Next Article in Journal
Impact of COVID-19 Stay-at-Home Restrictions on Employment Status, Physical Activity, and Sedentary Behavior
Next Article in Special Issue
A Randomized Feasibility Trial of a Fundamental Motor Skill Parent-Mediated Intervention for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders
Previous Article in Journal
Communication of Uncertainty about Preliminary Evidence and the Spread of Its Inferred Misinformation during the COVID-19 Pandemic—A Weibo Case Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
“… and after That Came Me”. Subjective Constructions of Social Hierarchy in Physical Education Classes among Youth with Visual Impairments in Germany
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Affordances for Motor Development in the Home Environment for Young Children with and without CHARGE Syndrome

Department of Kinesiology, Physical Education, Sport Studies, State University of New York Brockport, Brockport, NY 14420, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(22), 11936; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182211936
Submission received: 2 October 2021 / Revised: 25 October 2021 / Accepted: 10 November 2021 / Published: 13 November 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Movement Studies for Individuals with Visual Impairments)

Abstract

:
Affordances in the home environment are critical to early motor development. Currently, the home environment has not been examined in children with deafblindness or severe disabilities. The present study examined differences in, and relationships between, the home environment and motor development in children with and without CHARGE syndrome. CHARGE syndrome is a low-incidence, complex disorder with sensory and motor impairments. Participants included 28 parents of children with CHARGE syndrome and 32 parents of children without disabilities. Children with CHARGE syndrome achieved motor milestones significantly later and had fewer outside space affordances than children without disabilities. Older children had a greater variety of stimulation and fine motor toys, and those that achieved independent walking later had more outside space and fine and gross motor toys. Early experiences may be more important for children with CHARGE syndrome than children without disabilities. Moreover, parents can play a vital role in their children’s motor development to help them reach their motor milestones.

1. Introduction

During the early years, young children develop many motor milestones and fundamental motor skills [1]. The home environment is an important source of support for promoting motor development during this critical time of early childhood when a child is provided with a stimulating and supportive surrounding [2]. Infants and young children develop these motor milestones and motor skills through interacting with the environment in a meaningful way. Both gross motor skills, involving large muscles (e.g., kicking), and fine motor skills, involving smaller muscles (e.g., building a tower), are functionally integrated during development [3] and develop using the same higher-order neuromotor processes [4]. These motor skills are the building blocks for more advanced skills that are developed throughout childhood.
The notion of affordances within the ecological perspective provides a framework for understanding external influences on an individual’s motor development. From this perspective, affordances are aspects of the task (e.g., equipment) and environment (e.g., open space, parental support) that provide opportunities for action [5,6]. A supportive home environment for promoting motor development would include family members who are actively involved with the child as well as large spaces (e.g., backyard) and a variety of toys (e.g., balls) that encourage movement. Children with increased support and opportunities in the home would benefit while children who lack these affordances may lag behind their peers in their motor development [7].
Using the ecological perspective, Rodrigues and colleagues [7] created the first inventory addressing the presence of affordances in the home and the effect of these affordances upon motor development titled Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development Self-Report (AHEMD-SR). A positive relationship has been found among the five factors of the AHEMD-SR (outside space, inside space, variety of stimulation, gross motor toys, and fine motor toys) and motor development, with the greatest predictor being the child’s fine motor toys [8]. Moreover, children’s access to fine and gross motor toys and parents’ level of physical activity involvement with their children have been shown to significantly impact their motor development [9].
The AHEMD has also been used to examine differences in the home environment in infants and young children with and without disabilities, such as sensory impairments. Lage and colleagues [10] found that children with low vision had significantly less fine and gross motor toy affordances than their sighted peers; however, there were no differences in caregiver assistance between the two samples. Additionally, Araujo and colleagues [11] found that homes of infants with risk indicators for hearing loss displayed statistically fewer affordances when compared to homes of infants without the risk indicators. These findings revealed a variety of factors that influenced the development of fine and gross motor skills at home including toy availability, parental physical activity involvement, parental socioeconomic status (SES), age, and level of children’s hearing [11]. Although studies have been conducted upon young children with visual and hearing losses separately, the influence of the affordances for motor development in the home environment has not been examined in young children with both visual and hearing losses or severe disabilities. To examine the importance of the home environment upon motor development in young children with deafblindness and severe disabilities, this study sought to examine young children with CHARGE syndrome, which is the leading genetic cause of congenital deafblindness [12].
CHARGE syndrome is a rare genetic disorder identified by a broad range of multisensory deficits. One identifying factor is a mutation to the CHD7 gene, which has been identified as the cause of CHARGE syndrome [13]. A diagnosis of CHARGE syndrome is based on testing for the presence of a CHD7 gene mutation and/or a combination of the major and minor features [14]. Currently, the selection of these factors and the combination of factors needed for diagnosis are still under consideration. Major features currently include coloboma of the eye, choanal atresia, cranial nerve anomalies, and ear malformation. Minor features currently include heart defects, cleft lip and palate, kidney abnormalities, genital abnormalities, growth deficiency, typical CHARGE face, and palmar crease [14].
Children with CHARGE syndrome signify a heterogeneous population; therefore, individuals vary widely in their lived experiences of the medical and physical difficulties associated with the syndrome [15]. However, most individuals with CHARGE syndrome experience some level of hearing loss, vision loss, and balance problems, all of which lead to various levels of delays in motor development and communication [16]. In addition to physical limitations, children with CHARGE syndrome often experience multiple, prolonged hospitalizations, which may increase the likelihood of developmental delays due to their lack of consistent socialization and physical activity [17]. Based on the challenges described above, it is not surprising that children with CHARGE syndrome experience delays in the achievement of motor milestones [18]. Motor milestones are early motor skills that follow a particular sequence from holding the head up to independent walking. Children with CHARGE syndrome walk an average of 25 months later than their sighted peers [19,20]. One reason for this late onset for independent walking is that children with CHARGE syndrome experience more balance issues compared to their peers without disabilities [21,22]. This delayed walking can have drastic impacts on other gross motor skills, such as locomotor and object control skills [23,24], which are critical for engaging in many physical activities.
To date, it is not known if motor delays have been affected by the home environment. Although specific motor development characteristics have been assessed for children with CHARGE syndrome, the influence of the home environment upon their motor development has not been examined. The first aim of this study was to use the AHEMD to examine differences in affordances for motor development in the home environment between young children with and without CHARGE syndrome to better understand the multidimensional effects of affordances in the home related to motor development upon disability. A second aim of this study was to compare the relationship between home environment affordances with the age of onset of motor milestones in young children with CHARGE syndrome. To examine these aims, we proposed two hypotheses: 1. affordances as measured by the AHEMD will significantly differ in children with CHARGE syndrome in comparison to children without disabilities; 2. fine and gross motor toys available in the home will be significantly related to age of motor milestones.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants included parents or legal guardians of a child with or without CHARGE syndrome between the ages of 18 and 42 months. Twenty-eight parents of children with CHARGE syndrome and 32 parents of children without disabilities participated in this study. The children with CHARGE syndrome included 14 females and 14 males; 20 Caucasian, 1 American Indian or Alaskan Native, 2 Asian, 1 other, 3 more than one ethnicity, and 1 not indicated. Children without disabilities included 15 females and 17 males; 21 Caucasian, 2 Black or African American, 2 other, 4 more than one ethnicity, and 5 not indicated. Refer to Table 1 for descriptive data on the children of the participants and Table 2 for the CHARGE characteristics of the children with CHARGE syndrome. Independent-samples t-tests and chi-square analyses revealed no significant differences in age (t (5) = 0.40, p = 0.70), height (t (54) = −0.52, p = 0.61), gender (X2 (1, N = 60) = 0.06, p = 0.81), or ethnicity (X2 (4, N = 54) = 3.5, p = 0.48) between the children of the two samples; however, there was a significant difference in weight (t (57) = −2.86, p = 0.006), wherein the children with CHARGE syndrome weighed significantly less than the children without disabilities. This is not surprising because growth is often restricted in individuals with CHARGE syndrome [14].

2.2. Measure

Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development is a self-report instrument that assesses the quality and quantity of motor development opportunities in the home during early childhood [7,25]. The measure is designed for children between the ages of 18 and 42 months old and completed by parents or legal guardians, which are the best source of information on the home environment. The AHEMD inventory consists of 67 questions examining the home and characteristics of the family. The inventory consists of five sections: two on the family and child’s characteristics and three on affordances of the home environment: (1) physical space, (2) daily activities, and (3) play materials. There are three types of questions including Likert scale, multiple choice, and descriptive queries. The scoring method specifically designed for this inventory was used in this study. Total scores range from 5 to 20. Scores below 10 are categorized as low, meaning minimal affordances to promote motor development, scores between 10 and 15 are categorized as average, indicating sufficient opportunities, and scores between 15 and 20 are categorized as high, meaning very good affordances in the home for motor development. In addition, the survey includes a short family demographic survey consisting of the following variables: number of adults living in the house, number of children living in the house, number of rooms in the house, length of time the child has lived in the house, father’s education, mother’s education, annual family income, and length of childcare attendance. All demographic items are scored on a Likert scale. In addition, parents indicated the age of onset for their child’s motor milestones.
Construct validity and reliability were previously examined with families from the United States and Portugal [7]. Confirmatory factor analysis provided a 5-factor solution: outside space, inside space, variety of stimulation, fine motor toys, and gross motor toys. Fit indexes were all over 0.90 and reliability was established with high consistency, construct of interest ranging from 0.80 to 0.91.

2.3. Procedure

Parents of children with CHARGE syndrome were recruited from the CHARGE Syndrome Foundation via email and social media, and parents of children without disabilities were recruited from local daycare facilities (letters) and social media. Participants completed the survey by accessing a link or QR code that included the informed consent, demographics, CHARGE syndrome characteristics, age of onset of motor milestones, and AHEMD. Institutional review board approval was received prior to recruiting for this study.

2.4. Analyses

Scores from each participant on the AHEMD were entered into the AHEMD calculator provided by the developers to obtain standard scores for the five different factors: (1) outside space, (2) inside space, (3) variety of stimulation, (4) fine motor toys, and (5) gross motor toys [25]. Standard scores range from 1 (very low) to 4 (high). These scores were then combined to form a total AHEMD score ranging from 5 to 20.
A chi-square analysis and independent-samples t-tests were conducted to examine differences in the achievement and age of motor milestone between the children with CHARGE syndrome and the children without disabilities. Next, Mann–Whitney U tests were performed to determine differences in family demographic variables and the standard scores of the AHEMD between the two samples. Finally, Spearman rank order correlations were used to examine relationships between current age, age of each motor milestone, family demographics, and AHEMD scores for each sample.

3. Results

3.1. Motor Milestones

The chi-square analysis revealed significant differences in the achievement of several motor milestones between the two samples. Specifically, more children without disabilities could crawl, creep, stand without support, cruise, walk, run, jump, and hop than the children with CHARGE syndrome (see Table 3). There were also significant differences in age of motor milestones, wherein the children without disabilities achieved each milestone at an earlier age than the children with CHARGE syndrome (p < 0.001; see Table 4). Due to the limited number of children with CHARGE syndrome who could run, jump, and hop, these motor milestones were excluded from the analysis. Family demographic variables only differed significantly between the two samples for childcare length (U = 196, p < 0.001, r = 0.45), wherein the children without disabilities had longer childcare attendance than the children with CHARGE syndrome.

3.2. AHEMD Scores

Frequencies of affordance level for AHEMD scores across the two samples are presented in Table 5. Overall, most children across the two samples were categorized in the good to high range across all components of the AHEMD. One notable difference was that no children without disabilities were categorized as extremely low for any component whereas a small number of children with CHARGE syndrome were categorized as very low for outside space and variety of stimulation. Interestingly, all participants were categorized as high for inside space; thus, this component does not appear to be a discriminating factor between the two samples. For the total AHEMD score, all participants were categorized as average or high; however, a much larger majority of the children without disabilities were categorized as high compared to the children with CHARGE syndrome. Despite some of these initial observations, the analysis revealed that the two samples only differed on outside space (U = 299, p = 0.05, r = 0.25), suggesting that parents of children without disabilities offer better outside space affordances than the parents of children with CHARGE syndrome (see Table 6).
For the children with CHARGE syndrome, the correlation analysis revealed that current age had a significant positive relationship with variety of stimulation (r = 0.41, p = 0.039) and fine motor toys (r = 0.50, p = 0.01), indicating that the older the child, the more variety of movement opportunities and fine motor toys the child had access to in the home. The analysis also indicated that age of standing without support had a significant positive correlation with fine motor toys (rs = 0.60, p = 0.038) and total AHEMD (rs = 0.64, p = 0.024). Age of walking had significant positive relationships with outside space (rs = 0.63, p = 0.038), fine motor toys (rs = 0.70, p = 0.016), gross motor toys (rs = 0.84, p = 0.001), and total AHEMD (rs = 0.88, p < 0.001). However, these results must be interpreted cautiously due to the small number of children who had reached these milestones, n = 12 and n = 11, respectively. For the demographic variables, number of adults in the house had significant negative correlations with outside space (rs = −0.40, p = 0.041) and gross motor toys (rs = −0.42, p = 0.032), indicating that children had less access to outside space and gross motor toys when there were more adults living at the family house. Significant positive relationships were found for number of rooms in the house with both outside space (rs = 0.46, p = 0.019) and total AHEMD (rs = 0.43, p = 0.029); number of children in the house correlated with age of walking (rs = 0.66, p = 0.026), fine motor toys (rs = 0.47, p = 0.017) and total AHEMD (rs = 0.47, p = 0.019); and annual income correlated with age of walking (rs = 0.68, p = 0.022), gross motor toys (rs = 0.46, p = 0.017) and total AHEMD (rs = 0.51, p = 0.008).
For the children without disabilities, there were no significant relationships between current age and age of motor milestones with the AHEMD. However, there were significant correlations for some of the demographic variables. Specifically, number of children in the house had significant positive relationships with outside space (rs = 0.39, p = 0.03), variety of stimulation (rs = 0.51, p = 0.004), and total AHEMD (rs = 0.43, p = 0.018), indicating that outside space and varied movement opportunities were more available when more children were living in the home. There was also a significant positive association between annual income and variety of stimulation (rs = 0.41, p = 0.024), which suggests that wealthier families were able to provide more diverse movement opportunities at home.

4. Discussion

Motor skill development has an interactive effect with cognitive, emotional, and motor-perceptual development [8]. The purpose of this study was to examine the affordances in the home environment in young children with and without CHARGE syndrome to examine the multidimensional effects of affordances to motor development. Home environment resources include toys and space, but also parental and family support such as encouragement, guidance, and regular engagement with their child. To the author’s knowledge, this is the first study to examine the home environment in young children with deafblindness or severe disabilities. Specifically, we examined young children with CHARGE syndrome, the leading cause of congenital deafblindness [12]. Motor development is often significantly delayed in children with CHARGE syndrome due not only to sensory impairments, but motor impairments as well as environmental constraints, such as long hospital stays [23,24].
To examine the affordances for motor development in the home environment, parents of young children with and without CHARGE syndrome completed a validated survey on affordances in the home environment, AHEMD [7,25]. The rationale for this research is that more affordances in the home afford children more opportunities for movement experiences and physical activity that can promote fine and gross motor development [26]. This notion is based upon the ecological theory in which environments have individuals, objects, places, and events that provide action opportunities [6]. These action opportunities, affordances, are experienced differently depending upon the child’s functionalities and capabilities [6,27], and therefore, would be expected to be different in children with deafblindness and severe disabilities in comparison to children without disabilities.
Due to the delays in motor development, it was important to examine the role of demographics of the family and affordances in the home environment with the achievement of motor milestones in children with and without CHARGE syndrome. The findings revealed that children without disabilities were in childcare significantly more than children with CHARGE syndrome. These results are likely due to the medical needs of children with CHARGE syndrome at an early age [23,24]. Most daycares are typically not equipped to attend to the additional medical, functional, and communication needs of children with CHARGE syndrome. Outside space was also found to be higher for children without disabilities in comparison to the children with CHARGE syndrome. Although not as important as inside space, outdoor space is an important affordance contributing to motor development [28]. Outside space included more than one ground texture, one or more sloped surfaces, and apparatus to grasp, climb, or step, such as a playground. Limited space is a predictor of gross motor development in children without disabilities [28] and would likely restrict development in children with disabilities to an equal or potentially greater extent than children without disabilities. It is possible that parents of children with CHARGE syndrome purchase less outdoor equipment as a response to their child’s motor delays assuming that their child is not developmentally ready to use the equipment. It is also possible that parents perceive outdoor equipment as more dangerous than indoor toys. Parents of children with visual impairments are often found to be overprotective out of fear for their child’s safety [29].
The findings of this study also provided some implications of the family demographics as affordances to their child’s motor development. Interestingly, children with CHARGE syndrome lived in families with more adults than the families of children without disabilities. This may be due to the additional assistance and the presence of significant stress found in families with a non-verbal child [30]. Other demographic factors that influenced the affordances for children with CHARGE syndrome were SES and number of siblings. SES, which includes income and parental education, has been associated with the level of home affordances, wherein children who live in homes with higher incomes and parental education are provided with more affordances leading to higher motor skill development [28,31]. In the present study, SES, specifically income, was associated with age of walking, gross motor toys, and the total AHEMD score for children with CHARGE syndrome and SES was associated with a variety of stimulation for children without disabilities.
A second aim of this study was to examine the relationship between home environment affordances with the age of onset of motor milestones in young children with CHARGE syndrome due to their delays in motor development [32]. Similar to prior research [18,19,20], the results of the present study found significant delays in the acquisition of motor milestones in the children with CHARGE syndrome, such as age of standing and independent walking occurring significantly later than their peers without disabilities. Interestingly, however, age of walking was associated with the number of toys in the home. It is possible this is due to parents purchasing more toys to encourage their child to walk if they are delayed. Children who develop gross motor skills early likely would not need as many toys to promote walking. On another note, age was also positively associated with a variety of stimulation and fine motor toys in the present study for children with CHARGE syndrome. Older children benefitted from more fine motor toys, such as peg boards or lacing cubes, and more opportunities for movement than younger children. It is possible that there are more toys for older children due to the parents having more time to purchase toys and parents are more likely to purchase toys that they feel may benefit their child. Regardless of the reason for increased toys, it is important to note that fine motor toys are a significant predictor of fine motor skill development [28].

Limitations

This study provides some initial findings of affordances for motor development in the home environment in an underrepresented population, children with severe disabilities, specifically, CHARGE syndrome. Due to the low incidence of this population, obtaining large sample sizes, which is more typical of this type of research, is not possible. It is important to also understand the uniqueness of CHARGE syndrome, which can reduce generalizability. However, considering there has not been research conducted with children with severe disabilities, it is possible the findings may generalize to families with children with severe disabilities as they may likely have many similar challenges at home. As such, more research should be conducted on other populations with severe disabilities to strengthen these results and increase knowledge of the home affordances for children with severe disabilities.
It is also important to note that due to the delayed gross motor development of children with CHARGE syndrome, there was a particularly small sample size of participants who were able to independently walk. Children with CHARGE syndrome typically walk independently 30 months later than their peers without disabilities [22]. As such, future research should also examine broader age groups in families with children with severe disabilities.
Furthermore, it is important to mention the limitations of using a parental questionnaire. The questionnaire we used has been found to be valid and reliable; however, a thorough home assessment would provide more depth than parental report. For example, the assessment does not examine the length of time children spend using the affordances. The current research also relied upon parental recall for the onset of motor milestones. The researchers feel that although the assessment relied upon parental assessments and recall, the findings provide a foundation for understanding home affordances in children with severe disabilities, which, as mentioned, should be further explored. Future research should extend upon these findings by also examining the relationship of home affordances with objective measurements of fine and gross motor skills.

5. Conclusions

The home environment provides important affordances that can either promote or inhibit fine and gross motor development. The findings of this study indicate that early experiences may be even more important for children with CHARGE syndrome in comparison to children without disabilities. Parents should help guide their children with CHARGE syndrome on their motor development. Home environment resources include toys and space, but also parental and family support such as encouragement, guidance, and regular engagement with their child. In addition, it is recommended for parents of children with CHARGE syndrome to work with their child’s support team to determine optimal home affordances.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.B. and M.P.; methodology P.B.; software, P.B.; formal analysis, M.P.; investigation, P.B. and M.P.; resources, P.B. and L.L.; data curation, P.B. and M.P.; writing—original draft preparation, P.B., M.P. and L.L.; writing—review and editing, P.B., M.P. and L.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of SUNY Brockport IRB #00001431 on 22 March 2019 with a modification #00000869 approved on 24 October 2019.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

This study did not report any data.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Lipkin, P.H. Motor Development and Dysfunction. In Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics; Elsevier Inc.: Amsterdam, Netherlands, 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Iltus, S. Significance of Home Environments as Proxy Indicators for Early Childhood Care and Education; Paper Commissioned for the EFAGlobal Monitoring Report; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  3. Cameron, C.E.; Brock, L.L.; Murrah, W.M.; Bell, L.H.; Worzalla, S.L.; Grissmer, D.; Morrison, F.J. Fine Motor Skills and Executive Function Both Contribute to Kindergarten Achievement. Child Dev. 2012, 83, 1229–1244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Roebers, C.M.; Kauer, M. Motor and Cognitive Control in a Normative Sample of 7 Year Olds. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 175–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Gibson, E.J. Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1988, 39, 1–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Gibson, E.J. Perceiving the Affordances: A Portrait of Two Psychologists; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  7. Rodrigues, L.P.; Saraiva, L.; Gabbard, C. Development and construct validation of an inventory for assessing the home environment for motor development. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2005, 76, 140–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Haydari, A.; Askari, P.; Zarra Nezhad, M. Relationship between affordances in the home environment and motor development in children age 19–42 months. J. Soc. Sci. 2009, 5, 319–328. [Google Scholar]
  9. Mori, S.; Nakamoto, H.; Mizuochi, H.; Ikudome, S.; Gabbard, C. Influence of affordances in the home environment on motor development of young children in Japan. Child Dev. Res. 2013, 2013, 898406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lage, J.B.; Nascentes, G.A.N.; Pereira, K. Influence of the home environmental affordances in the mobility of children with low vision mobility: Functional skills and caregiver assistance. Rev. Bras. Oftalmol. 2016, 75, 290–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Araujo, D.M.; Santos, D.C.C.; Lima, M.C.M.P. Home environment of infants with risk indicators for hearing loss tends to be less stimulating. Int. J. Pediatric Otorhinolaryngol. 2019, 120, 146–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. National Center on Deaf-Blindness. The 2017 National Child Count of Children and You Who Are Deaf-Blind. Available online: https://nationaldb.org/reports/national-child-count-2017 (accessed on 4 October 2021).
  13. Janssen, N.; Bergman, J.E.H.; Swertz, M.A.; Tranebjaerg, L.; Lodahl, M.; Schoots, J.; Hoefsloot, L.H. Mutation update on the CHD7 gene involved in CHARGE syndrome. Hum. Mutat. 2012, 33, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Blake, K.D.; Davenport, S.L.; Hall, B.D.; Hefner, M.A.; Pagon, R.A.; Williams, M.S.; Graham, J.M. CHARGE association: An update and review for the primary pediatrician. Clin. Pediatrics 1998, 37, 159–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Blake, K.D.; Prasad, C. CHARGE syndrome. Orphanet J. Rare Dis. 2006, 1, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Hartshorne, T.S.; Hefner, M.A.; Davenport, S.L.H. Behavior in CHARGE syndrome: Introduction to the special topic. Am. J. Med. Genet. 2005, 133A, 228–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Lieberman, L.J.; Haibach, P.; Schedlin, H. Physical education and children with CHARGE syndrome: Research to practice. J. Vis. Impair. Blind. 2012, 106, 106–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Thelin, J.W.; Curtis, S.E.; Maddox, J.F.; Travis, L.S. Balance and mobility. In CHARGE Syndrome; Hartshorne, T.S., Hefner, M.A., Davenport, S.L.H., Thelin, J.W., Eds.; Plural Publishing: San Diego, CA, USA, 2011; pp. 55–62. [Google Scholar]
  19. Dammeyer, J. Development and characteristics of children with Usher syndrome and CHARGE syndrome. Int. J. Pediatric Otorhinolaryngol. 2012, 76, 1292–1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Foster, E.; Silliman-French, L.; Grenier, M. Parents’ perceptions of constraints impacting the development of walking in children with CHARGE syndrome. Res. Pract. Pers. Sev. Disabil. 2020, 45, 196–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Haibach, P.S.; Lieberman, L.J. Balance and self-efficacy of balance in children with CHARGE syndrome. J. Vis. Impair. Blind. 2013, 107, 297–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Haibach-Beach, P.; Perreault, M.; Lieberman, L.; Foster, E. Independent walking and balance in children with CHARGE Syndrome. Br. J. Vis. Impair. 2020, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Haibach-Beach, P.S.; Perreault, M.; Lieberman, L.; Foster, E. Gross motor skill performance in children with CHARGE Syndrome: Research to practice. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2019, 91, 103423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Perreault, M.; Haibach-Beach, P.S.; Foster, E.; Lieberman, L. Relationship between motor skills, balance, and physical activity in children with CHARGE Syndrome. J. Vis. Impair. Blind. 2020, 114, 315–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Rodrigues, L.P.; Gabbard, C.; Caçola, P. AHEMD Calculator. AHEMD Project. n.d. Available online: http://www.ese.ipvc.pt/dmh/AHEMD/ahemd_6.htm (accessed on 24 September 2021).
  26. Flôres, F.S.; Rodrigues, L.P.; Cordovil, R. Relationship between the affordances for motor behavior of schoolchildren (AMBS) and motor competence assessment (MCA) in Brazilian children. Children 2021, 8, 705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Heft, H. Foundations of an ecological approach to psychology. In The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  28. Valadi, S.; Gabbard, C. The effect of affordances in the home environment on children’s fine- and gross motor skills. Early Child Dev. Care 2020, 190, 1225–1232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Linsenbigler, K.; Petersen, S.; Lieberman, L.J. Barriers to physical activity for children with visual impairments: Where have we been and where do we still need to go? Palaestra 2017, 32, 26–31. [Google Scholar]
  30. Wulffaert, J.; Scholte, E.M.; Dijkxhoorn, Y.M.; Bergman, J.E.H.; van Ravenswaaij-Arts, C.M.A.; Berckelaer-Onnes, I.A. Parenting stress in CHARGE syndrome and the relationship with child characteristics. J. Phys. Disabil. 2009, 21, 301–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Freitas, T.C.B.; Gabbard, C.; Cacola, P.; Montebelo, M.I.L.; Santos, D.C.C. Family socioeconomic status and the provision of motor affordances in the home. Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 2013, 17, 319–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Perreault, M.; Haibach-Beach, P.; Lieberman, L.; Foster, E. Motor Competence in children with CHARGE syndrome. Res. Pract. Pers. Sev. Disabil. 2021, 46, 67–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Descriptive data of children with and without CHARGE syndrome.
Table 1. Descriptive data of children with and without CHARGE syndrome.
Children with CHARGEChildren without Disability
MeanSDMeanSD
Children’s Age29 mths9.6 mths28.1 mths7.5 mths
Weight25.1 lbs4.0 lbs28.8 lbs5.2 lbs
Height33.9 in9.8 in34.9 in2.8 in
Table 2. CHARGE characteristics.
Table 2. CHARGE characteristics.
Visual17Ocular Colobomas14Loss of Visual Field0CVI
Visual Acuity (right)1520/199 or better620/199 to 599120/600 and up
Visual Acuity (left)1320/199 or better620/199 to 600220/600 and up
Hearing Loss (right)2Normal (−10–15)5Slight (16–25)3Mild (26–40)0Moderate (41–55)3Moderate Severe (56–70)1Severe (71–90)13Profound (90+)
Hearing Loss (left)2Normal (−10–15)2Slight (16–25)0Mild (26–40)1Moderate (41–55)2Moderate Severe (56–70)4Severe (71–90)17Profound (90+)
Other Characteristics20Heart defects11Atresia of Choanae19Growth restrictions 26Missing or Malformed Semicircular Canals
Table 3. Cross tabulation of children with and without CHARGE syndrome and achievement of motor milestones.
Table 3. Cross tabulation of children with and without CHARGE syndrome and achievement of motor milestones.
Motor MilestoneCHARGE (N = 28)Control (N = 32)X2p
Holding headYes27321.160.28
No10
Rolling overYes2832
No00
Sitting w/o supportYes25323.610.06
No30
CrawlingYes20306.250.01
No71
CreepingYes203210.550.001
No80
Standing with supportYes26312.290.13
No20
Standing w/o supportYes133122.27<0.001
No150
CruisingYes23314.930.03
No40
WalkingYes123124.31<0.001
No160
RunningYes72934.44<0.001
No210
JumpingYes52321.53<0.001
No236
HoppingYes11414.68<0.001
No2715
Table 4. Results of t-tests and means and standard deviations for age of motor milestone between children with and without CHARGE syndrome.
Table 4. Results of t-tests and means and standard deviations for age of motor milestone between children with and without CHARGE syndrome.
Motor Milestone CHARGE (N = 28) Controls (N = 32) tdfpd
Mean SD Mean SD
Holding head 8.68 5.01 2.32 1.42 5.29 20.81 <0.001 1.73
Rolling over 10.05 3.40 3.87 2.00 6.21 28.43 <0.001 2.22
Sitting w/o support 13.83 7.41 6.13 1.72 4.30 18.72 <0.001 1.43
Crawling 16.19 5.85 7.61 2.56 5.42 20.05 <0.001 1.90
Creeping 19.54 7.71 8.47 2.44 5.01 13.65 <0.001 1.94
Standing with support 19.32 8.47 8.58 2.51 5.30 21.13 <0.001 1.72
Standing w/o support 23.50 6.19 10.58 3.10 6.73 14.50 <0.001 2.64
Cruising 22.39 10.21 10.76 2.46 4.71 18.87 <0.001 1.57
Walking 26.27 6.44 12.42 2.34 6.88 11.55 <0.001 2.86
Table 5. Frequencies of affordance level for AHEMD scores for children with and without CHARGE syndrome.
Table 5. Frequencies of affordance level for AHEMD scores for children with and without CHARGE syndrome.
Variable CHARGE (N = 28) Controls (N = 32)
Very Low Low Good or
Average
High Very Low Low Good or Average High
Outside Space 2 (7.7%) 6 (23.1%) 4 (15.4%) 14 (53.8%) 0 2 (6.5%) 6 (19.4%) 23 (74.2%)
Inside Space 0 0 0 26 (100%) 0 0 0 31 (100%)
Variety of Stimulation 1 (3.8%) 4 (15.4%) 3 (11.5%) 18 (69.2%) 0 0 7 (22.6%) 24 (77.4%)
Fine Motor Toys 0 4 (15.4%) 14 (53.8%) 8 (30.8%) 0 1 (3.2%) 19 (61.3%) 11 (35.5%)
Gross Motor Toys 0 0 11 (42.3%) 15 (57.7%) 0 1 (3.2%) 7 (22.6%) 23 (74.2%)
Total AHEMD 0 10 (38.5%) 16 (61.5%) 0 5 (16.1%) 26 (83.9%)
Table 6. Comparison of AHEMD scores between children with and without CHARGE syndrome.
Table 6. Comparison of AHEMD scores between children with and without CHARGE syndrome.
CHARGE (N = 28) Controls (N = 32) zpr
Median Mrank Median Mrank
Outside Space 4 25.00 4 32.35 1.97 0.05 0.25
Inside Space 4 29.00 4 29.00 0.00 1.00 0
Variety of Stimulation 4 27.06 4 30.63 1.05 0.29 0.14
Fine Motor Toys 3 27.08 3 30.61 0.91 0.36 0.12
Gross Motor Toys 4 26.65 4 30.97 1.19 0.23 0.15
Total AHEMD 16 24.88 19 32.45 1.75 0.08 0.23
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Beach, P.; Perreault, M.; Lieberman, L. Affordances for Motor Development in the Home Environment for Young Children with and without CHARGE Syndrome. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11936. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182211936

AMA Style

Beach P, Perreault M, Lieberman L. Affordances for Motor Development in the Home Environment for Young Children with and without CHARGE Syndrome. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(22):11936. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182211936

Chicago/Turabian Style

Beach, Pamela, Melanie Perreault, and Lauren Lieberman. 2021. "Affordances for Motor Development in the Home Environment for Young Children with and without CHARGE Syndrome" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 22: 11936. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182211936

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop