region were selected to capture the characteristics of urban and rural areas from each region. The present study considered the Yangon region representative of urban areas and the Bago region representative of rural areas [38].
The sample size was calculated using a method developed by the World Health Organization [45]. We arrive at an ideal sample size of approximately 1200, with 600 sampled from urban areas and 600 from rural areas with a 100-sample margin in respective areas. The study does not adjust for a certain anticipated response rate because the authors plan to continue collecting samples up to the target number. Further details are provided in the cohort profile of the study [46].
Trained surveyors visited homes with public health nurses from each community to conduct face-to-face interviews with study participants. In Yangon, the surveyors visited 1083 older adults, 610 of whom were at home. Ten were excluded due to refusal to participate in the survey (n = 6), severe dementia, or being bedridden (n = 4). The response rate was 98.4% in Yangon. In Bago, surveyors visited 1044 older adults, of whom 694 were at home. Ninety-four patients were excluded because of severe dementia or being bedridden; thus, the response rate was 86.5% in Bago. A total of 600 people each from the Yangon (222 men and 378 women) and Bago regions (261 men and 339 women) were surveyed (Figure 1).

2.2. Study Tools

A structured questionnaire for face-to-face interviews was developed for this study, following the Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study (JAGES), which is a nationwide, population-based, prospective cohort study of older community-dwelling Japanese adults [47]. The linguistic translation and validation process followed the Linguistic Validation Manual for Health Outcome Assessments [48]. The scale was first translated into English. Thereafter, it was translated into the local language and back-translated into English to ensure clarity and consistency.
Research staff was hired from the Myanmar Perfect Research Company, a group that conducts epidemiological surveys. The interviewers were recruited from a company. Before the commencement of the actual survey, a two-day training course on the research protocol, administration of the questionnaire, and ethical concerns was conducted for the interviewers.
A small pilot study was conducted before the actual survey for face validity at the Urban Health Center, Dagon Township, Yangon. Participants were adults aged >60 years who visited the outpatient clinic in the center. Twenty-five respondents were recruited who provided their consent to participate in the pilot study. During the pilot study, interviewers ensured the sequence, flow, and clarity of the study. After receiving feedback from the interviewers, the questionnaire was revised accordingly.
The inclusion criteria were an age ≥60 years and residence in a selected ward or village. The exclusion criteria were being bedridden and having severe dementia. Severe dementia was defined as an Abbreviated Mental Test score ≤ 6 [49,50].

2.3. Dependent Variable

Previous studies have generally measured subjective well-being using a single question such as “Taking all things together, would you say that you are very happy, pretty happy, not too happy, or not happy at all?” [16,51,52,53]. It has been shown that single-item measures of subjective well-being have moderate reliability. Happiness was assessed through the following question, which was previously validated [16,54]: “How do you rate your overall happiness level on a score of 0 for very unhappy to a score of 10 for very happy?” This scale was employed because it was the present study’s sole measure of self-perceived happiness.

2.4. Independent Variables

The wealth index, used as an objective economic indicator, was calculated from household asset items (radio, washing machine, TV, electric rice cooker, video/DVD player, air conditioner, electric fan, bicycle, refrigerator, motorcycle, computer, car/truck, store-bought furniture, microwave oven, personal music player, mobile phone, and Internet). The principal component score was calculated based on the participants’ possession of each item and used as the wealth index, following the method described in a previous report [55]. Subjective ES was assessed by asking the following question: “Which of the following best describes your current financial situation in light of general economic conditions?” The possible responses were (1) very difficult, (2) difficult, (3) average, (4) comfortable, and (5) very comfortable. Based on their responses, participants were categorized as “difficult or very difficult” (answering 1 or 2) or “average or higher” (answering 3–5).

2.5. Confounding Variables

The sociodemographic characteristics that were statistically significant with happiness scores in the univariate analysis included information regarding sex (male or female), subjective health status (excellent/good or fair/poor), illness during the preceding year (no or yes), depressive symptoms (geriatric depression scale [GDS] ≥5 or <5), educational level (no school, monastic, some/all primary school, middle/high school or higher), residential area (Yangon or Bago regions), marital status (married, widow/divorced/never married), living status (alone or not), and frequency of visits to religious facilities (less than once per week, once per week, or more). In addition, variables that were significantly related to happiness in previous studies [56,57,58] with similar analyses using similar outcomes were included: sex (male or female), social support (giving and receiving emotional and instrumental help), and religion (Buddhism or other).

2.6. Statistical Analysis

The mean happiness scores of the sociodemographic variables categorized as above were compared using a one-way analysis of variance test. The dependent variable (happiness level) was a categorical variable for which there was a clear ordering of the category levels, therefore, ordinal logistic regression analyses were performed to identify the factors associated with happiness. Variables with a variance inflation factor of 5 (indicating multicollinearity) were excluded. The remaining variables are included in the model. Univariate and multivariate adjusted results were expressed as the odds ratio (ORs) and adjusted odds ratio (AOR) with 95% confidence interval (CI). STATA 14 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA) was used to perform all statistical analyses, and the statistical significance level was set at p < 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Characteristics of Happy and Unhappy Respondents

Table 1 shows the happiness scores for each socio-demographic variable. For 1200 respondents, the mean happiness score was 6.58 (±2.01). Regarding ES, respondents with low objective ES had significantly lower happiness scores than those with middle/high objective ES (6.24 points versus 6.80 points, p < 0.001). Similarly, respondents with low subjective ES had significantly lower scores than those with average or higher subjective ES (5.62 points versus 6.83 points, p < 0.001). For more details, see Table 1.

3.2. Associations between Objective/Subjective ES and Happiness

Of the 1182 participants analyzed for multivariate analysis (Yangon: n = 591; Bago: n = 591), low objective and subjective ES were both negatively associated with happiness even after adjusting for confounding variables (AOR: 0.69, 95% CI: 0.52, 0.91; AOR: 0.46, 95% CI: 0.35, 0.62, respectively) (Table 2). After being stratified into an urban (Yangon) and a rural area (Bago), low objective and subjective ES were both still statistically or marginally associated with happiness in the urban and rural areas, even after adjusting for the confounding variables (for low objective ES in urban areas: AOR: 0.54, 95% CI: 0.32, 0.93; for low subjective ES in urban areas: AOR: 0.55, 95% CI: 0.34, 0.91; for low objective ES in rural areas: AOR: 0.73, 95% CI: 0.53, 1.01; and for low subjective ES in rural areas: AOR: 0.43, 95% CI: 0.30, 0.62).

4. Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate whether objective or subjective ES is associated with happiness using data from urban and rural areas in Myanmar. Project data from 2018 indicated that the mean happiness score was estimated to be 6.58 (±2.01) points. Overall, the model with adjusted potential confounding factors suggested that older adults with low objective ES were more likely to have lower happiness scores than those with average or higher objective ES, and the association was similar for low subjective ES.
To compare the happiness scores among other Asian populations, the mean happiness score of the participants of the JAGES was calculated [47]; the mean happiness score for adults aged ≥65 years was estimated to be 7.22 (±1.93) points in the JAGES study (n = 180,324). Although the mean happiness score in this study cannot be directly compared with that of the previous study due to differences in sample sizes and age ranges, the mean happiness score in Myanmar may be relatively lower than that in Japan. This may be related to Myanmar’s relatively lower SES and higher prevalence of poverty. According to the Myanmar Living Conditions Survey from 2017, which is a different survey from the 2018 longitudinal study “Healthy and Active Aging in Myanmar,” one in four people perceived themselves as poor, and another 32% were just above the poverty line, facing the risk of falling into the poverty trap in the event of adversity [59,60]. In addition, it might be related to the relatively higher prevalence of depressive symptoms in Myanmar (22.2%) compared with the median prevalence rate of depressive symptoms among adults aged ≥60 years worldwide (10.3%) [61,62].
In this study, both low objective and subjective ES were significantly or marginally associated with a lower happiness score, even after adjusting for confounding factors (Table 2). The reason why older adults with low objective/subjective ES are less likely to be happy than those with average/high objective/subjective ES in Myanmar can be speculated. A previous systematic review and meta-analysis found a seemingly consistent and statistically significant increase in the odds of cancer, angina, asthma, depression, and comorbidity prevalence when comparing low-objective ES with middle/high-objective ES [63,64]. Similarly, the risk of coronary artery disease, hypertension, diabetes, and dyslipidemia was higher when comparing low and high subjective social statuses [65]. Consistent with previous studies, in this study, low objective/subjective ES was potentially associated with physical health due to poverty, which is associated with a lack of public services such as education, health services, access to clean water, sanitation, and clean fuel. This exacerbates the vulnerability of poor people and their perception of status differentiation, which can lead to low happiness. Although we adjusted for self-rated health, health disparities due to differences in objective/subjective ES may be associated with happiness in older adults in Myanmar. Even in Japan, which is considered an egalitarian society with relatively few inequalities in health, subjective ES was negatively associated with happiness, and objective ES attenuated this association [26].
Although a previous study indicated that low subjective ES could directly increase stress or increase vulnerability to the effects of stress more than low objective ES [34], to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to reveal that this association is more pronounced in rural areas. One of the reasons may be its association with relative deprivation (RD). Previous studies have offered the RD hypothesis as a possible explanation for the damaging implications of inequalities in health [66,67,68,69,70]. The idea behind this hypothesis is that an individual’s health or health-related behavior is determined not only by their own resources (such as income or educational attainment), but also by their relative position in reference to those resources (i.e., how much others have versus how much they have), and by the distribution of income within society [68,70]. Subjective ES might be more strongly linked to RD than objective ES, especially in rural areas, since rural areas tend to be small and strongly united [71,72], making it easier to see the economic situation of people in the same community. Even if older adults are objectively poor, they may feel happy if there is little RD in their society. In contrast, it might be harder for them to be happy if they have high neighborhood social cohesion and feel poorer than others in rural areas.
Moreover, Myanmar is classified as a country with a critical shortage of health workers, which jeopardizes access to health services, resulting in poorer health status in people in hard-to-reach places [73]. This could be particularly detrimental for older adults whose healthcare needs are high [74]. Despite ongoing changes in rural Myanmar, a considerable rural-urban gap not only in human resources (education, knowledge, and skills), but also in access to material (land, farms, and savings) and social resources (trust-based bonds) could be associated with residents’ happiness and the psychological process of coping with chronic diseases [75]. In this context, it is possible that subjective ES was more likely to be associated with happiness than objective ES among older adults living in rural areas, although possible confounding variables were adjusted for.
The strength of this study is that, to the best of our knowledge, it is the first study to investigate happiness focusing on objective/subjective ES among older adults in Myanmar. In addition, it reported the current situation of older adults after significant circumstantial changes in Myanmar. Previous studies explored the sociodemographic status of older adults in Myanmar using survey data from 2012 [76,77,78,79] and from the national census conducted in 2014 [80]. However, the changes due to Myanmar’s democratization have occurred after surveys. Since Myanmar became a stratocracy again, this survey was unique, and no other data were available for analysis.
The findings of this study should be interpreted within the context of several limitations. First, this analysis adopted a cross-sectional design. Therefore, it may include potential confounding factors or reverse causation: causal relationships between objective/subjective ES and happiness could not be determined. We plan to employ a cohort study approach to explore potential causality. Second, the happiness and subjective ES measures were based on self-reports and a single item. Therefore, although this measure has been commonly used in previous studies and has been shown to be moderately reliable [16,38,51,52,53], the results do not necessarily translate into clinical significance for psychological health. Objective and subjective multi-item and well-established measures of happiness and ES should be considered in future studies [16]. However, the mitigation of the association of low objective/subjective ES on happiness (AOR: 0.69 and 0.46, respectively) was comparable to the aggravation of the association of low self-rated health and depressive symptoms (AOR: 0.70 and 0.37, respectively; data not shown), which has exceptional predictive validity with respect to mortality and morbidity [81,82,83]. Third, it is unknown whether these findings can be generalized beyond the Yangon and Bago regions of Myanmar. People living in the Bago region may enjoy better access to urban areas and health facilities than those living in rural areas further away from the Yangon region. Therefore, it is difficult to generalize the findings to populations in areas with limited access to urban areas and health services. However, the happiness and ES of older adults can be estimated in other regions by using the level of regional urbanization. This social epidemiological survey should be extended to include surrounding regions and states throughout the country in the future [38,84]. Finally, the survey sample excluded those who were bedridden or who had severe dementia. Assuming that the happiness of these people was lower (due to their poor health) [16], happiness may have been overestimated in the analysis, and the findings may not depict the whole picture of older adults in Myanmar.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, both objective and subjective ES may be associated with happiness among older adults in Myanmar. The associations remained significant or marginally significant in both urban (Yangon) and rural (Bago) areas, even after they were stratified by region. However, subjective, rather than objective, ES had a stronger association with happiness in rural areas.

6. Implications and Recommendations

In addition to material wealth, subjective ES may be associated with better psychological health among older adults in Myanmar. Intervention programs should use a wider range of strategies to address the economic disparities between rural and urban areas and within communities. Regional differences in objective and subjective ESs should also be considered. Future studies should examine the effectiveness of increasing happiness levels through specific interventions that consider regional differences. As population aging is a growing issue in Myanmar, it is necessary to look beyond merely ameliorating illnesses and implementing changes that enable longevity with good physical and psychological health and happiness.

Author Contributions

Y.S. (Yuri Sasaki) analyzed the data and produced the first draft of the manuscript. Y.S. (Yugo Shobugawa), I.N., D.T., Y.N., M.F., Y.S. (Yuki Shirakura), Y.C., K.T.L., P.E.Z., T.Z.B., T.S. and H.H.W. collected the data and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript, and Sasaki was the guarantor. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED), for the project titled “Development of Health Equity Assessment Tool Based on Social Epidemiological Survey for Older Adults in Myanmar and Malaysia”(17984739), Grants in aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, for the project titled, “Differences in Social Capital Influence on Depression among Older People—A Comparative Study of Three Asian Countries” (19K19472) and “What are the older persons who live with happiness even if they are ill or have their own disability? -Follow-up surveys of Japan and Southeast Asia-” (21K18453), and Grants in aid for Health and Labor Administration Promotion Research Project titled “Study on promotion of active and healthy aging in ASEAN” (20BA2002). In addition, the World Health Organization Kobe Centre for Health Development funded research to accelerate universal health coverage in light of population aging in ASEAN countries for research titled, “Development and Validation of Questionnaire Instrument for Evaluating the Determinants of Health Status and Universal Health Coverage in Older Adults in Selected Population in Myanmar and Ma-laysia” (WHO Kobe Centre—WKC: K18015).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The survey protocol was reviewed and approved by the ethical review committee at the Department of Medical Research at the Ministry of Health and Sports, the Republic of Myanmar, the World Health Organization (WHO) ethics committee (no. 014817, Ethics/DMR/2018/038), the ethics board at Niigata University (no. 2018-0096), and the National Institute of Public Health in Japan (no. NIPH-IBRA#12279).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before the interviews. Voluntary participation and the right to withdraw from participation at any time were ensured. This study conformed to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Data Availability Statement

An ethics committee has placed an ethical restriction on sharing de-identified data, as the data may contain sensitive information regarding the respondents’ physical and mental health.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the study participants and express our gratitude to Than Win Nyunt, from the Department of Geriatric Medicine, Yangon General Hospital, Yangon, Myanmar; Infectious Diseases Research Centre of Niigata University members. In addition, we thank Saw Thu Nander, Yi Mynt Kyaw, and Myanmar Perfect Research team members who were deeply involved in the project implemented to conduct the survey. We wish to express our gratitude to the Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study principle investigator, Katsunori Kondo, and the core members Naoki Kondo, Jun Aida, Toshiyuki Ojima, and Masashige Saito, who provided helpful advice regarding the project; Hiroshi Murayama from the University of Tokyo, who advised the authors on the aging survey based on his professional experiences; Akiko Tomita and Naoko Ito from the Japan International Cooperation Agency, who supported the conduct of the survey; Tomoko Manabe, who performed excellent secretarial support during the entire study; and Reiko Hayashi from the National Institute of Population and Social Security Research, Japan, who provided supportive advice.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Steptoe, A.; Deaton, A.; Stone, A.A. Subjective Wellbeing, Health, and Ageing. Lancet 2015, 385, 640–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Radermacher, W.J. Recent and Future Developments Related to “GDP and Beyond”. Rev. Income Wealth 2015, 61, 18–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Dolan, P.; White, M.P. How Can Measures of Subjective Well-Being Be Used to Inform Public Policy? Perspectives on Psychological Science. J. Assoc. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 2, 71–85. [Google Scholar]
  4. Veenhoven, R. Healthy Happiness: Effects of Happiness on Physical Health and the Consequences for Preventive Health Care. J. Happiness Stud. 2008, 9, 449–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Chei, C.-L.; Lee, J.M.-L.; Ma, S.; Malhotra, R. Happy Older People Live Longer. Age Ageing 2018, 47, 860–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Liu, B.; Floud, S.; Pirie, K.; Green, J.; Peto, R.; Beral, V. Does Happiness Itself Directly Affect Mortality? The Prospective UK Million Women Study. Lancet 2016, 387, 874–881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Chida, Y.; Steptoe, A. Positive Psychological Well-Being and Mortality: A Quantitative Review of Prospective Observational Studies. Psychosom. Med. 2008, 70, 741–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Ostir, G.V.; Markides, K.S.; Black, S.A.; Goodwin, J.S. Emotional Well-Being Predicts Subsequent Functional Independence and Survival. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2000, 48, 473–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Pressman, S.D.; Cohen, S. Does Positive Affect Influence Health? Psychol. Bull. 2005, 131, 925–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Koopmans, T.A.; Geleijnse, J.M.; Zitman, F.G.; Giltay, E.J. Effects of Happiness on All-Cause Mortality during 15 Years of Follow-Up: The Arnhem Elderly Study. J. Happiness Stud. 2010, 11, 113–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Moskowitz, J.T.; Epel, E.S.; Acree, M. Positive Affect Uniquely Predicts Lower Risk of Mortality in People with Diabetes. Health Psychol. 2008, 27, S73–S82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Lawrence, E.M.; Rogers, R.G.; Wadsworth, T. Happiness and Longevity in the United States. Soc. Sci. Med. 2015, 145, 115–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Schiffrin, H.H.; Nelson, S.K. Stressed and Happy? Investigating the Relationship between Happiness and Perceived Stress. J. Happiness Stud. 2010, 11, 33–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Fredrickson, B.L. The Value of Positive Emotions: The Emerging Science of Positive Psychology Is Coming to Understand Why it’s Good to Feel Good. Am. Sci. 2003, 91, 330–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Zhou, Y.; Zhou, L.; Fu, C.; Wang, Y.; Liu, Q.; Wu, H.; Zhang, R.; Zheng, L. Socio-Economic Factors Related with the Subjective Well-Being of the Rural Elderly People Living Independently in China. Int. J. Equity Health 2015, 14, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Sasaki, I.; Kondo, K.; Kondo, N.; Aida, J.; Ichikawa, H.; Kusumi, T.; Sueishi, N.; Imanaka, Y. Are Pension Types Associated with Happiness in Japanese Older people?: JAGES Cross-Sectional Study. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0197423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Sun, S.; Chen, J.; Johannesson, M.; Kind, P.; Burström, K. Subjective Well-Being and Its Association with Subjective Health Status, Age, Sex, Region, and Socio-Economic Characteristics in a Chinese Population Study. J. Happiness Stud. 2015, 17, 833–873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Tran, N.L.T.; Wassmer, R.W.; Lascher, E.L. The Health Insurance and Life Satisfaction Connection. J. Happiness Stud. 2017, 18, 409–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Keng, S.-H.; Wu, S.-Y. Living Happily Ever after? The Effect of Taiwan’s National Health Insurance on the Happiness of the Elderly. J. Happiness Stud. 2014, 15, 783–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Ergin, I.; Mandiracioglu, A. Demographic and Socioeconomic Inequalities for Self-Rated Health and Happiness in Elderly: The Situation for Turkey Regarding World Values Survey between 1990 and 2013. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2015, 61, 224–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kye, S.Y.; Park, K. Health-Related Determinants of Happiness in Korean Adults. Int. J. Public Health 2014, 59, 731–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Tan, J.H.; Abdin, E.; Shahwan, S.; Zhang, Y.; Sambasivam, R.; Vaingankar, J.A.; Mahendran, R.; Chua, H.C.; Chong, S.A.; Subramaniam, M. Happiness and Cognitive Impairment among Older Adults: Investigating the Mediational Roles of Disability, Depression, Social Contact Frequency, and Loneliness. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 4954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Sakamoto, R.; Okumiya, K.; Norboo, T.; Tsering, N.; Wada, T.; Fujisawa, M.; Imai, H.; Nose, M.; Ishimoto, Y.; Kimura, Y.; et al. Health and Happiness among Community-Dwelling Older Adults in Domkhar Valley, Ladakh, India. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 2017, 17, 480–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Yiengprugsawan, V.; Seubsman, S.-A.; Sleigh, A.C. Unhappiness and Mortality: Evidence from a Middle-Income Southeast Asian Setting. Biopsychosoc. Med. 2014, 8, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Hsu, H.-C.; Chang, W.-C.; Chong, Y.-S.; An, J.S. Happiness and Social Determinants across Age Cohorts in Taiwan. J. Health Psychol. 2016, 21, 1828–1839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Oshio, T.; Urakawa, K. The Association between Perceived Income Inequality and Subjective Well-Being: Evidence from a Social Survey in Japan. Soc. Indic. Res. 2014, 116, 755–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Tay, L.; Kuykendall, L. Promoting Happiness: The Malleability of Individual and Societal Subjective Wellbeing. Int. J. Psychol. 2013, 48, 159–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Read, S.; Grundy, E.; Foverskov, E. Socio-Economic Position and Subjective Health and Well-Being among Older People in Europe: A Systematic Narrative Review. Aging Ment. Health 2016, 20, 529–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Peiró, A. Happiness, Satisfaction and Socio-Economic Conditions: Some International Evidence. J. Socio-Econ. 2006, 35, 348–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Lu, L.; Kao, S.-F. Striving for Positive Aging among Chinese Older People: Effects of Life Stress and Control Beliefs in a Longitudinal Study. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 2017, 86, 382–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Oluwafunmilade, A.A.; Rojas, B.M.; Darboe, A.; Beogo, I. Socioeconomic Differential in Self-Assessment of Health and Happiness in 5 African Countries: Finding from World Value Survey. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0188281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Amorim, S.M.; França, L.H.D.F.P.; Valentini, F. Predictors of Happiness among Retired from Urban and Rural Areas in Brazil. Psicologia Reflexão Crítica 2017, 30, 2–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Navarro-Carrillo, G.; Alonso-Ferres, M.; Moya, M.; Valor-Segura, I. Socioeconomic Status and Psychological Well-Being: Revisiting the Role of Subjective Socioeconomic Status. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Adler, N.E.; Epel, E.S.; Castellazzo, G.; Ickovics, J.R. Relationship of Subjective and Objective Social Status with Psychological and Physiological Functioning: Preliminary Data in Healthy, White Women. Health Psychol. 2000, 19, 586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. HelpAge International. Ageing Population in Myanmar: Trends in Ageing and Health Myanmar. Available online: https://ageingasia.org/Ageing-Population-myanmar/ (accessed on 18 August 2020).
  36. HelpAge International. Improving Basic Mental Health Services in Myanmar’s Primary Health Care System. 2019. Available online: http://themimu.info/sites/themimu.info/files/assessment_file_attachments/Mental_health_policy_brief_FINAL.Pdf (accessed on 17 March 2021).
  37. Myanmar Profile. Institute for Health Metrics and Evaluation. University of Washington. 2018. Available online: http://www.healthdata.org/Myanmar (accessed on 17 March 2021).
  38. Sasaki, Y.; Shobugawa, Y.; Nozaki, I.; Takagi, D.; Nagamine, Y.; Funato, M.; Chihara, Y.; Shirakura, Y.; Lwin, K.T.; Zin, P.E.; et al. Association between Depressive Symptoms and objective/Subjective Socioeconomic Status among Older Adults of Two Regions in Myanmar. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0245489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Lee, Y. Report of the Special Rapporteur on the Situation of Human Rights in Myanmar; UN Human Rights Commission: New York, NY, USA, 2015; p. 9. [Google Scholar]
  40. Health in Myanmar. Ministry of Health, The Republic of the Union of Mynmar 2014. Available online: https://themimu.info/sites/themimu.info/files/documents/Report_Health_in_Myanmar_2014_MOH.Pdf (accessed on 17 March 2021).
  41. Htet, A.S.; Bjertness, M.B.; Sherpa, L.Y.; Kjøllesdal, M.K.; Oo, W.M.; Meyer, H.E.; Stigum, H.; Bjertness, E. Urban-Rural Differences in the Prevalence of Non-Communicable Diseases Risk Factors among 25–74 Years Old Citizens in Yangon Region, Myanmar: A Cross Sectional Study. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 1225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Selth, A. Modern Burma Studies: A Survey of the Field. Mod. Asian Stud. 2010, 44, 401–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Rezaee, M.; Hedayati, A.; Naghizadeh, M.M.; Farjam, M.; Sabet, H.R.; Paknahad, M. Correlation between Happiness and Depression According to Beck Depression and Oxford Happiness Inventory among University Students. Galen Med. J. 2016, 5, 75–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Deng, Y.; Paul, D.R. The Relationships between Depressive Symptoms, Functional Health Status, Physical Activity, and the Availability of Recreational Facilities: A Rural-Urban Comparison in Middle-Aged and Older Chinese Adults. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2018, 25, 322–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. World Health Organization. The WHO STEPS Surveillance Manual. 2017. Available online: https://www.who.int/ncds/surveillance/steps/STEPS_Manual.Pdf (accessed on 28 July 2020).
  46. Win, H.H.; Nyunt, T.W.; Lwin, K.T.; Zin, P.E.; Nozaki, I.; Bo, T.Z.; Sasaki, Y.; Takagi, D.; Nagamine, Y.; Shobugawa, Y. Cohort Profile: Healthy and Active Ageing in Myanmar (JAGES in Myanmar 2018): A Prospective Population-Based Cohort Study of the Long-Term Care Risks and Health Status of Older Adults in Myanmar. BMJ Open 2020, 10, e042877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Japan Gerontological Evaluation Study. Available online: https://www.jages.Net/?_layoutmode=off&lang=english (accessed on 30 July 2020).
  48. Acquadro, C.; Conway, K.; Giroudet, C.; Mear, I. Linguistic Validation Manual for Health Outcome Assessments; Mapi Institute: Lyon, France, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  49. Jitapunkul, S.; Pillay, I.; Ebrahim, S. The Abbreviated Mental Test: Its Use and Validity. Age Ageing 1991, 20, 332–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. MacKenzie, D.M.; Copp, P.; Shaw, R.J.; Goodwin, G.M. Brief Cognitive Screening of the Elderly: A Comparison of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Abbreviated Mental Test (AMT) and Mental Status Questionnaire (MSQ). Psychol. Med. 1996, 26, 427–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Krueger, A.; Schkade, D. The Reliability of Subjective Well-Being Measures. Reliab. Subj. Well Being Meas. 2007, 92, 1833–1845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lee, Y.; Hofferth, S.L.; Flood, S.M.; Fisher, K. Reliability, Validity, and Variability of the Subjective Well-Being Questions in the 2010 American Time Use Survey. Soc. Indic. Res. 2016, 126, 1355–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Diener, E.; Wirtz, D.; Tov, W.; Kim-Prieto, C.; Choi, D.-W.; Oishi, S.; Biswas-Diener, R. New Well-Being Measures: Short Scales to Assess Flourishing and Positive and Negative Feelings. Soc. Indic. Res. 2010, 97, 143–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Matsubayashi, K.; Kimura, S.; Iwasaki, T.; Okumiya, K.; Hamada, T.; Fujisawa, M.; Takeuchi, K.; Kawamoto, A.; Ozawa, T. Evaluation of Subjective Happiness in the Elderly Using a Visual Analogue Scale of Happiness in Correlation with Depression Scale. Nippon Ronen Igakkai Zasshi. Jpn. J. Geriatr. 1992, 29, 811–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Filmer, D.; Pritchett, L.H. Estimating Wealth Effects without Expenditure data—Or Tears: An Application to Educational Enrollments in States of India. Demography 2001, 38, 115–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Shimai, S.; Yamamiya, Y.; Fukuda, S. Subjective Happiness among Japanese Adults: An Upward Tendency Associated with age. Nihon Koshu Eisei Zasshi 2018, 65, 553–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Moeini, B.; Barati, M.; Farhadian, M.; Ara, M.H. The Association between Social Support and Happiness among Elderly in Iran. Korean J. Fam. Med. 2018, 39, 260–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Rizvi, M.A.K.; Hossain, M.Z. Relationship between Religious Belief and Happiness: A Systematic Literature Review. J. Relig. Health 2016, 56, 1561–1582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Myanmar Living Conditions Survey 2017: Socio-Economic Report Myanmar’s Central Statistical Organization of the Ministry of Planning, Finance and Industry, The World Bank, United Nations Development Programme. 2020. Available online: http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/151001580754918086/pdf/Myanmar-Living-Conditions-Survey-2017-Socio-Economic-Report.Pdf (accessed on 26 March 2021).
  60. Choragudi, U. Surveys Shows Inequality a Serious Problem in Myanmar. Mizzima. 2020. Available online: https://www.bnionline.net/en/news/survey-shows-inequality-serious-problem-myanmar (accessed on 15 February 2020).
  61. Barua, A.; Ghosh, M.K.; Kar, N.; Basilio, M.A. Prevalence of Depressive Disorders in the Elderly. Ann. Saudi Med. 2011, 31, 620–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Wada, T. Depression of Community-Dwelling Elderly in Three Asian Countries: Myanmar, Indonesia, and Japan. Kyoto Work. Pap. Area Stud. G-COE Ser. 2009, 18, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  63. Williams, J.; Allen, L.; Wickramasinghe, K.; Mikkelsen, B.; Roberts, N.; Townsend, N. A Systematic Review of Associations between Non-Communicable Diseases and Socioeconomic Status within Low- and Lower-Middle-Income Countries. J. Glob. Health 2018, 8, 020409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Hosseinpoor, A.R.; Bergen, N.; Mendis, S.; Harper, S.; Verdes, E.; Kunst, A.; Chatterji, S. Socioeconomic Inequality in the Prevalence of Noncommunicable Diseases in Low- and Middle-Income Countries: Results from the World Health Survey. BMC Public Health 2012, 12, 474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Tang, K.L.; Rashid, R.; Godley, J.; Ghali, W.A. Association between Subjective Social Status and Cardiovascular Disease and Cardiovascular Risk Factors: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e010137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Adjaye-Gbewonyo, K.; Kawachi, I. Use of the Yitzhaki Index as a Test of Relative Deprivation for Health Outcomes: A Review of Recent Literature. Soc. Sci. Med. 2012, 75, 129–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Wilkinson, R.G. Socioeconomic Determinants of Health: Health Inequalities: Relative or Absolute Material Standards? BMJ Clin. Res. Ed. 1997, 314, 591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Caner, A.; Yiğit, Y.C. Relative Deprivation and Its Association with Health Indicators: Lower Inequality May Not Improve Health. SSM Popul. Health 2019, 7, 100381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Smith, H.J.; Pettigrew, T.F.; Pippin, G.M.; Bialosiewicz, S. Relative Deprivation: A Theoretical and Meta-Analytic Review. Personality and Social Psychology Review. Off. J. Soc. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Inc. 2012, 16, 203–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Kawachi, I.; Kennedy, B.P. Income Inequality and Health: Pathways and Mechanisms. Health Serv. Res. 1999, 34, 215–227. [Google Scholar]
  71. Ziersch, A.M.; Baum, F.; Darmawan, I.G.N.; Kavanagh, A.M.; Bentley, R.J. Social Capital and Health in Rural and Urban Communities in South Australia. Aust. N. Z. J. Public Health 2009, 33, 7–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Riva, M.; Bambra, C.; Curtis, S.; Gauvin, L. Collective Resources or Local Social Inequalities? Examining the Social Determinants of Mental Health in Rural Areas. Eur. J. Public Health 2010, 21, 197–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Sommanustweechai, A.; Putthasri, W.; Nwe, M.L.; Aung, S.T.; Theint, M.M.; Tangcharoensathien, V.; Wynn, S.S. Community Health Worker in Hard-to-Reach Rural Areas of Myanmar: Filling Primary Health Care Service Gaps. Hum. Resour. Health 2016, 14, 64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Li, L.W.; Liu, J.; Xu, H.; Zhang, Z. Understanding Rural–Urban Differences in Depressive Symptoms among Older Adults in China. J. Aging Health 2016, 28, 341–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Ziarko, M.; Mojs, E.; Kaczmarek, L.D.; Warchol-Biedermann, K.; Malak, R.; Lisinski, P.; Samborski, W. Do Urban and Rural Residents Living in Poland Differ in Their Ways of Coping with Chronic Diseases? Eur. Rev. Med. Pharmacol. Sci. 2015, 19, 4227–4234. [Google Scholar]
  76. Knodel, J. The Situation of Older Persons in Myanmar: Results from the 2012 Survey of Older Persons; HelpAge International: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  77. Teerawichitchainan, B.; Knodel, J. Economic Status and Old-Age Health in Poverty-Stricken Myanmar. J. Aging Health 2015, 27, 1462–1484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Knodel, J.; Teerawichitchainan, B. Aging in Myanmar. Gerontology 2017, 57, 599–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Teerawichitchainan, B.; Knodel, J. Long-Term Care Needs in the Context of Poverty and Population Aging: The Case of Older Persons in Myanmar. J. Cross Cult. Gerontol. 2018, 33, 143–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Spoorenberg, T. Provisional Results of the 2014 Census of Myanmar: The Surprise That wasn’t. Asian Popul. Stud. 2015, 11, 4–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Schnittker, J.; Baćak, V. The Increasing Predictive Validity of Self-Rated Health. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e84933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Latham, K.; Peek, C.W. Self-Rated Health and Morbidity Onset among Late Midlife U.S. Adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2012, 68, 107–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  83. Pan, A.; Sun, Q.; Okereke, O.I.; Rexrode, K.M.; Hu, F.B. Depression and Risk of Stroke Morbidity and Mortality: A Meta-Analysis and Systematic Review. JAMA 2011, 306, 1241–1249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  84. Sasaki, Y.; Shobugawa, Y.; Nozaki, I.; Takagi, D.; Nagamine, Y.; Funato, M.; Chihara, Y.; Shirakura, Y.; Lwin, K.; Zin, P.; et al. Rural–Urban Differences in the Factors Affecting Depressive Symptoms among Older Adults of Two Regions in Myanmar. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 2818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Selection of participants for a study.
Figure 1. Selection of participants for a study.
Ijerph 19 03216 g001
Table 1. Happiness scores by socio-demographic characteristics (n = 1200).
Table 1. Happiness scores by socio-demographic characteristics (n = 1200).
NMean±SDp-Value
Happiness score 12006.582.01
Objective SESMiddle/High7186.801.88<0.001
(wealth index)Low4806.242.14
Subjective SESAverage or more9536.831.89<0.001
(self-rated economic status)Difficult/Very difficult2475.622.14
SexMale4836.862.00<0.001
Female7176.382.02
Age60–696706.612.000.25
70–793806.612.04
80+1506.322.06
Subjective health statusExcellent/Good3547.101.89<0.001
Fair/Poor8466.362.01
Illness during preceding yearNo5826.752.08<0.01
Yes6156.411.92
Depressive symptomsGDS < 59216.891.89<0.001
GDS ≥ 52655.482.01
EducationNo school1045.721.94<0.001
Monastic2926.471.98
Some/Finished primary4176.552.06
Middle school or higher3876.921.91
RegionYangon6006.711.86<0.05
Bago6006.452.13
Marital statusMarried6426.771.93<0.001
Widow/Divorced/Never5586.352.07
Living statusAlone686.042.45<0.05
Not alone11326.611.97
Social Support
Receiving emotional supportNo1766.602.140.85
Yes10246.571.98
Providing emotional supportNo1966.542.130.78
Yes10046.581.98
Receiving instrumental supportNo276.112.240.22
Yes11736.592.00
Providing instrumental supportNo2686.572.150.98
Yes9326.581.96
ReligionBuddhism11476.571.990.47
Other536.772.32
Frequency of religious visitsLess than once per week6176.401.99<0.05
Once per week or more5836.762.01
p-value for one-way analysis of variance test; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale; ES = Economic Status.
Table 2. Univariate and multivariate adjusted association between happiness and objective/subjective ES among the older adults in two Myanmar regions.
Table 2. Univariate and multivariate adjusted association between happiness and objective/subjective ES among the older adults in two Myanmar regions.
Happiness ORSE95%CIp-Value AORSE95%CIp-Value
Yangon & Bagon = 1198 n = 1182
Objective ESMiddle/High1.00 1.00
(wealth index)Low0.740.080.590.920.01 0.690.100.520.910.01
Subjective ESAverage or more1.00 1.00
(self-rated ES)Difficult/Very difficult0.330.480.250.440.00 0.460.070.350.620.00
Pseudo = 0.0191 Pseudo = 0.0459
n = 591 (Yangon)n = 599 n = 591
Objective ESMiddle/High1.00 1.00
(wealth index)Low0.520.140.310.870.01 0.540.150.320.930.03
Subjective ESAverage or more1.00 1.00
(self-rated ES)Difficult/Very difficult0.460.110.290.750.00 0.550.140.340.910.02
Pseudo = 0.0100 Pseudo = 0.0359
n = 591 (Bago)n = 599 n = 591
Objective ESMiddle/High1.00 1.00
(wealth index)Low0.760.120.551.040.08 0.730.120.531.010.06
Subjective ESAverage or more1.00 1.00
(self-rated ES)Difficult/Very difficult0.290.050.200.410.00 0.430.080.300.620.00
Pseudo = 0.0191 Pseudo = 0.0643
AOR: Adjusted Odds Ratio; CI: Confidence Interval; ES: Economic Status; GDS: Geriatric Depression Scale; OR: Odds Ratio; SE: Standard Error. Adjusted for age, sex, subjective health, illness during preceding year, depressive symptoms, education, region, marital status, living status, social supports, religion, frequency of religious visits.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Sasaki, Y.; Shobugawa, Y.; Nozaki, I.; Takagi, D.; Nagamine, Y.; Funato, M.; Chihara, Y.; Shirakura, Y.; Lwin, K.T.; Zin, P.E.; et al. Association between Happiness and Economic Status among Older Adults in Two Myanmar Regions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 3216. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063216

AMA Style

Sasaki Y, Shobugawa Y, Nozaki I, Takagi D, Nagamine Y, Funato M, Chihara Y, Shirakura Y, Lwin KT, Zin PE, et al. Association between Happiness and Economic Status among Older Adults in Two Myanmar Regions. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(6):3216. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063216

Chicago/Turabian Style

Sasaki, Yuri, Yugo Shobugawa, Ikuma Nozaki, Daisuke Takagi, Yuiko Nagamine, Masafumi Funato, Yuki Chihara, Yuki Shirakura, Kay Thi Lwin, Poe Ei Zin, and et al. 2022. "Association between Happiness and Economic Status among Older Adults in Two Myanmar Regions" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 6: 3216. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063216

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop