Next Article in Journal
The Effect of Static and Dynamic Stretching during Warm-Up on Running Economy and Perception of Effort in Recreational Endurance Runners
Previous Article in Journal
Stress among Emergency Health Care Workers on Nuclear or Radiation Disaster: A Preliminary State Survey
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Is Experience the Best Teacher? Knowledge, Perceptions, and Awareness of Wildfire Risk

1
Department of Agricultural and Environmental Sciences, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Via Amendola 165/A, 70126 Bari, Italy
2
Department of Geography, The University of Hong Kong, Centennial Campus, Pokfulam Road, Hong Kong
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(16), 8385; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168385
Submission received: 23 June 2021 / Revised: 30 July 2021 / Accepted: 6 August 2021 / Published: 8 August 2021

Abstract

:
Wildfires represent a natural phenomenon with detrimental effects on natural resources and human health. A better knowledge, perception, and awareness of wildfire risk may help communities at risk of exposure to prevent future events and safeguard their own lives. The aim of this study is to explore differences between individuals with and without previous wildfire experience, in terms of (1) subjective and advanced wildfire knowledge, (2) self-reported perceptions, (3) level of information, (4) self-protection measures, and (5) importance of community involvement. As a second step, we investigated differences in the same variables, focusing more deeply on a group of individuals with previous wildfire experience, classifying them according to fire-related employment (fire-related workers vs. non-workers) and wildland–urban interface (WUI) proximity (WUI residents vs. non-WUI residents). The Kruskal–Wallis test was applied to establish differences between the pairs of subsamples. Our results partially confirmed our hypothesis, that direct experience leads individuals to have a greater preparedness on the topic of wildfires. Perception of knowledge is reflected only at a shallow level of expertise, and, therefore, no relevant within-group differences related to fire-related employment or to WUI proximity were detected. Moreover, available information was perceived to be insufficient, thus we report a strong need for developing effective communication to high-risk groups, such as homeowners and fire-related workers.

1. Introduction

The effects of climate change coupled with unsustainable anthropic pressure are increasing wildfire risk, making European landscapes more vulnerable to wildfires [1]. Currently, the data reported by the European Commission suggest that wildfires still represent a critical issue, especially for the Mediterranean countries of Southern Europe, such as Italy. Statistics reveal that between 2009 and 2018, Italy experienced a yearly average of more than 5500 wildfire events, covering a mean annual surface area of 67,000 ha [2]. These alarming numbers reflect wildfires’ detrimental effects on natural resources and human health [3]. In this context, national and regional institutions must integrate their efforts to improve prevention, by enhancing the preparedness of communities located in areas threatened by wildfires [4]. A better knowledge, perception, and awareness of wildfire risk may help people and communities that have experienced wildfires to prevent future events and thus safeguard their own lives.
Indeed, numerous studies have suggested that past experience of wildfires in experts, homeowners, and individuals in communities at high risk of wildfire is reflected in a greater subjective knowledge related to wildfires (e.g., risk perception, management, reduction behavior) [5,6,7,8,9]. In general, subjective knowledge related to wildfire, which can be defined as what individuals believe to know [10], has important implications for the difference in perception of risk severity, and thus in the response to that risk [8]. For instance, the behavior of homeowners has shown to be predicted by a combination of attitudes and cognitive factors (e.g., perceived behavioral control and beliefs on self-efficacy) affected by past experience with wildfires [6,7,8,9,11]. Although the relationship between past experience and subjective knowledge related to wildfire is established, mixed results are available on the relation between past experience and behavior, e.g., risk mitigation measures [12]. While some evidence states that direct experience of wildfire leads to a greater preparedness on mitigation measures [13], especially when emotional components have a crucial role, e.g., in case of property loss or evacuation [14,15], others did not confirm this relationship [11,16]. Bihari and Ryan [17] pointed out that past experience with wildfires predicts social capital and involvement in high-risk communities, which in turn affect wildfire awareness and mitigation actions.
Similarly, individuals with professional activities related to forests and fires (i.e., professionals who work directly with or manage forest areas, such as a lumberjack, forest firefighter, or forester) have shown to be more qualified in risk knowledge and perception than others, perhaps due to their prior experience [18]. However, this high-risk group has been far less investigated than wildland–urban interface (WUI) communities.
In the face of such fragmented and inconsistent findings, it is therefore difficult to disentangle the role of direct experience in wildfire risk knowledge, perception, and awareness in individuals, whether they are homeowners, fire-related workers, or laypersons.
The overarching aim of this study is to uncover the role of direct wildfire experience in specific groups. To do this, we explored differences between individuals with and without previous wildfire experience, in terms of: (I) subjective and advanced wildfire knowledge, (II) self-reported perceptions, (III) level of information, (IV) self-protection measures, and (V) the importance of community involvement. Secondly, we investigated differences in the same variables with a greater focus on the group of individuals with previous wildfire experience, classifying them according to:
  • Fire-related employment: fire-related workers vs. non-fire-related workers; and
  • Wildland–urban interface (WUI) proximity: WUI residents vs. non-WUI residents.
According to the literature, we hypothesized that direct experience leads individuals to have a greater preparedness on the topic of wildfires, and that therefore no within-group differences regarding fire-related employment or WUI proximity are expected.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design and study Population

This study is based on a cross-sectional design, where a single survey questionnaire was administered throughout Italy. The questionnaire was completed by 775 participants (age: mean = 37.4; standard deviation = 14.6; gender: female = 393; male = 382). The questionnaire was disseminated through various channels, including social networks and personal and professional contacts, according to a recruitment technique called “snowball sampling” or “chain-referral sampling” [19], where participants are invited to recruit other potential participants. Inclusion criteria were: (a) age ≥18 years and (b) residency in Italy. To verify that the interviewees were currently residents in Italy, they were asked to provide the municipality of residence. Geographic distribution of the sample was not homogenous; however, all the Italian regions were represented. Most of the questionnaires were received from Apulia, (southern) Sardinia, Tuscany, (central) Lazio, and Lombardy (Northern Italy).

2.2. Item Selection and Development

No validated questionnaire on the variables of interest is currently available in the literature. Therefore, for the item selection and development of our questionnaire we adapted previously used items for investigating the perception of wildfire risk in WUI residents in Algeria [20], Portugal [18], and the United States [6]. After merging the three aforementioned questionnaires, an adapted questionnaire resulted (see Table S1 in Supplementary Materials for the English version), suitable for the administration to a wider pool of users, including the general population, fire-related workers, and WUI residents. The adapted questionnaire consisted of six sections:
  • Sociodemographic variables and other general information. Information on age, gender, education, occupation, and city of residence was collected. Participation in voluntary organizations or associations connected with firefighting activities was also registered, since these participants were classified as “fire-related workers”. In this section, we also asked participants to declare if they ever had a direct wildfire experience.
  • Subjective knowledge. Variables in this section were intended to explore objective and perceived knowledge about fires, such as wildfire and WUI definitions, their occurrence, and self-perceived topic knowledge.
  • Advanced knowledge. The four items in this section more specifically investigated those aspects related to wildfire drivers, the potential risk of an increasing number of wildfires, levels of flammability, and the role of climate change. In this section, the questions were also dichotomous.
  • Awareness of level of information. This section explored personal beliefs about mitigation measures adopted by landscape and forest managers, and about the tendency to retrieve information on the topic. The first variable was classified as “yes” and “no”, while the second was based on a 5-point Likert scale classification, ranging from “Never” (1) to “Always” (5).
  • Self-protection measures. This section refers to the response strategies that the participant would implement in the event of a wildfire. The two variables explored were about the response to the threat categorized as “tackling” or “avoiding the threat” (i.e., “fight or flight” response), and about the willingness to equip one’s home with fire protection systems, such as fire extinguishers and smoke detectors (“yes” and “no”).
  • Community involvement. In this last section, we investigated the perception of the importance of scientific research, to improve the awareness of wildfire risk in the general population using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “Not at all” (1) to “Very much” (5), and of an informed community to prevent and mitigate wildfire risk (“yes” and “no”).
Internal consistency of the total scale, as a measure of reliability, showed to be acceptable (ω = 0.7; see Dunn et al. [21] for more details).

2.3. Procedure

The survey was carried out in a single phase, in the period between 22 December 2020 and 22 January 2021. The amount of time required to complete the questionnaire was approximately 5 to 10 min. To administer the questionnaire, the Google Forms online platform was employed; the platform easily reaches the highest number of potential participants via several devices (e.g., tablet, smart-phone, and laptop). Moreover, COVID-19-related restrictive measures prevented face-to-face interviews.
The study procedure was designed in accordance with the ethical standards of the Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards [22]. Potential participants were informed that participation was on a voluntary basis, the questionnaire was anonymous, and that data would be processed in an aggregate manner, in compliance with national and European data protection laws for scientific and statistical purposes (GDPR 2016/679). By choosing the option “I give my consent” all participants gave their consent to voluntarily participate in the survey and were aware that they could interrupt participation at any stage and that anonymity was guaranteed.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

The general survey sample was grouped into two subsamples: (a) participants with direct wildfire experience and (b) participants without direct wildfire experience. Subsequently, the subsample of participants with direct experience was further classified as follows: (c) fire-related workers vs. (d) non-fire-related workers; and (e) WUI residents vs. (f) non-WUI residents. WUI residents detected in the entire sample were 36 individuals. We paired the participants of the last two subgroups—(e) and (f)—using the propensity score matching analysis (PSM). This statistical technique allows us to balance the distributions of potentially confounding covariates, such as age, gender, and education. PSM is used to reduce selection bias by comparing groups based on covariates, and matches each participant of a sample on certain characteristics, especially in the case of statistical analyses that cannot control for such variables. [23]. Thirty-six non-WUI residents who matched for age, gender, and education with the 36 WUI residents were extracted from the total sample. In doing so, we obtained a new subsample of 36 non-WUI residents, comparable to the subsample of 36 WUI residents. PSM was performed with the package MathIt of the R Software [24]. Descriptive statistics were calculated on the six subsamples for sociodemographic variables.
To establish differences in the responses between the pairs of subsamples, the Kruskal–Wallis test [25] was applied. Kruskal–Wallis is a nonparametric test which is used as an alternative to the analysis of variance (ANOVA) when heteroscedastic and non-normally distributed data are given [26]. The use of a nonparametric test is justified by the fact that the variables examined did not have a normal distribution, and the observations were represented by ordinal classifications. Furthermore, in the case of subsamples of WUI residents and non-WUI-residents, the sample size was too small to be able to understand if there was a normal distribution [25]. The test was performed using the statistics package of the R software.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

The sociodemographic characteristics of each survey subsample are displayed in Table 1. Age, gender, and education are satisfactorily balanced in each pair of subsamples. The subsample of participants with fire experience was further grouped according to the fire-related employment and WUI proximity. The descriptive statistics presented for WUI residents and non-residents refer to the matched groups after PSM analysis with the nearest-neighbor matching method, and with a ratio of one to one. Using this statistical method, we extracted from the total sample 36 non-WUI residents with a comparable age, gender, and education to the 36 WUI residents.

3.2. Fire Experience

As shown in Table 2, significant differences were found in the scores of the four variables related to the topic “Subjective knowledge”, namely Wildfire_definition (x2 = 152.7, p < 0.001), Self-perceived_knowledge (x2 = 17.2, p < 0.001), WUI_definition (x2 = 189.2, p < 0.001), and Wildfire_occurrence (x2 = 23.4, p < 0.001). Moreover, a significant difference in Selflearning (x2 = 0.3, p < 0.001) was observed. The test results suggest that all the scores of the previously mentioned variables were higher in participants with fire experience than those without fire experience.

3.3. Fire-Related Employment

Significant differences between the subsamples of non-fire-related and fire-related workers among participants with fire experience confirmed previous findings (Table 3). “Subjective knowledge” and “Level of information” are the significant topics that yielded higher scores for fire-related than non-fire-related workers. Within them, significant differences were found for Wildfire_definition (x2 = 6.2, p < 0.05), Self-perceived_knowledge (x2 = 73.3, p < 0.001), WUI_definition (x2 = 18.1, p < 0.001), Wildfire_occurrence (x2 = 34.8, p < 0.001), and Selflearning (x2 = 104.2, p < 0.001).

3.4. WUI Proximity

As a result of the Kruskal–Wallis test, three variables presented significant differences in response scores between the matched groups (Table 4). Self-perceived_knowledge within the topic “Subjective knowledge” (x2 = 73.3, p < 0.05) and Selflearning within the topic “Level of information” (x2 = 104.2, p < 0.001) were found to be significantly different with higher scores for WUI residents than the matched group of non-residents. On the contrary, Response_threat_categ (x2 = 0.5, p < 0.05) showed significantly higher scores in non-WUI residents than in the WUI residents group.

4. Discussion

The current study investigated the differences in attitudes towards wildfires among individuals who have or have not been exposed to a wildfire. Our hypothesis was that direct wildfire experience significantly increases the perceived and actual knowledge about wildfires, their causes, mitigation measures, and need for the involvement of the social and scientific communities. Our results partially confirmed our assumptions. Individuals who have been directly exposed to a wildfire have a significantly higher level of subjective knowledge of the topic themes, e.g., definitions of “wildfire” and “wildland–urban interface”. Knowledge is also reflected by the degree of self-evaluation of the topic, which is significantly higher in the group of individuals with direct experience of a wildfire, as recent evidence supports. On the contrary, our results revealed no difference between the two groups on advanced knowledge, risk, and drivers/causes of wildfires [18].
However, we found that participants with a direct experience of wildfires requested more information on the topic than those who had never been exposed to such an event, confirming the fact that knowledge and experience influence the number of sources from which individuals usually seek information [27]. This result was confirmed in the subsamples of individuals with direct experience who have a fire-related employment, including firefighters, members of local associations, volunteers, and forest scientists. It could be inferred that these subgroups might be at risk for wildfire consequences, concerned about their own protection for work and personal reasons, and, consequently, more likely to gather information on the topic. This is in line with previous studies on both forest and wildfire specialists and WUI residents, pointing out the perceived need of further knowledge and evidence on wildfires [20,28].
Nevertheless, in our study, surprisingly no significant differences were detected among subgroups on the perception of importance of scientific research and community involvement in improving and spreading awareness of wildfire risk. In fact, previous results have revealed that forest experts consider public awareness, in particular, concerning the impact of human activities, as a crucial element for risk reduction and fire management, especially in view of the ensuing effects of climate change [29,30,31].
The perception of “mental preparedness” by experts, however, does not always translate into safe decisions [29,32]. Nevertheless, laypersons often place their trust in the knowledge and skills of experts when mitigating wildfire risk. For instance, a recent study [33] showed that not only experience with wildfires but also the perception of the effectiveness of mitigation measures shape both risk perception and the intention to implement those measures [11,34]. This is particularly true when considering homeowners, who may perceive some mitigation measures as a threat to their sense of connectedness to nature [35]. Our results showed that WUI residents, also referred to as homeowners, have a significantly higher perception of wildfire knowledge than non-WUI residents. However, this was not observed by comparing knowledge between our two subgroups. Mixed results on WUI residents’ preparedness are available in the literature, probably due to a lack of standards of measuring tools and appropriate statistical analyses [36]. Another possible explanation has been provided by Arvai et al. [37], and recently supported by Larsen et al. [38], highlighting the fact that in the phase immediately following a near-miss wildfire event there is an increase in perceived risk. At a later time, the perceived risk level naturally decreases as the individual begins to think of the event as something unlikely, and, therefore, unlikely to happen again [38]. We hypothesize that this interesting theory is a plausible explanation for the lack of difference we found in the awareness about protective measures between WUI residents and non-WUI residents. This finding is also consistent with a result in our study regarding self-protection measures. In support of previous evidence on residents’ willingness to stay and defend their properties in Victoria, Australia [39], we found that our subsample of non-residents was more willing to avoid or flee the threat of a fire, unlike WUI residents who claimed to counter it. In fact, WUI residents are almost entirely homeowners in the WUI area, with a strong place attachment to the surrounding natural environment and their communities [17,40], which makes it more difficult to leave land, homes, and other possessions to escape a fire. A possible explanation might be that WUI residents give fire protection responsibility to the government for educating residents and visitors about fire hazards and for managing public land for fire safety, whereas the responsibility is their own for fireproofing their property [9].
Lastly, an interesting result concerned the importance of an informed community and scientific research. In all subgroups, high scores were found for both dimensions as major proof of the correct perception of the usefulness of what we might define as environmental education [41].

5. Study Strengths and Limitations

We believe the present study is the first to have surveyed both WUI residents and fire workers, all with previous wildfire experience, throughout the Italian territory. The study, however, includes some limitations that warrant further discussion. First and foremost is the low sample number of WUI residents who have had at least one wildfire experience, and, consequently, after matching, the low number of paired non-residents. It is worth pointing out that the presence of restrictive measures resulting from the COVID-19 pandemic has made it impossible to collect data with paper and pencil tools in these areas [42]. However, considering that the WUI population in Italy is highly variable and heterogeneous [2,43], we were able to detect only a small percentage of participants recruited by word-of-mouth and social media sharing in our general sample. Another limitation is related to the use of web surveys for data collection. Despite their rapid spread as a low-cost tool for retrieving data on target populations that are difficult to reach, the reliability of web surveys depends on the level of digital education of respondents. In fact, our participant subsamples have a rather low average age, and, therefore, a predictable medium-high level of education. Such unbalanced stratification of the sample in favor of the most educated and youngest could leave aside important considerations on wildfire impact and management in at-risk communities, such as those with poor education, low income, and the elderly [44,45,46]. Future research aimed at evaluating the interplay between the characteristics of vulnerable groups and fire-related environmental variables, such as climate variability, forest fuel distribution, and topographic features is warranted [47,48]. Lastly, we acknowledge the need to use validated scales for assessing wildfire risk perception. Despite the presence of a number of scales investigating wildfire risk perception, no well-validated measurement tool is currently available. We presented an initial measure of reliability, but we hope in the future to carry out a validation and a psychometric evaluation of the proposed questionnaire.

6. Conclusions

Our study attempted to answer the question of whether the experience of a wildfire event is enough to acquire knowledge about wildfires. The answer is: not quite. Subjective knowledge related to wildfires is reflected only at a shallow level of expertise. In particular, contrary to our expectations, we found that the self-perceived level of knowledge of individuals with direct experience of wildfire does not translate into a real greater preparedness, except for superficial information on the subject (e.g., definitions of “wildfire” and “WUI”). Additionally, among individuals with past fire experience, not even the expected highly prepared groups (e.g., fire-related workers and WUI residents) seem to possess perceived advanced knowledge of wildfires, self-protection measures, or encouraging awareness on the importance of community involvement. However, the core of our results revealed that those groups of individuals declare to be actively engaged in seeking information on the topic, which may indicate a certain degree of self-awareness concerning the knowledge gap. It is therefore clear that the level of information of those who should provide it (e.g., the scientific community, responsible bodies) is perceived to be insufficient. Our conclusions corroborate previous studies [49,50], which highlighted the need for developing effective communication for high-risk groups, such as homeowners and fire-related workers, in order to effectively prepare them for threats and potential impacts of wildfires, and avoid the adverse health impacts of the exposure.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph18168385/s1, Table S1: survey questionnaire (English translation).

Author Contributions

G.S. (Giuseppina Spano): conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, investigation, writing and revising. M.E.: conceptualization, formal analysis, investigation, writing, and revising. O.C., V.G., G.C. and M.D.: data collection and revising. R.L.: supervising and revising. G.S. (Giovanni Sanesi): supervising, revising, and funding acquisition. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was carried out under the project “Establishing Urban FORest based solutions In Changing Cities (EUFORICC) and financially supported by the Ministry of Education, University and Research (MIUR) of Italy (PRIN 20173RRN2S).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The study did not report any data.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Yole DeBellis for support in revising the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Elia, M.; D’Este, M.; Ascoli, D.; Giannico, V.; Spano, G.; Ganga, A.; Colangelo, G.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G. Estimating the Probability of Wildfire Occurrence in Mediterranean Landscapes Using Artificial Neural Networks. Environ. Impact Assess. Rev. 2020, 85, 106474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. European Commission. Joint Research Centre. European Wildfire Danger and Vulnerability in a Changing Climate: Towards Integrating Risk Dimensions: JRC PESETA IV Project: Task 9 Forest Fires; Publications Office of the European Union: Luxembourg, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  3. Spano, G.; Giannico, V.; Elia, M.; Bosco, A.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G. Human Health–Environment Interaction Science: An Emerging Research Paradigm. Sci. Total Environ. 2020, 704, 135358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Elia, M.; Giannico, V.; Spano, G.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G.; Elia, M.; Giannico, V.; Spano, G.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G. Likelihood and Frequency of Recurrent Fire Ignitions in Highly Urbanised Mediterranean Landscapes. Int. J. Wildland Fire 2020, 29, 120–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Langer, E.R.; McGee, T.K.; Langer, E.R.; McGee, T.K. Wildfire Risk Awareness and Prevention by Predominantly Māori Rural Residents, Karikari Peninsula, Aotearoa New Zealand. Int. J. Wildland Fire 2017, 26, 820–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Martin, W.E.; Martin, I.M.; Kent, B. The Role of Risk Perceptions in the Risk Mitigation Process: The Case of Wildfire in High Risk Communities. J. Environ. Manag. 2009, 91, 489–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. McGee, T.K.; McFarlane, B.L.; Varghese, J. An Examination of the Influence of Hazard Experience on Wildfire Risk Perceptions and Adoption of Mitigation Measures. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2009, 22, 308–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Martin, I.M.; Bender, H.; Raish, C. What Motivates Individuals to Protect Themselves from Risks: The Case of Wildland Fires. Risk Anal. 2007, 27, 887–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Greg Winter, J.S.F. Homeowner Perspectives on Fire Hazard, Responsibility, and Management Strategies at the Wildland-Urban Interface. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2000, 13, 33–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lindell, M.K.; Perry, R.W. Household Adjustment to Earthquake Hazard: A Review of Research. Environ. Behav. 2000, 32, 461–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hall, T.E.; Slothower, M. Cognitive Factors Affecting Homeowners’ Reactions to Defensible Space in the Oregon Coast Range. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2009, 22, 95–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Brenkert-Smith, H.; Champ, P.A.; Flores, N. Trying Not to Get Burned: Understanding Homeowners’ Wildfire Risk–Mitigation Behaviors. Environ. Manag. 2012, 50, 1139–1151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. McCaffrey, S.M.; Stidham, M.; Toman, E.; Shindler, B. Outreach Programs, Peer Pressure, and Common Sense: What Motivates Homeowners to Mitigate Wildfire Risk? Environ. Manag. 2011, 48, 475–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Siegel, J.M.; Shoaf, K.I.; Afifi, A.A.; Bourque, L.B. Surviving Two Disasters: Does Reaction to the First Predict Response to the Second? Environ. Behav. 2003, 35, 637–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Nguyen, L.H.; Shen, H.; Ershoff, D.; Afifi, A.A.; Bourque, L.B. Exploring the Causal Relationship between Exposure to the 1994 Northridge Earthquake and Pre- and Post-Earthquake Preparedness Activities. Earthq. Spectra 2006, 22, 569–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Schulte, S.; Miller, K.A. Wildfire Risk and Climate Change: The Influence on Homeowner Mitigation Behavior in the Wildland–Urban Interface. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2010, 23, 417–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Bihari, M.; Ryan, R. Influence of Social Capital on Community Preparedness for Wildfires. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2012, 106, 253–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Oliveira, R.; Oliveira, S.; Zêzere, J.L.; Viegas, D.X. Uncovering the Perception Regarding Wildfires of Residents with Different Characteristics. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2020, 43, 101370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Mann, H.B.; Whitney, D.R. On a Test of Whether One of Two Random Variables Is Stochastically Larger than the Other. Ann. Math. Stat. 1947, 18, 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sahar, O.; Leone, V.; Limani, H.; Rabia, N.; Meddour, R. Wildfire Risk and Its Perception in Kabylia (Algeria). IForest Biogeosci. For. 2018, 11, 367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Dunn, T.J.; Baguley, T.; Brunsden, V. From Alpha to Omega: A Practical Solution to the Pervasive Problem of Internal Consistency Estimation. Br. J. Psychol. 2014, 105, 399–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. World Medical Association. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. JAMA 2013, 310, 2191–2194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Harder, V.S.; Stuart, E.A.; Anthony, J.C. Propensity Score Techniques and the Assessment of Measured Covariate Balance to Test Causal Associations in Psychological Research. Psychol. Methods 2010, 15, 234–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Stuart, E.A.; King, G.; Imai, K.; Ho, D. MatchIt: Nonparametric Preprocessing for Parametric Causal Inference. J. Stat. Softw. 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Kruskal, W.H.; Wallis, W.A. Use of Ranks in One-Criterion Variance Analysis. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1952, 47, 583–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Elia, M.; Lafortezza, R.; Lovreglio, R.; Sanesi, G. Developing Custom Fire Behavior Fuel Models for Mediterranean Wildland–Urban Interfaces in Southern Italy. Environ. Manag. 2015, 56, 754–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Velez, A.-L.K.; Diaz, J.M.; Wall, T.U.; Velez, A.-L.K.; Diaz, J.M.; Wall, T.U. Public Information Seeking, Place-Based Risk Messaging and Wildfire Preparedness in Southern California. Int. J. Wildland Fire 2017, 26, 469–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Roberts, R.M.; Jones, K.W.; Duke, E.; Shinbrot, X.; Harper, E.E.; Fons, E.; Cheng, A.S.; Wolk, B.H. Stakeholder Perceptions and Scientific Evidence Linking Wildfire Mitigation Treatments to Societal Outcomes. J. Environ. Manag. 2019, 248, 109286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Raftoyannis, Y.; Nocentini, S.; Marchi, E.; Calama Sainz, R.; Garcia Guemes, C.; Pilas, I.; Peric, S.; Amaral Paulo, J.; Moreira-Marcelino, A.C.; Costa-Ferreira, M.; et al. Perceptions of Forest Experts on Climate Change and Fire Management in European Mediterranean Forests. IForest Biogeosci. For. 2014, 7, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Elia, M.; Giannico, V.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G. Modeling Fire Ignition Patterns in Mediterranean Urban Interfaces. Stoch. Environ. Res. Risk Assess. 2019, 33, 169–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. D’Este, M.; Ganga, A.; Elia, M.; Lovreglio, R.; Giannico, V.; Spano, G.; Colangelo, G.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G. Modeling Fire Ignition Probability and Frequency Using Hurdle Models: A Cross-Regional Study in Southern Europe. Ecol. Process. 2020, 9, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Eriksen, C.; Prior, T. Defining the Importance of Mental Preparedness for Risk Communication and Residents Well-Prepared for Wildfire. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2013, 6, 87–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Ghasemi, B.; Kyle, G.T.; Absher, J.D. An Examination of the Social-Psychological Drivers of Homeowner Wildfire Mitigation. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 70, 101442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Nelson, K.C.; Monroe, M.C.; Johnson, J.F.; Bowers, A.; Nelson, K.C.; Monroe, M.C.; Johnson, J.F.; Bowers, A. Living with Fire: Homeowner Assessment of Landscape Values and Defensible Space in Minnesota and Florida, USA. Int. J. Wildland Fire 2004, 13, 413–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. McFarlane, B.L.; McGee, T.K.; Faulkner, H.; McFarlane, B.L.; McGee, T.K.; Faulkner, H. Complexity of Homeowner Wildfire Risk Mitigation: An Integration of Hazard Theories. Int. J. Wildland Fire 2011, 20, 921–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Walpole, H.; McCaffrey, S.; Rapp, C.; Wilson, R.; Walpole, H.; McCaffrey, S.; Rapp, C.; Wilson, R. Operationalising Homeowner Wildfire Risk Mitigation in Fire-Prone Areas. Int. J. Wildland Fire 2020, 30, 161–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Arvai, J.; Gregory, R.; Ohlson, D.; Blackwell, B.; Gray, R. Letdowns, Wake-Up Calls, and Constructed Preferences: People’s Responses to Fuel and Wildfire Risks. J. For. 2006, 104, 173–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Larsen, L.N.D.; Howe, P.D.; Brunson, M.; Yocom, L.; McAvoy, D.; Helen Berry, E.; Smith, J.W. Risk Perceptions and Mitigation Behaviors of Residents Following a Near-Miss Wildfire. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2021, 207, 104005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. McLennan, J.; Elliott, G.; Omodei, M.; Whittaker, J. Householders’ Safety-Related Decisions, Plans, Actions and Outcomes during the 7 February 2009 Victorian (Australia) Wildfires. Fire Saf. J. 2013, 61, 175–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Spano, G.; D’Este, M.; Giannico, V.; Carrus, G.; Elia, M.; Lafortezza, R.; Panno, A.; Sanesi, G. Are Community Gardening and Horticultural Interventions Beneficial for Psychosocial Well-Being? A Meta-Analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Ryan, R.L. The Influence of Landscape Preference and Environmental Education on Public Attitudes toward Wildfire Management in the Northeast Pine Barrens (USA). Landsc. Urban Plan. 2012, 107, 55–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Spano, G.; D’Este, M.; Giannico, V.; Elia, M.; Cassibba, R.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G. Association between Indoor-Outdoor Green Features and Psychological Health during the COVID-19 Lockdown in Italy: A Cross-Sectional Nationwide Study. Urban For. Urban Green. 2021, 62, 127156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mancini, L.D.; Elia, M.; Barbati, A.; Salvati, L.; Corona, P.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G. Are Wildfires Knocking on the Built-Up Areas Door? Forests 2018, 9, 234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Giannico, V.; Spano, G.; Elia, M.; D’Este, M.; Sanesi, G.; Lafortezza, R. Green Spaces, Quality of Life, and Citizen Perception in European Cities. Environ. Res. 2021, 196, 110922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Masri, S.; Scaduto, E.; Jin, Y.; Wu, J. Disproportionate Impacts of Wildfires among Elderly and Low-Income Communities in California from 2000–2020. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Spano, G.; Caffò, A.O.; Bosco, A. Cognitive Functioning, Subjective Memory Complaints and Risky Behaviour Predict Minor Home Injuries in Elderly. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2018, 30, 985–991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Ascoli, D.; Vacchiano, G.; Scarpa, C.; Arca, B.; Barbati, A.; Battipaglia, G.; Elia, M.; Esposito, A.; Garfì, V.; Lovreglio, R.; et al. Harmonized Dataset of Surface Fuels under Alpine, Temperate and Mediterranean Conditions in Italy. A Synthesis Supporting Fire Management. IForest Biogeosci. For. 2020, 13, 513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. D’Este, M.; Elia, M.; Giannico, V.; Spano, G.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G. Machine Learning Techniques for Fine Dead Fuel Load Estimation Using Multi-Source Remote Sensing Data. Remote Sens. 2021, 13, 1658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Belleville, G.; Ouellet, M.-C.; Morin, C.M. Post-Traumatic Stress among Evacuees from the 2016 Fort McMurray Wildfires: Exploration of Psychological and Sleep Symptoms Three Months after the Evacuation. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Powell, T.; Wegmann, K.M.; Backode, E. Coping and Post-Traumatic Stress in Children and Adolescents after an Acute Onset Disaster: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 4865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the six survey subsamples.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the six survey subsamples.
Fire ExperienceWith Fire Experience
Fire-Related EmploymentWUI Proximity
WithWithoutWorkersNon-WorkersResidentsNon-Residents
Frequency (N)2605151101503636
Age (M ± DS)(38.1 ± 15.3)(37 ± 14.3)(37.3 ± 14.7)(38.6 ± 15.7)(38.7 ± 15)(39.1 ± 15.4)
Gender [N (%)]
Male133 (17)249 (32.1)56 (22)77 (29.6)13 (18)13 (18.1)
Female127 (16)266 (34.3)54 (21)73 (28.1)23 (32)23 (31.9)
Education [N (%)]
No education000000
Primary school02 (0.3)0000
Secondary school25 (3.2)27 (3.5)8 (3.1)17 (6.5)6 (8.3)6 (8.3)
High School122 (16)226 (29.2)57 (22)65 (25)17 (24)17 (23.6)
University or higher113 (15)260 (33.5)45 (17)68 (26.2)13 (18)13 (18.1)
Occupation [N (%)]
Unemployed15 (1.9)23 (3)8 (3.1)7 (2.7)1 (1.4)2 (2.78)
Student64 (8.3)137 (17.7)33 (13)31 (11.9)3 (4.2)9 (12.5)
Tradesman5 (0.6)4 (0.5)2 (0.8)3 (1.2)3 (4.2)0
Employee97 (13)214 (27.6)30 (12)67 (25.8)15 (21)15 (20.8)
Freelancer29 (3.7)62 (8)14 (5.4)15 (5.8)6 (8.3)3 (4.1)
Retired15 (1.9)16 (2.1)5 (1.9)10 (3.8)1 (1.4)3 (4.1)
Other35 (4.5)59 (7.6)18 (6.9)17 (6.5)7 (9.7)4 (5.5)
N = frequency; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; WUI = wildland–urban interface.
Table 2. Kruskal-Wallis test for differences in the responses between participants with and without direct wildfire experience.
Table 2. Kruskal-Wallis test for differences in the responses between participants with and without direct wildfire experience.
TopicItemFire Experiencex2p Value
Without (N)With (N)Without (M)With (M)Without (SD)With (SD)
Subjective knowledgeWildfire_definition (Q12)5152600.8410.9380.3660.241152.6670.000***
Self-perceived_knowledge (Q13)5152600.3771.1040.5380.74117.2380.000***
WUI_definition (Q14)5152600.5730.7150.4950.452189.1940.000***
Wildfire_occurrence (Q15)5152600.8620.9730.6810.50023.4360.000***
Advanced knowledgeDrivers_WUI (Q16)5152600.1500.1350.3570.34272.0020.578
Flammability_levels (Q17)5152600.6120.6620.4880.4740.1330.175
Climate_change (Q18)5152600.9400.9460.2380.2262.0290.721
Risk_worsening (Q19)5152600.8390.9120.3680.2850.0060.005
Level of informationSite_specific_mit (Q20)5152600.3860.3690.4870.4842.8970.642
Selflearning (Q21)5152602.1203.5851.0611.3880.2620.000***
Self-protection measuresResponse_threat_categ (Q22)5152600.7280.6960.4450.461195.8010.350
Home_protection (Q23)5152600.9510.9350.2150.2482.8410.329
Community involvementScientific_research (Q24)5152604.2004.2580.9400.9340.3120.361
Informed_community (Q25)5152600.9610.9650.1930.1830.5290.770
Q = question; N = frequency; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; WUI = wildland–urban interface; x2 = Kruskal–Wallis chi-square; *** p < 0.001. Red and green shadings indicate the lower and the higher mean, respectively.
Table 3. Kruskal–Wallis test for differences in responses between the subsamples of non-fire-related and fire-related workers among participants with direct wildfire experience.
Table 3. Kruskal–Wallis test for differences in responses between the subsamples of non-fire-related and fire-related workers among participants with direct wildfire experience.
TopicItemFire-Related Employmentx2p Value
Non-Workers (N)Workers (N)Non-Workers (M)Workers (M)Non-Workers (SD)Workers (SD)
Subjective knowledgeWildfire_definition (Q12)1501100.9070.9820.2920.1346.1820.013*
Self-perceived_knowledge (Q13)1501100.7671.5640.6890.53473.3360.000***
WUI_definition (Q14)1501100.6130.8550.4890.35418.0650.000***
Wildfire_occurrence (Q15)1501100.9600.9900.1960.09534.8490.000***
Advanced knowledgeDrivers_WUI (Q16)1501100.1400.1270.3480.3350.0880.767
Flammability_levels (Q17)1501100.6400.6910.4820.4640.7320.392
Climate_change (Q18)1501100.9400.9550.2380.2090.2630.608
Risk_worsening (Q19)1501100.9270.8910.2620.3131.0020.317
Level of informationSite_specific_mit (Q20)1501100.3470.4000.4780.4920.7720.380
Selflearning (Q21)1501102.8534.5821.2820.771104.2210.000***
Self-protection measuresResponse_threat_categ (Q22)1501100.7130.6730.4540.4710.4930.483
Home_protection (Q23)1501100.9400.9270.2380.2610.1680.682
Community involvementScientific_research (Q24)1501104.2334.2910.9580.9020.0880.767
Informed_community (Q25)1501100.9600.9730.1970.1640.3060.580
Q = question; N = frequency; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; WUI = wildland–urban interface; x2 = Kruskal–Wallis chi-square; * p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001. Red and green shadings indicate the lower and the higher mean, respectively.
Table 4. Kruskal–Wallis test for differences in responses between the subsamples of non-WUI and WUI residents among participants with direct wildfire experience.
Table 4. Kruskal–Wallis test for differences in responses between the subsamples of non-WUI and WUI residents among participants with direct wildfire experience.
TopicItemWUI Proximityx2p Value
Non-Resident (N)Resident (N)Non-Resident (M)Resident (M)Non-Resident (SD)Resident (SD)
Subjective knowledgeWildfire_definition (Q12)36360.8890.9720.3190.1676.1820.167
Self-perceived_knowledge (Q13)36360.9171.3610.8060.59373.3360.015*
WUI_definition (Q14)36360.6940.8330.4670.37818.0650.168
Wildfire_occurrence (Q15)36360.9720.9440.1660.23234.8490.336
Advanced knowledgeDrivers_WUI (Q16)36360.0830.1110.2800.3190.0880.693
Flammability_levels (Q17)36360.6940.6110.4670.4940.7320.461
Climate_change (Q18)36360.9171.0000.2800.0000.2630.079
Risk_worsening (Q19)36360.9440.9440.2320.2321.0021.000
Level of informationSite_specific_mit (Q20)36360.3330.3610.4780.4870.7720.806
Selflearning (Q21)36363.1114.1671.3891.134104.220.001**
Self-protection measuresResponse_threat_categ (Q22)36360.6390.3890.4870.4940.4930.035*
Home_protection (Q23)36360.8610.9170.3510.2800.1680.456
Community involvementScientific_research (Q24)36364.1114.3891.0080.8030.0880.261
Informed_community (Q25)36361.0001.0000.0000.0000.306NA
Q = question; N = frequency; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; WUI = wildland–urban interface; NA = not applicable; x2 = Kruskal–Wallis chi-square; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01. Red and green shadings indicate the lower and the higher mean, respectively.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Spano, G.; Elia, M.; Cappelluti, O.; Colangelo, G.; Giannico, V.; D’Este, M.; Lafortezza, R.; Sanesi, G. Is Experience the Best Teacher? Knowledge, Perceptions, and Awareness of Wildfire Risk. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 8385. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168385

AMA Style

Spano G, Elia M, Cappelluti O, Colangelo G, Giannico V, D’Este M, Lafortezza R, Sanesi G. Is Experience the Best Teacher? Knowledge, Perceptions, and Awareness of Wildfire Risk. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(16):8385. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168385

Chicago/Turabian Style

Spano, Giuseppina, Mario Elia, Onofrio Cappelluti, Giuseppe Colangelo, Vincenzo Giannico, Marina D’Este, Raffaele Lafortezza, and Giovanni Sanesi. 2021. "Is Experience the Best Teacher? Knowledge, Perceptions, and Awareness of Wildfire Risk" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 16: 8385. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18168385

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop