Next Article in Journal
Responding to COVID-19: The Suitability of Primary Care Infrastructure in 33 Countries
Next Article in Special Issue
The Relationship between Social Support and Mental Health Problems of Peri- and Postmenopausal Women during the SARS-CoV-2 Pandemic
Previous Article in Journal
Ending the Social Normalization of Violence against Children in Canada: A Framework, Rationale, and Appeal to Canadian Faith Leaders
Previous Article in Special Issue
Ableism, Human Rights, and the COVID-19 Pandemic: Healthcare-Related Barriers Experienced by Deaf People in Aotearoa New Zealand
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Division of Labour and Parental Mental Health and Relationship Well-Being during COVID-19 Pandemic-Mandated Homeschooling

by
Mariam M. Elgendi
1,*,
Sherry H. Stewart
1,
Danika I. DesRoches
1,
Penny Corkum
1,
Raquel Nogueira-Arjona
2 and
S. Hélène Deacon
1
1
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS B3H 4R2, Canada
2
School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, Brighton BN1 9QH, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(24), 17021; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192417021
Submission received: 11 October 2022 / Revised: 14 December 2022 / Accepted: 15 December 2022 / Published: 18 December 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Social and Emotional Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic)

Abstract

:
While the COVID-19 pandemic has impacted the way parents partition tasks between one another, it is not clear how these division of labour arrangements affect well-being. Pre-pandemic research offers two hypotheses: economic theory argues optimal outcomes result from partners specialising in different tasks, whereas psychological theory argues for a more equitable division of labour. The question of which approach optimizes well-being is more pressing in recent times, with COVID-19 school closures leaving many couples with the burden of homeschooling. It is unknown whether specialisation or equity confer more benefits for mandated homeschoolers, relative to non-homeschoolers or voluntary homeschoolers. Couples (n = 962) with children in grades 1–5 completed measures of workload division and parental well-being. A linear mixed modelling in the total sample revealed that specialisation, but not equity, promoted increased parental emotional and relationship well-being. These relations were moderated by schooling status: voluntary homeschoolers’ well-being benefitted from specialisation, whereas mandated homeschoolers’ well-being did not benefit from either strategy; non-homeschoolers well-being benefitted from both strategies. Across the mixed-gender couples, mothers’ and fathers’ well-being both benefitted from specialisation; equity was only beneficial for mothers’ well-being. Overall, couples might be advised to adopt highly equitable and specialised arrangements to promote both parents’ well-being.

1. Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has caused disruptions to work and family life on a global scale. In March 2020, the World Health Organization (WHO) proclaimed the COVID-19 outbreak a global pandemic. Shortly thereafter, massive containment measures were implemented as part of efforts to curb the spread of the virus [1]. In North America, states and provinces issued stay-at-home orders, prohibited large gatherings, and closed businesses, workplaces, and schools. In compliance with these measures, many people were forced to limit their time outside home. For many parents, traditional care supports were suspended, leaving them to balance mandated homeschooling of their children with working from home during a time of already heightened uncertainty. Given these massive disturbances to work and family life, couples were forced to readjust their roles and responsibilities. Accordingly, this study intended to explore the impact of these new work and family life arrangements on the mental health and relationship well-being of parents during the COVID-19 pandemic, with a specific focus on how homeschooling due to the COVID-19 pandemic affected these relationships.
Many health experts have expressed that the psychological footprint of a large-scale pandemic such as COVID-19 is likely to far exceed the medical footprint of the disease [2]. Research has already shown the negative impacts of these containment measures on the mental health of couples [3], particularly those with children [4]. Women in particular were at an increased risk of developing mental health and substance use problems during the pandemic [5,6,7,8]. A recent meta-analysis uncovered that both being female and having children increased the likelihood of psychological distress in response to viral outbreaks [9].
One of the most important factors affecting couples’ mental health and relationship well-being is the division of labour [10,11]. Division of labour, defined as the partitioning and assignment of tasks to different individuals [12], is a particularly significant issue among couples with children, who must negotiate the division of both household and childcare tasks. During the pandemic, this division of labour also included the division of homeschooling responsibilities for many families.
Homeschooling is a practice in which parents educate children at home rather than send them to school [13]. Traditionally, this has been a voluntary choice, but COVID-19 restrictions brought this to many families on a non-voluntary basis. Here, we use the term “Mandated homeschooling” to refer to the practice where parents are responsible for supervising and assisting their children to navigate school-related tasks and online learning at home due to COVID-19 school closures. Couples mandatorily homeschooling due to the COVID-19 pandemic are likely to experience more disruptions to their work–life arrangements compared to non-homeschoolers (i.e., parents whose children attend school in-person) or those voluntarily homeschooling their children (i.e., parents who chose to homeschool for reasons unrelated to the COVID-19 pandemic). Indeed, couples mandated to homeschool due to the COVID-19 pandemic have been shown to be less optimistic and more likely to engage in coping-related substance use than non-homeschooling couples [14]. Mandatory homeschooling was thrust upon parents with no notice and with varying degrees of support from education systems. Children have been forced to be at home for longer periods of time and require more support from their parents than usual. Consistent with this, there is a growing body of research highlighting that the COVID-19 pandemic has decreased paid work responsibilities and increased unpaid work responsibilities such as childcare and household management for both parents [15,16,17].
Experts, however, are conflicted on how the division of labour has been impacted by the COVID-19 pandemic. Some argue that women are disproportionately impacted by the increased unpaid work responsibilities such as homeschooling [17], while others suggest that the pandemic has provided an opportunity to reduce gender inequalities around paid and unpaid labour [16]. Further, it is not yet clear how these new arrangements affect parents’ mental health and relationship well-being, particularly among mandated homeschooling parents.
Pre-pandemic research offers two plausible theories as to what type of division of labour is most beneficial to parents’ mental health and relationship outcomes [11]. On the one hand, economic theory argues that partners specialising in performing particular roles and tasks improves mental health and promotes stronger and healthier relationships in comparison to sharing roles [18]. An example of specialisation is having one partner engage in household labour, while the other takes on paid labour. On the other hand, psychological theory argues that an equity of the division of labour is more beneficial than an inequitable division of labour in relationships, e.g., both partners put in the same time into unpaid and/or paid labour [19,20,21]. Equity and specialisation are often regarded as being mutually exclusive in the literature, particularly because of the increased focus on examining how couples divide one of the forms of labour, such as looking at household labour or childcare alone [22,23,24,25,26]. However, this is not true in practice given all of the responsibilities that couples are tasked with and must simultaneously balance. A couple can both specialise (in conventional or counter-conventional ways) and be equitable in their division of labour under some circumstances. For instance, this would be the case if each member of a couple spent the same number of hours working every week, but one exclusively participated in paid work while the other exclusively in childcare and/or household work. The scarce extant literature [27] examining the differential effects of equity and specialisation finds that these two strategies independently vary relative to one another, although they are negatively related (r = −0.26). Research also finds that couples adopt various combinations of these strategies (e.g., high levels of both equity and specialisation or high levels of equity and low levels of specialisation), although traditional arrangements are more common (e.g., high levels of specialisation and low levels of equity) [27]. Accordingly, the present study examines how specialisation and equity might each be separately predictive of emotional and relationship well-being by examining parents’ division of both paid and unpaid labour (childcare and household labour) simultaneously. In the sections below, we review each of these theories and examine how they fit in the broader context of the COVID-19 pandemic, particularly in the mandated homeschooling context.

1.1. Economic Theory—Specialisation

The economic principle of specialisation proposes that the division of labour in a relationship is most successful when couples specialise in different tasks [18]. Specialisation refers to when partners of a couple perform different activities; for example, a couple with high levels of specialisation might have one partner who contributes the majority of their time to paid labour, whilst the other mostly engages in unpaid labour. This theory was initially applied to explain the division of labour in the workplace and was later adapted to explain household dynamics [18,28]. Specialisation is based on the rationale that when partners devote their time to a specific type of task, they each develop unique and specific skills and motivations that maximise the output of the household [18]. Accordingly, it has been suggested that specialisation might have positive impacts on relationship functioning and couples’ mental health because it is more efficient than equity. For instance, in couples who specialise, only one member will have to spend time and mental effort attending to work demands, or must balance work and family commitments, e.g., devoting time to their child’s needs [29,30,31]. Theoretically, specialisation also posits that one partner may have a comparative advantage in certain types of tasks as a result of societal circumstances (e.g., gender-specific wage discrimination in the workplace, favourable tax policies for dependent spouses, women’s rising economic autonomy) and/or intrinsic biological differences (e.g., hormones and physical body structure) [18]. Moreover, less specialised couples are likely to have to bargain and coordinate more with their partner and are also likely to monitor each other’s efforts to ensure that each is equally contributing to the relationship [18,28]. Research shows that dual-income couples report more relationship conflict, distress, and time pressure than single-income couples [29,30], suggesting that having one partner who specialises in paid labour might confer more benefits for the couple. Further, Kalmijn et al. found that high levels of specialisation compared to lower levels of specialisation (i.e., more role-sharing arrangements) were protective against dissolution of marriage [32].
Although the economic principle of specialisation is theorised to be effective in improving relationship satisfaction regardless of which partner contributes to paid vs. unpaid labour, research has found more traditional arrangements of specialisation to be more beneficial, i.e., when men are responsible for all or the majority of paid labour [32]. Further, research consistently finds that higher levels of specialisation (i.e., less role-sharing) disproportionately benefits women’s well-being [31,33]. Although both parents in dual-income families reportedly spend an equal amount of time thinking about work and family responsibilities, women are more distressed by these thoughts [31]. Further, studies on dual-income couples show that mothers, as compared to fathers, report more instances of work interfering with family responsibilities and family interfering with work commitments, particularly when children younger than 12 years old are residing at home [33,34,35]. Increased levels of work–family interference have been associated with increased levels of physical (e.g., more health-related problems [36]), emotional (e.g., increased levels of depression [37]), and relational problems (e.g., increased levels of relationship conflict [38,39]). Further, the participation of women in the labour force and their income levels have increased more so than for men, with this shift being associated with an increased risk of divorce and separation [32,40,41,42]. A couple is less specialised when both partners spend more hours engaging in paid labour and share the burden of contributing to household income. Accordingly, the negative impacts of women’s participation in the workforce on relationship well-being has been theorised to be due to the adverse effects of reduced specialisation on couples’ well-being [43,44]. Collectively, this highlights important gender differences associated with adopting more specialised roles within a relationship.
In the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, some experts have argued that the disruptions of the pandemic are more likely to cause parents to adopt traditional gender roles, with mothers exclusively taking on the responsibility of increased housework and childcare and fathers assuming paid work responsibilities [15]. Recent parent testimonials in Nature [45] and The New York Times [46] support this conjecture. Indeed, reports from couples with young children during the first wave of the pandemic showed that the burden of increased household and childcare work had largely fallen to mothers [47]. On the paid work front, there is evidence that women were more likely than men to have reduced their working hours or to have lost their jobs due to the COVID-19 pandemic [48], leaving more time to spend at home caring for their families and homeschooling children [49].
However, it is not yet clear how specialisation among couples during the COVID-19 pandemic impacts mental health or relationship well-being. On the one hand, spending more time with children at home might have protective effects on parents’ mental health. Indeed, some pre-pandemic studies show that having children at home is associated with higher levels of overall well-being and lower levels of depressive symptoms [50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57]. On the other hand, the COVID-19 pandemic has increased unpaid and decreased paid labour tasks in the home [17]. Therefore, specialising in tasks during a pandemic may lead to one partner taking on a greater workload than the other, which can be detrimental to mental health and relationship well-being. A recent UN policy brief noted that the COVID-19 pandemic will increase both women’s home responsibilities [58] and conflict in the home, largely directed at women [59]. If women are expected to specialise in childcare and household work, including taking on the additional responsibility of mandatory homeschooling, with little to no support from the educational system and in the absence of their typical supports (e.g., shared time with families and access to libraries, parks, and gyms), this will likely have detrimental effects on their mental health [9].

1.2. Psychological Theory—Equity

The equity hypothesis predicts that an equitable division of labour will promote parental mental health and relationship well-being [19,20,21]. This hypothesis is built on the premise that a relationship is more agreeable when the division of labour is perceived to be just and fair. Indeed, research indicates that a person’s subjective interpretation of the distribution of work in a relationship has a greater impact on relationship well-being than the actual objective division of labour [60,61]. The equity hypothesis is derived from evaluations of the amount of work, time, and effort that both partners contribute into a relationship and the relative rewards that they gain. Accordingly, equity occurs in a relationship if the efforts one puts forth and the rewards one gains are equal to those of their partner. Following this rationale, an unequal division of labour, in which one person is doing drastically more than the other, can lead to negative impacts on mental and relational health [20]. A person who perceives themselves to be doing more than their partner is likely to develop negative emotions, such as frustration, anger, and sadness, which can quickly escalate into anxiety and depression [62]. Similarly, the under-involved partner can also develop anxiety and depression due to feelings of shame and guilt [62,63]. Furthermore, inequity can also lead to conflict in the relationship which, in turn, is likely to negatively impact mental health and relational satisfaction. Indeed, research has shown that inequity is associated with greater marital conflict and lower marital satisfaction [21,64,65], both of which contribute to poorer mental health [66]. Couples with more equitable divisions of labour also report higher levels of relationship well-being, including increased emotional intimacy and commitment [23,67,68]. Equity has also been associated with higher reported levels of perceived social support in romantic couples [68,69], which has been established to buffer the negative impact of stress on an individual’s mental health [70]. In contrast, inequity within a relationship is associated with an increased risk of marital disruption and dissolution, particularly if a woman believes she was contributing more to the relationship [71,72]. Reflecting this, longitudinal research suggests that inequity can have long-lasting negative effects on marital quality, with known effects persisting up to 20 years [73].
While the equity hypothesis predicts benefits for both members of a couple, research has underscored the gendered effects of equitable labour divisions at home. Specifically, women tend to have stronger beliefs than men that labour should be divided equally in a relationship and attribute more importance to these arrangements in a relationship [74]. Intuitively, these gender differences in beliefs around equity might arise from women’s continued experiences with inequity. Furthermore, while an inequitable division of labour negatively impacts both men and women, women’s experiences of inequity tend to be more intense and/or have more severe consequences on their psychological distress, role strain, and physical health [75,76,77,78,79,80]. Women are also more emotionally responsive to inequity than men, particularly when the division of labour benefits them more, and report experiencing more days of distress and negative emotions including fear and anger related to their experiences of inequity [81]. Collectively, this suggests that inequity places women at a greater disadvantage than men.
In the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, some experts predict that the imposed containment measures might lead to a more equitable division of labour [16,17,82]. Over the past several decades, research has shown a decreasing gender gap in couples’ division of labour [83]. This is reflected in men’s increased participation in household and childcare tasks and women’s increased involvement in paid labour. Despite this trend, pre-pandemic research suggests that women today still contribute more of their time and effort into household and childcare tasks compared to men, regardless of their employment status [84,85,86]. The COVID-19 crisis led many employees to adopt more flexible work arrangements, which might have exposed many fathers specifically to the increased burden of childcare responsibilities and household demands, which women may be more accustomed to balancing. This exposure led many fathers to become more involved with household and child-rearing tasks during the pandemic than before [16,17,82], which serves as evidence of a more equal sharing of domestic labour. This shift makes sense given that pre-pandemic research highlights that working from home is associated with a more equal sharing of household labour by men [87]. Moreover, COVID-19 containment measures eliminated many unsupportive workplace cultures and policies, e.g., inflexible scheduling or lack of paid family leave, which are not conducive to an equitable division of labour [88]. Indeed, studies on fathers taking parental leave suggests that sudden exposures to childcare and household management can have long-lasting effects on men’s participation in unpaid care work [89,90,91]. Intuitively, men sharing domestic labour more equally with their women partners during the pandemic might reduce the pressure put on women to take on additional domestic/childcare responsibilities and thus promote better mental health for both partners [92]. Accordingly, a more equitable division of labour during the COVID-19 pandemic might have protective impacts on parental well-being.

1.3. Homeschooling and Division of Labour

The impact of COVID-19-mandated homeschooling on the division of labour and on parent mental health has yet to be explored. Prior to the pandemic, most voluntary homeschooling parents (approximately 80%) reported that mothers were more likely to take on homeschooling their child, while fathers assumed more paid labour responsibilities [93,94,95,96]. Reflecting on the voluntary homeschooling literature [97], mothers who voluntarily homeschool their children frequently report experiencing intense role strain and emotional burnout [98,99,100]. In the context of the pandemic, the effects of such homeschooling arrangements might be exacerbated in couples mandated to homeschool due to COVID-19-associated school closures. Specifically, women who are mandated to homeschool their children may be at greater risk for psychological distress because they must balance these additional homeschooling responsibilities with their massively increased household workloads and reduced time and emotional capacity. This suggests that an equitable rather than a more specialised division of labour might be more beneficial for couples mandated to be responsible for educating their children at home. Indeed, preliminary research into the impact of mandated homeschooling on mothers vs. fathers seems to hint at a protective effect of fathers taking a greater share of the homeschooling responsibility on mothers’ alcohol use [92]. However, research has yet to compare voluntary and mandated homeschoolers, which are likely to have different ways of dividing labour and/or responding to labour arrangements. Accordingly, it is also important to distinguish between those mandated to homeschool and those who voluntarily take up this responsibility. Specifically, parents voluntarily homeschooling prior to the pandemic may already have household arrangements in place [101] that would serve to help them educate their children at home during the COVID-19 pandemic. However, the pandemic is also likely to have negatively impacted voluntary homeschooling parents, particularly because of decreased access to the supports on which voluntary homeschooling communities may normally rely upon, such as libraries, public gym facilities, and other like-minded homeschooling families [102,103]. However, the rapid onset of the COVID-19 pandemic and consequent unexpected school closures might put mandated homeschoolers, particularly mothers, at an increased risk for mental health and relationship problems. Accordingly, mandated homeschoolers might benefit more from protective factors such as an equitable division of labour relative to voluntary homeschoolers. In summary, while both equity and specialisation seem to confer specific advantages for parents in general (i.e., non-homeschooling couples), whether equity is more beneficial to homeschooling parents, particularly to women and those mandated to homeschool due to COVID-19, has yet to be determined.

1.4. The Current Study

The primary goal of this study was to examine how equity and specialisation separately predict well-being in parents during the COVID-19 pandemic and how this differs in mandated homeschooling couples relative to non-homeschooling and voluntary homeschooling couples. We further aimed to explore whether equity and specialisation predicted different outcomes for men and women among mixed-gender couples, given the differential impact of the division of labour on mothers and fathers in pre-pandemic research [72,77,79]. We intended to examine the impacts on both emotional (i.e., anxiety, depression, and stress) and relationship well-being (relationship conflict and satisfaction) to accurately capture effects on both individual- and couple-level stability, which are most likely to be impacted by the division of labour in the home [10,11]. Lastly, we also conducted supplementary analyses to explore potential three-way interactions between gender, homeschooling status, and specialisation/equity.
Consistent with predictions emerging from the equity and specialisation hypotheses, we expected: (H1) a greater specialisation of tasks between parents to predict better emotional and relationship well-being; (H2) a more equitable division of labour in parents to also predict better emotional and relationship well-being. Specifically, we expected more specialisation and equity to predict lower levels of depression, anxiety, stress, and relationship conflict, as well as higher levels of relationship satisfaction in parents, though this effect might be stronger for equity vs. specialisation [27]. Given that previous research suggests that the division of labour has more negative impacts for mothers than for fathers [72,75], we also expected mothers to benefit more from (H3) more specialisation and (H4) equity than fathers. Given the uncertainty and stress around homeschooling during the COVID-19 pandemic, we also expected (H5) parents of in-person learners (i.e., non-homeschooling parents) to benefit from both equity and specialisation, and we expected both mandated and voluntary homeschooling parents to benefit more from an equitable division of labour than from specialisation, with the former being more beneficial to a greater extent than the latter.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedures

A total of 962 adult (19+ years old) Canadian (n = 806) and American (n = 156) romantic couples were recruited in March–May 2021 (In-person learners = 386 couples; Mandated homeschooling due to the COVID-19 pandemic = 332 couples; Voluntary homeschooling for reasons unrelated to the COVID-19 pandemic = 244 couples). The demographics information and relationship characteristics for the study sample are presented in Table 1.
Initially, 764 couples were recruited through Qualtrics Survey Panels, 332 couples of which were homeschooling due to COVID-19, 46 of which were homeschooling for reasons unrelated to COVID-19, and 386 of which were not homeschooling. Participants were first screened for eligibility and then asked to provide informed consent. Eligible respondents were those who had at least one school-aged child (Grade 1 to 5) living with them, and who were involved in a romantic relationship for at least 3 months with a partner who was also willing and available to participate. Both partners had to be: at least 19 years old, living in Canada or the US, and cohabiting together during the one month period of 15 January to 15 February 2021. Following this, respondents were asked to complete a series of measures, which included two filter questions to ensure that the respondent was paying attention (e.g., “Please select disagree”). The panelist completed the survey first before handing their device to their partner to complete the same set of measures (the partner had to complete the survey within 48 h of the panelist). Qualtrics also performed a speeder check (i.e., checking to identify responses that were more than two standard deviations from the mean duration) to ensure that adequate time was spent completing the survey. Couples who were ineligible, failed the filter questions or speeder check, or failed to provide consent, were screened out. Finally, after completing the survey, couples were compensated via Qualtrics according to their guidelines.
To supplement the recruitment by Qualtrics, 198 couples homeschooling for reasons unrelated to COVID-19 were recruited (during March–May 2021) from homeschooling associations and social media homeschooling groups. This was needed because our recruitment strategies through Qualtrics did not adequately capture this subset of homeschoolers (This was noted early in recruitment. Accordingly, this second method of data collection was instituted early in the data collection process and substantially overlapped with Qualtrics Survey Panels’ recruitment). These participants followed the same procedures outlined above (i.e., Qualtrics software was used to collect and link de-identified data for these parents across couples, and the same data quality screening measures such as speeder checks were utilised). The only exception was that participants were directed to a different location at the end of the survey, where they provided their email addresses so that they could be compensated ($10 Amazon gift cards for each member of the couple) by a co-investigator or trained research assistant. The participant email addresses were not stored with the data to help protect confidentiality. Lastly, this study received approval from Dalhousie University’s Social Sciences Research Ethics Board.

2.2. Sample Size Justification

Participants were part of an existing longitudinal study on the factors that support and impede family well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. A post hoc power analysis also revealed that our sample was adequately powered (power of approximately 1) to detect a small effect size (β = 0.2) of equity/specialisation on parent well-being (H1H2), assuming an alpha of 0.01. Our estimate of effect size was based on the results of extant literature on the effects of the division of labour on emotional and relationship well-being [11,27]. To assess if we had a sufficient sample size to detect moderation by gender or homeschooling status (H3H5), we conducted additional post hoc power analyses (one assuming a sample size of 831 mixed-gender couples and one assuming a sample size of 962 couples). These analyses yielded similar findings (i.e., power of approximately 1). Given the complexity of estimating power for multilevel models, these calculations were based upon power analyses with actor–partner interdependence models (APIMPowerR) [104].

2.3. Measures

Both members of the couple completed the same set of measures in March–May 2021 using the same one month time frame as a reference (i.e., 15 January–15 February 2021. This timeframe was chosen as the only time during the pandemic when all three forms of schooling were taking place in both Canada and the US (Qualtrics targeted specific regions in both Canada and the US to attempt to ensure that our sample had adequate numbers of all three forms of schooling).

2.3.1. Demographics

The demographics questionnaire asked about the respondents’ homeschooling status, gender, age, race, number and age of children, relationship status, family income, highest level of education, employment, ethnicity, and length of relationship. These were used in the sample description and some were used as covariates (i.e., age, number of children, relationship length, family income, and highest level of education), given previous research linking these variables with levels of emotional and relationship problems [105,106,107,108,109]. Further, these variables were significantly different across our three schooling conditions (see Table 1; p < 0.05) and were significantly and/or marginally associated with emotional and/or relationship well-being in bivariate correlational analyses in the current study.

2.3.2. Workload Division Measure

Division of labour was assessed with questions on paid and unpaid service from the General Social Use survey (GSU) [110], to which we added questions on homeschooling labour (e.g., “Last week, how many hours did you spend looking after one or more children in your household?”). In line with past research [11,27], equity and specialisation were calculated for each couple as follows ( P a [paid hours of partner a], P b [paid hours of partner b], U a [unpaid hours, i.e., household and childcare hours, of partner a], and U b [unpaid hours, i.e., household and childcare hours, of partner b]. Total number of hours is defined as T a b = P b + U b + P a + U a ):
Equity = 1 | ( P a + U a ) ( P b + U b ) | T a b
Specialisation = | ( P a P a + U a ) ( P b P b + U b ) |
Equity was defined as the absolute total difference in the proportion of time spent by both partners on paid and unpaid labour; to ensure that higher values represent more equity, scores were subtracted from 1. In contrast, specialisation was conceptualised as the absolute total difference in the proportion of hours that each partner spends on paid work relative to the total amount of work [27]. Scores could range from 0 to 1 for equity (0 = no equity; 1= full equity) and specialisation (0 = full role-sharing; 1 = full role specialisation). For homeschooling couples, hours spent homeschooling was included as part of childcare hours, which was part of unpaid labour.

2.3.3. Emotional Well-Being

The 7-item Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD-7) scale [111] was used to assess generalised anxiety symptoms (e.g., “Feeling nervous, anxious, or on edge”) and the 9-item Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) [112] was used to assess depressive symptoms (e.g., “Feeling down, depressed, or hopeless”). Items on both measures were answered on a 4-point rating scale, ranging from “Not at all” (scored as 0) to “Nearly Every Day” (scored as 3), for the period of interest of 15 January–15 February 2021. For each measure, items were summed to create a total score. These anxiety [111,113] and depression [112] measures have good internal consistency (α-values > 0.89) and test-retest reliability, as well as good procedural and criterion-related validity across numerous populations, e.g., patients in primary care, patients with medical comorbidities, and cognitively impaired patients [114]. The PHQ-9 and GAD-7 tools are widely used as brief severity measures for depressive and anxious symptoms, respectively [114]. Both scales showed excellent internal consistency (α = 0.93 [GAD-7]; α = 0.91 [PHQ-9]) in the present sample. We also assessed perceived stress using the 4-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4) [115]. This scale asked parents to rate how often they experienced stressful thoughts and feelings during the month of interest from 15 January–15 February 2021 (e.g., “In the last month, how often have you felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life?”). Items were answered on a 5-point rating scale ranging from “Never” (Scored as 0) to “Very Often” (scored as 4). The PSS-4 tool has been demonstrated to be an internally consistent and valid measure of stress in a sample of pregnant women and across a sample of adults in the UK (α = 0.77) [116,117]. Though the PSS-4 showed a lower internal consistency (α = 0.58) in the present sample, this is very close to the acceptable cut-off of 0.60 for short scales of 10 items or less [118].

2.3.4. Relationship Well-Being

Relationship well-being was assessed using two scales, which tapped relationship conflict and relationship satisfaction, respectively. Relationship conflict was assessed with the 7-item Partner-Specific Rejecting Behaviors Scale (PSRBS) [119], completed for conflict with partner (e.g., “I was angry or irritated with my partner”). Items were answered on a 9-point scale ranging from “Strongly disagree” (scored as 1) to “Strongly agree” (scored as 9) for the period of interest of 15 January–15 February 2021. Items were summed to create a total conflict score for each member of the couple. This scale has been shown to possess excellent psychometric properties (i.e., internal consistency, α-values > 0.93; test-retest reliability, and validity) in prior studies using romantic dyads [120]. Relationship satisfaction was assessed using the 4-item dyadic Couples Satisfaction Index (CSI) [121]. Three items assess the level of agreement with the partner (e.g., “I had a warm and comfortable relationship with my partner”). These items were answered on a 6-point rating scale ranging from “Not at all” (scored as 0) to “Completely true” (scored as 5) for the one month period of interest. One item assesses the degree of happiness with the relationship, which was answered on a 7-point scale ranging from “Extremely unhappy” (scored as 0) to “Perfect” (Scored as 6). The CSI tool has been demonstrated to be reliable in romantic couples [122]; it highly correlates with other relationship satisfaction measures and discriminates between distressed and non-distressed relationships, establishing its validity [121]. Both scales showed excellent internal consistency (α = 0.95 [PSRBS]; α = 0.90 [CSI]) in the present sample.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

All analyses were carried out using R software version 4.1.2. The five outcome variables were initially screened and inspected for the approximate normality of residuals, linearity, constant variance, and extreme outliers. There were some violations of the normality and constant variance assumptions. Thus, we ran all the models using more robust estimation methods and computed heteroscedastic consistent standard errors [123,124]. Our results were unchanged.
In terms of testing our hypothesis, separate analyses were conducted for each predictor (i.e., equity and specialisation) and outcome variable (i.e., depression, anxiety, stress, relationship satisfaction, and relationship conflict). In all models, parent age, the number of children, relationship length, family income, and highest level of education were included as covariates, and all predictors were grand mean-centred. Due to the dyadic nature of the data, we used linear mixed models (LMMs) with indistinguishable dyads to test the impact of specialisation and equity on the emotional and relationship well-being of couples in the full sample (H1 and H2). For each outcome variable, equity (or specialisation) and covariates (see above) were modelled as fixed effects while the participants were modelled as random effects nested within couples. An example of a model specification looking at the impact of equity on depression is as follows: Depression = β00 + β01 (Equity) + β10 (Covariates) + r0 + e. LMMs with gender-distinguishable dyads in the mixed-gender couples only were then used to assess gender differences on the impact of specialisation and equity (H3 and H4). To assess if the impacts of equity and specialisation were different for homeschooling (voluntary and COVID-19-mandated homeschoolers) and non-homeschooling couples, we conducted all of the above analyses using homeschooling status as a moderator (H5). All moderators were effect coded and interactions were probed by separately estimating the simple slopes of equity and specialisation on the outcomes for different levels of the moderators (Gender: Male, Female; Homeschooling Status: In-person learners, Mandated homeschoolers due to COVID-19, Voluntary Homeschoolers). Model fit statistics (Bayes information criterion [BIC], Akaike information criterion [AIC], and R2) are presented for each model. Further, given our multiple outcome measures (n = 5), we used a more stringent alpha for determining statistical significance. Thus, all effects significant at p < 0.01 were considered statistically significant and discussed as so. However, as this was also a novel study (the first of its kind during the pandemic), we wanted to ensure that we also protected against type 1 error, and thus we note that all effects that are significant at traditional levels of significance p < 0.05 and discuss all effects between p < 0.05 and p = 0.01 as trends in the data.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Correlations

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations between all study variables are presented in Table 2. Overall, couples on average reported high levels of equity and low levels of specialisation. Further, based on population norms, parents in our sample endorsed mild symptoms of depression and anxiety but moderate to high symptoms of perceived stress [112,125,126,127]. Lastly, reports of both relationship conflict and satisfaction were higher (i.e., greater than 1 SD) than previous samples of romantic dyads [120,128,129].
With regard to bivariate correlations, correlations for the entire sample are presented below the diagonal, and the correlations for the mandated homeschooling sample are presented above the diagonal. In the whole sample, equity was only negatively correlated (marginally) with perceived stress, and specialisation was negatively correlated with all of our outcome variables (except relationship satisfaction). In the mandated homeschoolers sample, equity was marginally and negatively associated with depression and perceived stress, and specialisation was not correlated with any of our outcome variables. Equity and specialisation were significantly negatively intercorrelated in the whole sample, but the magnitude of this intercorrelation decreased in the mandated homeschooling sample.

3.2. Effect of Specialisation and Equity (H1 and H2)

Consistent with the specialisation hypothesis (H1), a more specialised division of labour across parents was associated with significantly lower depression, anxiety, perceived stress (marginal), and relationship conflict (see Table 3).
Contrary to H2, a more equitable division of labour was not associated with any of our parental well-being outcomes (see Table 4).

3.3. Gender Differences in Specialisation and Equity (H3 and H4)

Gender significantly moderated the effect of specialisation on all measures of parents’ emotional and relationship well-being (see Table 5); however, these effects were not as predicted. A simple slopes analysis was used to probe the interactions at both levels of the moderator: Male and Female.
Contrary to H3, more specialisation of tasks across parents was significantly associated with decreased depression (β = −3.82; p < 0.001; see Figure 1) and anxiety (β = −2.99; p < 0.001) in males and only marginally associated with decreased anxiety in females (β = −1.26; p = 0.04). Specialisation was also associated with decreased perceived stress (β = −1.12; p = 0.003) and relationship conflict (β = −5.16; p < 0.001) in males but not females (perceived stress [β = 0.02; p = 0.968]; relationship conflict [β = −1.35; p = 0.44]). Lastly, specialisation was not significantly associated with relationship satisfaction for either males (β = 0.38; p = 0.51) or females (β = −0.90; p = 0.12). However, the opposing effects of specialisation on males’ and females’ relationship satisfaction is likely driving the observed interaction.
Gender moderated the effect of equity on depression (only marginally), anxiety (only marginally), and relationship satisfaction, but not of equity on perceived stress or relationship conflict in parents in mixed-gender couples (see Table 6 and Figure 2).
Consistent with H3, simple slopes analysis indicated that in females, a more equitable division of labour was associated with decreased depression (β = −2.00; p = 0.02) and increased relationship satisfaction (β = 2.35; p < 0.001; see Figure 2) but was not associated with anxiety symptoms (β = −1.16; p = 0.13). There were no significant associations between being male and any of these outcomes (depression [β = −0.09; p = 0.92]; anxiety [β = 0.59; p = 0.44]; relationship satisfaction [β = 0.34; p = 0.63]). Despite the nonsignificant values, the opposing effects of equity on males and females anxiety is likely driving the observed interaction. Further, gender did not moderate the effect of equity on relationship conflict.

3.4. Differences by Homeschooling Status (H5)

With regard to the effects of specialisation on parents’ well-being, homeschooling status significantly moderated the effect of specialisation (Table 7) on parental depression, anxiety, and relationship satisfaction. Homeschooling status also marginally moderated the effect of specialisation on relationship conflict. Simple slopes analyses were used to probe the interactions at all levels of the moderator: In-person learners, Mandated Homeschoolers, and Voluntary Homeschoolers.
In contrast to H5, voluntary homeschoolers experienced significantly less depression (β = −6.80; p < 0.001), anxiety (β = −6.84; p < 0.001; see Figure 3), and relationship conflict (β = −12.36; p < 0.001) and reported significantly higher relationship satisfaction (β = 3.27; p < 0.001) with higher, as compared to lower, levels of specialisation across tasks. Similarly, but to a lesser degree, in-person learners also reported marginally less depression (β = −2.37; p = 0.01) and relationship conflict (β = −4.58; p = 0.04) with higher vs. lower specialisation. However, none of the associations between specialisation and measures of parents’ emotional and relationship well-being were significant for mandated homeschoolers (all p-values > 0.05).
Homeschooling status also significantly moderated the effect of equity on relationship satisfaction (Table 8), but not on other measures of parent well-being (all p-values > 0.05).
Partially consistent with H5, simple slopes analysis indicated that there was a marginally positive association between relationship satisfaction and equity for in-person learners (β = 2.09; p = 0.02; see Figure 4). In contrast to H5, the opposite trend was observed in voluntary homeschoolers; relationship satisfaction and equity were marginally negatively associated (β = −2.99; p = 0.04). Equity and relationship satisfaction were positively associated in mandated homeschoolers, but this was not significant (β = 1.73; p = 0.10).

3.5. Supplementary Analyses

Supplementary analyses were run to test potential three-way interactions between gender, homeschooling status, and specialisation/equity. There was a significant three-way interaction between specialisation, parent gender, and homeschooling status with regard to relationship conflict (Table 9; see Figure 5), but not any other outcomes. A simple slopes analysis was used to probe the interaction at all levels of the moderators: Homeschooling status (In-person learners, Mandated Homeschoolers and Voluntary Homeschoolers) and Gender (Male, Female). Men from voluntary homeschooling couples had a significant negative relationship between specialisation and relationship conflict (β = −21.34; p < 0.001). All other simple slopes were non-significant (all p-values > 0.05).
There was also a significant three-way interaction between equity, parent gender, and homeschooling status with regard to parent depression (only marginally) and relationship conflict (Table 10; see Figure 6 and Figure 7), but not any other outcomes. Simple slopes analyses indicated that female parents of in-person learners (β = −3.25; p = 0.01) had marginally significant negative associations between equity and depression (see Figure 6); this association was not significant in male and female mandated homeschoolers (β = −2.24; p = 0.13; β = −2.80, p = 0.06) as well as male parents of in-person learners (β = 0.34; p = 0.79). Equity and depression were not significantly associated in male or female voluntary homeschoolers (β = 0.01, p = 1.00; β = 0.63, p = 0.74). With regard to relationship conflict, male and female voluntary homeschoolers reported opposing effects of equity (β = 3.80, p = 0.42; β = −8.99, p = 0.06), although these relations were not significant. Further, there was a marginally significant negative association between equity and relationship conflict in male parents of in-person learners (β = −6.52, p = 0.04); this was similar but not significant in their female counterparts (β = −5.76, p = 0.07). Lastly, equity and relationship conflict were not significantly associated in mandated homeschoolers (both male and female parents; all p-values > 0.05).

4. Discussion

In the present study, we investigated whether equity and/or specialisation was associated with greater well-being among parents during the COVID-19 pandemic and tested whether these effects varied by gender and/or homeschooling status. It was predicted that parents with a highly specialised and/or equitable division of labour would report lower levels of depression, anxiety, stress, and/or relationship conflict, as well as higher levels of relationship satisfaction. We also predicted that these relations would be stronger in mothers vs. fathers and that mandated and voluntary homeschooling parents would benefit more from equity than specialisation, while parents of in-person learners would benefit from both equity and specialisation. Our predictions were partially supported by our data analyses.
In terms of investigating the impact of the division of labour on parent well-being overall, we found high levels of specialisation, but not equity, to have positive effects on parents’ emotional and relationship well-being. Consistent with our hypothesis (H1) and with economic theory [18], high levels of specialisation among couples with children living through the COVID-19 pandemic was associated with increased emotional and relationship well-being in parents (i.e., lower reported levels of depression, anxiety, perceived stress, and relationship conflict). Building on pre-pandemic research showing the benefits of specialisation [32], our results suggest that specialisation might be an effective division of labour strategy to promote parents’ well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic, overall. Interestingly, a more equitable division of labour was not associated with similar benefits on parental well-being. This is in contrast with our hypothesis (H2) and past research demonstrating the benefits of equity in a relationship [71,73]. One possibility is that couples with an inequitable division of labour may not perceive it as being unfair or inequitable. This is consistent with research that finds that majority of couples who report an inequitable division of household labour also perceive these household arrangements to be fair to themselves and their partners [130,131,132]. It is possible that couples who adopt more traditional gender role attitudes might be particularly more accustomed to expect and/or accept an inequitable division of labour [133] because performing gendered tasks (regardless of the time and effort it takes) provides individuals the satisfaction of presenting themselves as competent members of their gender category [134,135]. Overall, our results offer evidence In support of the economic theory of specialisation rather than the psychological theory of equity. However, these overall effects should be interpreted in conjunction with the moderating effects of gender and homeschooling status discussed next.
Inconsistent with our hypothesis (H3), the specialised division of labour was associated with more positive impacts on emotional and relationship well-being in men relative to women. Specifically, men in relationships with more specialised strategies for dividing labour reported lower levels of depression, anxiety, stress, and relationship conflict. We did find some evidence to suggest that women might also experience decreased anxiety with more specialisation, although this effect was marginal and smaller in magnitude then in the men and there were no associations with reduced stress, depression, or relationship conflict (See Figure 1). The observation of greater beneficial effects for men is partially consistent with pre-pandemic findings that the division of labour has more negative mental health consequences for women relative to men [64,81]. Moreover, these gendered effects of specialisation favouring men are consistent with pre-pandemic findings that show that specialisation confers more financial and employment benefits for men relative to women [136]. Furthermore, research shows that compared to mothers, fathers attribute more social value to paid work [137,138] and report more guilt, failure, and depression when unemployed [138,139]. Accordingly, men may find more specialised strategies for dividing labour to be beneficial for their mental and relational health. In contrast, research suggests that mothers attribute more meaning and happiness with domestic chores, leisure activities, and childcare tasks [140] and experience more guilt, depression, and anxiety when they perceive their work to interfere with their ability to care for their children [141]. Accordingly, women may also find more specialised strategies for dividing labour to be beneficial for their mental and relational health; however, this was not evident in the current study. Specialisation was only marginally associated with decreased anxiety in women. A potential explanation for the gender difference that emerges in the effects of specialisation on the well-being of fathers vs. mothers might be the gender norms that permeate society. An individual’s perceptions of equity and/or fairness in the division of household labour is filtered through gender ideology, and in turn influence emotional and relationship well-being [74]. Although many stereotypes of the “typical woman” have evolved over the past decade, gender norms regarding men are more resistant to change [142]. Further, changes such as parenthood may cause couples to adopt and/or maintain traditional gender roles [143,144]. However, our measures did not ask about gender role attitudes, limiting our ability to firmly attribute these gender differences to gender ideology. Future studies should investigate mechanisms underlying why specialisation might confer mental health and relational benefits to men more so than to women during the pandemic. Further, studies should explore whether specialisation was enforced on women during the COVID-19 pandemic (i.e., due to job loss and workplace closures) and whether such circumstances might influence parental well-being.
Consistent with our hypothesis (H4), an equitable division of labour was associated with more positive impacts for women as compared to men’s well-being. Specifically, women in relationships with a more equitable division of labour reported marginally lower levels of depression and significantly higher levels of relationship satisfaction compared with women in less equitable arrangements. In contrast, an equitable division of labour was not significantly associated in either a beneficial or detrimental direction with men’s emotional and relationship well-being (see Figure 2). This is consistent with pre-pandemic research, which suggests that an inequitable division of labour has greater psychological and interpersonal consequences on women relative to men [64,71,72,81]. Despite evolving societal ideas around men’s and women’s work and family roles, mothers are still expected to take on more physically and emotionally taxing responsibilities (e.g., managing and overseeing the family and household schedule) compared to their partners [85]. Pre-pandemic evidence suggests that these additional responsibilities take a toll on the mental health of mothers [31,97,145,146], a finding that has been replicated in COVID-19 pandemic research [14,92]. Our study builds on current research to show that women’s greater emotional responsiveness to the inequitable division of labour persists during the COVID-19 pandemic. This is worrisome in the context of COVID-19 containment measures (e.g., social distancing, school closures), when many household and childcare responsibilities have increased (e.g., Carlson et al., 2020b; Craig & Churchill, 2021; Shafer et al., 2020). These increased demands and their consequent effects on mental health and relationship well-being are also likely to interfere with a mother’s ability to parent during this challenging time and place them at increased risk for escalating conflict and domestic violence [147].
Our findings also highlighted the role of homeschooling status as a potential moderator of relationships between the division of labour strategies and parental well-being. In contrast with our predictions (H5), specialisation more so than equity emerged as a useful strategy, with beneficial effects on the emotional and relationship well-being of parents who are voluntarily homeschooling their children. Specifically, while higher levels of specialisation were associated with significantly less depression, anxiety, and relationship conflict and higher relationship satisfaction in these parents (See Figure 3), more equity was associated with marginally lower relationship satisfaction (see Figure 4). This is consistent with the voluntary homeschooling literature, which suggests that mothers are often largely responsible for homeschooling children alongside other domestic responsibilities, although fathers may less frequently take on the primary homeschooler role [93]. Specialisation likely promotes more positive interpersonal relationships (as compared to equity) because it is consistent with a lot of the characteristics of homeschooling families. For instance, homeschooling families often decide to live on one income [93] and thus might find it more efficient if only one member of the couple must balance work and family commitments.
In contrast, none of the aforementioned relations between equity/specialisation and parental well-being were significant for parents who were mandated to homeschool their children due to the COVID-19 pandemic (see Figure 3 and Figure 4). This is inconsistent with our predictions (H5) and recent findings highlighting the protective effect of an egalitarian division of homeschooling labour on drinking frequency in couples during COVID-19 [92]. A potential explanation for our findings might be that couples who have experienced more waves of the pandemic and subsequently more instances of school closures have adapted in other ways to the increased demands of homeschooling opportunities. The supports for mandated homeschoolers have also likely evolved relative to the first wave of the pandemic (e.g., increased hours of online learning offered by schools, more educational supports). Furthermore, our mandatory homeschooling sample was composed of families with relatively high SES (see Table 1). Accordingly, we are not able to capture impacts on families who might be most susceptible to the negative impacts of the pandemic [148,149] and are consequently more likely to benefit from a more equal division of labour during mandated homeschooling. Interestingly, however, both specialisation and equity appeared to promote parental well-being for parents whose children attend school in-person, though the former had a larger effect. Consistent with H5, parents of in-person learners had a marginally negative association between specialisation and parental depression and relationship conflict, and a marginally positive association between equity and relationship satisfaction (see Figure 4). These findings suggest that parents who do not homeschool are susceptible to the benefits of either approach to the division of labour. However, it is also important to be aware that all these relationships might be bi-directional. Accordingly, factors such as better emotional adjustment and more relationship satisfaction are likely to promote positive and more frequent discussions regarding the division of labour in the home. More longitudinal analyses in the future might unpack the direction of these relations.
However, it is also important to recognise and interpret results considering the 3-way interactions between gender, homeschooling status, and specialisation/equity that were revealed in our supplementary analyses (See Figure 5, Figure 6 and Figure 7). Specifically, our results indicate that among couples who were voluntarily homeschooling, fathers reported more benefits associated with greater role specialisation (i.e., significantly lower relationship conflict). In contrast, in couples whose children were attending school in-person, both mothers and fathers appeared to benefit from equity (i.e., marginally lower depression or relationship conflict) but not specialisation. Furthermore, there were no significant relations between either equity or specialisation and parental well-being among mothers or fathers of mandated homeschoolers or in mothers who were voluntarily homeschooling. To reconcile these opposing reports on the effects of equity and specialisation, parents might be advised to adopt combinations of both specialised and equitable divisions of labour. Research that views these concepts as mutually exclusive or that examines equity alone might fail to capture the distinct impacts of these two approaches. It is also important to note that although some of the simple slopes noted above (see Figure 5, Figure 6 and Figure 7) were not significant (and/or were approaching significance), these are likely due to the reduced power of the current sample to detect effects for these higher order interactions; future studies should attempt to see if these preliminary findings are replicated using a larger sample. Overall, this work extends pre-pandemic research on division of labour and well-being [27] to the context of the COVID-19 pandemic and clarifies the role that homeschooling status and gender may play in moderating these relations.

Limitations and Future Directions

Our study was limited in several ways. First, given the difficulty of obtaining objective measures of the division of labour, we relied on more subjective reports. Accordingly, our reports of equity and specialisation may not be reflective of the actual objective division of labour in a relationship. Nonetheless, subjective perceptions of the distribution of work in a relationship have been found to have greater impacts on relationship well-being than the actual, objective division of labour [60,61]. Future work should attempt to look at whether these effects persist with more objective measures of division of labour. Second, we relied on retrospective reports of individuals. Accordingly, time delays, changes in COVID-19 containment measures, and changes in the division of labour might have influenced participants’ responses. Nonetheless, research shows that division of labour is resistant to short-term changes in relationships [150]. Additionally, COVID-19 containment measures were relatively stable over the course of January–February 2021, and parents’ experiences of these conditions (during the second wave of the pandemic) are crucial to document, despite the time delay between the experience and the report. Future work should attempt to examine whether effects persist over time and across future waves of the pandemic. Third, most of our sample was university educated, Caucasian, and in a mixed-gender relationship; we also only recruited couples. Thus, we are limited in our ability to generalise findings. Future work should attempt to recruit more socially diverse samples (i.e., single-parent families and individuals of different race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, gender, and/or sexual orientation). This is vital given predictions that COVID-19 is exacerbating pre-existing inequalities, e.g., across income levels and ethnic backgrounds [148,149]. It is important to note though that our sample’s socioeconomic, ethnic, and familial compositions (e.g., families with same-gender parents) are generally representative of the North American population [151,152,153,154,155]. Furthermore, although we attempted to be more inclusive in our initial analyses (H1H2; H5) by using indistinguishable dyads, our small sample of same-gender couples prevented more in-depth analyses of the potential role of sexual orientation. Future work should attempt to see if our findings can be replicated in same-gender couples, particularly in light of research suggesting that equity might be particularly beneficial in these couples [32,156].
Fourth, it is likely that children affect their parents’ mental health, particularly if they require more support during mandated homeschooling. We did not ask about children’s ability to cope with homeschooling; thus, we are limited in our ability to attribute our results on parental well-being to division of labour strategies. However, our analyses controlled for the number of children in the household, and we did restrict recruitment to school-aged children in grades 1–5, who are more likely to experience mental health problems and require support and direction during mandated homeschooling [157]. In addition, the average age of the children in our sample did not differ across the three groups (Table 1). Collectively, this increases the likelihood that the effects observed in our study are a consequence of the division of labour rather than due to group differences in a potentially confounding variable. Despite our attempts to control for confounding effects in several ways, future research should attempt to clarify these relationships by inquiring about children’s experiences during mandated homeschooling. Fifth, the current study conceptualised childcare, housework, and homeschooling all under the umbrella of unpaid labour, which prevented us from examining the effects of partners specialising in specific kinds of unpaid labour. The purpose of the current study was to explore the effects of equity and specialisation (as conceptualised in the literature) on parents’ well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic; thus, such types of more specific analyses are beyond the scope of the current study. Nonetheless, future studies should aim to develop techniques aimed at exploring the effects of specialising in specific kinds of unpaid labour.
Sixth, our use of the Qualtrics Survey Panels as a recruitment tool in addition to our supplementary efforts to recruit voluntary homeschoolers may limit our ability to generalise findings. However, a recent meta-analysis found that samples drawn from online panel data are comparable to conventionally sourced data with respect to both psychometric properties and criterion validity [158]. Nonetheless, future research should attempt to investigate these relations in other samples and be obtained using more conventional recruitment methods. Seventh, our study’s measure of relationship conflict was limited in its focus on perpetration of conflict rather than victimization by conflict perpetrated by the partner. Consequently, future studies should attempt to see if effects differ when examining both the perpetration of and victimization by conflict. Further, our two relationship outcomes showed differing effects. For instance, equity was best for enhancing relational satisfaction in women, whereas specialisation was best for minimizing conflict in men. This might reflect how different genders contextualise relationship well-being and the relative importance that they attribute to relationship satisfaction and/or conflict in a partnership; indeed, this presents a potential avenue for future research. Eighth, due to the small sample size of parents who reside in the US (n = 156) in this study, we were unable to examine differences in parents based on their country of residence, as such analyses were likely to be underpowered. Responses to the COVID-19 pandemic likely differed between Canada and the US (e.g., travel restrictions, government support, school closures, health system capacity), which may have had differing impacts on parents’ responsibilities and work–life arrangements [159]. Future work may wish to examine whether the COVID-19 pandemic has had different effects on the division of labour in parents living in Canada vs. the United States.

5. Conclusions

In a more general sense, our findings offer support for the effects of the economic theory of specialisation on couples’ wellbeing [18]. When contrasting the effects of equity and specialisation on parents’ emotional and relationship well-being, consistent with the economic theory, specialisation emerged as a more beneficial strategy for couples to adopt during the COVID-19 pandemic to promote their well-being. However, these effects should be interpreted in the context of the observed moderating effects of an individual’s gender and homeschooling status. Although more role specialisation was beneficial for both mothers and fathers’ well-being, particularly fathers, equity was only beneficial for mothers’ well-being. These relations were further distinguished by a parent’s homeschooling status such that the well-being of parents who were voluntarily homeschooling benefitted more from specialisation while the well-being of parents with children attending school in-person benefited from the use of either strategy; mandated homeschoolers’ well-being did not appear to strongly benefit from the use of either equity or specialisation strategies. These results suggest that mandated homeschoolers have likely resorted to other strategies and supports to be able to adapt in other ways to the increased demands of homeschooling opportunities during COVID-19; as such, these parents might require more external and educational supports to promote their well-being.
Overall, our findings advance the current literature on the division of labour and its impacts on emotional and relationship well-being by isolating the specific impacts of specialisation and equity and by examining the division of various forms of labour (paid and unpaid), including homeschooling labour. Furthermore, the large amount of homeschooling literature to date has studied almost exclusively voluntary contexts [160,161,162]. Moreover, the emerging COVID-19 pandemic-mandated homeschooling literature [14,92] has focused on comparing mandated homeschooling couples with couples without children. However, there might be important similarities and differences between mandated and voluntary homeschoolers. This study presents an incremental advance in the voluntary homeschooling and emerging COVID-19 mandatory homeschooling literature by being among the first to compare the outcomes of both mandated and voluntary homeschooling populations with those of in-person learners during the pandemic.
Practically speaking, our results inform our understanding of how different divisions of labour support (or interfere with) emotional well-being for parents impacted by COVID-19 pandemic-related disruptions. Indeed, approximately 4–8% of the variance in our well-being outcomes is accounted for by specialisation and/or equity, suggesting that they have meaningful impacts on well-being. This is vital information to have in the face of the current pandemic and the increased need for mental health services [163], especially as the division of labour might present itself as a potential therapeutic target for couples in therapy [164]. Division of labour is also uniquely suited as a target for interventions given its adaptability and malleable nature in comparison to other risk factors that might impact couples’ wellbeing, e.g., personality, genetics. For instance, clinicians might provide guidance on how to divide up work and family tasks in a relationship to avoid conflict, promote relationship satisfaction, and support the mental health of both members of a couple. Specifically, based on the results of this study, clinicians might advise couples who are voluntarily homeschooling to promote a more specialised division of labour strategies, and parents with children studying at in-person school might be best advised to adopt highly equitable and/or specialised division of labour arrangements (with potential additive effects of adopting both). However, when dealing with mixed-gender couples specifically, regardless of their homeschooling status, couples should be advised to attempt to adopt both equitable and specialised division of labour strategies to ensure positive impacts on both mothers’ and fathers’ emotional and relationship well-being.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, M.M.E., S.H.D. and S.H.S.; methodology, M.M.E., S.H.D. and S.H.S.; validation, M.M.E. and D.I.D.; formal analysis, M.M.E.; investigation, M.M.E., S.H.D. and S.H.S.; resources, S.H.D. and S.H.S.; data curation, M.M.E. and D.I.D.; writing—original draft preparation, M.M.E.; writing—review and editing, M.M.E., S.H.D., D.I.D., R.N.-A., P.C. and S.H.S.; visualization, M.M.E.; supervision, S.H.D. and S.H.S.; project administration, M.M.E. and D.I.D.; funding acquisition, S.H.D., R.N.-A., P.C. and S.H.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) under Partnership Engage Grant number 1008-2020-0220 (PI = S.H.S.). M.M.E. is funded by a SSHRC Doctoral Fellowship and a Nova Scotia Graduate Scholarship. S.H.S. is funded by a Tier 1 Canada Research Chair in Addictions and Mental Health from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Social Sciences Institutional Review Board (or Research Ethics Board) of Dalhousie University (2020-5336; 5 May 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available in de-identified form on reasonable request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Viner, R.M.; Russell, S.J.; Croker, H.; Packer, J.; Ward, J.; Stansfield, C.; Mytton, O.; Bonell, C.; Booy, R. School closure and management practices during coronavirus outbreaks including COVID-19: A rapid systematic review. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2020, 4, 397–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Lee, J. Mental health effects of school closures during COVID-19. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2020, 4, 421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Brooks, S.K.; Webster, R.K.; Smith, L.E.; Woodland, L.; Wessely, S.; Greenberg, N.; Rubin, G.J. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 2020, 395, 912–920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Shevlin, M.; McBride, O.; Murphy, J.; Miller, J.G.; Hartman, T.K.; Levita, L.; Mason, L.; Martinez, A.P.; McKay, R.; Stocks, T.V.A.; et al. Anxiety, depression, traumatic stress and COVID-19-related anxiety in the UK general population during the COVID-19 pandemic. BJPsych Open 2020, 6, e125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Wenham, C.; Smith, J.; Morgan, R. Gender and COVID-19 Working Group. COVID-19: The gendered impacts of the outbreak. Lancet 2020, 395, 846–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Rodriguez, L.M.; Litt, D.M.; Stewart, S.H. Drinking to cope with the pandemic: The unique associations of COVID-19-related perceived threat and psychological distress to drinking behaviors in American men and women. Addict. Behav. 2020, 110, 106532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Thibaut, F.; Van Wijngaarden-Cremers, P.J.M. Women’s Mental Health in the Time of COVID-19 Pandemic. Front. Glob. Women’s Health 2020, 1, 588372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Paschke, K.; Arnaud, N.; Austermann, M.I.; Thomasius, R. Risk factors for prospective increase in psychological stress during COVID-19 lockdown in a representative sample of adolescents and their parents. BJPsych Open 2021, 7, e94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Kisely, S.; Warren, N.; McMahon, L.; Dalais, C.; Henry, I.; Siskind, D. Occurrence, prevention, and management of the psychological effects of emerging virus outbreaks on healthcare workers: Rapid review and meta-analysis. BMJ 2020, 369, m1642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Newkirk, K.; Perry-Jenkins, M.; Sayer, A.G. Division of Household and Childcare Labor and Relationship Conflict among Low-Income New Parents. Sex Roles 2016, 76, 319–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Blom, N.; Kraaykamp, G.; Verbakel, E. Couples’ Division of Employment and Household Chores and Relationship Satisfaction: A Test of the Specialization and Equity Hypotheses. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 2017, 33, 195–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kim, S.; Mohtadi, H. Labor Specialization and Endogenous Growth. Am. Econ. Rev. 1992, 82, 404–408. [Google Scholar]
  13. Neuman, A.; Guterman, O. Homeschooling Is Not Just about Education: Focuses of Meaning. J. Sch. Choice 2017, 11, 148–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Deacon, S.H.; Rodriguez, L.M.; Elgendi, M.; King, F.E.; Nogueira-Arjona, R.; Sherry, S.B.; Stewart, S.H. Parenting through a pandemic: Mental health and substance use consequences of mandated homeschooling. Couple Fam. Psychol. Res. Pract. 2021, 10, 281–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Shafer, K.; Scheibling, C.; Milkie, M.A. The Division of Domestic Labor before and during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Canada: Stagnation versus Shifts in Fathers’ Contributions. Can. Rev. Sociol. Can. de Sociol. 2020, 57, 523–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Carlson, D.L.; Petts, R.; Pepin, J. Men and Women Agree: During the COVID-19 Pandemic Men Are Doing More at Home; Council on Contemporary Families: Austin, TX, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  17. Craig, L.; Churchill, B. Dual-earner Parent Couples’ Work and Care during COVID-19. Gend. Work. Organ. 2021, 28, 66–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Becker, G.S. A Treatise on the Family; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1991; ISBN 0674020669. [Google Scholar]
  19. Brines, J. Economic Dependency, Gender, and the Division of Labor at Home. Am. J. Sociol. 1994, 100, 652–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sprecher, S. The Relation Between Inequity and Emotions in Close Relationships. Soc. Psychol. Q. 1986, 49, 309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Sprecher, S. Equity and Social Exchange in Dating Couples: Associations with Satisfaction, Commitment, and Stability. J. Marriage Fam. 2001, 63, 599–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Barstad, A. Equality Is Bliss? Relationship Quality and the Gender Division of Household Labor. J. Fam. Issues 2014, 35, 972–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Carlson, D.L.; Miller, A.J.; Sassler, S.; Hanson, S. The Gendered Division of Housework and Couples’ Sexual Relationships: A Reexamination. J. Marriage Fam. 2016, 78, 975–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Carlson, D.L.; Miller, A.J.; Sassler, S. Stalled for Whom? Change in the Division of Particular Housework Tasks and Their Consequences for Middle-to Low-Income Couples. Socius Sociol. Res. Dyn. World 2018, 4, 2378023118765867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. DeMaris, A.; Longmore, M.A. Ideology, Power, and Equity: Testing Competing Explanations for the Perception of Fairness in Household Labor. Soc. Forces 1996, 74, 1043–1071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Lewis, S.N.C.; Cooper, C.L. The transition to parenthood in dual-earner couples. Psychol. Med. 1988, 18, 477–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Kalmijn, M.; Monden, C.W. The division of labor and depressive symptoms at the couple level. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2011, 29, 358–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. England, P.; Farkas, G. Households, Employment, and Gender: A Social, Economic, and Demographic View, 1st ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  29. Van Der Lippe, T.; Jager, A.; Kops, Y. Combination Pressure. Acta Sociol. 2006, 49, 303–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Dai, W. Dual-Earner Couples in the United States. Encycl. Fam. Stud. 2016, 1, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Offer, S. The Costs of Thinking about Work and Family: Mental Labor, Work–Family Spillover, and Gender Inequality among Parents in Dual-earner Families. Sociol. Forum 2014, 29, 916–936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kalmijn, M.; Loeve, A.; Manting, D. Income dynamics in couples and the dissolution of marriage and cohabitation. Demography 2007, 44, 159–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Martinengo, G.; Jacob, J.I.; Hill, E.J. Gender and the Work-Family Interface: Exploring Differences across the Family Life Course. J. Fam. Issues 2010, 31, 1363–1390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hill, E.J.; Jacob, J.I.; Shannon, L.L.; Brennan, R.T.; Blanchard, V.L.; Martinengo, G. Exploring the relationship of workplace flexibility, gender, and life stage to family-to-work conflict, and stress and burnout. Community Work. Fam. 2008, 11, 165–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Higgins, C.; Duxbury, L.; Lee, C. Impact of Life-Cycle Stage and Gender on the Ability to Balance Work and Family Responsibilities. Fam. Relat. 1994, 43, 144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Frone, M.R.; Russell, M.; Cooper, M.L. Relation of work-family conflict to health outcomes: A four-year longitudinal study of employed parents. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 1997, 70, 325–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Zhang, Y.; Duffy, J.F.; De Castillero, E.R. Do sleep disturbances mediate the association between work-family conflict and depressive symptoms among nurses? A cross-sectional study. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 2017, 24, 620–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Higgins, C.A.; Duxbury, L.E.; Irving, R.H. Work-family conflict in the dual-career family. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1992, 51, 51–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Barling, J.; Macewen, K.E. Linking work experiences to facets of marital functioning. J. Organ. Behav. 1992, 13, 573–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Blossfeld, H.-P.; Müller, R. Union Disruption in Comparative Perspective: The Role of Assortative Partner Choice and Careers of Couples. Int. J. Sociol. 2002, 32, 3–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Manting, D.; Loeve, J.A. Economic Circumstances and Union Dissolution of Couples in the 1990s in The Netherlands; Statistics Netherlands Voorburg/Heerlen: Voorburg, Heerlen, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  42. Poortman, A.-R.; Kalmijn, M. Women’s Labour Market Position and Divorce in the Netherlands: Evaluating Economic Interpretations of the Work Effect. Eur. J. Popul. 2002, 18, 175–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Oppenheimer, V.K. Women’s Employment and the Gain to Marriage: The Specialization and Trading Model. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 1997, 23, 431–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Schoen, R.; Astone, N.M.; Kim, Y.J.; Rothert, K.; Standish, N.J. Women’s Employment, Marital Happiness, and Divorce. Soc. Forces 2002, 81, 643–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Minello, A. The pandemic and the female academic. Nature 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Lerer, L.; Medina, J. When Mom’s Zoom Meeting Is the One That Has to Wait. New York Times. 2020. Available online: https://www.nytimes.com/2020/04/22/us/politics/women-coronavirus-2020.html (accessed on 2 January 2021).
  47. Waddell, N.; Overall, N.C.; Chang, V.T.; Hammond, M.D. Gendered division of labor during a nationwide COVID-19 lockdown: Implications for relationship problems and satisfaction. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2021, 38, 1759–1781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Hipp, L.; Bünning, M. Parenthood as a driver of increased gender inequality during COVID-19? Exploratory evidence from Germany. Eur. Soc. 2020, 23, S658–S673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. van der Lippe, T.; Treas, J.; Norbutas, L. Unemployment and the Division of Housework in Europe. Work. Employ. Soc. 2017, 32, 650–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Becker, C.; Kirchmaier, I.; Trautmann, S.T. Marriage, parenthood and social network: Subjective well-being and mental health in old age. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0218704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Helbig, S.; Lampert, T.; Klose, M.; Jacobi, F. Is Parenthood Associated with Mental Health? Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2006, 41, 889–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Kalucza, S.; Hammarström, A.; Nilsson, K. Mental health and parenthood—A longitudinal study of the relationship between self-reported mental health and parenthood. Health Sociol. Rev. 2015, 24, 283–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Balbo, N.; Arpino, B. The Role of Family Orientations in Shaping the Effect of Fertility on Subjective Well-being: A Propensity Score Matching Approach. Demography 2016, 53, 955–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Clark, A.E.; Diener, E.; Georgellis, Y.; Lucas, R. Lags and Leads in Life Satisfaction: A Test of the Baseline Hypothesis. Econ. J. 2008, 118, F222–F243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Frijters, P.; Johnston, D.W.; Shields, M.A. Life Satisfaction Dynamics with Quarterly Life Event Data. Scand. J. Econ. 2011, 113, 190–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Kohler, H.-P.; Behrman, J.R.; Skytthe, A. Partner + Children = Happiness? An Assessment of the Effect of Fertility and Partnerships on Subjective Well-Being in Danish Twins. Popul. Dev. Rev. 2005, 31, 407–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Pollmann-Schult, M. Parenthood and Life Satisfaction: Why Don’t Children Make People Happy? J. Marriage Fam. 2014, 76, 319–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. United Nations Sustainable Development Group [UNSDG]. Policy Brief: The Impact of COVID-19 on Women; UNSDG: New York, NY, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  59. Douglas, M.; Katikireddi, S.V.; Taulbut, M.; McKee, M.; McCartney, G. Mitigating the wider health effects of COVID-19 pandemic response. BMJ 2020, 369, m1557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Meuwly, N.; Wilhelm, P.; Eicher, V.; Perrez, M. Welchen Einfluss Hat Die Aufteilung von Hausarbeit Und Kinderbetreuung Auf Partnerschaftskonflikte Und Partnerschaftszufriedenheit Bei Berufstätigen Paaren? [Division of Housework and Child Care, Conflict, and Relationship Satisfaction in Dual-Earner Coup. Z. Für Fam. 2011, 23, 37–56. [Google Scholar]
  61. Stevens, D.P.; Kiger, G.; Mannon, S.E. Domestic Labor and Marital Satisfaction: How Much or How Satisfied? Marriage Fam. Rev. 2005, 37, 49–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Guerrero, L.K.; La Valley, A.G.; Farinelli, L. The experience and expression of anger, guilt, and sadness in marriage: An equity theory explanation. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2008, 25, 699–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Kim, S.; Thibodeau, R.; Jorgensen, R.S. Shame, guilt, and depressive symptoms: A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 2011, 137, 68–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Ruppanner, L.; Brandén, M.; Turunen, J. Does Unequal Housework Lead to Divorce? Evidence from Sweden. Sociology 2017, 52, 75–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Schieman, S.; Ruppanner, L.; Milkie, M.A. Who Helps with Homework? Parenting Inequality and Relationship Quality Among Employed Mothers and Fathers. Day Care Early Educ. 2017, 39, 49–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K.; Newton, T.L. Marriage and Health: His and Hers. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 127, 472–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Damaske, S. For the Family? How Class and Gender Shape Womens Work; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2011; ISBN 0199912041. [Google Scholar]
  68. Schwartz, P. Love between Equals: How Peer Marriage Really Works; Simon and Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 1995; ISBN 1439105219. [Google Scholar]
  69. Morgan, D.H. Superdads: How fathers balance work and family in the twenty-first century. J. Gend. Stud. 2014, 23, 219–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Gellert, P.; Häusler, A.; Suhr, R.; Gholami, M.; Rapp, M.; Kuhlmey, A.; Nordheim, J. Testing the stress-buffering hypothesis of social support in couples coping with early-stage dementia. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0189849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. DeMaris, A. The role of relationship inequity in marital disruption. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2007, 24, 177–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Frisco, M.L.; Williams, K. Perceived Housework Equity, Marital Happiness, and Divorce in Dual-Earner Households. J. Fam. Issues 2003, 24, 51–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. DeMaris, A. The 20-year trajectory of marital quality in enduring marriages: Does equity matter? J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2010, 27, 449–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Lavee, Y.; Katz, R. Divison of Labor, Perceived Fairness, and Marital Quality: The Effect of Gender Ideology. J. Marriage Fam. 2002, 64, 27–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Bird, C.E. Gender, Household Labor, and Psychological Distress: The Impact of the Amount and Division of Housework. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1999, 40, 32–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Goldberg, A.E.; Perry-Jenkins, M. Division of Labor and Working-Class Women’s Well-Being Across the Transition to Parenthood. J. Fam. Psychol. 2004, 18, 225–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Harryson, L.; Novo, M.; Hammarström, A. Is gender inequality in the domestic sphere associated with psychological distress among women and men? Results from the Northern Swedish Cohort. J. Epidemiol. Community Heal. 2010, 66, 271–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Nomaguchi, K.M.; Milkie, M.A. Costs and Rewards of Children: The Effects of Becoming a Parent on Adults’ Lives. J. Marriage Fam. 2003, 65, 356–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Riina, E.M.; Feinberg, M.E. Involvement in Childrearing and Mothers’ and Fathers’ Adjustment. Fam. Relat. 2012, 61, 836–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Thurston, R.C.; Sherwood, A.; Matthews, K.A.; Blumenthal, J.A. Household Responsibilities, Income, and Ambulatory Blood Pressure among Working Men and Women. Psychosom. Med. 2011, 73, 200–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Lively, K.J.; Steelman, L.C.; Powell, B. Equity, Emotion, and Household Division of Labor Response. Soc. Psychol. Q. 2010, 73, 358–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Sevilla, A.; Smith, S. Baby Steps: The Gender Division of Childcare during the COVID-19 Pandemic; IZA Working Paper Series; ISER: London, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  83. Bianchi, S.M.; Milkie, M.A.; Sayer, L.C.; Robinson, J.P. Is Anyone Doing the Housework? Trends in the Gender Division of Household Labor. Soc. Forces 2000, 79, 191–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Bianchi, S.M.; Sayer, L.C.; Milkie, M.A.; Robinson, J.P. Housework: Who Did, Does or Will Do It, and How Much Does It Matter? Soc. Forces 2012, 91, 55–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Rizavi, S.S.; Sofer, C. The Division of Labour within the Household: Is There Any Escape from Traditional Gender Roles? 2008; unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
  86. Horne, R.M.; Johnson, M.D.; Galambos, N.L.; Krahn, H.J. Time, Money, or Gender? Predictors of the Division of Household Labour across Life Stages. Sex Roles 2017, 78, 731–743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Carlson, D.L.; Petts, R.J.; Pepin, J.R. Flexplace Work and Partnered Fathers’ Time in Housework and Childcare. Men Masculinities 2021, 24, 547–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Pedulla, D.S.; Thébaud, S. Can We Finish the Revolution? Gender, Work-Family Ideals, and Institutional Constraint. Am. Sociol. Rev. 2015, 80, 116–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Farré, L.; González, L. Does paternity leave reduce fertility? J. Public Econ. 2019, 172, 52–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development [OECD]. Women at the Core of the Fight against COVID-19 Crisis; OECD: Paris, France, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  91. Tamm, M. Fathers’ parental leave-taking, childcare involvement and labor market participation. Labour Econ. 2019, 59, 184–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. DesRoches, D.; Deacon, S.; Rodriguez, L.; Sherry, S.; Nogueira-Arjona, R.; Elgendi, M.; Meier, S.; Abbass, A.; King, F.; Stewart, S. Homeschooling during COVID-19: Gender Differences in Work–Family Conflict and Alcohol Use Behaviour among Romantic Couples. Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Jackson, G.; Allan, S. Fundamental Elements in a Child’s Right to Education: A Study of Home Education Research and Regulation in Australia. Int. Electron. J. Elem. Educ. 2010, 2, 350–366. [Google Scholar]
  94. McDowell, S.A. The Home Schooling Mother-Teacher: Toward a Theory of Social Integration. Peabody J. Educ. 2000, 75, 187–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Ruth, M. Home Education: Constructions of Choice. Int. Electron. J. Elem. Educ. 2010, 3, 45–56. [Google Scholar]
  96. Stevens, M.L. Kingdom of Children: Culture and Controversy in the Homeschooling Movement; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  97. Baker, E.E. Motherhood, homeschooling, and mental health. Sociol. Compass 2019, 13, e12725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Lois, J. The Temporal Emotion Work of Motherhood: Homeschoolers’ Strategies for Managing Time Shortage. Gend. Soc. 2010, 24, 421–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Lois, J. Home Is Where the School Is: The Logic of Homeschooling and the Emotional Labor of Mothering; New York University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; ISBN 0814789439. [Google Scholar]
  100. Sherfinski, M.; Chesanko, M. Disturbing the data: Looking into gender and family size matters with US Evangelical homeschoolers. Gend. Place Cult. 2014, 23, 18–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Howell, J.S.; Sheran, M.E. Homeschooling in the United States: Revelation or Revolution? Unpubl. Work. Pap. 2008. [Google Scholar]
  102. Furness, A. Helping Homeschoolers in the Library; American Library Association: Chicago, IL, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  103. Hanna, L.G. Homeschooling Education. Educ. Urban Soc. 2011, 44, 609–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Ackerman, R.A.; Kenny, D.A. APIMPowerR: An Interactive Tool for Actor-Partner Interdependence Model Power Analysis. Comput. Softw. 2016. [Google Scholar]
  105. Fleury, M.-J.; Grenier, G.; Bamvita, J.-M.; Perreault, M.; Caron, J. Typology of adults diagnosed with mental disorders based on socio-demographics and clinical and service use characteristics. BMC Psychiatry 2011, 11, 67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  106. Plass-Christl, A.; Haller, A.-C.; Otto, C.; Barkmann, C.; Wiegand-Grefe, S.; Hölling, H.; Schulte-Markwort, M.; Ravens-Sieberer, U.; Klasen, F. Parents with mental health problems and their children in a German population based sample: Results of the BELLA study. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0180410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  107. Harris, K.M.; Edlund, M.J.; Larson, S. Racial and Ethnic Differences in the Mental Health Problems and Use of Mental Health Care. Med. Care 2005, 43, 775–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Jose, O.; Alfons, V. Do Demographics Affect Marital Satisfaction? J. Sex Marital Ther. 2007, 33, 73–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Zainah, Z.; Nasir, R.; Hashim, R.S.; Yusof, N. Effects of Demographic Variables on Marital Satisfaction. Asian Soc. Sci. 2012, 8, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Statistics Canada. General Social Survey on Time Use. Available online: https://www23.statcan.gc.ca/imdb/p3Instr.pl?Function=assembleInstr&lang=en&Item_Id=217656 (accessed on 5 May 2020).
  111. Spitzer, R.L.; Kroenke, K.; Williams, J.B.W.; Löwe, B. A Brief Measure for Assessing Generalized Anxiety Disorder: The GAD-7: The GAD-7. Arch. Intern. Med. 2006, 166, 1092–1097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Kroenke, K.; Spitzer, R.L.; Williams, J.B.W. The PHQ-9: Validity of a Brief Depression Severity Measure. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2001, 16, 606–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Löwe, B.; Decker, O.; Müller, S.; Brähler, E.; Schellberg, D.; Herzog, W.; Herzberg, P.Y. Validation and Standardization of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Screener (GAD-7) in the General Population. Med. Care 2008, 46, 266–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Kroenke, K.; Spitzer, R.L.; Williams, J.B.W.; Löwe, B. The Patient Health Questionnaire Somatic, Anxiety, and Depressive Symptom Scales: A systematic review. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 2010, 32, 345–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Cohen, S.; Kamarck, T.; Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983, 24, 385–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Karam, F.; Bérard, A.; Sheehy, O.; Huneau, M.-C.; Briggs, G.; Chambers, C.; Einarson, A.; Johnson, D.; Kao, K.; Koren, G.; et al. Reliability and validity of the 4-item perceived stress scale among pregnant women: Results from the OTIS antidepressants study. Res. Nurs. Health 2012, 35, 363–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Warttig, S.L.; Forshaw, M.J.; South, J.; White, A. New, normative, English-sample data for the Short Form Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4). J. Health. Psychol. 2013, 18, 1617–1628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Loewenthal, K.M.; Lewis, C.A. An Introduction to Psychological Tests and Scales, 2nd ed.; Psychology Press: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Murray, S.L.; Griffin, D.W.; Rose, P.; Bellavia, G.M. Calibrating the sociometer: The relational contingencies of self-esteem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 63–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Mackinnon, S.P.; Sherry, S.B.; Antony, M.M.; Stewart, S.H.; Sherry, D.L.; Hartling, N. Caught in a bad romance: Perfectionism, conflict, and depression in romantic relationships. J. Fam. Psychol. 2012, 26, 215–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  121. Funk, J.L.; Rogge, R.D. Testing the ruler with item response theory: Increasing precision of measurement for relationship satisfaction with the Couples Satisfaction Index. J. Fam. Psychol. 2007, 21, 572–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Graham, J.M.; Diebels, K.J.; Barnow, Z.B. The reliability of relationship satisfaction: A reliability generalization meta-analysis. J. Fam. Psychol. 2011, 25, 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  123. Koller, M. robustlmm: An R Package for Robust Estimation of Linear Mixed-Effects Models. J. Stat. Softw. 2016, 75, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  124. Field, A.P.; Wilcox, R.R. Robust statistical methods: A primer for clinical psychology and experimental psychopathology researchers. Behav. Res. Ther. 2017, 98, 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Vallejo, M.A.; Vallejo-Slocker, L.; Fernández-Abascal, E.G.; Mañanes, G. Determining Factors for Stress Perception Assessed with the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4) in Spanish and Other European Samples. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  126. Malik, A.O.; Peri-Okonny, P.; Gosch, K.; Thomas, M.; Mena, C.; Hiatt, W.R.; Jones, P.G.; Provance, J.B.; Labrosciano, C.; Jelani, Q.-U.; et al. Association of Perceived Stress Levels With Long-term Mortality in Patients With Peripheral Artery Disease. JAMA Netw. Open 2020, 3, e208741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Johnson, S.U.; Ulvenes, P.G.; Øktedalen, T.; Hoffart, A. Psychometric Properties of the General Anxiety Disorder 7-Item (GAD-7) Scale in a Heterogeneous Psychiatric Sample. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  128. Lambe, L.; Mackinnon, S.P.; Stewart, S.H. Dyadic conflict, drinking to cope, and alcohol-related problems: A psychometric study and longitudinal actor–partner interdependence model. J. Fam. Psychol. 2015, 29, 697–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  129. Cacioppo, J.T.; Cacioppo, S.; Gonzaga, G.C.; Ogburn, E.L.; VanderWeele, T.J. Marital satisfaction and break-ups differ across on-line and off-line meeting venues. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 10135–10140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  130. Baxter, J. The Joys and Justice of Housework. Sociology 2000, 34, 609–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Braun, M.; Lewin-Epstein, N.; Stier, H.; Baumgärtner, M.K. Perceived Equity in the Gendered Division of Household Labor. J. Marriage Fam. 2008, 70, 1145–1156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Young, M.; Wallace, J.E.; Polachek, A.J. Gender Differences in Perceived Domestic Task Equity. J. Fam. Issues 2013, 36, 1751–1781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  133. West, C.; Zimmerman, D.H. Doing Gender. Gend. Soc. 1987, 1, 125–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Coltrane, S. Research on Household Labor: Modeling and Measuring the Social Embeddedness of Routine Family Work. J. Marriage Fam. 2000, 62, 1208–1233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Chen, J.; Houser, D. When are women willing to lead? The effect of team gender composition and gendered tasks. Leadersh. Q. 2019, 30, 101340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Killewald, A.; Gough, M. Does Specialization Explain Marriage Penalties and Premiums? Am. Sociol. Rev. 2013, 78, 477–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  137. Andreeva, E.; Hanson, L.L.M.; Westerlund, H.; Theorell, T.; Brenner, M.H. Depressive symptoms as a cause and effect of job loss in men and women: Evidence in the context of organisational downsizing from the Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health. BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  138. Gili, M.; López-Navarro, E.; Castro, A. Gender differences in mental health during the economic crisis. Psicothema 2016, 28, 407–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. Poortman, A.-R. How Work Affects Divorce. J. Fam. Issues 2005, 26, 168–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  140. Connelly, R.; Kimmel, J. If You’re Happy and You Know It: How Do Mothers and Fathers in the US Really Feel about Caring for Their Children? Fem. Econ. 2014, 21, 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Borelli, J.L.; Nelson, S.K.; River, L.M.; Birken, S.A.; Moss-Racusin, C. Gender Differences in Work-Family Guilt in Parents of Young Children. Sex Roles 2016, 76, 356–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Sendén, M.G.; Klysing, A.; Lindqvist, A.; Renström, E.A. The (Not So) Changing Man: Dynamic Gender Stereotypes in Sweden. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Chesley, N. What Does It Mean to Be a “Breadwinner” Mother? J. Fam. Issues 2016, 38, 2594–2619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Huinink, J.; Reichart, E. Der Weg in Die Traditionelle Arbeitsteilung – Eine Einbahnstraße? The Path to the Traditional Division of Work: A One Way Road? In Familiale Beziehungen, Familienalltag und Soziale Netzwerke: Family Relationships, Everyday Life, and Social Networks; Bien, W., Marbach, J.H., Eds.; VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2008; pp. 43–80. ISBN 978-3-531-91980-5. [Google Scholar]
  145. Pinho, P.D.S.; De Araújo, T.M. Associação entre sobrecarga doméstica e transtornos mentais comuns em mulheres. Rev. Bras. de Epidemiologia 2012, 15, 560–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. Ciciolla, L.; Luthar, S.S. Invisible Household Labor and Ramifications for Adjustment: Mothers as Captains of Households. Sex Roles 2019, 81, 467–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Pietromonaco, P.R.; Overall, N.C. Applying relationship science to evaluate how the COVID-19 pandemic may impact couples’ relationships. Am. Psychol. 2021, 76, 438–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Dooley, D.G.; Simpson, J.N.; Beers, N.S. Returning to School in the Era of COVID-19. JAMA Pediatr. 2020, 174, 1028–1029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  149. Gauthier, G.R.; A Smith, J.; García, C.; A Garcia, M.; A Thomas, P. Exacerbating Inequalities: Social Networks, Racial/Ethnic Disparities, and the COVID-19 Pandemic in the United States. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2020, 76, e88–e92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  150. Lam, C.B.; McHale, S.M.; Crouter, A.C. The Division of Household Labor: Longitudinal Changes and Within-Couple Variation. J. Marriage Fam. 2012, 74, 944–952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  151. Statistics Canada. Canadian Income Survey. 2019. Available online: https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/daily-quotidien/210323/dq210323a-eng.htm (accessed on 15 April 2021).
  152. Statistics Canada. Ethnic and Cultural Origins of Canadians: Portrait of a Rich Heritage. 2017. Available online: https://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2016/as-sa/98-200-x/2016016/98-200-x2016016-eng.cfm (accessed on 15 April 2021).
  153. Semega, J.; Kollar, M.; Shrider, E.A.; Creamer, J. Income and Poverty in the United States: 2019. Available online: https://www.census.gov/library/publications/2020/demo/p60-270.html (accessed on 15 April 2021).
  154. United States Census Bureau. QuickFacts. Available online: https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/fact/table/US/PST045219 (accessed on 15 April 2021).
  155. Statistics Canada. Census in Brief: Portrait of Children’s Family Life in Canada in 2016; Statistics Canada: Ottawa, ON, Canada, 2017; Available online: https://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2016/as-sa/98-200-x/2016006/98-200-x2016006-eng.cfm (accessed on 20 April 2021).
  156. Goldberg, A.E.; Smith, J.Z.; Perry-Jenkins, M. The Division of Labor in Lesbian, Gay, and Heterosexual New Adoptive Parents. J. Marriage Fam. 2012, 74, 812–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Zhao, Y.; Guo, Y.; Xiao, Y.; Zhu, R.; Sun, W.; Huang, W.; Liang, D.; Tang, L.; Zhang, F.; Zhu, D.; et al. The Effects of Online Homeschooling on Children, Parents, and Teachers of Grades 1–9 During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Med. Sci. Monit. 2020, 26, e925591-e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  158. Walter, S.L.; Seibert, S.E.; Goering, D.; O’Boyle, E.H. A Tale of Two Sample Sources: Do Results from Online Panel Data and Conventional Data Converge? J. Bus. Psychol. 2019, 34, 425–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  159. Unruh, L.; Allin, S.; Marchildon, G.; Burke, S.; Barry, S.; Siersbaek, R.; Thomas, S.; Rajan, S.; Koval, A.; Alexander, M.; et al. A comparison of 2020 health policy responses to the COVID-19 pandemic in Canada, Ireland, the United Kingdom and the United States of America. Health Policy 2021, 126, 427–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  160. Guterman, O.; Neuman, A. Emotion, behaviour, and the structuring of home education in Israel: The role of routine. Aust. Educ. Res. 2020, 48, 67–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  161. Guterman, O.; Neuman, A. Personality, socio-economic status and education: Factors that contribute to the degree of structure in homeschooling. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 2017, 21, 75–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  162. Guterman, O.; Neuman, A. Parental attachment and internalizing and externalizing problems of Israeli school-goers and homeschoolers. Sch. Psychol. 2020, 35, 41–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  163. World Health Organization. COVID-19 Disrupting Mental Health Services in Most Countries. 2020. Available online: https://www.who.int/news/item/05-10-2020-covid-19-disrupting-mental-health-services-in-most-countries-who-survey (accessed on 20 April 2021).
  164. Schulz, M.S.; Cowan, C.P.; Cowan, P.A. Promoting healthy beginnings: A randomized controlled trial of a preventive intervention to preserve marital quality during the transition to parenthood. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2006, 74, 20–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the interaction between specialisation and gender on parents’ depression in mixed-sex couples.
Figure 1. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the interaction between specialisation and gender on parents’ depression in mixed-sex couples.
Ijerph 19 17021 g001
Figure 2. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the interaction between equity and gender on parents’ relationship satisfaction in mixed-sex couples.
Figure 2. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the interaction between equity and gender on parents’ relationship satisfaction in mixed-sex couples.
Ijerph 19 17021 g002
Figure 3. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the interaction between specialisation and schooling status on parents’ anxiety.
Figure 3. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the interaction between specialisation and schooling status on parents’ anxiety.
Ijerph 19 17021 g003
Figure 4. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the interaction between equity and schooling status on parents’ relationship satisfaction.
Figure 4. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the interaction between equity and schooling status on parents’ relationship satisfaction.
Ijerph 19 17021 g004
Figure 5. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the three-way interaction between specialisation, gender, and schooling status on parents’ relationship conflict in mixed-gender couples.
Figure 5. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the three-way interaction between specialisation, gender, and schooling status on parents’ relationship conflict in mixed-gender couples.
Ijerph 19 17021 g005
Figure 6. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the three-way interaction between equity, gender, and schooling status on parents’ depression in mixed-gender couples.
Figure 6. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the three-way interaction between equity, gender, and schooling status on parents’ depression in mixed-gender couples.
Ijerph 19 17021 g006
Figure 7. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the three-way interaction between equity, gender, and schooling status on parents’ relationship conflict in mixed-gender couples.
Figure 7. Simple slopes analyses demonstrating the three-way interaction between equity, gender, and schooling status on parents’ relationship conflict in mixed-gender couples.
Ijerph 19 17021 g007
Table 1. Demographics information and relationship characteristics for the study sample.
Table 1. Demographics information and relationship characteristics for the study sample.
VariableIn-Person
Learners
(n = 772)
Mandated
Homeschoolers
(n = 664)
Voluntary
Homeschoolers
(n = 488)
Age—M (SD) ***39.13 (6.70)38.70 (6.91)36.17 (5.61)
Number of children—M (SD) ***2.10 (0.85)1.97 (0.85)1.48 (0.88)
Average child age—M (SD) 8.64 (2.75)8.80 (2.86)8.44 (2.03)
Relationship length (yrs)—M (SD) ***13.63 (6.24)13.05 (6.19)10.64 (5.05)
Gender
 Male388334242
 Female383328244
 Other/ Prefer not to answer122
Relationship Status ***
 Mixed gender642562458
 Same gender1289826
 Other/Prefer not to answer244
Family Income ***
 CAD 25,000 or less per year36338
 Between CAD 26,000 and 50,000787683
 Between CAD 51,000 and 75,00098110160
 Between CAD 76,000 and 100,000172106122
 Between CAD 101,000 and 125,0001108460
 Between CAD 126,000 and 150,0001179438
 CAD 151,000 or more per year12813210
 Prefer not to answer33297
Highest Level of Education ***
 Some high school171510
 High school graduate817444
 Some college/university8867130
 College/university graduate414301214
 Some post-graduate413457
 Post-graduate degree13117333
Employment ***
 Employed (Full/Part time)634529389
 Unemployed11612989
 Student (Full/Part time)2141
 Prefer not to answer129
Ethnicity ***
 White552438416
 Asian or Arab/West Asian 113112623
 Latin American or Black or First Nations 1526037
 Multiracial25286
 Other/Unknown12126
Notes. 1 Combined to maintain confidentiality of respondents due to low numbers in one or more of these categories.  p < 0.10; *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations among study variables.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations among study variables.
1234567In-Person Learners—Mean (SD)Mandated HS—
Mean (SD)
Voluntary HS—
Mean (SD)
  • Depression
0.83 **0.56 **−0.42 **0.52 **−0.08 −0.066.19 (5.96)6.29 (5.61)7.41 (5.65)
2.
Anxiety
0.86 ** 0.58 **−0.42 **0.50 **−0.07−0.035.05 (5.11)5.24 (4.96)5.88 (4.78)
3.
Perceived Stress
0.51 **0.53 ** −0.46 **0.39 **−0.08 −0.026.32 (2.87)6.41 (3.10)7.10 (2.46)
4.
Relationship Satisfaction
−0.36 **−0.35 **−0.40 ** −0.55 **0.07−0.0614.45 (4.48)13.39 (4.79)12.64 (4.27)
5.
Relationship Conflict
0.55 **0.50 **0.38 **−0.51 ** −0.07−0.0319.36 (14.07)19.22 (13.46)24.26 (15.38)
6.
Equity
0.000.02−0.05 0.020.01 −0.18 **0.74 (0.25)0.75 (0.23)0.84 (0.19)
7.
Specialisation
−0.14 **−0.13 **−0.06 *0.01−0.12 **−0.33 ** 0.29 (0.30)0.28 (0.26)0.22 (0.24)
Mean
(Total sample)
6.545.336.5513.6220.550.770.27
SD
(Total sample)
5.784.982.874.5914.370.230.27
Notes. Correlations for the whole sample (n = 1924 individuals; 962 couples) are below the diagonal; for the mandated homeschoolers (n = 664 individuals; 332 couples), the correlations are above the diagonal. HS stands for Homeschoolers.  p < 0.05; * p < 0.01; ** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Effects of specialisation on parents’ well-being.
Table 3. Effects of specialisation on parents’ well-being.
OutcomePredictorsBSE (b)95% CIp
Depression
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.044/0.622
AIC/BIC = 11,303/11,353
Intercept10.46 **1.218.08–12.83<0.001
Parent age−0.060.03−0.12–0.000.068
No. of children−0.48 0.2−0.87–−0.090.015
Relationship length−0.040.04−0.11–0.030.253
Family Income−0.070.1−0.26–0.120.470
Education0.010.11−0.20–0.220.927
Specialisation−2.91 **0.62−4.12–−1.69<0.001
Anxiety
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.036/0.544
AIC/BIC = 10,882/10,932
Intercept8.60 **1.036.57–10.62<0.001
Parent age−0.040.03−0.10–0.010.106
No. of children−0.34†0.17−0.66–−0.010.043
Relationship length−0.040.03−0.10–0.010.135
Family Income−0.010.08−0.17–0.150.878
Education−0.070.1−0.26–0.120.497
Specialisation−2.20 **0.52−3.22–−1.17<0.001
Perceived Stress
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.030/0.495
AIC/BIC = 8878.0/8927.8
Intercept9.22 **0.598.07–10.36<0.001
Parent age−0.04 *0.02−0.07–−0.010.007
No. of children−0.170.09−0.35–0.020.076
Relationship length−0.0010.02−0.03–0.030.935
Family Income−0.14 *0.05−0.23–−0.050.003
Education−0.030.06−0.14–0.080.559
Specialisation−0.68 0.3−1.26–−0.100.022
Relationship Satisfaction
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.026/0.779
AIC/BIC = 10,054/10,103
Intercept12.49 **0.9910.54–14.43<0.001
Parent age−0.020.03−0.08–0.030.362
No. of children0.76 **0.170.44–1.08<0.001
Relationship length0.040.03−0.02–0.100.200
Family Income0.040.08−0.11–0.190.625
Education−0.010.07−0.16–0.140.901
Specialisation0.020.52−1.01–1.040.972
Relationship Conflict
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.043/0.703
AIC/BIC = 14,513/14,562
Intercept30.33 **3.0624.34–36.33<0.001
Parent age−0.150.08−0.31–0.010.058
No. of children−1.58 *0.5−2.57–−0.590.002
Relationship length−0.150.09−0.33–0.030.094
Family Income0.360.24−0.11–0.830.138
Education−0.080.26−0.58–0.420.749
Specialisation−5.07 *1.59−8.19–−1.960.001
Note.  p < 0.05; * p < 0.01; ** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Effects of equity on parents’ well-being.
Table 4. Effects of equity on parents’ well-being.
OutcomePredictorsBSE (b)95% CIp
Depression
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.026/0.622
AIC/BIC =11,324/11,374.2
Intercept10.98 **1.228.59–13.37<0.001
Parent age−0.060.03−0.12–0.000.055
No. of children−0.65 *0.2−1.04–−0.260.001
Relationship Length−0.050.04−0.12–0.020.153
Family Income−0.050.1−0.24–0.140.602
Education0.010.11−0.21–0.220.961
Equity−0.340.74−1.79–1.100.642
Anxiety
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.022/0.544
AIC/BIC =10,899/10,949.0
Intercept8.99 **1.046.96–11.02<0.001
Parent age−0.050.03−0.10–0.010.090
No. of children−0.45 *0.17−0.78–−0.120.007
Relationship Length−0.050.03−0.11–0.010.082
Family Income−0.010.08−0.17–0.160.941
Education−0.070.1−0.26–0.120.469
Equity0.120.62−1.10–1.340.848
Perceived Stress
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.029/0.495
AIC/BIC = 8880/8929.7
Intercept9.34 **0.588.19–10.48<0.001
Parent age−0.04 *0.02−0.07–−0.010.006
No. of children−0.23 0.09−0.41–−0.040.015
Relationship Length0.0010.02−0.04–0.030.789
Family Income−0.12 *0.05−0.22–−0.030.009
Education−0.030.06−0.14–0.080.571
Equity−0.630.35−1.31–0.050.070
Relationship Satisfaction
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.027/0.779
AIC/BIC = 10,052/10,101.7
Intercept12.47 **0.9810.54–14.40<0.001
Parent age−0.020.03−0.08–0.030.369
No. of children0.79 **0.170.47–1.12<0.001
Relationship Length0.040.03−0.02–0.100.189
Family Income0.020.08−0.13–0.170.760
Education−0.010.07−0.16–0.130.879
Equity0.770.61−0.43–1.970.208
Relationship Conflict
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.034/0.703
AIC/BIC = 14,523/14,572.4
Intercept31.25 **3.0625.25–37.24<0.001
Parent age−0.160.08−0.32–−0.000.050
No. of children−1.88 **0.5−2.87–−0.90<0.001
Relationship Length−0.170.09−0.35–0.010.061
Family Income0.390.24−0.09–0.870.107
Education−0.090.26−0.59–0.410.732
Equity−0.941.87−4.61–2.730.616
Note.  p < 0.05; * p < 0.01; ** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Effects of specialisation on Male vs. Female parents’ well-being in mixed-sex couples.
Table 5. Effects of specialisation on Male vs. Female parents’ well-being in mixed-sex couples.
OutcomePredictorsBSE (b)95% CIp
Depression
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.054/0.598
AIC/BIC = 9669.8/9729.0
Intercept10.39 **1.247.96–12.81<0.001
Parent age−0.050.03−0.11–0.010.127
No. of children−0.52 *0.2−0.91–−0.140.008
Relationship length−0.040.04−0.11–0.030.283
Family Income−0.120.1−0.31–0.070.229
Education−0.020.11−0.24–0.200.873
Specialisation−2.58 **0.63−3.81–−1.34<0.001
Gender0.40 **0.090.22–0.57<0.001
Specialisation × Gender1.24 **0.320.62–1.87<0.001
Anxiety
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.050/0.532
AIC/BIC = 9345.8/9405.0
Intercept8.62 **1.076.51–10.72<0.001
Parent age−0.040.03−0.09–0.020.166
No. of children−0.4 0.17−0.74–−0.070.018
Relationship length−0.040.03−0.10–0.020.199
Family Income−0.040.09−0.21–0.130.614
Education−0.070.1−0.27–0.130.468
Specialisation−2.12 **0.54−3.19–−1.06<0.001
Gender0.45 **0.080.28–0.61<0.001
Specialisation × Gender0.87 *0.30.27–1.460.004
Perceived Stress
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.047/0.512
AIC/BIC = 7708.7/7767.8
Intercept9.52 **0.648.26–10.77<0.001
Parent age−0.04 *0.02−0.08–−0.010.008
No. of children−0.190.1−0.39–0.010.057
Relationship length0.0010.02−0.04–0.040.982
Family Income−0.16 *0.05−0.26–−0.060.001
Education−0.050.06−0.17–0.070.430
Specialisation−0.550.32−1.18–0.090.090
Gender0.27 **0.050.17–0.37<0.001
Specialisation × Gender0.56 *0.180.20–0.920.002
Relationship Satisfaction
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.033/0.788
AIC/BIC = 8646.9/8706.1
Intercept12.57 **1.0510.50–14.64<0.001
Parent age−0.020.03−0.08–0.030.426
No. of children0.82 **0.170.48–1.16<0.001
Relationship length0.040.03−0.03–0.100.236
Family Income0.020.08−0.14–0.180.811
Education−0.040.08−0.19–0.110.599
Specialisation−0.260.56−1.35–0.830.641
Gender−0.19 **0.05−0.29–−0.09<0.001
Specialisation × Gender−0.64 **0.19−1.02–−0.270.001
Relationship Conflict
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.043/0.667
AIC/BIC = 12,483.8/12,543.0
Intercept30.08 **3.1223.96–36.19<0.001
Parent age−0.120.08−0.28–0.040.156
No. of children−1.76 **0.5−2.74–−0.77<0.001
Relationship length−0.180.09−0.36–0.010.059
Family Income0.190.25−0.30–0.670.445
Education−0.150.27−0.67–0.380.584
Specialisation−3.26 1.61−6.41–−0.110.042
Gender0.30.2−0.09–0.700.129
Specialisation × Gender1.91 *0.720.49–3.320.008
Note.  p < 0.05; * p < 0.01; ** p < 0.001. Gender-coded −1 (Male), +1 (Female).
Table 6. Effects of equity on Male vs. Female parents’ well-being in mixed-sex couples.
Table 6. Effects of equity on Male vs. Female parents’ well-being in mixed-sex couples.
OutcomePredictorsBSE (b)95% CIp
Depression
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.039/0.593
AIC/BIC = 9693.5/9752.7
Intercept10.97 **1.248.54–13.40<0.001
Parent age−0.050.03−0.12–0.010.101
No. of children−0.73 **0.2−1.12–−0.34<0.001
Relationship length−0.050.04−0.12–0.030.210
Family Income−0.080.1−0.28–0.110.411
Education−0.050.11−0.27–0.170.675
Equity−1.040.77−2.55–0.470.177
Gender0.42 **0.090.25–0.60<0.001
Equity × Gender−0.96 0.39−1.72–−0.190.014
Anxiety
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.036/0.531
AIC/BIC = 9363.3/9422.5
Intercept9.07 **1.086.96–11.19<0.001
Parent age−0.040.03−0.10–0.010.140
No. of children−0.55 *0.17−0.88–−0.210.001
Relationship length−0.050.03−0.11–0.020.144
Family Income−0.030.09−0.20–0.150.760
Education−0.10.1−0.30–0.100.346
Equity−0.280.67−1.59–1.020.670
Gender0.46 **0.080.30–0.63<0.001
Equity × Gender−0.88 0.37−1.60–−0.160.017
Perceived Stress
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.045/0.508
AIC/BIC = 7714.6/7773.8
Intercept9.66 **0.648.42–10.91<0.001
Parent age−0.05 *0.02−0.08–−0.010.006
No. of children−0.26 0.1−0.45–−0.060.010
Relationship length0.0010.02−0.04–0.030.925
Family Income−0.14 *0.05−0.25–−0.040.005
Education−0.060.06−0.18–0.060.355
Equity−0.710.39−1.48–0.060.069
Gender0.28 **0.050.18–0.38<0.001
Equity × Gender−0.370.22−0.81–0.070.099
Relationship Satisfaction
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.038/0.789
AIC/BIC =8635.5/ 8694.7
Intercept12.45 **1.0510.40–14.50<0.001
Parent age−0.020.03−0.08–0.030.459
No. of children0.87 **0.170.53–1.21<0.001
Relationship length0.040.03−0.03–0.100.253
Family Income−0.010.08−0.17–0.150.859
Education−0.020.08−0.17–0.130.830
Equity1.34 0.670.03–2.660.046
Gender−0.20 **0.05−0.31–−0.10<0.001
Equity × Gender1.00 **0.230.55–1.46<0.001
Relationship Conflict
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.041/0.664
AIC/BIC = 12,491.6/ 12,550.8
Intercept30.86 **3.1124.77–36.96<0.001
Parent age−0.120.08−0.29–0.040.133
No. of children−2.10 **0.5−3.09–−1.12<0.001
Relationship length−0.18 0.09−0.37–−0.000.048
Family Income0.270.25−0.22–0.760.278
Education−0.180.27−0.70–0.340.501
Equity−3.211.95−7.04–0.620.100
Gender0.340.2−0.05–0.730.089
Equity × Gender−0.650.88−2.38–1.070.458
Note.  p < 0.05; * p < 0.01; ** p < 0.001. Gender-coded −1 (Male), +1 (Female).
Table 7. Effects of specialisation and homeschooling status on parents’ well-being.
Table 7. Effects of specialisation and homeschooling status on parents’ well-being.
OutcomePredictorsBSE (b)95% CIp
Depression
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 =
0.054/0.623
AIC/BIC =
11,291.4/11,363.2
Intercept10.20 **1.387.49–12.91<0.001
Parent age−0.050.03−0.11–0.010.116
No. of children−0.370.2−0.77–0.030.069
Relationship Length−0.040.04−0.11–0.030.240
Family Income−0.080.1−0.27–0.110.433
Education0.030.11−0.18–0.250.760
Specialisation−12.33 **3.53−19.26–−5.41<0.001
Homeschooling 1−0.010.24−0.47–0.460.979
Homeschooling 2−0.180.27−0.72–0.360.511
Specialisation × Homeschooling 11.110.83−0.52–2.730.182
Specialisation × Homeschooling 23.32 *1.021.33–5.320.001
Anxiety
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 =
0.053/0.546
AIC/BIC =
10,866.6/10,938.4
Intercept7.99 **1.175.70–10.28<0.001
Parent age−0.040.03−0.09–0.020.183
No. of children−0.240.17−0.57–0.100.169
Relationship Length−0.050.03−0.11–0.010.112
Family Income−0.030.08−0.20–0.130.686
Education−0.040.1−0.23–0.150.648
Specialisation−13.79 **2.97−19.61–−7.97<0.001
Homeschooling 10.030.2−0.36–0.420.885
Homeschooling 20.0030.23−0.45–0.450.989
Specialisation × Homeschooling 11.52 0.70.15–2.880.029
Specialisation × Homeschooling 23.92 **0.852.24–5.59<0.001
Perceived Stress
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 =
0.034/0.496
AIC/BIC = 8879.1/8951.0
Intercept9.61 **0.678.30–10.92<0.001
Parent age−0.04 0.02−0.07–−0.010.012
No. of children−0.120.1−0.31–0.070.210
Relationship Length−0.0000.02−0.03–0.0310.000
Family Income−0.13 *0.05−0.22–−0.030.008
Education−0.020.06−0.13–0.090.683
Specialisation−0.881.69−4.19–2.430.603
Homeschooling 1−0.090.11−0.31–0.130.436
Homeschooling 2−0.250.13−0.51–0.000.052
Specialisation × Homeschooling 1−0.110.4−0.89–0.660.776
Specialisation × Homeschooling 20.220.49−0.73–1.170.651
Relationship
Satisfaction
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 =
0.049/0.780
AIC/BIC =
10,036.6/10,108.4
Intercept10.93 **1.138.71–13.15<0.001
Parent age−0.040.03−0.09–0.020.173
No. of children0.57 **0.170.24–0.900.001
Relationship Length0.040.03−0.02–0.090.226
Family Income0.020.08−0.13–0.170.815
Education−0.020.07−0.16–0.130.832
Specialisation8.42 *2.952.64–14.200.004
Homeschooling 10.73 **0.20.34–1.11<0.001
Homeschooling 20.51 0.230.07–0.960.024
Specialisation × Homeschooling 1−1.180.69−2.53–0.180.088
Specialisation × Homeschooling 2−2.80 **0.85−4.46–−1.130.001
Relationship Conflict
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 =
0.060/0.704
AIC/BIC =
14,498.5/14,570.4
Intercept33.55 **3.4926.71–40.39<0.001
Parent age−0.120.08−0.28–0.040.139
No. of children−1.05†0.52−2.07–−0.040.042
Relationship Length−0.140.09−0.32–0.030.114
Family Income0.460.24−0.02–0.930.060
Education−0.010.26−0.50–0.500.995
Specialisation−21.41 8.99−39.04–−3.780.017
Homeschooling 1−0.860.6−2.04–0.320.153
Homeschooling 2−2.29 **0.7−3.66–−0.930.001
Specialisation × Homeschooling 11.272.11−2.87–5.400.548
Specialisation × Homeschooling 26.52†2.591.44–11.590.012
Notes. p < 0.05; * p < 0.01; ** p < 0.001. Homeschooling status 1 coded as 0 (voluntary homeschoolers), 1 (not homeschooling, i.e., child attending school in-person), and −1 (mandated homeschooling due to COVID-19). Homeschooling status 2 coded as −1 (voluntary homeschoolers), 0 (not homeschooling, i.e., child attending school in-person), and 1 (mandated homeschooling due to COVID-19).
Table 8. Effects of equity and homeschooling status on parents’ well-being.
Table 8. Effects of equity and homeschooling status on parents’ well-being.
OutcomePredictorsBSE (b)95% CIp
Depression
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.029/0.623
AIC/BIC =
11,328.5/11,400.3
Intercept11.23 **1.428.44–14.02<0.001
Parent age−0.060.03−0.12–0.010.078
No. of children−0.59 *0.2−0.99–−0.190.004
Relationship Length−0.050.04−0.12–0.020.184
Family Income−0.020.1−0.22–0.170.815
Education0.030.11−0.19–0.240.815
Equity0.334.45−8.39–9.060.941
Homeschooling 1−0.060.24−0.54–0.410.792
Homeschooling 2−0.330.29−0.89–0.230.248
Equity × Homeschooling 10.581.01−1.39–2.560.561
Equity × Homeschooling 2−0.971.28−3.48–1.540.449
Anxiety
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.025/0.546
AIC/BIC =
10,903.5/10,975.3
Intercept8.78 **1.216.41–11.14<0.001
Parent age−0.040.03−0.10–0.010.116
No. of children−0.42 0.17−0.75–−0.080.016
Relationship Length−0.050.03−0.11–0.010.102
Family Income−0.010.09−0.16–0.170.955
Education−0.060.1−0.25–0.140.574
Equity3.013.75−4.34–10.350.422
Homeschooling 100.2−0.40–0.400.988
Homeschooling 2−0.090.24−0.56–0.390.723
Equity × Homeschooling 10.140.85−1.52–1.800.871
Equity × Homeschooling 2−1.51.08−3.61–0.610.164
Perceived Stress
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.035/0.496
AIC/BIC =
8879.9/8951.7
Intercept9.71 **0.688.38–11.04<0.001
Parent age−0.04 0.02−0.07–−0.010.013
No. of children−0.170.1−0.36–0.020.083
Relationship Length0.0010.02−0.04–0.030.896
Family Income−0.1 0.05−0.20–−0.010.033
Education−0.020.06−0.13–0.090.735
Equity1.962.09−2.14–6.060.350
Homeschooling 1−0.120.11−0.34–0.110.310
Homeschooling 2−0.29 0.13−0.56–−0.030.030
Equity × Homeschooling 1−0.50.47−1.43–0.430.290
Equity × Homeschooling 2−0.770.6−1.95–0.410.199
Relationship
Satisfaction
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.051/0.780
AIC/BIC =
10,033.7/10,105.5
Intercept11.06 **1.158.81–13.31<0.001
Parent age−0.030.03−0.09–0.020.185
No. of children0.61 **0.170.28–0.94<0.001
Relationship Length0.030.03−0.03–0.090.301
Family Income−0.020.08−0.17–0.140.841
Education−0.020.07−0.16–0.130.818
Equity−9.89 *3.65−17.05–−2.730.007
Homeschooling 10.72 **0.20.33–1.11<0.001
Homeschooling 20.51 0.230.05–0.970.029
Equity × Homeschooling 11.81 0.830.20–3.430.028
Equity × Homeschooling 23.27 *1.051.21–5.330.002
Relationship Conflict
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.048/0.704
AIC/BIC =
14,514.7/14,586.5
Intercept35.69 **3.5628.71–42.68<0.001
Parent age−0.130.08−0.29–0.030.106
No. of children−1.47 *0.52−2.48–−0.460.004
Relationship Length−0.150.09−0.33–0.020.089
Family Income0.58 0.250.10–1.070.019
Education−0.010.26−0.51–0.490.967
Equity−2.4511.24−24.47–19.570.827
Homeschooling 1−1.030.61−2.23–0.170.091
Homeschooling 2−2.69 **0.72−4.10–−1.27<0.001
Equity × Homeschooling 11.332.54−3.64–6.310.599
Equity × Homeschooling 2−1.213.23−7.55–5.120.707
Notes. p < 0.05; * p < 0.01; ** p < 0.001. Homeschooling status 1 coded as 0 (voluntary homeschoolers), 1 (not homeschooling, i.e., child attending school in-person), and −1 (mandated homeschooling due to COVID-19). Homeschooling status 2 coded as −1 (voluntary homeschoolers), 0 (not homeschooling, i.e., child attending school in-person), and 1 (mandated homeschooling due to COVID-19).
Table 9. Effects of specialisation and homeschooling status on male vs. female parents’ well-being in mixed-sex couples.
Table 9. Effects of specialisation and homeschooling status on male vs. female parents’ well-being in mixed-sex couples.
OutcomePredictorsBSE (b)95% CIp
Depression 
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.078/0.601
AIC/BIC = 9655.9/9758.1
Intercept11.55 ***1.328.97–14.13<0.001
Parent age−0.050.03−0.10–−0.000.050
No. of children−0.310.2−0.70–0.090.125
Relationship length0.0010.002−0.00–0.000.433
Family Income−0.110.1−0.31–0.080.247
Education−0.0010.11−0.22–0.220.969
Specialisation−16.43 ***3.44−23.18–−9.69<0.001
Gender−0.550.46−1.46–0.350.232
Homeschooling 1−0.460.24−0.93–0.020.060
Homeschooling 2−0.61 0.27−1.15–−0.080.024
Gender × Specialisation0.281.77−3.19–3.750.876
Gender × Homeschooling 10.230.12−0.01–0.470.060
Gender × Homeschooling 20.230.13−0.03–0.490.081
Specialisation × Homeschooling 12.34 **0.830.71–3.970.005
Specialisation × Homeschooling 24.21 ***0.992.27–6.15<0.001
Specialisation × Gender × Homeschooling 1−0.040.43−0.89–0.810.935
Specialisation × Gender × Homeschooling 20.460.51−0.54–1.460.369
Anxiety
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.077/0.538
AIC/BIC = 9324.5/9426.7
Intercept9.01 ***1.146.77–11.25<0.001
Parent age−0.05 0.02−0.09–−0.000.036
No. of children−0.280.17−0.61–0.060.109
Relationship length0.0010.001−0.00–0.000.569
Family Income−0.070.09−0.24–0.100.418
Education−0.070.1−0.27–0.130.482
Specialisation−16.18 ***2.96−21.98–−10.37<0.001
Gender−0.87 0.44−1.73–−0.010.047
Homeschooling 1−0.220.21−0.63–0.180.282
Homeschooling 2−0.180.23−0.63–0.280.454
Gender × Specialisation−0.481.67−3.76–2.800.773
Gender × Homeschooling 10.31 **0.120.08–0.530.008
Gender × Homeschooling 20.33 **0.130.09–0.580.008
Specialisation × Homeschooling 12.34 **0.720.93–3.740.001
Specialisation × Homeschooling 24.29 ***0.852.62–5.95<0.001
Specialisation × Gender × Homeschooling 10.110.41−0.69–0.920.782
Specialisation × Gender × Homeschooling 20.480.48−0.47–1.420.327
Perceived Stress
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.052/0.515
AIC/BIC = 7715.8/7818.0
Intercept9.93 ***0.698.58–11.27<0.001
Parent age−0.04 **0.01−0.07–−0.010.003
No. of children−0.130.1−0.33–0.080.219
Relationship length0.0010.001−0.00–0.000.390
Family Income−0.15 **0.05−0.25–−0.040.005
Education−0.040.06−0.16–0.080.520
Specialisation−1.261.78−4.74–2.230.480
Gender0.100.27−0.42–0.620.708
Homeschooling 1−0.110.13−0.35–0.140.382
Homeschooling 2−0.29 0.14−0.57–−0.010.039
Gender × Specialisation 1.631.02−0.38–3.630.112
Gender × Homeschooling 10.030.07−0.11–0.170.676
Gender × Homeschooling 20.050.08−0.10–0.200.476
Specialisation × Homeschooling 10.060.43−0.79–0.900.897
Specialisation × Homeschooling 20.290.51−0.71–1.300.564
Specialisation × Gender × Homeschooling 1−0.430.25−0.92–0.060.087
Specialisation × Gender × Homeschooling 2−0.080.3−0.66–0.500.786
Relationship Satisfaction
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.061/0.789
AIC/BIC = 8631.5/8733.7
Intercept10.49 ***1.138.28–12.71<0.001
Parent age−0.020.02−0.06–0.030.415
No. of children0.62 ***0.180.27–0.97<0.001
Relationship length−0.0020.001−0.00–0.000.192
Family Income0.000.08−0.16–0.160.979
Education−0.040.08−0.19–0.120.649
Specialisation8.69 **3.042.74–14.640.004
Gender0.400.28−0.14–0.950.144
Homeschooling 10.77 ***0.210.36–1.19<0.001
Homeschooling 20.66 **0.240.19–1.130.006
Gender × Specialisation 0.431.06−1.64–2.500.683
Gender × Homeschooling 1−0.140.07−0.29–0.000.051
Gender × Homeschooling 2−0.140.08−0.30–0.010.068
Specialisation × Homeschooling 1−1.270.73−2.71–0.170.084
Specialisation × Homeschooling 2−2.98 **0.87−4.69–−1.270.001
Specialisation × Gender × Homeschooling 1−0.150.26−0.65–0.360.570
Specialisation × Gender × Homeschooling 2−0.340.31−0.94–0.260.267
Relationship Conflict
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.083/0.679
AIC/BIC = 12,435.3/12,537.5
Intercept37.55 ***3.3131.05–44.04<0.001
Parent age−0.14 0.07−0.27–−0.010.038
No. of children−1.01 0.51−2.01–−0.010.047
Relationship length0.0020.004−0.01–0.010.592
Family Income0.290.25−0.20–0.770.243
Education−0.020.26−0.54–0.500.945
Specialisation−30.63 ***8.75−47.77–−13.49<0.001
Gender2.56 1.030.54–4.580.013
Homeschooling 1−2.40 ***0.61−3.61–−1.20<0.001
Homeschooling 2−3.24 ***0.69−4.59–−1.89<0.001
Gender × Specialisation21.05 ***3.9413.33–28.77<0.001
Gender × Homeschooling 1−0.310.27−0.84–0.230.258
Gender × Homeschooling 2−0.72 0.29−1.30–−0.150.014
Specialisation × Homeschooling 14.89 2.110.75–9.040.021
Specialisation × Homeschooling 28.16 **2.513.24–13.090.001
Specialisation × Gender × Homeschooling 1−3.42 ***0.96−5.31–−1.53<0.001
Specialisation × Gender × Homeschooling 2−5.58 ***1.14−7.81–−3.34<0.001
Notes. p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001. Gender coded −1 (Male), +1 = (Female). Homeschooling status 1 coded as 0 (voluntary homeschoolers), 1 (not homeschooling, i.e., child attending school in-person), and −1 (mandated homeschooling due to COVID-19). Homeschooling status 2 coded as −1 (voluntary homeschoolers), 0 (not homeschooling, i.e., child attending school in-person), and 1 (mandated homeschooling due to COVID-19).
Table 10. Effects of equity and homeschooling status on male vs. female parents’ well-being in mixed-sex couples.
Table 10. Effects of equity and homeschooling status on male vs. female parents’ well-being in mixed-sex couples.
OutcomePredictorsBSE (b)95% CIp
Depression
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.050/0.599
AIC/BIC = 9689.9/9792.1
Intercept12.25 **1.439.45–15.05<0.001
Parent age−0.04 0.03−0.11–0.020.185
No. of children−0.57 *0.2−0.97–−0.170.005
Relationship length−0.040.04−0.11–0.040.316
Family Income−0.010.1−0.21–0.190.902
Education−0.030.11−0.26–0.190.767
Equity2.334.45−6.39–11.050.601
Gender−0.670.48−1.61–0.280.168
Homeschooling 1−0.480.25−0.96–0.010.054
Homeschooling 2−0.74 *0.29−1.31–−0.180.009
Gender × Equity3.622.28−0.85–8.090.112
Gender × Homeschooling 10.240.12−0.01–0.480.056
Gender × Homeschooling 20.28 0.140.01–0.550.040
Equity × Homeschooling 1−0.241.04−2.28–1.810.822
Equity × Homeschooling 2−1.541.28−4.04–0.960.228
Equity × Gender × Homeschooling 1−1.21 0.54−2.27–−0.150.025
Equity × Gender × Homeschooling 2−0.900.65−2.18–0.390.170
Anxiety
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.044/0.537
AIC/BIC = 9364.7/9467.0
Intercept9.31 **1.246.87–11.74<0.001
Parent age−0.040.03−0.09–0.020.195
No. of children−0.47 *0.18−0.82–−0.130.007
Relationship length−0.040.03−0.10–0.020.207
Family Income0.000.09−0.18–0.180.986
Education−0.090.1−0.29–0.110.371
Equity4.533.85−3.01–12.070.239
Gender−0.880.46−1.78–0.010.052
Homeschooling 1−0.200.21−0.62–0.220.346
Homeschooling 2−0.210.25−0.70–0.280.398
Gender × Equity0.912.15−3.31–5.130.674
Gender × Homeschooling 10.31 *0.120.08–0.540.008
Gender × Homeschooling 20.34 *0.130.08–0.600.009
Equity × Homeschooling 1−0.390.9−2.16–1.370.663
Equity × Homeschooling 2−1.931.1−4.09–0.240.081
Equity × Gender × Homeschooling 1−0.320.51−1.32–0.680.527
Equity × Gender × Homeschooling 2−0.430.62−1.65–0.780.483
Perceived Stress
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.053/0.509
AIC/BIC = 7721.3/7823.5
Intercept10.11 **0.738.68–11.55<0.001
Parent age−0.04 0.02−0.07–−0.010.014
No. of children−0.190.1−0.39–0.010.069
Relationship length0.000.02−0.04–0.040.935
Family Income−0.12 0.05−0.22–−0.010.028
Education−0.040.06−0.17–0.080.475
Equity1.172.25−3.25–5.590.603
Gender0.140.28−0.41–0.680.625
Homeschooling 1−0.140.13−0.39–0.110.265
Homeschooling 2−0.35 0.15−0.63–−0.060.018
Gender × Equity−0.311.32−2.90–2.270.811
Gender × Homeschooling 10.010.07−0.13–0.150.847
Gender × Homeschooling 20.060.08−0.10–0.210.465
Equity × Homeschooling 1−0.460.53−1.50–0.570.379
Equity × Homeschooling 2−0.520.65−1.79–0.750.424
Equity × Gender × Homeschooling 1−0.150.31−0.77–0.460.623
Equity × Gender × Homeschooling 20.140.38−0.60–0.890.707
Relationship Satisfaction
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.064/0.790
AIC/BIC = 8622.4/8724.6
Intercept10.96 **1.28.60–13.32<0.001
Parent age−0.030.03−0.09–0.020.227
No. of children0.67 **0.180.33–1.02<0.001
Relationship length0.020.03−0.04–0.090.444
Family Income−0.070.08−0.23–0.090.389
Education−0.020.08−0.17–0.130.820
Equity−8.32 3.85−15.86–−0.780.031
Gender0.360.29−0.20–0.920.210
Homeschooling 10.75 **0.210.33–1.16<0.001
Homeschooling 20.65 *0.250.17–1.130.009
Gender × Equity−0.551.35−3.20–2.110.685
Gender × Homeschooling 1−0.140.07−0.28–0.000.056
Gender × Homeschooling 2−0.130.08−0.29–0.030.114
Equity × Homeschooling 11.560.9−0.21–3.330.084
Equity × Homeschooling 23.15 *1.10.99–5.320.004
Equity × Gender × Homeschooling 10.290.32−0.34–0.920.367
Equity × Gender × Homeschooling 20.410.39−0.35–1.170.288
Relationship Conflict
Marginal R2/Conditional R2 = 0.071/0.670
AIC/BIC = 12,465.1/12,567.3
Intercept38.01 **3.5531.06–44.97<0.001
Parent age−0.080.08−0.24–0.080.344
No. of children−1.40 *0.51−2.40–−0.400.006
Relationship length−0.150.09−0.33–0.030.097
Family Income0.53 0.250.03–1.030.037
Education−0.060.27−0.58–0.470.836
Equity2.3511.14−19.48–24.180.833
Gender2.89 *1.090.76–5.010.008
Homeschooling 1−2.48 **0.62−3.70–−1.27<0.001
Homeschooling 2−3.58 **0.72−4.99–−2.18<0.001
Gender × Equity−15.39 *5.13−25.45–−5.340.003
Gender × Homeschooling 1−0.370.28−0.92–0.170.181
Gender × Homeschooling 2−0.79 0.31−1.40–−0.190.011
Equity × Homeschooling 1−1.402.61−6.51–3.720.592
Equity × Homeschooling 2−2.153.2−8.41–4.120.502
Equity × Gender × Homeschooling 12.221.22−0.16–4.600.068
Equity × Gender × Homeschooling 24.56 *1.471.67–7.450.002
Notes. p < 0.05; * p < 0.01; ** p < 0.001. Gender-coded −1 (Male), +1 = (Female). Homeschooling status 1 coded as 0 (voluntary homeschoolers), 1 (not homeschooling, i.e., child attending school in-person), and −1 (mandated homeschooling due to COVID-19). Homeschooling status 2 coded as −1 (voluntary homeschoolers), 0 (not homeschooling, i.e., child attending school in-person), and 1 (mandated homeschooling due to COVID-19).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Elgendi, M.M.; Stewart, S.H.; DesRoches, D.I.; Corkum, P.; Nogueira-Arjona, R.; Deacon, S.H. Division of Labour and Parental Mental Health and Relationship Well-Being during COVID-19 Pandemic-Mandated Homeschooling. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 17021. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192417021

AMA Style

Elgendi MM, Stewart SH, DesRoches DI, Corkum P, Nogueira-Arjona R, Deacon SH. Division of Labour and Parental Mental Health and Relationship Well-Being during COVID-19 Pandemic-Mandated Homeschooling. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(24):17021. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192417021

Chicago/Turabian Style

Elgendi, Mariam M., Sherry H. Stewart, Danika I. DesRoches, Penny Corkum, Raquel Nogueira-Arjona, and S. Hélène Deacon. 2022. "Division of Labour and Parental Mental Health and Relationship Well-Being during COVID-19 Pandemic-Mandated Homeschooling" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 24: 17021. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192417021

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop