Next Article in Journal
Experiences of Life and Intersectionality of Transgender Refugees Living in Italy: A Qualitative Approach
Previous Article in Journal
How Phantom Networks, Provider Qualities, and Poverty Sway Medicaid Dental Care Access: A Geospatial Analysis of Manhattan
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Theoretical Foundations to the Impact of Dog-Related Activities on Human Hedonic Well-Being, Life Satisfaction and Eudaimonic Well-Being

1
School of Life Sciences, University of Lincoln, Lincoln LN6 7TS, UK
2
School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln LN6 7TS, UK
3
Department of Neuroscience, Psychology and Behaviour, University of Leicester, Leicester LE1 7RH, UK
4
School of Medicine, University of Nottingham, Nottingham NG7 2RD, UK
5
Department of Statistics, StatsAdvice.com, Ltd., 10551 Berlin, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(23), 12382; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182312382
Submission received: 22 October 2021 / Revised: 22 November 2021 / Accepted: 22 November 2021 / Published: 25 November 2021

Abstract

:
Cross-sectional comparisons of well-being between dog owners and non-owners commonly generate inconsistent results. Focusing on the uniqueness of the relationship might help address this issue and provide a stronger foundation for dog-related psychotherapeutic interventions. This study aims to evaluate the impact of dog-related activities (e.g., exercising the dog) on owner hedonic well-being, life satisfaction and eudaimonic well-being. It was also hypothesised that psychological closeness to the dog would affect these well-being outcomes. For this study, 1030 dog owners aged over 18 years old answered an online questionnaire about the impact of 15 groups of dog-related activities on their well-being. Ordinal regressions were used to estimate the mean response (and its uncertainty) for each outcome, while conditioning for psychological closeness to the dog and controlling for several key covariates. Tactile interactions and dog playing were significantly more beneficial than other activities for hedonic well-being, and dog training and dog presence for eudaimonic well-being. In contrast, dog health issues and behavioural problems were linked to decrements in these well-being outcomes. Higher psychological closeness to the dog predicted greater improvement in well-being in positive dog-related activities. Our quantitative study validates the general findings of previous qualitative work and lays the groundwork for future longitudinal studies.

1. Introduction

Despite frequent reports of the benefits of pet ownership to human well-being based on qualitative studies, quantitative findings based largely on cross-sectional comparisons between owners and non-owners are inconsistent [1,2,3,4,5]. These mixed results highlight not only the complexity of the pet-human relationship but also the need for theoretical frameworks that account for the diversity of the relationship [4]. For example, although McNicholas et al. [6] have proposed a range of mechanisms whereby pet ownership might have a direct, indirect (via enhancement of social interactions) or no effect at all on human well-being, this is clearly not a comprehensive list. By contrast, Gee et al. [7] have suggested that a biopsychological model may explain the benefits of dog-human interaction. However, while this might seem more comprehensive, it lacks detail concerning the specific behaviours associated with ownership that impact human well-being. For example, are the benefits related to dog walking, tactile interactions, the routine of looking after the animal, or simply to the animal’s presence? Similar questions are being increasingly asked, not only in studies of pet ownership [8,9,10,11,12,13], but also in experiments featuring human-animal interaction [14,15,16,17]. By addressing the specific activities/situations associated with pet ownership, we can begin to objectively address the psychological basis to any benefits.
Barcelos et al. [18] have recently proposed a framework that describes 58 dog-related activities/situations (e.g., dog walking, feeding the dog and dog aggression), organised into 15 themes (e.g., exercise, providing for the dog and unwanted behaviours), and their impact on hedonic well-being (positive and negative affect) [19,20], life satisfaction [21] and eudaimonic well-being (i.e., six areas of life functioning: autonomy, purpose in life, personal growth, control over own environment, self-acceptance and positive relations with others) [22,23] of UK dog owners. This thematic framework has also been shown to be relatively robust in other cultures (e.g., Brazilian dog owners) [24], amongst a more neurodiverse population (e.g., autistic adults dog owners) [25], and even in other species (e.g., cat owners) [26], illustrating how pet ownership can be deconstructed into a range of related activities that might positively or negatively impact human well-being. Besides accommodating the uniqueness of the pet-owner relationship, this approach, unlike many others, considers both hedonia and eudaimonia, as recommended for investigations of well-being, given their complementary psychological functions. Hedonic pursuit is associated more with immediate well-being (e.g., everyday affect), while eudaimonic is more closely associated with longer-term well-being (e.g., life engagement) [27]. Findings of other studies also indicate that the psychological closeness between owner and dog could be an important moderator of owner well-being outcomes [28,29]. The latter found a positive correlation with well-being (i.e., life satisfaction and happiness) and a negative correlation with psychological distress, and in the study by Cavanaugh et al. [28], owners who reported higher psychological closeness to their dog tended to be more satisfied with their lives.
Having used qualitative methods to build a framework of dog owner interactions which impact on their well-being [18], the aim of the current study was to use quantitative methods to deduce the significance of the reported relationships between types of dog owner activity and their impact on owner well-being in a wider population [30]. Furthermore, given the potential importance of psychological closeness on the proposed effects, we included a measure of this in order to illustrate the importance of psychological closeness in relation to different types of activity.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Ethical Approval

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Lincoln (protocol code 2021_0503). Electronic informed consent was obtained from participants prior to their participation.

2.2. Questionnaire Design and Structure

Participants completed an online questionnaire (QualtricsTM) divided into two sections: (1) general questions about the participant and their dog, used as covariates in the statistical analysis, and (2) questions about the impact of 15 groups of dog-related activities on participants’ hedonic well-being, life satisfaction and eudaimonic well-being. A total of eleven aspects of well-being were self-reported in relation to these activities: life satisfaction [21], four elements of hedonic well-being [31,32,33] and six elements of eudaimonic well-being [22].
Most questions of the survey were not mobile friendly. Therefore, participants were advised at the introduction of the survey to answer the survey from a desktop and not from their phones. The questionnaire is available in the Supplementary Material.

2.3. Measurements

2.3.1. Dog-Related Activities/Situations

The dog-related activities (Table 1) were based on our previous framework [18,24,25]. To decrease potential order effects, the order of the questions about dog-related activities (for example, how does ‘dog playing’ impact on your satisfaction with your own life?) was randomised for each participant. The impact of each of these activities on the 11 well-being outcomes was assessed using a 7-point Likert response (−3 worsen a lot, −2 worsen moderately, −1 worsen a bit, 0 no impact, +1 improve a bit, +2 improve moderately, +3 improve a lot).

2.3.2. Life Satisfaction and Hedonic Well-Being

Life satisfaction was referred to as ‘satisfaction with your own life’. The four aspects of hedonic well-being were described in terms of states: (1) ‘positive affect of high arousal (e.g., happiness, excitement, joy, fun, activation, etc)’, (2) ‘positive affect of low arousal (e.g., calmness, relaxation, serenity, peacefulness, etc)’, (3) ‘negative affect of high arousal (e.g., stress, annoyance, worry, frustration, anxiety, anger, etc)’, and (4) ‘negative affect of low arousal (e.g., sadness, tiredness, unhappiness, guilt, feeling low, grief, etc)’.

2.3.3. Eudaimonic Well-Being

The six elements of eudaimonia considered were: (1) ‘autonomy (your independence, freedom from others’ approval)’, (2) ‘your control over situations/events in your life or in your surroundings’, (3) ‘personal growth (your growth or achievement of your potentials)’, (4) ‘positive relations with others (good social relations with other people)’, (5) ‘purpose in life (having aims/goals in life)’, and (6) ‘self-acceptance (accepting yourself, the good and bad in you)’.

2.3.4. Closeness to the Dog

Finally, the level of psychological closeness to the dog was measured with an adapted version of the Inclusion of Other in the Self scale [34], a visual scale, ranging from 1 to 7, originally developed to assess interpersonal closeness.

2.4. Participants

A power analysis was conducted prior to data collection. Aiming for 99% chance of reaching narrow confidence intervals equivalent to ≤20% of the one-point distance between the options given to participants (e.g., improve moderately and improve a lot) in each activity, a sample of 1000 dog owners was estimated. The power analysis was based on the estimated marginal means of an ordinal regression analysis applied to each of the 1000 simulations. Each simulation assumed weak correlations (R = 0.1) between all pairs of covariates and moderate correlations (R = 0.3) between the DV and three of the covariates (zero correlation between DV and the remaining covariates). The model was further challenged in the power analysis by capturing an unequal spread of responses across the seven ordinal response categories (the seven categories were chosen to have marginal probabilities of selection of 0.01, 0.01, 0.01, 0.07, 0.1, 0.2 and 0.6, respectively).
Using convenience and snowball sampling, volunteer participants were recruited via social media (i.e., Facebook, Twitter, Quora, Reddit, WhatsApp) and local media (i.e., University of Lincoln News). To be included, they needed to be at least 18 years old and own a dog.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Estimated means were calculated using ordinal probit regression models on the R packages ‘ordinal’ (v.2019.12-10) and ‘emmeans’ (v.1.6.1). The following participants’ characteristics were included as covariates in the statistical models, as they could influence owners’ well-being: age (continuous variable [35]), gender (female, male, non-binary [36]), country (UK, US, other [37]), living alone (yes/no [38]), on the autism spectrum (yes/no [39]), diagnosed or experiencing a mental health problem (yes/no [40]), and level of psychological closeness to the dog (1 to 7 [29]). Dogs’ characteristics were also controlled in the models, as they might influence the performance of dog-related activities and/or human well-being (e.g., dog size can affect dog walking performance [41]): sex of the dog(s) (female, male, mixed—e.g., one female and one male in the household was coded as mixed), young dog—under one year old (yes/no), senior dog—10 years old or more (yes/no), reproductive status (intact, neutered/spayed, mixed—i.e., having both intact and neutered dogs in the household was coded as mixed), very small/small dog (yes/no), and very large/giant dog (yes/no).
Given the focus of this study on how dog-related activities differ in their impact on well-being, we have used a graphical approach to present our results (Figures 1–11—presented later in the manuscript). We show the estimated means for each pair of activity and well-being outcome (e.g., life satisfaction rating due to exercise with the dog versus life satisfaction due to dog playing). Such an approach facilitates the visualisation of statistical differences between dog-related activities. Plots (Figures 1–11) show, through error bars (99% CI), the regressed well-being outcomes of each dog-related activity for both owners with lower (score 2—lower quartile) and higher (score 6—upper quartile) psychological closeness to their dogs. Plots were created using the R package ‘ggplot2’ (v.3.3.3). Due to the large number of ordinal regressions performed, a more conservative confidence interval (i.e., 99%) has been used in the error bars in Figures 1–11 to provide some mitigation of Type I errors.

3. Results

3.1. Demographics

Of the 2041 people who started the survey, 1030 met the inclusion criteria and completed the entire survey (50.5%). Respondents (Table S1) were aged 18 to 82 years old (mean 39.7 years, sd = 14.1), were mostly female (83.9%), and were mostly living in the US (38.4%) or UK (33.9%). Further details of the participants and their dogs are available in the Supplementary Material (Table S1). Of those respondents who did not complete the questionnaire, 598 had similar demographics to the actual participants of the study: 18–79 years old (mean 38.9, sd = 13.4), mostly female (79.9%), and mostly living in the US (38.3%) or UK (35.6%). Demographic information of the remaining 413 respondents is not available, as those individuals either did not meet the inclusion criteria, being redirected to the end of the survey, or stopped at the introductory stage due to a warning message telling them to answer the survey from a desktop and not from their phones

3.2. Overview of the Reported Impact of Dog-Related Activities on Well-Being

Nine dog-related activities tended to be reported as predominantly positive for well-being across the aspects of well-being assessed (Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4, Figure 5, Figure 6, Figure 7, Figure 8, Figure 9, Figure 10 and Figure 11): exercise with the dog (e.g., walking); the presence of the dog; tactile interactions with the dog; other close interactions (e.g., dog greeting the owner); training the dog; looking after the dog (e.g., feeding); dog playing; talking to others because of the dog; simply having the dog. These activities will be referred to as ‘positive activities’ from hereon. In contrast, five activities were mainly detrimental to participant well-being: dog health problems (e.g., dog being sick); sensory behaviour problems (e.g., dog barking); lacking control over the dog (e.g., dog pulling on the lead); dog aggressive behaviours; other dog unwanted behaviours. These will be referred to as ‘negative activities’ from hereon. One activity (maintenance of the dog, e.g., taking dog to the vet) mainly had no to little impact across participants’ well-being.
In Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4, Figure 5, Figure 6, Figure 7, Figure 8, Figure 9, Figure 10 and Figure 11, the non-overlap of error bars between different dog-related activities indicates that the activities compared are significantly different (p < 0.01). For example, in terms of the ‘positive affect of high arousal’ (Figure 1), dog playing increases happiness significantly more than looking after the dog in both owners with higher and lower closeness to their dogs. To avoid repetition of information provided in the figures, only the most relevant activities—highest/lowest score in our sample and overlapping the least with other activities—are reported in the text of each figure. However, this does not imply that this activity is significantly different to all of the others, as it could still overlap with other activities in the figure. The overlap of activities indicates that they might have a similar impact on the respective well-being outcome. Finally, the estimated means for each pair of dog-related activity and well-being outcome is available in the Supplementary Material (Tables S2–S12).

3.3. Relationship between Closeness to the Dog and Reported Well-Being Outcomes

On average, participants reported moderate-high level of psychological closeness to their dogs (mean = 4.82, sd = 1.89). The effect of closeness to the dog is illustrated in Figure 1 and Figure 2. Asterisks next to the error bars indicate the level of significance of this variable. Overall, when rating ‘positive activities’ or ‘maintenance of the dog’, owners who were closer to their dogs reported greater improvement in well-being outcomes than owners with lower closeness. By contrast, in relation to ‘negative activities’, the difference in well-being between owners with higher or lower closeness to their dogs were generally not significant. The exceptions to this were in relation to ‘health problems’, being more negatively rated for its impact on ‘positive affect of high arousal’ and ‘life satisfaction’ by owners who were closer to their dogs (Figure 1 and Figure 5); and in relation to ‘other unwanted behaviours’, being more detrimental for ‘positive affect of high arousal’ in owners with higher closeness to their dogs (Figure 1). Full statistical details for the independent variable ‘closeness to the dog’ and the confidence intervals of well-being outcomes at lower and higher closeness to the dog are available in the Supplementary Material (Tables S13–S33).

3.4. Effect of Dog-Related Activities on Specific Well-Being Outcomes

3.4.1. Positive Affect of High Arousal (e.g., Happiness, Excitement, Joy)

‘Dog playing’, ‘simply having the dog’ and ‘dog presence’ were the most positive activities for happiness, whereas ‘health problems’ and, secondly, ‘dog aggression’ were the most detrimental ones (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Reported impact of dog-related activities on positive affect of high arousal, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 1. Reported impact of dog-related activities on positive affect of high arousal, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g001

3.4.2. Positive Affect of Low Arousal (e.g., Calmness, Relaxation, Peacefulness)

‘Tactile interactions’ and ‘dog presence’ led the rating scores for calmness improvement. ‘Health problems’ and ‘dog aggression’ were, again, the most detrimental activities (Figure 2).
Figure 2. Reported impact of dog-related activities on positive affect of low arousal, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 2. Reported impact of dog-related activities on positive affect of low arousal, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g002

3.4.3. Negative Affect of High Arousal (e.g., Stress, Anger, Frustration)

‘Tactile interactions’ and, secondly, ‘dog playing’ and ‘other close interactions’ were reported to improve most the negative affect of high arousal (Figure 3). ‘Health problems’ and ‘dog aggression’ were, again, the worst for this aspect of well-being.
Figure 3. Reported impact of dog-related activities on negative affect of high arousal, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 3. Reported impact of dog-related activities on negative affect of high arousal, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g003

3.4.4. Negative Affect of Low Arousal (e.g., Sadness, Feeling Low, Frustration)

Similar to what has been described in terms of the negative affect of high arousal, ‘tactile interactions’, ‘other close interactions’ and ‘dog playing’ were the most positive for negative affect of low arousal (Figure 4). Again, ‘health problems’, followed by ‘dog aggression’, were the worst for this aspect of owners’ well-being.
Figure 4. Reported impact of dog-related activities on negative affect of low arousal, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 4. Reported impact of dog-related activities on negative affect of low arousal, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g004

3.4.5. Life Satisfaction

Similar to positive affect of high arousal, ‘simply having the dog’ and ‘dog presence’ were the activities that improved most the owners’ life satisfaction (Figure 5), whereas ‘health problems’ was the activity that worsened this well-being outcome the most.
Figure 5. Reported impact of dog-related activities on life satisfaction, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 5. Reported impact of dog-related activities on life satisfaction, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g005

3.4.6. Autonomy

All ‘positive activities’ were rated as having a very similar positive impact on owners’ autonomy. However, ‘exercising’ with the dog seems to be slightly better than other activities (Figure 6). The ‘negative activities’ were also rated similarly, but ‘dog aggression’ had the most negative effect, particularly among owners with higher closeness to their dogs.
Figure 6. Reported impact of dog-related activities on autonomy, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 6. Reported impact of dog-related activities on autonomy, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g006

3.4.7. Environmental Mastery

‘Training the dog’ was the activity reported to most improve an owner’s sense of environmental mastery (Figure 7). In contrast, ‘health problems’ and ‘dog aggression’ were the worst for environmental mastery.
Figure 7. Reported impact of dog-related activities on environmental mastery, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 7. Reported impact of dog-related activities on environmental mastery, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g007

3.4.8. Personal Growth

According to owner ratings, ‘training the dog’ and ‘simply having the dog’ were the most beneficial activities for personal growth (Figure 8). Although the negative activities did not seem to have a considerable impact on personal growth, ‘health problems’ was the most detrimental for this well-being outcome, particularly for owners with higher closeness to their dogs.
Figure 8. Reported impact of dog-related activities on personal growth, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 8. Reported impact of dog-related activities on personal growth, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g008

3.4.9. Positive Relations with Others

Positive relations with others was reported to improve most due to ‘talk to others because of the dog’, followed by ‘simply having the dog’, ‘dog presence’, ‘dog training’, ‘dog playing’ and ‘exercise’ (Figure 9). As with autonomy, ‘dog aggression’ was the most detrimental event impacting on this aspect of well-being.
Figure 9. Reported impact of dog-related activities on positive relations with others, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 9. Reported impact of dog-related activities on positive relations with others, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g009

3.4.10. Purpose in Life

‘Simply having the dog’, ‘training the dog’ and ‘dog presence’ were reported to increase owners’ purpose in life the most. Such as observed in personal growth, ‘health problems’ was the only activity significantly bad for purpose in life in both closer and more distant owners (Figure 10).
Figure 10. Reported impact of dog-related activities on purpose in life, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 10. Reported impact of dog-related activities on purpose in life, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g010

3.4.11. Self-Acceptance

As with autonomy, the ratings of the ‘positive activities’ were very similar to each other. Still, ‘dog presence’, ‘tactile interactions’ and ‘simply having the dog’ seem to be the most beneficial for self-acceptance. ‘Dog aggression’ was the only activity significantly detrimental for self-acceptance independently of level of closeness to the dog (Figure 11), although high closeness made ‘health problems’ more likely to be associated with a negative impact.
Figure 11. Reported impact of dog-related activities on self-acceptance, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Figure 11. Reported impact of dog-related activities on self-acceptance, at lower and higher level of closeness to the dog.
Ijerph 18 12382 g011

4. Discussion

The findings indicate that different dog-related activities have varying impacts on human hedonic well-being, life satisfaction and eudaimonic well-being, as suggested in our previous qualitative work [18,24,25]. These specific findings have the potential to lay the foundation for more targeted psychotherapeutic interventions using owners’ own pet dogs and, possibly, also assistance animals in AAI (e.g., positive tactile interaction with a dog to help decrease negative affect of high arousal in a patient). We suggest that specific aspects of human well-being might be more effectively boosted using a more tailored activity-based approach, based on the leading activities in Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4, Figure 5, Figure 6, Figure 7, Figure 8, Figure 9, Figure 10 and Figure 11. Further these results provide important insight into why the reported benefits of pet ownership are so inconsistent [1,2,3,4,5]; i.e., different owners will engage in different activities and thus receive varying amounts of benefit or cost to diverse types of well-being. The activities’ high variability in their level of impact across different well-being outcomes also highlights the importance of a comprehensive assessment of well-being in this type of work [27]. In this way, the potential benefits or costs associated with a specific activity are less likely to be missed. Another important finding was the potential moderating role of the closeness of the relationship between the owner and dog, on many of the potential effects.

4.1. Closeness to the Dog

A higher level of closeness to the dog was associated with greater perceived benefits from all ‘positive activities’ and ‘maintenance of the dog’ in all aspects of well-being, which might explain why owners who are closer to their dogs have greater well-being [28,29] and are more satisfied with their relationship with their dogs than other owners [42]. In contrast, ‘negative activities’ were generally scored similarly by owners regardless of their closeness. In other words, dog behavioural and health problems generally worsen the well-being of a dog owner to a similar degree. These findings have important implications for the management of people’s mental health: since closer owners seem to benefit more from ‘positive activities’, increased frequency/duration of those activities (e.g., walking the dog more often) are likely to be much more efficacious for them. It is also worth speculating that when these activities are used within an AAI context, their impact may be less than for an owner, until a close bond is formed. Thus, focusing on building emotional closeness between a client and therapeutic animal may be important to the efficacy of a proposed activity. By contrast, psychological interventions focused on decreasing ‘negative activities’ (e.g., dog behaving less aggressively) appear to have a similar effect on the owner regardless of their emotional closeness.

4.2. Hedonic Well-Being and Life Satisfaction

Positive interactions with the dog (e.g., touching, being greeted by the animal) and routine-like activities (e.g., walking, feeding) were rated as beneficial to owners’ hedonic well-being, which is consistent with the findings of Barcelos et al. [18,25] and Corrêa et al. [24]. Tactile interactions, in particular, led the rating score in three aspects of hedonic well-being, increasing owners’ calmness and decreasing both stress and sadness. Indeed, it has been suggested that human-dog tactile interactions are a major component of the ‘pet effect’ [15], being associated with lower human cortisol [43,44,45], blood pressure, heart rate [15], and higher oxytocin [43,44]. Playing with the dog or watching the animal play were also very important to three aspects of hedonic well-being, improving owners’ happiness, stress and sadness. Horowitz and Hecht [46] reported that, from their observations, 61% of dog-human playful interactions appear to elevate owners’ positive affect. Human cortisol has also been reported to decrease after ‘low-key playful interactions’ with dogs [44] and qualitative studies have consistently reported the hedonic benefits of dog play (e.g., is enjoyable/fun, improves sadness, stress [18,24,25,47,48]. However, in a cross-sectional study, owners’ reported frequency of dog playing was not correlated with well-being scores (e.g., stress, psychosomatic symptoms) [49], but this may reflect the issue of less precise measurement of well-being in many studies, that we commented on in the introduction to this discussion.
Life satisfaction was rarely mentioned by participants in our previous qualitative studies [18,24,25], however all ‘positive activities’ were rated here as positive to life satisfaction. Indeed, simply having a dog seems to improve life satisfaction [50], even if this is not something that often comes to mind in interviews or focus groups. In the current study, the mere ‘presence of the dog’ led the rating scores for life satisfaction and positive affect of high arousal. The benefits of the dog’s presence for humans is one area of human-animal interaction where the scientific results are fairly consistent: being associated not only with beneficial neurophysiological changes [16,51] but also with improvements in self-reported well-being [8,11,14,18,24,25]. By contrast, dog health problems (e.g., disease, injury) and behavioural problems, particularly dog aggression, were reported to worsen owners’ hedonic well-being and life satisfaction. Caring for sick animals is a burden and source of stress for pet owners [52,53,54,55,56]; likewise, pet behaviour problems undermine owner well-being [48,57,58] and increase dissatisfaction with their animal [42]. Thus, interventional strategies that minimise detrimental activities and augment positive ones will likely improve owners’ hedonic well-being and life satisfaction.

4.3. Eudaimonic Well-Being

Exercising the dog (e.g., walking), looking after the animal (e.g., feeding) and the presence of the dog were positive across all elements of eudaimonic well-being. Dog walking has been reported to give purpose in life and facilitate positive social relationships [9,59,60]. Caring for an animal can give owners routine, life structure and purpose [59,61,62], even potentially helping in suicide prevention [25,63]. Dog presence/company is an important social lubricant [60,64,65,66] and is potentially a source of social support [45]. Interestingly, dog training led the rating score of most eudaimonic elements (e.g., environmental mastery, personal growth). Positive effects from dog training on eudaimonia have been reported in qualitative studies of owners (e.g., [18,24,25]), and in relation to formal dog training programs (e.g., with prisoners, or soldiers with post-traumatic stress disorder). This might be because improving participants’ skills, autonomy, trust in others and emotional control might give them a sense of achievement and self-development [67,68,69,70,71].
Interestingly, while positive activities were universally beneficial, negative events (e.g., health problems, dog aggression) had little to no impact on personal growth, purpose in life and self-acceptance; the significant detrimental effects were mostly related to autonomy, environmental mastery and positive relations with others. The latter negative results might therefore be linked largely to the stress and burden of these activities in the lives of pet owners in terms of owners being unable to make independent choices (autonomy), manage everyday affairs (environmental mastery), and due to interpersonal conflicts (relation with others), e.g., frequent disputes with other people because of dog showing aggression [48,52,53,54,55,56,57,58].

4.4. Strengths and Limitations

The strengths of this study include its large sample size and the comprehensiveness of the dog-related activities and well-being outcomes evaluated, providing rich findings that can be empirically tested in future studies. This study had a very solid foundation, building on the consistency of our previous qualitative investigations [18,24,25]. Two main limitations of the study are its cross-sectional methodology, which does not permit inferences of causality, and the large proportion of female participants (83.9%), a common issue in studies of human-animal bond (see [72]), such as those about dog ownership [73]. Another limitation of the study is the lack of assessment of additional covariates that could have impacted how owners perceive the interactions with their dogs, such as participants’ socioeconomic status [74] and personality traits [75]. Utz [76], for example, found that socioeconomic status is an important confounder in the association between pet ownership and human physical health. Despite these limitations, our study controls for several important covariates (e.g., age, gender, country, age of the dog, size of the dog, etc.) which is not always the case in studies on pet ownership [77].
Future investigations could test the impact of fluctuations in dog-related activities on the well-being of dog owners longitudinally, as a way to assess causality. Additionally, the same methodology could be extended to other pets and non-pet animals, such as those studies which have already been initiated with cats [26]. To increase male participation, targeted recruitment strategies can be applied, such as using more male dog owner pictures in advertisement materials, asking participants to invite male friends to the study, targeting social media groups/venues more populated by men.

5. Conclusions

By employing a wider spectrum of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being herein, we may have begun to specify where the greatest impact of various human-dog interactions lies. Overall, different dog-related activities were found to have specific effects on owner well-being. Accordingly, we caution against over-simplified generalisations of a ‘pet effect’ or recommendations of pet acquisition as a psychological aid. A more individualised approach, based on increments or decrements in dog-related activities aiming to improve specific aspects of one’s well-being is likely to result in more predictable outcomes and thus be more fruitful. Attention should also be given to the level of closeness between the person and the dog, as this may have significant moderating effects. Future psychotherapeutic interventions with pet dogs or studies about dog ownership and human well-being could use the findings presented here to provide guidance for their hypotheses and therapeutic goals. We hope that greater understanding of the complexity of the dog-human relationship and the impact that dogs have on human mental health will not only help shape a better human-dog relationship, but also a better society for all.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph182312382/s1, Questionnaire, Table S1: Characteristics of participants (n = 1030) of the study and of their dogs, Tables S2–S12: Estimated means and sd for each pair of activity and well-being outcome, Tables S13–S23: Statistics of the independent variable ‘closeness to the dog’ in the ordinal regressions, Tables S24–S33: Confidence intervals of each activity at lower and higher ‘closeness to the dog’.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.M.B., N.K., J.M., S.H. and D.S.M.; Data curation, A.M.B., N.K. and S.H.; Formal analysis, A.M.B., P.A. and D.S.M.; Investigation, A.M.B.; Methodology, A.M.B., N.K., J.M., S.H., P.A. and D.S.M.; Project administration, A.M.B., N.K. and D.S.M.; Supervision, N.K., J.M., S.H. and D.S.M.; Writing—original draft, A.M.B., N.K., J.M., S.H., P.A. and D.S.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Lincoln (protocol code 2021_0503, approved on 12 April 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/n6m2k/?view_only=3e70af2059564120a0383f1ed8937b30, accessed on 20 November 2021.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all the participants of the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Jacobson, K.C.; Chang, L. Associations Between Pet Ownership and Attitudes Toward Pets With Youth Socioemotional Outcomes. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Herzog, H. The Impact of Pets on Human Health and Psychological Well-Being. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 20, 236–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Islam, A.; Towell, T. Cat and dog companionship and well-being: A systematic review. Int. J. Appl. Psychol. 2013, 3, 149–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Gee, N.R.; Mueller, M.K. A Systematic Review of Research on Pet Ownership and Animal Interactions among Older Adults. Anthrozoös 2019, 32, 183–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Brooks, H.L.; Rushton, K.; Lovell, K.; Bee, P.; Walker, L.; Grant, L.; Rogers, A. The power of support from companion animals for people living with mental health problems: A systematic review and narrative synthesis of the evidence. BMC Psychiatry 2018, 18, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. McNicholas, J.; Gilbey, A.; Rennie, A.; Ahmedzai, S.; Dono, J.-A.; Ormerod, E. Pet ownership and human health: A brief review of evidence and issues. BMJ 2005, 331, 1252–1254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Gee, N.R.; Rodriguez, K.E.; Fine, A.H.; Trammell, J.P. Dogs Supporting Human Health and Well-Being: A Biopsychosocial Approach. Front. Veter–Sci. 2021, 8, 630465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bennett, P.C.; Trigg, J.L.; Godber, T.; Brown, C. An Experience Sampling Approach to Investigating Associations between Pet Presence and Indicators of Psychological Wellbeing and Mood in Older Australians. Anthrozoös 2015, 28, 403–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Carr, D.; Friedmann, E.; Gee, N.; Gilchrist, C.; Sachs-Ericsson, N.; Koodaly, L. Dog Walking and the Social Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Loneliness in Older Adults. Animal 2021, 11, 1852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Cui, Y.; Russell, M.; Davern, M.; Christian, H. Longitudinal evidence of the impact of dog ownership and dog walking on mental health. J. Public Health 2021, 43, e145–e152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Janssens, M.; Eshuis, J.; Peeters, S.; Lataster, J.; Reijnders, J.; Enders-Slegers, M.-J.; Jacobs, N. The Pet-Effect in Daily Life: An Experience Sampling Study on Emotional Wellbeing in Pet Owners. Anthrozoös 2020, 33, 579–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Pereira, J.M.; Fonte, D. Pets enhance antidepressant pharmacotherapy effects in patients with treatment resistant major depressive disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2018, 104, 108–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Ng, M.; Wenden, E.; Lester, L.; Westgarth, C.; Christian, H. A study protocol for a randomised controlled trial to evaluate the effectiveness of a dog-facilitated physical activity minimal intervention on young children’s physical activity, health and development: The PLAYCE PAWS trial. BMC Public Health 2021, 21, 51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Aydin, N.; Krueger, J.I.; Fischer, J.; Hahn, D.; Kastenmüller, A.; Frey, D.; Fischer, P. “Man’s best friend:” How the presence of a dog reduces mental distress after social exclusion. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 48, 446–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Vormbrock, J.K.; Grossberg, J.M. Cardiovascular effects of human-pet dog interactions. J. Behav. Med. 1988, 11, 509–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Allen, K. Cardiovascular reactivity and the presence of pets, friends, and spouses: The truth about cats and dogs. Psychosom. Med. 2002, 64, 727–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Petersson, M.; Uvnäs-Moberg, K.; Nilsson, A.; Gustafson, L.-L.; Hydbring-Sandberg, E.; Handlin, L. Oxytocin and cortisol levels in dog owners and their dogs are associated with behavioral patterns: An exploratory study. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Barcelos, A.M.; Kargas, N.; Maltby, J.; Hall, S.; Mills, D.S. A framework for understanding how activities associated with dog ownership relate to human well-being. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 11363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Deci, E.L.; Ryan, R.M. Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: An introduction. J. Happiness Stud. 2008, 9, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Joshanloo, M. Investigating the relationships between subjective well-being and psychological well-being over two decades. Emotion 2019, 19, 183–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Diener, E. Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. Am. Psychol. 2000, 55, 34–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ryff, C.D. Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 57, 1069–1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ryff, C.D. Psychological Well-Being Revisited: Advances in the science and practice of Eudaimonia. Psychother. Psychosom. 2014, 83, 10–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Corrêa, G.F.; Barcelos, A.M.; Mills, D.S. Dog-related activities and human well-being in Brazilian dog owners: A framework and cross-cultural comparison with a British study. Sci. Prog. 2021, in press. [Google Scholar]
  25. Barcelos, A.M.; Kargas, N.; Packham, C.; Mills, D.S. Understanding the impact of dog ownership on autistic adults: Implications for mental health and suicide prevention. Sci. Rep. 2021, in press. [Google Scholar]
  26. Ravenscroft, S.J.; Barcelos, A.M.; Mills, D.S. Cat-human related activities associated with human well-being. Hum. Anim. Interact. Bull. 2021, in press. [Google Scholar]
  27. Huta, V. An overview of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being concepts. In Handbook of Media Use and Well-Being; Reinecke, L., Oliver, M.B., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 32–51. [Google Scholar]
  28. Cavanaugh, L.A.; Leonard, H.A.; Scammon, D.L. A tail of two personalities: How canine companions shape relationships and well-being. J. Bus. Res. 2008, 61, 469–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kanat-Maymon, Y.; Wolfson, S.; Cohen, R.; Roth, G. The benefits of giving as well as receiving need support in human–pet relations. J. Happiness Stud. 2021, 22, 1441–1457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Queirós, A.; Faria, D.; Almeida, F. Strengths and limitations of qualitative and quantitative research methods. Eur. J. Educ. Stud. 2017, 3, 369–387. [Google Scholar]
  31. Scherer, K.R. What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Soc. Sci. Inf. 2005, 44, 695–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Yik, M.; Russell, J.A.; Steiger, J.H. A 12-point circumplex structure of core affect. Emotion 2011, 11, 705–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Russell, J.A. Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 110, 145–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Aron, A.; Aron, E.N.; Smollan, D. Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 63, 596–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Gulick, E.E.; Krause-Parello, C.A. Factors related to type of companion pet owned by older women. J. Psychosoc. Nurs. Ment. Heal. Serv. 2012, 50, 30–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Christian, H.; Wood, L.; Nathan, A.; Kawachi, I.; Houghton, S.; Martin, K.; McCune, S. The association between dog walking, physical activity and owner’s perceptions of safety: Cross-sectional evidence from the US and Australia. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Lai, L.C.H.; Cummins, R.A.; Lau, A.L.D. Cross-cultural difference in subjective wellbeing: Cultural response bias as an explanation. Soc. Indic. Res. 2013, 114, 607–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Himsworth, C.G.; Rock, M. Pet ownership, other domestic relationships, and satisfaction with life among seniors: Results from a Canadian national survey. Anthrozoös 2013, 26, 295–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Eaves, L.C.; Ho, H.H. Young adult outcome of autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2008, 38, 739–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Simpson, P.L.; Schumaker, J.F.; Dorahy, M.J.; Shrestha, S.N. Depression and life satisfaction in Nepal and Australia. J. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 136, 783–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Westgarth, C.; Christley, R.M.; Marvin, G.; Perkins, E. I walk my dog because it makes me happy: A qualitative study to under-stand why dogs motivate walking and improved health. Int. J. Environ. Res. 2017, 14, 936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Van Herwijnen, I.R.; Van Der Borg, J.A.M.; Naguib, M.; Beerda, B. Dog ownership satisfaction determinants in the owner-dog relationship and the dog’s behaviour. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0204592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Handlin, L.; Hydbring-Sandberg, E.; Nilsson, A.; Ejdebäck, M.; Jansson, A.; Uvnäs-Moberg, K. Short-term tnteraction between dogs and their owners: Effects on oxytocin, cortisol, insulin and heart rate—an exploratory study. Anthrozoös 2011, 24, 301–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Odendaal, J.; Meintjes, R. Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behaviour between humans and dogs. Vet. J. 2003, 165, 296–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Beetz, A.; Julius, H.; Turner, D.; Kotrschal, K. Effects of social support by a dog on stress modulation in male children with insecure attachment. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Horowitz, A.; Hecht, J. Examining dog–human play: The characteristics, affect, and vocalizations of a unique interspecific interaction. Anim. Cogn. 2016, 19, 779–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Swall, A.; Ebbeskog, B.; Hagelin, C.L.; Fagerberg, I. Can therapy dogs evoke awareness of one’s past and present life in persons with Alzheimer′s disease? Int. J. Older People Nurs. 2014, 10, 84–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Chandler, C.K.; Fernando, D.M.; Minton, C.A.B.; Portrie-Bethke, T.L. Eight domains of pet-owner wellness: Valuing the owner-pet relationship in the counseling process. J. Ment. Health Couns. 2015, 37, 268–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Ramírez, M.T.G.; Hernández, R.L. Benefits of dog ownership: Comparative study of equivalent samples. J. Vet. Behav. 2014, 9, 311–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Bao, K.J.; Schreer, G. Pets and happiness: Examining the association between pet ownership and wellbeing. Anthrozoös 2016, 29, 283–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Polheber, J.P.; Matchock, R.L. The presence of a dog attenuates cortisol and heart rate in the Trier Social Stress Test compared to human friends. J. Behav. Med. 2014, 37, 860–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Nettifee, J.A.; Munana, K.; Griffith, E.H. Evaluation of the impacts of epilepsy in dogs on their caregivers. J. Am. Anim. Hosp. Assoc. 2017, 53, 143–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Pergande, A.E.; Belshaw, Z.; Volk, H.A.; Packer, R.M.A. “We have a ticking time bomb”: A qualitative exploration of the impact of canine epilepsy on dog owners living in England. BMC Vet. Res. 2020, 16, 443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Spitznagel, M.B.; Jacobson, D.M.; Cox, M.D.; Carlson, M.D. Caregiver burden in owners of a sick companion animal: A cross-sectional observational study. Vet. Rec. 2017, 181, 321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Spitznagel, M.B.; Carlson, M.D. Caregiver burden and veterinary client well-Being. Vet. Clin. Small Anim. Pract. 2019, 49, 431–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Christiansen, S.B.; Kristensen, A.T.; Sandøe, P.; Lassen, J. Looking after chronically ill dogs: Impacts on the caregiver’s life. Anthrozoös 2013, 26, 519–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Buller, K.; Ballantyne, K.C. Living with and loving a pet with behavioral problems: Pet owners’ experiences. J. Vet. Behav. 2020, 37, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Ben-Michael, J.; Korzilius, H.; Felling, A.; Vossen, J. An exploratory model of dog disciplining. Anthrozoös 2000, 13, 150–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Carr, E.C.J.; Wallace, J.E.; Onyewuchi, C.; Hellyer, P.W.; Kogan, L. Exploring the meaning and experience of chronic pain with people who live with a dog: A qualitative study. Anthrozoös 2018, 31, 551–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. McNicholas, J.; Collis, G.M. Dogs as catalysts for social interactions: Robustness of the effect. Br. J. Psychol. 2000, 91, 61–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Kabel, A.; Khosla, N.; Teti, M. The Dog Narratives: Benefits of the human-animal bond for women with HIV. J. HIV/AIDS Soc. Serv. 2015, 14, 405–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Glaw, X.; Kable, A.; Hazelton, M.; Inder, K. Meaning in life and meaning of life in mental health care: An integrative literature review. Issues Ment. Health Nurs. 2016, 38, 243–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Love, H.A. Best friends come in all breeds: The role of pets in suicidality. Anthrozoös 2021, 34, 175–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Serpell, J.; McCune, S.; Gee, N.; Griffin, J.A. Current challenges to research on animal-assisted interventions. Appl. Dev. Sci. 2017, 21, 223–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Wells, D.L. The facilitation of social interactions by domestic dogs. Anthrozoös 2004, 17, 340–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Guéguen, N.; Ciccotti, S. Domestic dogs as facilitators in social interaction: An evaluation of helping and courtship behaviors. Anthrozoös 2008, 21, 339–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Flynn, E.; Combs, K.M.; Gandenberger, J.; Tedeschi, P.; Morris, K.N. Measuring the psychological impacts of prison-based Dog training programs and in-prison outcomes for inmates. Prison. J. 2020, 100, 224–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Leonardi, R.J.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M.; McIvor, G.; Vick, S.-J. “You think you’re helping them, but they’re helping you too”: Experiences of Scottish male young offenders participating in a dog training program. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Yount, R.; Ritchie, E.C.; Laurent, M.S.; Chumley, P.; Olmert, M.D. The role of service dog training in the treatment of combat-related PTSD. Psychiatr. Ann. 2013, 43, 292–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Cooke, B.J.; Farrington, D. The effects of dog-training programs: Experiences of incarcerated females. Women Crim. Justice 2015, 25, 201–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Antonio, M.E.; Davis, R.G.; Shutt, S.R. Dog training programs in Pennsylvania’s Department of Corrections. Soc. Anim. 2017, 25, 475–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Herzog, H. Women Dominate Research on the Human-Animal Bond. Available online: https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/animals-and-us/202105/women-dominate-research-the-Human-animal-bond (accessed on 24 May 2021).
  73. Chia, D.; Powell, L.; Lee, V.; Haghighi, M.M.; Podberscek, A.; Ding, D.; Sherrington, C.; Stamatakis, E. Sociodemographic correlates of prospective dog owners’ intentions to participate in controlled trials of dog ownership and human health. BMC Res. Notes 2018, 11, 169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Jenkins, R.; Bhugra, D.; Bebbington, P.; Brugha, T.; Farrell, M.; Coid, J.; Fryers, T.; Weich, S.; Singleton, N.; Meltzer, H. Debt, income and mental disorder in the general population. Psychol. Med. 2008, 38, 1485–1493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. Soto, C.J. Is Happiness Good for your personality? Concurrent and prospective relations of the Big Five with subjective well-being. J. Pers. 2014, 83, 45–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Utz, R.L. Walking the dog: The effect of pet ownership on human health and health behaviors. Soc. Indic. Res. 2013, 116, 327–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Mueller, M.K.; King, E.K.; Callina, K.; Dowling-Guyer, S.; McCobb, E. Demographic and contextual factors as moderators of the relationship between pet ownership and health. Health Psychol. Behav. Med. 2021, 9, 701–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Dog-related activities/situations assessed in the questionnaire.
Table 1. Dog-related activities/situations assessed in the questionnaire.
1. Exercise (e.g., walking, running, hiking with the dog, etc)6. Look after the dog (e.g., feeding, giving water)11. Health problems (dog health issues, e.g., injury, sickness)
2. Dog presence (i.e., the presence/company of the dog)7. Dog playing (i.e., play with the dog or watch the dog play)12. Sensory problems (i.e., behavioural issues that disturb owner’s senses, e.g., dog barking, house soiling)
3. Tactile interactions (i.e., touching or being touched by the dog)8. Talk to others because of the dog (e.g., say hello to others because of dog)13. Dog out of control (i.e., loss or lack of control over the dog, such as when the dog pulls on the lead, does not respond to recall)
4. Other close interactions (e.g., dog greeting, talking to the dog)9. Simply having the dog (i.e., being a dog owner)14. Dog aggression (e.g., growling, trying to bite, biting)
5. Train the dog (e.g., commands, housetraining)10. Maintenance (i.e., involvement in indirect tasks to provide for the dog, such bills, vet visits, purchases)15. Other unwanted behaviours (e.g., destruction of items, attention seeking, separation-related problems, etc)
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Barcelos, A.M.; Kargas, N.; Maltby, J.; Hall, S.; Assheton, P.; Mills, D.S. Theoretical Foundations to the Impact of Dog-Related Activities on Human Hedonic Well-Being, Life Satisfaction and Eudaimonic Well-Being. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 12382. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182312382

AMA Style

Barcelos AM, Kargas N, Maltby J, Hall S, Assheton P, Mills DS. Theoretical Foundations to the Impact of Dog-Related Activities on Human Hedonic Well-Being, Life Satisfaction and Eudaimonic Well-Being. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(23):12382. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182312382

Chicago/Turabian Style

Barcelos, Ana Maria, Niko Kargas, John Maltby, Sophie Hall, Phil Assheton, and Daniel S. Mills. 2021. "Theoretical Foundations to the Impact of Dog-Related Activities on Human Hedonic Well-Being, Life Satisfaction and Eudaimonic Well-Being" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 23: 12382. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182312382

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop