Next Article in Journal
Knowledge on Pre-Hospital Emergency Management of Tooth Avulsion among Croatian Students of the Faculty of Education
Next Article in Special Issue
Effects of Incubation of Human Brain Microvascular Endothelial Cells and Astrocytes with Pyridostigmine Bromide, DEET, or Permethrin in the Absence or Presence of Metal Salts
Previous Article in Journal
Analyzing Workers’ Compensation Claims and Payments Made Using Data from a Large Insurance Provider
Previous Article in Special Issue
Radiation Exposure Predicts Reported Vaccine Adverse Effects in Veterans with Gulf War Illness
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Prevalence of Mild Cognitive Impairment in a Convenience Sample of 202 Gulf War Veterans

1
Department of Radiology and Biomedical Imaging, University of California, San Francisco, CA 94143, USA
2
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, University of California, San Francisco, CA 94143, USA
3
San Francisco Veterans Affairs Health Care System, San Francisco, CA 94121, USA
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(19), 7158; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17197158
Submission received: 31 July 2020 / Revised: 1 September 2020 / Accepted: 10 September 2020 / Published: 30 September 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Gulf War Illness, A Drug and Environmentally-Triggered Condition)

Abstract

:
Gulf War Illness (GWI) is a chronic, multisymptom disorder estimated to affect approximately 25–32% of Gulf War veterans (GWVs). Cognitive dysfunction is a common symptom of GWI. On the continuum of cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is conceptualized as a transitional phase between normal aging and dementia. Individuals with MCI exhibit cognitive decline but have relatively spared activities of daily function and do not meet criteria for dementia. The current study sought to investigate the prevalence of MCI in a convenience sample of 202 GWVs (median age: 52 years; 18% female). Twelve percent of the sample (median age: 48 years) had MCI according to an actuarial neuropsychological criterion, a rate materially higher than expected for this age group. GWVs with MCI also had a smaller hippocampal volume and a thinner parietal cortex, higher rates of current posttraumatic stress disorder and major depressive disorder compared to GWVs without MCI. Because people with MCI are more likely to progress to dementia compared to those with normal cognition, these results may portend future higher rates of dementia among deployed GWVs.

1. Introduction

An estimated 250,000 Gulf War veterans (GWVs) are afflicted by a chronic health condition known as Gulf War Illness (GWI) or Chronic Multi-symptom Illness (CMI) [1,2]. GWI/CMI is associated with several concomitant symptoms including persistent fatigue, musculoskeletal pain, gastrointestinal and respiratory problems, skin rashes, and cognitive dysfunction [1]. Longitudinal studies suggest that most GWVs with GWI/CMI have not improved with time [3,4,5]; instead, many are getting worse [2]. A recent study suggests that GWVs have more chronic medical conditions and are developing these chronic medical conditions earlier than the general civilian population [6]. There have also been reports of a higher than expected incidence of cognitive impairment among GWVs [7,8,9,10]. One study found that at least half of the veterans from a population-based cohort of 1200 GWVs with and without GWI reported symptoms of cognitive dysfunction [8]. A recent meta-analysis concluded that Gulf War (GW) service is associated with impairment in three cognitive domains: attention and executive function, visuospatial ability, and learning and memory [9].
On the continuum of cognitive decline, impairment greater than that expected for one’s age but not severe enough to meet the criteria for dementia is classified as mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [11,12]. Individuals with MCI exhibit objective cognitive impairment, typically in one or two cognitive domains, but have relatively spared daily functioning abilities. Thus, they do not meet the criteria for dementia [13]. Research suggests that individuals with MCI are 3 to 5 times more likely to progress to any form of dementia compared to those with normal cognition [14,15,16,17], with an annual rate of progression of 12% in the general population and up to 20% in populations at higher risk [18].
Because memory is severely impaired in AD, historically MCI was characterized as an amnestic disorder [14]. The original criteria for MCI, proposed in 1999, include subjective memory complaint, objective memory impairment, preserved general cognitive function, intact activities of daily living, and not meeting criteria for dementia [14]. Subsequently, broader classification schemes of MCI were introduced that include non-amnestic forms of MCI and clinical subtypes that include single and multiple cognitive domains [19,20,21]. The conventional MCI criteria have remained essentially unchanged in the past 20 years, and some shortcomings with the original criteria have been noted. For example, subjective reports of memory decline can sometimes be more strongly related to emotional factors than to objective memory abilities [22]. In many large-scale research studies and clinical trials, the conventional MCI criteria have been operationalized to rely on impaired performance on a single measure of episodic memory (generally delayed memory for a one-paragraph story) to determine impaired memory functioning [23], which can be unreliable [24]. It has also been common for large-scale research studies and clinical trials to use performance on a screening measure of global cognition (e.g., the Mini Mental State Exam) as an indication of intact abilities in other cognitive domains, which can be insensitive for distinguishing MCI from cognitively normal individuals [25]. Reliance of the conventional MCI criteria on a clinician’s judgment of mild impairment based on a semi-structured clinical interview [23] can introduce variability across clinicians, sites, and timepoints. Finally, patients diagnosed with the conventional MCI criteria can sometimes revert back to normal cognitive status [26,27,28,29,30] and have heterogenous neuropsychological [31] and AD biomarker profiles [32].
In an attempt to make the MCI diagnosis more consistent and reliable, an actuarial method for diagnosing MCI was proposed in 2009 [33]. The actuarial method differs from the conventional method in the following ways: first, cognitive impairment is defined as performance that is one standard deviation (SD) below the age-adjusted norm on a neuropsychological test measure. In contrast, the conventional MCI criteria defines cognitive impairment as performance that is 1.5 SDs below the age-adjusted norm on a neuropsychological measure [14]. Second, the actuarial method requires two impaired scores within a cognitive domain because neurologically normal older adults can have high base rates of isolated low neuropsychological test scores [24]. In contrast, the conventional MCI criteria requires only one impaired score. Research suggests that patients classified as MCI by the actuarial neuropsychological criteria have stronger associations between cognitive function and hippocampal volume [34], higher levels of AD biomarkers (i.e., beta-amyloid, Aβ, total and hyperphosphorylated tau in cerebral spinal fluid, CSF), are more likely to possess the APOE ε4 allele, a genetic risk factor for AD, and more likely to convert to dementia than patients classified as MCI by the conventional criteria [35].
Most MCI studies have focused on individuals in their 70s [36]; however, it has been argued that MCI can be identified in adults before the age of 60 using neuropsychological assessments that do not have ceiling effects and cover multiple cognitive domains [37]. We previously reported that GWVs with subjective memory complaints have objective memory impairment [38]. Because this satisfies two of the original Petersen criteria for MCI [14], and because there is suggestive evidence that GWVs exhibit a higher than expected incidence of cognitive impairment [7,8,9,10] and may be experiencing accelerated aging [6], the current study sought to investigate whether GWVs exhibit a higher than expected prevalence of MCI. We also explored demographic, clinical, military, and deployment-related differences between GWVs with and without MCI.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Human Subjects

The study participants were 202 consecutive GWVs recruited from 2014 to 2018 at the San Francisco Veterans Affairs Health Care System (SFVAHCS). The parent study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of California, San Francisco (UCSF) and the SFVAHCS. All participants provided written informed consent prior to enrollment in the parent study. Because this study consisted of secondary analyses of pre-existing de-identified data, additional IRB approval was not required.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Clinical Interview Assessments

Participants in the parent study were clinically screened with the following instruments: the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Diagnosis (SCID) [39], the Life Stressor Checklist-Revised [40], the Clinician Administered Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) Scale (CAPS) [41], and the Ohio State University TBI Identification Method (OSU TBI-ID) Short Form [42]. See [43] for additional details about the clinical screening of this sample.

2.2.2. Kansas Military History and Health Questionnaire

The Kansas Gulf War Military History and Health Questionnaire [44] was used to assess Kansas Gulf War Illness (GWI) case status [44], the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Chronic Multisymptom Illness (CMI) case status [45], and GW deployment-related experiences. See [43] for additional details about classification of GWI and CMC cases in this sample.

2.3. Classification of MCI

The present study sought to examine the prevalence of MCI in deployed GWVs. Although we previously reported evidence that GWVs with subjective memory complaints have objective memory impairment [38], which meets two of the original Petersen criteria for MCI [14], we could not use the conventional MCI criteria to classify GWVs because the parent study did not access activities of daily living or the clinical presence/absence of dementia. However, all participants in the parent study underwent neuropsychological assessment. Therefore, we used the actuarial neuropsychological criteria [33,46] to determine MCI status in the GWVs. The neuropsychological measures used to classify MCI are listed in Table 1.
In the original publication by Jak et al. describing the actuarial neuropsychological criteria [33], three neuropsychological measures in five cognitive domains were used to classify MCI. A subject was classified as MCI if s/he scored ≥1 SD below the age-adjusted norm on two measures in a cognitive domain (i.e., a subject with ≥2 scores ≥1 SD below the age-adjusted norm in the memory domain was classified as an amnestic MCI; a subject with ≥2 scores ≥1 SD below the age-adjusted norm in the executive function domain was classified as an executive function MCI). In a subsequent publication, Bondi et al. [35] used the actuarial neuropsychological criteria to classify subjects from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Because ADNI did not have as extensive a neuropsychological battery as that used in the original study by Jak et al. [33], the authors adapted the actuarial criteria to consider two neuropsychological measures in three cognitive domains for classifying MCI [35]. An ADNI subject was classified as MCI if s/he scored ≥1 SD below the age-adjusted norm on both measures in a cognitive domain (i.e., memory, executive function, or attention) or if s/he scored ≥1 SD below the age-adjusted norm on ≥1 measure in each of the three cognitive domains (i.e., a subject with one score 1 SD below the age-adjusted norm in all three domains was classified as MCI).
In this study, we used the same five cognitive domains describe by Jak et al. [33]. However, because we did not have three separate neuropsychological measures for the visuospatial and language domains, we employed a hybrid of the actuarial criteria described by Jak et al. [33] and Bondi et al. [35]: we considered two neuropsychological measures in three cognitive domains (i.e., episodic memory, executive function, and attention) and one neuropsychological measure in the other two cognitive domains (i.e., language and visuospatial function). A GWV was classified as MCI if s/he scored ≥1 SD below the age-adjusted norm on both measures in the episodic memory, executive function, or attention domain. A GWV was also classified as MCI if s/he scored ≥1 SD below the age-adjusted norm on ≥4 cognitive domains. A GWV was classified as having “intermediate” cognitive impairment if s/he scored ≥1 SD below the age-adjusted norm on ≤3 cognitive domains. GWVs with no score below the age-adjusted norm in any cognitive domain were classified as “cognitively normal” (CN). See Table 2.

2.4. Brain MRI

GWVs were scanned at the SFVSHCS on a 3 Tesla (T) Siemens Skyra MRI system equipped with a 32-channel receiver head coil. The MRI scan protocol included the following: T1-weighted 3D whole brain gradient echo MRI TR/TE/TI = 2500/2.98/1100 ms, 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm3 resolution and T2-weighted turbo spin echo MRI TR/TE 3200/11 ms, 0.9 × 0.9 × 3.0 mm3 resolution. The T2-weighted image was used to estimate intracranial volume (ICV). One hundred and ninety-four GWVs in the sample had artifact-free MRI data, from which we derived hippocampal volumes and measures of cortical thickness.

2.4.1. Image Processing

Freesurfer Version 5.1 was used to label cortical and subcortical tissue classes and derive quantitative estimates of cortical thickness and regional brain volume [52,53,54]. To protect against type I error, and because we had no a priori hypotheses about laterality, volumes of the right and left hippocampus, and cortical thicknesses of the lobes of the brain and the insula were combined across the hemispheres to reduce the number of measurements. To further reduce the number of measures, we averaged across the Freesurfer parcels to examine cortical thickness of the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes of the brain and the insula, as described in [55].

2.4.2. Intracranial Volume (ICV) Measurement

After checking that all extracranial and skull structures were removed and all intracranial structures were fully preserved in the skull stripped images, the BET program (FMRIB Image Analysis Group, Oxford University, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) was used to determine intracranial volume (ICV) from the T2-weighted scan.

2.5. Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses of the demographic, clinical, neuropsychological, and volumetric measures were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 26 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA). Demographic and descriptive characteristics were compared across three cognitive groups with analysis of variance (ANOVA) for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables. Because we previously reported significant effects of predicted exposure to the Khamisiyah plume on hippocampal and total cortical gray matter volumes [56,57,58], Khamisiyah exposure status was included as a covariate in analyses of the hippocampal volume and measures of cortical thickness. We also accounted for apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype in analyses of hippocampal volume and cortical thickness. This is because the APOE ε4 allele is a genetic risk factor for sporadic AD [59,60,61], while the APOE ε2 allele is considered protective against AD [62,63]. The APOE ε4 allele has been associated with a smaller hippocampal volume [64,65] and thinner cortex [66] in cognitively normal individuals and in patients with AD [67] while the APOE ε2 allele has been associated with thicker cortex in cognitively intact individuals [68]. GWVs with APOE 3/4 or 4/4 genotype were classified as ε4 positive while GWVs with APOE 2/3 or 2/4 genotype were classified as ε2 positive. Because the APOE ε2 allele is considered protective against AD [62,63] GWVs with the 2/4 genotype were not classified as APOE ε4 positive.

2.5.1. Covariates

Age [69,70], sex [71], and years of education [72,73] were included as covariates in analyses of hippocampal volume and cortical thickness because these variables have been shown to influence measures of brain volume and thickness. We also accounted for the presence or absence of Kansas GWI exclusionary condition (e.g., diabetes, heart disease other than hypertension, stroke, lupus, rheumatoid arthritis, cancer, liver and/or kidney disease) as a proxy measure of general overall health. ICV was included as a covariate in analysis of hippocampal volume to account for variations in head size between subjects that could reduce reliability [74]. Finally, we included any demographic and clinical variables that differed significantly between groups as covariates in the analyses.

2.5.2. Correction for Multiple Comparisons

Because the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons can be overly conservative when applied to non-independent (i.e., correlated) measures, we adjusted the analyses of cortical thickness of the four lobes of the brain and insula according to the number of regions analyzed (n = 5) and the average intercorrelations among the regions [75]. With an average intercorrelation r = 0.58, a 2-sided adjusted p = 0.03 was considered statistically significant. Similarly, analyses of GW deployment-related exposures were adjusted for multiple comparisons according to the number of exposures analyzed (n = 15) and the average intercorrelations among the exposures [75]. With an average intercorrelation r = 0.21, a 2-sided adjusted p = 0.006 was considered statistically significant.

2.5.3. Post-Hoc Analyses

We used hierarchical linear regression to examine the ability of current PTSD, current MDD, and history of alcohol abuse/dependence (independent variables entered in the last step of the regression model) to predict MCI status (dependent variable) over and above demographic and military characteristics that differed significantly between the groups (independent variables entered into the first step of the regression model). We used also hierarchical linear regression to examine the ability of MCI status (independent variable entered in the last step of the regression model) to predict hippocampal volume (dependent variable in second post-hoc analysis) and parietal cortex thickness (dependent variable in third post-hoc analysis) over and above demographic and clinical variables (i.e., age, sex, years of education, current PTSD and MDD, history of alcohol abuse/dependence, ICV, APOE ε4 and ε2 status, CDC CMI, and Khamisiyah plume exposure status, entered into the first step of the regression models). In a final post-hoc stepwise linear regression analysis, we examined the ability of all the demographic, clinical, and deployment-related exposures that differed significantly between the groups to predict MCI status (dependent variable).

3. Results

Twelve percent of the GWVs (n = 25) in the sample were classified as MCI according to the actuarial neuropsychological method, while 39% (n = 79) were classified as “cognitively normal” (CN, i.e., had no impaired scores in any cognitive domains). The rest of the GWVs (49%, n = 98) were classified as “intermediate.” Five of the intermediate GWVs had impaired scores (i.e., >1 SD below the age-corrected norm) in 3 cognitive domains; 25 had impaired scores in 2 cognitive domains; 68 had impaired scores in 1 cognitive domain.
Table 3 summarizes the demographic and clinical characteristics of the three cognitive groups. The MCI group tended to be younger (F2.201 = 2.49, p = 0.09) and have fewer years of formal education (F2.201 = 2.49, p = 0.09) compared to the other two groups. Significantly more GWVs with MCI were enlisted personnel during the GW (96%, χ2 = 13.32, df = 2, p = 0.003), had current PTSD (40%, χ2 = 17.12, df = 2, p = 0.001) and current MDD (24%, χ2 = 7.30, df = 2, p = 0.03), and a history of alcohol abuse/dependence (44%, χ2 = 6.73, df = 2, p = 0.04) compared to the other groups. There were significantly more CDC CMI cases, both mild–moderate (80%, χ2 = 6.52, df = 2, p = 0.04) and severe (40%, χ2 = 12.89, df = 2, p = 0.002), in the MCI than the other groups. There were no differences in Kansas GWI cases or in the number of GWVs with Kansas GWI exclusionary conditions. The CN group had fewer African Americans compared to the other two groups (1% versus 11% in the intermediate and 24% in the MCI, χ2 = 16.10, df = 2, p = 0.001).
In post-hoc analysis, we used hierarchical linear regression to examine the ability of the clinical variables (i.e., current PTSD, current MDD, and history of alcohol abuse/dependence, independent variables entered in the last step of the regression model) to predict MCI status (dependent variable) over and above demographic and military characteristics that differed significantly between the groups (i.e., race and rank during the GW, entered as independent variables in Step 1 of the model). The post-hoc regression models were significant (p’s ≤ 0.03, 0.08 ≤ R2 ≤ 0.11). In the first step of the model, rank (i.e., enlisted, standardized coefficient β = 0.21, t = 2.99, p = 0.003) and race (i.e., non-white, standardized coefficient β = 0.21, t = 3.04, p = 0.003) were significantly associated with MCI status. Even after accounting for rank and race in the first step of the model, current PTSD status (standardized coefficient β = 0.17, t = 2.47, p = 0.01) was significantly associated with MCI status in the second step of the model. Current MDD and history of alcohol abuse/dependence were not significantly associated with MCI status.
Table 4 summarizes the neuroimaging results. The MCI group had a smaller hippocampal volume compared to the other groups, even after accounting for ICV, age, sex, education, rank during the GW, current PTSD and MDD diagnoses, history of alcohol abuse/dependence, CDC CMI case status, predicted exposure to the Khamasiyah plume, Kansas GWI exclusionary conditions (as a proxy measure for general overall health) and APOE ε2/ε4 genotype (F2.189 = 6.76, p = 0.001). Planned contrasts revealed that the MCI group had a smaller hippocampal volume compared to CN (p < 0.001) and intermediate (p = 0.001) groups, but there were no significant hippocampal volume differences between intermediate and CN groups. The MCI group also had a thinner parietal cortex compared to the intermediate and CN groups, even after accounting for age, sex, education, rank during the GW, current PTSD and MDD diagnoses, history of alcohol abuse/dependence, CDC CMI case status, predicted exposure to the Khamasiyah plume, Kansas GWI exclusionary status, and APOE ε2/ε4 genotype (F2,181 = 4.10, p = 0.018). Planned contrasts revealed that the MCI group had a thinner parietal cortex compared to CN (p = 0.005) and intermediate (p = 0.03) groups, but there were no significant parietal cortex thickness differences between intermediate and CN groups. Although there was a group difference in temporal cortex thickness (F2,183 = 3.29, p = 0.04), this difference was not significant after adjustments for multiple comparisons according to the number of regions analyzed (n = 5) and the average intercorrelations among those regions [75]. With an average intercorrelation of r = 0.58 between the thickness of the four lobes of the brain and insula, a 2-sided adjusted p = 0.03 was considered statistically significant.
In post-hoc analyses, we used hierarchical linear regression to examine the ability of MCI status (independent variable entered in the last step of the model) to predict hippocampal volume and parietal cortex thickness (dependent variables) over and above demographic and clinical variables. All fits in the post-hoc regression for hippocampal volume were significant (p’s ≤ 0.002, 0.30 ≤ R2 ≤ 0.33). After accounting for ICV (standardized coefficient β = 0.51, t= 7.40, p < 0.001), age (standardized coefficient β = −0.20, t= −3.01, p = 0.001), and Khamisiyah exposure status (standardized coefficient β = −0.13, t= −1.99, p < 0.05) in the first step of the model, MCI status was still significantly associated with hippocampal volume (standardized coefficient β = 0.20, t= 3.07, p = 0.002). In the post-hoc regression for parietal cortex thickness, age was inversely related to parietal cortex thickness (standardize coefficient β = −0.20, t= −2.44, p = 0.016); however, the first model with demographic and clinical variables was not statistically significant (R2 = 0.04, p = 0.09). Only the second model was significant (R2 = 0.07, p = 0.008) because MCI status was significantly associated with parietal cortex thickness (standardized coefficient β = 0.21, t= 2.67, p = 0.008).
Table 5 summarizes the group differences in GW deployment-related exposures and experiences. More GWVs in the MCI group reported witnessing smoke from burning oil well fires, hearing chemical alarms sound, coming into contact with dead animals, using powdered pesticides directly on skin, witnessing their living areas being sprayed with pesticides, and coming into contact with chemical agent resistant coating (CARC) paint compared to the other groups (see Table 5). However, after adjustment for multiple comparisons according to the number of exposures (n = 15) and the average intercorrelation among the exposures (r = 0.21) [75], only differences in the frequency of contact with dead animals and witnessing living areas being sprayed with pesticides remained significant.
In a final post-hoc analysis, we examined the relationship between MCI status (dependent variable) and demographic and military characteristics (race and rank during GW), clinical characteristics (current PTSD and MDD, history of alcohol abuse/dependence, and CDC CMI status) and deployment-related experiences (coming into contact with dead animals and witnessing living area sprayed with pesticides) that differed significantly between the groups. The stepwise linear regression models (p’s ≤ 0.01, 0.07 ≤ R2 ≤ 0.15) revealed three significant predictors of MCI status: race (being non-white, standardized coefficient β = 0.20, t = 2.82, p = 0.005), current PTSD diagnosis (standardized coefficient β = 0.20, t= 2.94, p = 0.004), and rank (enlisted during the GW, standardized coefficient β = 0.17, t= 2.48, p = 0.01). GW deployment-related experiences were not significantly associated with MCI status after accounting for these three variables.

4. Discussion

The main finding of this study is that 12% of GWVs in a convenience sample originally recruited for a VA-funded study on the effects of predicted exposure to the Khamisiyah plume on brain structure and function had MCI according to an actuarial neuropsychological criteria [33,35]. MCI, conceptualized as the transition between normal cognitive aging and dementia [76], is widely considered to be a prodromal condition indicative of future dementia such as AD [76]. Although not every patient with MCI progresses to dementia [14,77], it is noteworthy that GWVs with MCI had smaller hippocampal volume and thinner parietal cortex, two hallmarks of AD pathogenesis [78,79] compared to GWVs without MCI. The group difference in hippocampal volume and parietal cortex thickness remained significant even after accounting for demographic and clinical variables, predicted exposure to the Khamisiyah plume, APOE genotype, and general overall health, approximated by using Kansas GWI exclusionary status.
How significant is a prevalence of 12% of MCI in GWVs who had a median age of 48 years at the time they were studied? According to the American Academy of Neurology Practice update summary of MCI [80], the prevalence of MCI in the general population is 6.7% for ages 60–64, 8.4% for 65–69, 10.1% for 70–74, 14.5% for 75–79, and 25.2% for ages 80–84. Very few research studies of MCI focus on adults under the age of 60 [36]. The few studies that have reported prevalence in the range of 0%−13% [81,82,83]. If the higher estimates of MCI in adults under 60 is valid, then our finding that 12% of deployed GWVs had MCI is not out of the ordinary. In a study of 1126 twins from the Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging (VETSA) who were 51–59 years old at the time of study, Kremen et al. reported a prevalence of 1%−65% for MCI [37]. However, it seems unlikely that 65% of Vietnam veterans in their 50s would have MCI given that the prevalence of MCI increases with age [84] and 65% is significantly higher than the prevalence of MCI in octogenarians, estimated to be 25% by the American Academy of Neurology [80]. Consequently, Kremen et al. proposed that lower rates of MCI are more likely to be valid in adults under 60 [37]. If we assume that Kremen et al.’s proposal is correct, and that the prevalence of MCI in adults under 60 is lower than the prevalence of MCI in people aged 60–64, estimated to be 6.7% by the American Academy of Neurology [80], then 12% MCI in deployed GWVs is almost twice the prevalence of MCI expected in the general population.
Two case definitions for GWI have been endorsed by the Institute of Medicine for use in clinical diagnosis and research investigations [1]: the CDC CMI definition [45] and the Kansas GWI definition [44]. Although cognitive dysfunction is a symptom of both, it is interesting that was no difference in the rate of Kansas GWI between the three cognitive groups. In contrast, there were significantly more CDC CMI cases, both severe and mild–moderate cases, in the MCI group. This is likely because the Kansas GWI definition requires cases to have multiple chronic symptoms in at least three of six categories and cognitive difficulties falls within the broader category of neurological/cognitive/mood symptoms. In contrast, the CDC CMI definition requires only symptoms in two of three categories and the mood-cognition category captures many symptoms common in individuals with MCI (i.e., difficulty remembering or concentrating, word finding difficulties, feeling depressed, feeling moody, feeling anxious, trouble sleeping).
The mechanisms underlying higher rates of MCI in GWVs are likely to be complex and interlinking with no single process explaining the relationship. However, post-hoc regression analysis revealed a strong association between current PTSD and MCI status, even after accounting for demographic characteristics. PTSD is a stress-related condition that develops in some individuals after experiencing a traumatic event [85]. It has been postulated that dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis occurs in some people following exposure to severe trauma [86]. Chronic hyperactivation of the HPA axis may lead to aberrant neuroimmune responses [87], which, in turn, can result in damage to the hippocampus [88,89,90,91]. A large (n = 1868) consortium study recently confirmed the negative association between PTSD and hippocampal volume [91]. Notably, hippocampal atrophy is also a pathological hallmark of AD [92]. There is also suggestive evidence that PTSD may induce epigenetic changes that disrupt a number of physiological mechanisms/systems such as the metabolic, immune, and inflammatory systems, which renders individuals with PTSD vulnerable for developing a variety of co-morbid chronic diseases [93]. Aberrant immune responses may also interrupt anti-inflammatory Aβ clearance mechanisms that creates a switch towards pro-inflammatory mechanisms which could cause neuronal necrosis [94]. One consequence of this would be cognitive impairment. Indeed, PTSD has been associated with impaired cognition [95,96,97,98,99], increased risk for dementia [94,99,100], and increased Aβ burden [101], but see [102].
Although there were higher rates of current MDD among GWVs with MCI compared to the other groups (24% vs. 8% and 6% in intermediate and CN), post-hoc analysis suggested that current MDD was not significantly associated with MCI status in the present study. Nevertheless, it is noteworthy that MDD has been associated with impaired cognitive function [103,104,105,106]. Furthermore, there is a large body of literature suggesting that depression in late life [107,108,109,110,111] as well as middle age [112,113] increases the risk of developing cognitive impairment and dementia. An epidemiological study of more than 13,000 subjects found that the risk of developing AD was approximately doubled in individuals with late-life depressive symptoms while the risk of vascular dementia was more than tripled in those with both mid- and late-life depression [114]. Neuropathology studies have linked history of depression with increased amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles, two neuropathological hallmarks of AD [115,116]. Prolonged damage to the hippocampus due to hypercorisolaemia may play a role linking both PTSD and MDD to cognitive impairment and risk for dementia [100,117].
Consistent with epidemiological findings that substance use disorders are commonly comorbid with PTSD [118,119,120,121] and MDD [122,123], there was a higher prevalence of history of alcohol abuse/dependence in MCI group compared to the other two groups (44% vs. 19% and 23% in intermediate and CN). Although post-hoc analysis suggested that history of alcohol use disorder was not significantly associated with MCI status in the present study, like PTSD and MDD, heavy alcohol use has been associated with deleterious effects on the hippocampus [124] and cognitive function [125,126,127,128]. Future studies will be necessary to ascertain the relationship between PTSD, MDD, history of alcohol abuse/dependence and MCI in GWVs.
Contact with dead animals during the GW has been considered a proxy for contact with chemical warfare agents [129]. There have been reports of negative associations between self-reported exposure to pesticides and cognitive performance in non-demented individual who live in areas near pesticide-sprayed fields [130]. In the current study, GWVs with MCI reported higher frequencies of coming into contact with dead animals and seeing their living area being sprayed or fogged with pesticide during the GW; however, neither deployment-related exposure was significantly associated with MCI status after accounting for race, rank, and PTSD in post-hoc regression analyses.
The current study also found a significant association between race (i.e., being non-white) and MCI status. This is consistent with the reports of higher rates of MCI and dementia in African Americans [131,132,133,134,135]. In fact, it has been suggested that African Americans and Hispanics may be more likely to develop AD and other dementias than their non-Hispanic White counterparts [136,137] because of differences in underlying risk factors [136,137,138,139]. There were also significantly more enlisted personnel in the MCI group compared to the other two cognitive groups. In the U.S. Armed Forces, military rank is commonly considered a proxy for socioeconomic status (SES) because higher SES correlates with higher rank and there is a direct relationship between military rank and annual income [140]. Many studies have found higher risk of AD and other dementias [141,142,143,144,145,146] and higher rates of MCI [82,146,147] among individuals with lower SES. There is a well-established relationship between higher SES levels and cognitive function, particularly for executive function and language tasks [148]. One explanation may be that higher cognitive function leads to more intellectual and higher paying occupations, and greater wealth affords enriching lifestyles than can contribute to cognitive reserve [149], which may confer protection against the effects of neurodegeneration [150].
The findings of this study should be considered in the context of some limitations: first, this study had a cross-sectional, non-random, non-experimental design because the parent study from which data for the secondary analyses were derived was not originally designed to investigate the relationship between GW deployment and the prevalence of MCI. Thus, the current findings may not accurately reflect the true prevalence or severity of MCI among GWVs and cannot determine causal links. For example, history of TBI with loss of consciousness (LOC) has been associated with an approximately 2.5 year earlier diagnosis of MCI [151]. However, TBI with LOC was exclusionary in the parent study. Further, the parent study excluded participants with a lifetime history of psychotic or bipolar disorders and/or drug abuse or dependence in the past 12 months, which may have biased the current findings. Second, MCI was defined using an actuarial neuropsychological criteria [33] and the GWVs did not undergo neurological evaluations. Nevertheless, it is worth noting that MCI patients identified with the actuarial neuropsychological criteria have been shown to exhibit significant cerebral spinal fluid AD biomarker associations, more stable diagnoses, and have a larger percentage of individuals who progressed to dementia than MCI patients diagnosed by the conventional MCI criteria [35]. Third, the present study used a hybrid of the actuarial neuropsychological criteria described by Jak et al. [33] and Bondi et al. [35]. The original description of the actuarial neuropsychological criteria employed three neuropsychological measures in five cognitive domains. Patients were categorized as MCI if they had impaired scores on two measures in a cognitive domain [33]. In a subsequent publication, the actuarial neuropsychological criteria was adapted for the ADNI sample and two neuropsychological measures in three cognitive domains were used [35]. ADNI subjects were classified as MCI if they had an impaired score on two measures in one cognitive domain or one impaired score in three cognitive domains. Because the neuropsychological battery used in the parent study only had one measure in the visuospatial domain and one measure in the language domain, we modified the actuarial neuropsychological criteria to categorize GWVs as MCI if they had two impaired scores in the cognitive domains with two measures (i.e., memory, executive function and attention), or one impaired score in at least four of the five domains. Therefore, it may be possible that some of the GWVs classified as “intermediate” in the present study may, in fact, have MCI. Finally, we did not have information about the veterans’ premorbid cognitive abilities (e.g., from the Armed Forces Qualification Test). Previous research has shown that low premorbid cognitive abilities may be a risk factor for MCI, whereas high premorbid cognitive abilities may be protective against MCI [151].

5. Conclusions

Assuming that the prevalence of MCI in people under 60 is lower than the prevalence of MCI in people aged 60–64, estimated to be 6.7% [80], the finding that 12% of GWVs (median 48 years at the time of testing) had MCI is nearly twice the prevalence rate of MCI expected in the general population. This finding is consistent with the idea that GWVs are aging at a faster rate than the general population [6]. Furthermore, GWVs with MCI had hippocampal atrophy and a thinner parietal cortex, two hallmarks of AD pathogenesis [78,79], compared to GWVs without MCI. Because individuals with MCI develop dementia at a higher rate than the general population (10–15% versus 1–2% per year) [12], if these results are confirmed in a larger, more general sample of GWVs, it may portend higher rates of future dementia in deployed GWVs. With the advent of in vivo biomarkers of amyloid and tau, AD is increasingly being conceptualized as a biomarker-driven diagnosis rather than a clinical syndrome [152]. Therefore, it will be informative to examine in vivo levels of amyloid and tau in GWVs with MCI in future studies.

Funding

This research was funded by the Department of Veterans Affairs grant I01CX000798.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank all the GW veterans who participated in this research project.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Institute of Medicine. Chronic Multisymptom Illness in Gulf War veterans: Case Definitions Reexamined; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  2. White, R.F.; Steele, L.; O’Callaghan, J.P.; Sullivan, K.; Binns, J.H.; Golomb, B.A.; Bloom, F.E.; Bunker, J.A.; Crawford, F.; Graves, J.C.; et al. Recent research on Gulf War illness and other health problems in veterans of the 1991 Gulf War: Effects of toxicant exposures during deployment. Cortex 2016, 74, 449–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Ozakinci, G.; Hallman, W.K.; Kipen, H.M. Persistence of symptoms in veterans of the First Gulf War: 5-year follow-up. Environ. Health Perspect. 2006, 114, 1553–1557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Kang, H.K.; Li, B.; Mahan, C.M.; Eisen, S.A.; Engel, C.C. Health of US veterans of 1991 Gulf War: A follow-up survey in 10 years. J. Occup Environ. Med. 2009, 51, 401–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Blanchard, M.S.; Eisen, S.A.; Alpern, R.; Karlinsky, J.; Toomey, R.; Reda, D.J.; Murphy, F.M.; Jackson, L.W.; Kang, H.K. Chronic multisymptom illness complex in Gulf War I veterans 10 years later. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2006, 163, 66–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Zundel, C.G.; Krengel, M.H.; Heeren, T.; Yee, M.K.; Grasso, C.M.; Janulewicz Lloyd, P.A.; Coughlin, S.S.; Sullivan, K. Rates of chronic medical conditions in 1991 Gulf War veterans compared to the general population. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Smith, B.N.; Wang, J.M.; Vogt, D.; Vickers, K.; King, D.W.; King, L.A. Gulf War Illness: Symptomatology among veterans 10 years after deployment. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2013, 55, 104–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Yee, M.; Seichepine, D.R.; Janulewicz, P.A.; Sullivan, K.A.; Proctor, S.P.; Krengel, M.H. Self-Reported traumatic brain injury, health and rate of chronic multisymptom illness in veterans from the 1990–1991 Gulf War. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2016, 31, 320–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Janulewicz, P.A.; Krengel, M.H.; Maule, A.; White, R.F.; Cirillo, J.; Sisson, E.; Heeren, T.; Sullivan, K. Neuropsychological characteristics of Gulf War illness: A meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0177121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Jeffrey, M.G.; Krengel, M.; Kibler, J.L.; Zundel, C.; Klimas, N.; Sullivan, K.; Craddock, T.J.A. Neuropsychological findings in Gulf War Illness: A Review. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Langa, K.M.; Levine, D.A. The diagnosis and management of mild cognitive impairment: A Clinical Review. JAMA 2014, 312, 2551–2561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Petersen, R.C. Mild cognitive impairment. N. Engl. J. Med. 2011, 364, 2227–2234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Hugo, J.; Ganguli, M. Dementia and cognitive impairment: Epidemiology, diagnosis, and treatment. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 2014, 30, 421–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Petersen, R.C.; Smith, G.E.; Waring, S.C.; Ivnik, R.J.; Tangalos, E.G.; Kokmen, E. Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch. Neurol. 1999, 56, 303–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Yaffe, K.; Petersen, R.C.; Lindquist, K.; Kramer, J.; Miller, B. Subtype of mild cognitive impairment and progression to dementia and death. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2006, 22, 312–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Mitchell, A.J.; Shiri-Feshki, M. Temporal trends in the long term risk of progression of mild cognitive impairment: A pooled analysis. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2008, 79, 1386–1391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Petersen, R.C.; Roberts, R.O.; Knopman, D.S.; Boeve, B.F.; Geda, Y.E.; Ivnik, R.J.; Smith, G.E.; Jack, C.R., Jr. Mild cognitive impairment: Ten years later. Arch. Neurol. 2009, 66, 1447–1455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Plassman, B.L.; Langa, K.M.; Fisher, G.G.; Heeringa, S.G.; Weir, D.R.; Ofstedal, M.B.; Burke, J.R.; Hurd, M.D.; Potter, G.G.; Rodgers, W.L.; et al. Prevalence of cognitive impairment without dementia in the United States. Ann. Intern. Med. 2008, 148, 427–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Petersen, R.C.; Doody, R.; Kurz, A.; Mohs, R.C.; Morris, J.C.; Rabins, P.V.; Ritchie, K.; Rossor, M.; Thal, L.; Winblad, B. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 2001, 69, 1985–1992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Petersen, R.C.; Morris, J.C. Mild cognitive impairment as a clinical entity and treatment target. Arch. Neurol. 2005, 62, 1160–1163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Manly, J.J.; Bell-McGinty, S.; Tang, M.X.; Schupf, N.; Stern, Y.; Mayeux, R. Implementing diagnostic criteria and estimating frequency of mild cognitive impairment in an urban community. Arch. Neurol. 2005, 62, 1739–1746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Yates, J.A.; Clare, L.; Woods, R.T. Subjective memory complaints, mood and MCI: A follow-up study. Aging Ment. Health 2017, 21, 313–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Petersen, R.C.; Aisen, P.S.; Beckett, L.A.; Donohue, M.C.; Gamst, A.C.; Harvey, D.J.; Jack, C.R., Jr.; Jagust, W.J.; Shaw, L.M.; Toga, A.W.; et al. Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): Clinical characterization. Neurology 2010, 74, 201–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Brooks, B.L.; Iverson, G.L.; Holdnack, J.A.; Feldman, H.H. Potential for misclassification of mild cognitive impairment: A study of memory scores on the Wechsler Memory Scale-III in healthy older adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol Soc. 2008, 14, 463–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Mitchell, A.J. A meta-analysis of the accuracy of the mini-mental state examination in the detection of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2009, 43, 411–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Gallassi, R.; Oppi, F.; Poda, R.; Scortichini, S.; Maserati, M.S.; Marano, G.; Sambati, L. Are subjective cognitive complaints a risk factor for dementia? Neuro. Sci. 2010, 31, 327–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Tyas, S.L.; Salazar, J.C.; Snowdon, D.A.; Desrosiers, M.F.; Riley, K.P.; Mendiondo, M.S.; Kryscio, R.F. Transitions to mild cognitive impairments, dementia, and death: Findings from the Nun Study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 165, 1231–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. de Jager, C.A.; Budge, M.M. Stability and predictability of the classification of mild cognitive impairment as assessed by episodic memory test performance over time. Neurocase 2005, 11, 72–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Nordlund, A.; Rolstad, S.; Klang, O.; Edman, A.; Hansen, S.; Wallin, A. Two-year outcome of MCI subtypes and aetiologies in the Goteborg MCI study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2010, 81, 541–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ganguli, M.; Snitz, B.E.; Saxton, J.A.; Chang, C.C.H.; Lee, C.W.; Vander Bilt, J.; Hughes, T.F.; Lowenstein, D.A.; Unverzagt, F.W.; Petersen, R.C. Outcomes of mild cognitive impairment by definition: A population study. Arch. Neurol. 2011, 68, 761–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Edmonds, E.C.; Delano-Wood, L.; Clark, L.R.; Jak, A.J.; Nation, D.A.; McDonald, C.R.; Libon, D.J.; Au, R.; Galasko, D.; Salmon, D.P.; et al. Susceptibility of the conventional criteria for mild cognitive impairment to false positive diagnostic errors. Alzheimers Dement. 2015, 11, 415–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Nettiksimmons, J.; Decarli, C.; Landau, S.; Beckett, L. Biological heterogeneity in ADNI amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 2014, 10, 511–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Jak, A.J.; Bondi, M.W.; Delano-Wood, L.; Wierenga, C.; Corey-Bloom, J.; Salmon, D.P.; Delis, D.C. Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2009, 17, 368–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Jak, A.J.; Urban, S.; McCauley, A.; Bangen, K.J.; Delano-Wood, L.; Corey-Bloom, J.; Bondi, M.W. Profile of hippocampal volumes and stroke risk varies by neuropsychological definition of mild cognitive impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2009, 15, 890–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Bondi, M.W.; Edmonds, E.C.; Jak, A.J.; Clark, L.R.; Delano-Wood, L.; McDonald, C.R.; Nation, D.A.; Libon, D.J.; Au, R.; Galasko, D.; et al. Neuropsychological criteria for mild cognitive impairment improves diagnostic precision, biomarker associations, and prediction of progression. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2014, 42, 275–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Ward, A.; Arrighi, H.M.; Michels, S.; Cedarbaum, J.M. Mild cognitive impairment: Disparity of incidence and prevalence estimates. Alzheimers Dement. 2012, 8, 14–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kremen, W.S.; Jak, A.J.; Pznizzon, M.S.; Spoon, K.M.; Franz, C.E.; Thompson, W.K.; Jacobson, K.C.; Vasilopoulos, T.; Vuoksimaa, E.; Xian, H.; et al. Early identification and heritability of mild cognitive impairment. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2014, 43, 600–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Chao, L.L. Evidence of objective memory impairments in deployed Gulf War veterans with subjective memory complaints. Mil. Med. 2017, 182, e1625–e1631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. First, M.; Spitzer, R.; Gobbon, M.; Williams, J. Structured Clinical Interview for DMS-IV; New Work State Psychiatric Institute Biometrics Research Department: New York, NY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  40. Wolfe, J.; Kimerling, R.; Brown, P.; Chresman, K.; Levin, K. Psychometric review of the life stressor checklist-revised. In Instrumentation in Stress, Trauma, and Adaptation; Stamm, B.H., Ed.; Sidran Press: Lutherville, MD, USA, 1996; pp. 144–151. [Google Scholar]
  41. Blake, D.D.; Weathers, F.W.; Nagy, L.M.; Kaloupek, D.G.; Gusman, F.D.; Charney, D.S.; Keane, T.M. The development of a Clinican-Administered PTSD Scale. J. Trauma. Stress 1995, 8, 75–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Corrigan, J.D.; Bogner, J.A. Initial reliability and validity of the OSU TBI identification method. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2007, 22, 318–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Chao, L.L. The relationship between traumatic brain injury and rates of chronic symptomatic illness in 202 Gulf War Veerans. Mil. Med. 2018, 183, e571–e579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Steele, L. Prevalence and patterns of Gulf War illness in Kansas veterans: Association of symptoms with characteristics of person, place, and time of military service. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2000, 152, 992–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Fukuda, K.; Nisenbaum, R.; Stewart, G.; Thompson, W.W.; Robin, L.; Washko, R.M.; Noah, D.L.; Barrett, D.H.; Randall, B.; Herwaldt, B.L.; et al. Chronic multisymptom illness affecting Air Force veterans of the Gulf War. JAMA 1998, 280, 981–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Bondi, M.W.; Jak, A.J.; Delano-Wood, L.; Jacobson, M.W.; Delis, D.C.; Salmon, D.P. Neuropsychological contributions to the early identification of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2008, 18, 73–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Delis, D.C.; Kramer, J.H.; Kaplan, E.; Ober, B.A. California Verbal Learning Test, 2nd ed.; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  48. Reitan, R.M.; Wolfson, D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Interpetation; Neuropsychological Press: Tucson, AZ, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  49. Delis, D.C.; Kaplan, E.; Kramer, J.H. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  50. Wechsler, D. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd ed.; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  51. Kaplan, E.; Goodglass, H.; Wintraub, S. The Boston Naming Test; Lea and Febiger: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  52. Dale, A.M.; Fischl, B.; Sereno, M.I. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 1999, 9, 179–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Desikan, R.S.; Segonne, F.; Fischl, B.; Quinn, B.T.; Dickerson, B.C.; Blacker, D.; Buckner, R.L.; Dale, A.M.; Maguire, R.P.; Hyman, B.T.; et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 2006, 31, 968–980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Fischl, B.; Sereno, M.I.; Dale, A.M. Cortical surface-based analysis: II. Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 1999, 9, 195–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Chao, L.L. Associations between the self-reported frequency of hearing chemical alarms in theater and visuospatial function in Gulf War Veterans. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2016, 58, 1014–1020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Chao, L.L.; Reeb, R.; Esparza, I.L.; Abadjian, L.R. Associations between the self-reported frequency of hearing chemical alarms in theater and regional brain volume in Gulf War Veterans. Neurotoxicology 2016, 53, 246–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Chao, L.L.; Rothlind, J.C.; Cardenas, V.A.; Meyerhoff, D.J.; Weiner, M.W. Effects of low-level exposure to sarin and cyclosarin during the 1991 Gulf War on brain function and brain structure in US veterans. Neurotoxicology 2010, 31, 493–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Chao, L.L.; Kriger, S.; Buckley, S.; Ng, P.; Mueller, S.G. Effects of low-level sarin and cyclosarin exposure on hippocampal subfields in Gulf War Veterans. Neurotoxicology 2014, 44, 263–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Brouwers, N.; Sleegers, K.; Van Broeckhoven, C. Molecular genetics of Alzheimer’s disease: An update. Ann. Med. 2008, 40, 562–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Harold, D.; Abraham, R.; Hollingworth, P.; Sims, R.; Gerrish, A.; Hamshere, M.L.; Pahwa, J.S.; Moskvina, V.; Dowzell, K.; Williams, A.; et al. Genome-wide association study identifies variants at CLU and PICALM associated with Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Genet. 2009, 41, 1088–1093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Lambert, J.C.; Heath, S.; Even, G.; Campion, D.; Sleegers, K.; Hiltunen, M.; Combarros, O.; Zelenika, D.; Bullido, M.J.; Tavernier, B.; et al. Genome-wide association study identifies variants at CLU and CR1 associated with Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Genet. 2009, 41, 1094–1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Corder, E.H.; Saunders, A.M.; Risch, N.J.; Strittmatter, W.J.; Schmechel, D.E.; Gaskell, P.C., Jr.; Rimmler, J.B.; Locke, P.A.; Conneally, P.M.; Schmader, K.E.; et al. Protective effect of apolipoprotein E type 2 allele for late onset Alzheimer disease. Nat. Genet. 1994, 7, 180–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Wu, L.; Zhao, L. ApoE2 and Alzheimer’s disease: Time to take a closer look. Neural. Regen. Res. 2016, 11, 412–413. [Google Scholar]
  64. Lehtovirta, M.; Laakso, M.P.; Soininen, H.; Helisalmi, S.; Mannermaa, A.; Helkala, E.L.; Partanen, K.; Ryynänen, M.; Vainio, P.; Hartikainen, P.; et al. Volumes of hippocampus, amygdala and frontal lobe in Alzheimer patients with different apolipoprotein E genotypes. Neuroscience 1995, 67, 65–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. O’Dwyer, L.; Lamberton, F.; Matura, S.; Tanner, C.; Scheibe, M.; Miller, J.; Rujescu, D.; Prvulovic, D.; Hampel, H. Reduced hippocampal volume in healthy young ApoE4 carriers: An MRI study. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e48895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Burggren, A.C.; Zeineh, M.M.; Ekstrom, A.D.; Braskie, M.N.; Thompson, P.M.; Small, G.W.; Bookheimer, S.Y. Reduced cortical thickness in hippocampal sub-regions among cognitively normal apolipoprotein E e4 carriers. Neuroimage 2008, 41, 1177–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Mueller, S.G.; Schuff, N.; Raptentsetsang, S.; Elman, J.; Weiner, M.W. Selective effect of Apo e4 on CA3 and dentate in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease using high resolution MRI at 4 T. Neuroimage 2008, 42, 42–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Piers, R.J. Structural brain volume differences between cognitively intact ApoE4 carriers and non-carriers across the lifespan. Neural Regen. Res. 2018, 13, 1309–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Scahill, R.I.; Frost, C.; Jenkins, R.; Whitwell, J.L.; Rossor, M.N.; Fox, N.C. A longitudinal study of brain volume changes in normal aging using serial registered magnetic resonance imaging. Arch. Neurol. 2003, 60, 989–994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Peters, R. Ageing and the brain. Postgrad. Med. J. 2006, 82, 84–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Ritchie, S.J.; Cox, S.R.; Shen, X.; Lombardo, M.V.; Reus, L.M.; Alloza, C.; Harris, M.A.; Alderson, H.L.; Hunter, S.; Neilson, E.; et al. Sex Differences in the Adult Human Brain: Evidence from 5216 UK Biobank Participants. Cereb. Cortex 2018, 28, 2959–2975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Foubert-Samier, A.; Catheline, G.; Amieva, H.; Dilharreguy, B.; Helmer, C.; Allard, M.; Dartigues, J.F. Education, occupation, leisure activities, and brain reserve: A population-based study. Neurobiol. Aging 2012, 33, e15–e25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Arenaza-Urquijo, E.M.; Landeau, B.; La Joie, R.; Mevel, K.; Mézenge, F.; Perrotin, A.; Desgranges, B.; Bartres-Faz, D.; Eustache, F.; Chételat, G. Relationships between years of education and gray matter volume, metabolism and functional connectivity in healthy elders. Neuroimage 2013, 83, 450–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Mathalon, D.H.; Sullivan, E.V.; Rawles, J.M.; Pfefferbaum, A. Correction for head size in brain-imaging measurements. Psychiatry Res. 1993, 50, 121–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Sankoh, A.J.; Huque, M.F.; Dubey, S.D. Some comments on frequently used multiple endpoint adjustment methods in clinical trials. Stat. Med. 1997, 16, 2529–2542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Tangalos, E.G.; Petersen, R.C. Mild cognitive impairment in geriatrics. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 2018, 34, 563–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Albert, M.S.; DeKosky, S.T.; Dickson, D.; Dubois, B.; Feldman, H.H.; Fox, N.C.; Gamst, A.; Holtzman, D.M.; Jagust, W.J.; Petersen, R.C.; et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011, 7, 270–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Devanand, D.P.; Pradhaban, G.; Liu, X.; Khandji, A.; De Santi, S.; Segal, S.; Rusinek, H.; Pelton, G.H.; Honing, L.S.; Mayeux, R.; et al. Hippocampal and entorhinal atrophy in mild cognitive impairment: Prediction of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2007, 68, 828–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Dickerson, B.C.; Bakkour, A.; Salat, D.H.; Feczko, E.; Pacheco, J.; Greve, D.N.; Grodstein, F.; Wright, C.I.; Blacker, D.; Rosas, H.D.; et al. The cortical signature of Alzheimer’s disease: Regionally specific cortical thinning relates to symptom severity in very mild to mild AD dementia and is detectable in asymptomatic amyloidpositive individuals. Cereb. Cortex 2009, 19, 497–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  80. Petersen, R.C.; Lopez, O.; Armstrong, M.J.; Getchius, T.S.D.; Ganguli, M.; Gloss, D.; Gronseth, G.S.; Marson, D.; Pringsheim, T.; Day, G.S.; et al. Practice guideline update summary: Mild cognitive impairment. Report of the Guideline Development, Dissemination, and Implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 2018, 90, 126–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Coria, F.; Gomez de Caso, J.A.; Minguez, L.; Rodriguez-Atalejo, F.; Claveria, L.E. Prevalence of age-associated memory impairment and dementia in a rural community. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 1993, 56, 973–976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  82. Schorder, J.; Kratz, B.; Pantel, J.; Minnemann, E.; Lehr, U.; Sauer, H. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in an elderly community sample. J. Neural Trans. Suppl. 1998, 54, 51–59. [Google Scholar]
  83. Hänninen, T.; Hallikainen, M.; Tousainen, S.; Vanhanen, M.; Soininen, H. Pravalence of mild cognitive impairment: A population-based study in elderly subjects. Acta Neurol. Scand. 2002, 106, 148–154. [Google Scholar]
  84. Petersen, R.C.; Roberts, R.O.; Knopman, D.S.; Geda, Y.E.; Cha, R.H.; Pankratz, V.S.; Boeve, B.F.; Tangalos, E.G.; Ivnik, R.J.; Rocca, W.A. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment is higher in men. The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Neurology 2010, 75, 889–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013; Volume 5. [Google Scholar]
  86. McEwen, B.S.; Rasgon, N.L. The brain and body on stress: Allostatic load and mechanisms for depression and dementia. In Depression as a Systemic Illness; Strain, J.J., Blumenfield, M., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018; pp. 14–36. [Google Scholar]
  87. Menard, C.; Pfau, M.; Hodes, G.E.; Russo, S.J. Immune and neuroendocrine mechanisms of stress vulnerability and resilience. Neuropsychopharmacology 2017, 42, 62–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Bremner, J.D.; Randall, P.; Scott, T.M.; Bronen, R.A.; Seibyl, J.P.; Southwick, S.M.; Delaney, R.C.; McCarthy, G.; Innis, R.B. MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 1995, 152, 973–981. [Google Scholar]
  89. Bremner, J.D.; Randall, P.; Vermetten, E.; Staib, L.; Bronen, R.A.; Mazure, C.; Capelli, S.; McCarthy, G.; Innis, R.B.; Charney, D.S. Magnetic resonance imaging-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse—A preliminary report. Biol. Psychiatry 1997, 41, 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Bremner, J.D.; Narayan, M.; Anderson, E.R.; Staib, L.H.; Miller, H.L.; Charney, D.S. Hippocampal volume reduction in major depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 2000, 157, 115–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Logue, M.W.; van Rooij, S.J.H.; Dennis, E.L.; Davis, S.L.; Hayes, J.P.; Stevens, J.S.; Densmore, M.; Haswell, C.C.; Ipser, J.; Koch, S.B.J.; et al. Smaller hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder: A multisite ENIGMA-PGC study: Subcortical volumetry results from posttraumatic stress disorder consortia. Biol. Psychiatry 2018, 83, 244–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Halliday, G. Pathology and hippocampal atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2017, 16, 862–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Perroud, N.; Rutembesa, E.; Paoloni-Giacobino, A.; Mutabaruka, J.; Mutesa, L.; Stenz, L.; Malafosse, A.; Karege, F. The Tutsi genocide and transgenerational transmission of maternal stress: Epigenetics and biology of the HPA axis. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 2014, 15, 334–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Calsolaro, V.; Edison, P. Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s disease: Current evidence and future directions. Alzheimers Dement. 2016, 12, 719–732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Bremner, J.D.; Scott, T.M.; Delaney, R.C.; Southwick, S.M.; Mason, J.W.; Johnson, D.R.; Innis, R.B.; McCarthy, G.; Charney, D.S. Deficits in short-term memory in post-traumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 1993, 150, 1015–1019. [Google Scholar]
  96. Bremner, J.D.; Randall, P.; Scott, T.M.; Capelli, S.; Delaney, R.; McCarthy, G.; Charney, D.S. Deficits in short-term memory in adult survivors of childhood abuse. Psych. Res. 1995, 59, 97–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Bremner, J.D.; Vermetten, E.; Afzal, N.; Vythilingam, M. Deficits in verbal declarative memory function in women with childhood sexual abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2004, 192, 643–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Schuitevoerder, S.; Rosen, J.W.; Twamley, E.W.; Ayers, C.R.; Sones, H.; Lohr, J.B.; Goetter, E.M.; Fonzo, G.W.; Holloway, K.J.; Thorp, S.R. A meta-analysis of cognitive functioning in older adults with PTSD. J. Anxiety Disord. 2013, 7, 550–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Qureshi, S.U.; Long, M.E.; Bradshaw, M.R.; Pyne, J.M.; Magruder, K.M.; Kimbrell, T.; Hudson, T.J.; Jawaid, A.; Schulz, P.E.; Kunik, M.E. Does PTSD impair cognition beyond the effect of trauma? J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2011, 23, 16–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Yaffe, K.; Vittinghoff, E.; Lindquist, K.; Barnes, D.; Covinsky, K.E.; Neylan, T.; Kluse, M.; Marmar, C. Post-traumatic stress disorder and risk of dementia among U.S. veterans. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 608–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Mohamed, A.Z.; Cumming, P.; Srour, H.; Gunasena, T.; Uchida, A.; Haller, C.N.; Nasrallah, F.; Department of Defense Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Amyloid pathology fingerprint differentiates post-traumatic stress disorder and traumatic brain injury. Neuroimage Clin. 2018, 19, 716–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Landau, S.; Harvey, D.; Weiner, M.W. Letter to the Author, concerning the publication: Amyloid pathology fingerprint differentiates post-traumatic stress disorder and traumatic brain injury. Neuroimage Clin. 2019, 23, 101868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Nebes, R.D.; Butters, M.A.; Houck, P.R.; Zmuda, M.D.; Aizenstein, H.; Pollock, B.G.; Mulsant, B.H.; Reynolds, C.F., III. Dual-task performance in depressed geriatric patients. Psychiatry Res. 2001, 102, 139–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Nebes, R.D.; Reynolds, C.F., III; Boada, F.; Metzler, C.C.; Fukui, M.B.; Saxton, J.; Halligan, E.M.; DeKosky, S.T. Longitudinal increase in the volume of white matter hyperintensities in late-onset depression. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2002, 17, 526–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Butters, M.A.; Becker, J.T.; Nebes, R.D.; Zmuda, M.D.; Mulsant, B.H.; Pollock, B.G.; Reynolds, C.F., III. Changes in cognitive functioning following treatment of late-life depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 2000, 157, 1949–1954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Butters, M.A.; Whyte, E.M.; Nebes, R.D.; Begley, A.E.; Dew, M.A.; Mulsant, B.H.; Zmuda, M.D.; Rhalla, R.; Metzler, C.C.; Pollock, B.G.; et al. The nature and determinants of neuropsychological functioning in late-life depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2004, 61, 587–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Jorm, A.F. History of depression as a risk factor for dementia: An updated review. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2001, 35, 776–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Ownby, R.L.; Crocco, E.; Acevedo, A.; John, V.; Lowenstein, D. Depression and risk for Alzheimer disease: Systematic review, meta-analysis, and metaregression analysis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2006, 63, 530–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Yaffe, K.; Blackwell, T.; Gore, R.; Sands, L.; Reus, V.; Browner, W.S. Depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in nondemented elderly women: A prospective study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1999, 56, 425–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Barnes, D.E.; Alexopoulos, G.S.; Lopez, O.L.; Williamson, J.D.; Yaffe, K. Depressive symptoms, vascular disease, and mild cognitive impairment: Findings from the Cardiovascular Health Study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2006, 63, 273–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  111. Wilson, R.S.; Arnold, S.E.; Beck, T.L.; Bienias, J.L.; Bennett, D.A. Change in depressive symptoms during the prodromal phase of Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008, 65, 439–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  112. Green, R.C.; Cupples, L.A.; Kurz, A.; Auerbach, S.; Go, R.; Sadovnick, D.; Duara, R.; Kukull, W.A.; Chui, H.; Edeki, T.; et al. Depression as a risk factor for Alzheimer disease: The MIRAGE Study. Arch. Neurol. 2003, 60, 753–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Dotson, V.M.; Beydoun, M.A.; Zonderman, A.B. Recurrent depressive symptoms and the incidence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 2010, 75, 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Barnes, D.E.; Yaffe, K.; Byers, A.L.; McCormick, M.; Schaefer, C.; Whitmer, R.A. Mid-life versus late-life depressive symptoms and risk of dementia: Differential effects for Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2013, 69, 493–498. [Google Scholar]
  115. Rapp, M.A.; Schnaider-Beeri, M.; Grossman, H.T.; Sano, M.; Perl, D.P.; Purohit, D.P.; Gorman, J.M.; Haroutunian, V. Increased hippocampal plaques and tangles in patients with Alzheimer disease with a lifetime history of major depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2006, 63, 161–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Rapp, M.A.; Schnaider-Beeri, M.; Purohit, D.P.; Perl, D.P.; Haroutunian, V.; Sano, M. Increased neurofibrillary tangles in patients with Alzheimer disease with comorbid depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2008, 16, 168–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Köhler, S.; Thomas, A.J.; Lloyd, A.; Barber, R.; Almeida, O.P.; O’Brien, J.T. White matter hyperintensities, cortisol levels, brain atrophy and continuing cognitive deficits in late-life depression. Br. J. Psychiatry 2010, 196, 143–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  118. Kessler, R.C.; Sonnega, A.; Bromet, E.; Hughes, M.; Nelsen, C.B. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1995, 52, 1048–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Jacobsen, L.K.; Southwick, S.M.; Kosten, T.R. Substance use disorders in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder: A review of the literature. Am. J. Psychiatry 2001, 158, 1184–1190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Mills, K.L.; Teesson, M.; Ross, J.; Peters, L. Trauma, PTSD, and substance use disorders: Findings from the Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Well-Being. Am. J. Psychiatry 2006, 163, 652–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Najt, P.; Fusar-Poli, P.; Brambilla, P. Co-occurring mental and substance abuse disorders: A review on the potential predictors and clinical outcomes. Psychiatry Res. 2011, 186, 159–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. McHugh, R.K.; Weiss, R.D. Alcohol Use Disorder and Depressive Disorders. Alcohol Res. 2019, 40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Rich, J.S.; Martin, P.R. Co-occurring psychiatric disorders and alcoholism. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2014, 125, 573–588. [Google Scholar]
  124. Beresford, T.P.; Arciniegas, D.B.; Alfers, J.; Clapp, L.; Martin, B.; Du, Y.; Liu, D.; Shen, D.; Davatzikos, C. Hippocampus volume loss due to chronic heavy drinking. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2006, 30, 1866–1870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Goldman, M.S. Cognitive impairment in chronic alcoholics: Some cause for optimism. Am. Psychol. 1983, 38, 1045–1054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Fein, G.; Bachman, L.; Fisher, S.; Davenport, L. Cognitive impairments in abstinent alcoholics. West. J. Med. 1990, 152, 531–537. [Google Scholar]
  127. Oscar-Berman, M.; Marinkovic, K. Alcohol: Effects on neurobehavioral functions and the brain. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2007, 17, 239–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Sullivan, E.V.; Harris, R.A.; Pfefferbaum, A. Alcohol’s effects on brain and behavior. Alcohol. Res. Health 2010, 33, 127–143. [Google Scholar]
  129. Fulco, C.E.; Liverman, C.T.; Sox, H.C. Depleted Uranium, Sarin, Pyridostigmine, Vaccines, In Gulf War and Health; Committee on Health Effects Associated with Exposures During the Gulf War, Division of Health Promotion and Disease Prevention: Washington, DC, USA, 2000; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
  130. Dardiotis, D.; Siokas, V.; Moza, S.; Kosmidis, M.H.; Vogiatzi, C.; Aloizou, A.M.; Geronikola, N.; Ntanasi, E.; Zalonis, I.; Yannakoulia, M.; et al. Pesticide exposure and cognitive function: Results from the Hellenic Longitudinal Investigation of Aging and Diet (HELIAD). Environ. Res. 2019, 177, 108632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Fillenbaum, G.G.; Heyman, A.; Huber, M.S.; Woodbury, M.A.; Leiss, J.; Schmader, K.E.; Bohannon, A.; Trapp-Moen, B. The prevalence and 3-year incidence of dementia in older black and white community residents. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 1998, 51, 587–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Plassman, B.L.; Langa, K.M.; Fisher, G.G.; Heeringa, S.G.; Weir, D.R.; Ofstedal, M.B.; Burke, J.R.; Hurd, M.D.; Potter, G.G.; Rodgers, W.L.; et al. Prevalence of dementia in the United States: The aging, demographics, and memory study. Neuroepidemiology 2007, 29, 125–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  133. Hall, K.S.; Gao, S.; Baiyewu, O.; Lane, K.A.; Gureje, O.; Shen, J.; Ogunniyi, A.; Murrell, J.R.; Unverzagt, F.W.; Dickens, J.; et al. Prevalence rates for dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in African Americans: 1992 versus 2001. Alzheimers Dement. 2009, 5, 227–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  134. Katz, M.J.; Lipton, R.B.; Hall, C.B.; Zimmerman, M.E.; Sanders, A.E.; Verghese, J.; Dickson, D.W.; Derby, C.A. Age-specific and sex-specific prevalence and incidence of mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and Alzheimer dementia in blacks and whites: A report from the Einstein Aging Study. Alzheimer. Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2012, 26, 335–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  135. Knopman, D.S.; Gottesman, R.F.; Sharrett, A.R.; Wruck, L.M.; Windham, B.G.; Coker, L.; Schneider, A.L.; Hengrui, S.; Alonso, A.; Coresh, J.; et al. Mild cognitive impairment and dementia prevalence: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Neurocognitive Study (ARIC-NCS). Alzheimer’s Dement. Diagn. Assess. Dis. Monit. 2016, 2, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  136. Alzheimer’s Association. Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer Dementia 2017, 13, 325–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Mayeda, E.R.; Glymour, M.M.; Quesenberry, C.P.; Whitmer, R.A. Inequalities in dementia incidence between six racial and ethnic groups over 14 years. Alzheimers Dement. 2016, 12, 216–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  138. Chin, A.L.; Negash, S.; Hamilton, R. Diversity and disparity in dementia: The impact of ethnoracial differences in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2011, 25, 187–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  139. Yaffe, K.; Falvey, C.; Harris, T.B.; Newman, A.; Satterfield, S.; Koster, A.; Ayonayon, H.; Simonsick, E.; Health ABC Study. Effect of socioeconomic disparities on incidence of dementia among biracial older adults: Prospective study. BMJ 2013, 347, f7051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  140. Clark, J.Y.; Thompson, I.M. Military rank as a measure of socioeconomic status and survival from prostate cancer. South. Med. J. 1994, 87, 1141–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Stern, Y.; Gurland, B.; Tatemichi, T.K.; Tang, M.X.; Wilder, D.; Mayeux, R. Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA 1994, 271, 1004–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Evans, D.A.; Hebert, L.E.; Beckett, L.A.; Scherr, P.A.; Albert, M.S.; Chown, M.J.; Pilgrim, D.M.; Taylor, J.O. Education and other measures of socioeconomic status and risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a defined population of older persons. Arch. Neurol. 1997, 54, 1399–1405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  143. Koster, A.; Penninx, B.W.J.H.; Bosma, H.; Kempen, G.I.J.M.; Newman, A.B.; Rubin, S.M.; Satterfield, S.; Atkinson, H.H.; Ayonayon, H.N.; Rosano, C.; et al. Socioeconomic differences in cognitive decline and the role of biomedical factors. Ann. Epidemiol. 2005, 15, 564–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  144. Goldbourt, U.; Schnaider-Beeri, M.; Davidson, M. Socioeconomic status in relationship to death of vascular disease and late-life dementia. J. Neurol. Sci. 2007, 257, 177–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  145. Scazufca, M.; Menezes, P.R.; Vallada, H.P.; Crepaldi, A.L.; Pastor-Valero, M.; Coutinho, L.M.S.; DiRienzo, V.D.; Almeida, O.P. High prevalence of dementia among older adults from poor socioeconomic backgrounds in Sao Paulo, Brazil. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2008, 20, 394–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  146. Sattler, C.; Toro, P.; Schonknecht, P.; Schroder, J. Cognitive activity, education and socioeconomic status as preventive factors for mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Psychiatry Res. 2012, 196, 90–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Tervo, S.; Kivipelto, M.; Hanninen, T.; Vanhanen, M.; Hillikainen, M.; Mannermaa, A.; Soininen, H. Incidence and risk factors for mild cognitive impairment: A population-based three-year follow-up study of cognitively healthy elderly subjects. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn Disord. 2004, 17, 196–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Krishnadas, R.; McLean, J.; Batty, G.D.; Burnes, H.; Deans, K.A.; Ford, I.; McConnachie, A.; McLean, J.S.; Millar, K.; Sattar, N.; et al. Socioeconomic deprivation and cortical morphology: Psychological, social, and biological determinants of ill health study. Psychosom. Med. 2013, 75, 616–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Marioni, R.E.; van den Hout, A.; Valenzuela, M.J.; Brayne, C.; Matthews, F.E.; MRC Cognitive Function and Ageing Study. Active cognitive lifestyle associates with cognitive recovery and a reduced risk of cognitive decline. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2012, 28, 223–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  150. Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2006, 20, s69–s74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. LoBue, C.; Denney, D.; Hynan, L.S.; Rossetti, H.C.; Lacritz, L.H.; Hart, J.; Womack, K.B.; Woon, F.L.; Cullum, C.M. Self-Reported Traumatic Brain Injury and Mild Cognitive Impairment: Increased Risk and Earlier Age of Diagnosis. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2016, 51, 727–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  152. Jack, C.R., Jr.; Bennett, D.A.; Glennow, K.; Carrillo, M.C.; Dunn, B.; Haeberlin, S.B.; Holtzman, D.M.; Jagust, W.; Jennsen, F.; Karlawish, J.; et al. NIA-AA research framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2018, 14, 535–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Neuropsychological measures used in the actuarial neuropsychological criteria for classifying MCI.
Table 1. Neuropsychological measures used in the actuarial neuropsychological criteria for classifying MCI.
DomainMeasures
Episodic MemoryCalifornia Verbal Learning Test-II [47] (CVLT) Trials 1–5 total recall
CVLT long-delay free recall
Executive FunctionTrail Making Test [48] (TMT) Part B
Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System [49] (D-KEFS) Color-Word Inference Test, color-word inhibition condition
AttentionWeschler Adult Intelligence Scale-III [50] (WAIS-III) Digit Span
TMT [48] Part A
LanguageBoston Naming Test [51]
Visuospatial FunctionWAIS-III [50] Block Design
Table 2. Operationalization of MCI, intermediate, and cognitively normal status.
Table 2. Operationalization of MCI, intermediate, and cognitively normal status.
MCIScore ≤ 1 SD Below the Norm on both Measures in Episodic Memory Domain or Executive Function Domain or Attention Domain or Score ≤ 1 SD Below the Norm on ≥1 Measure in ≥4 Cognitive Domains
Intermediate cognitive impairmentScore ≤ 1 SD below the norm on ≤3 cognitive domains
Cognitively normalNo score < 1 SD below the norm in any cognitive domains
Table 3. Demographic and clinical characteristics in the entire study sample and by cognitive status.
Table 3. Demographic and clinical characteristics in the entire study sample and by cognitive status.
AllMCIIntermediateCNStatistics
N20225 (12%)98 (49%)79 (39%)
Age (years)54.1 (7.7)51.8 (6.7)53.7 (7.3)55.4 (8.4)F = 2.49
Education (years)15.6 (2.3)14.8 (2.2)15.5 (2.3)16.0 (2.3)F = 2.49
Male166 (82%)22 (88%)76 (78%)68 (86%)χ2 = 2.83
Race χ2 = 16.10 **
Caucasian148 (73%)15 (60%)66 (67%)67 (85%)
African American18 (9%)6 (24%)11 (11%)1 (1%)
Other36 (18%)4 (16%)21 (21%)11 (14%)
Enlisted personnel during GW150 (74%)24 (96%)77 (79%)49 (62%)χ2 = 13.32 **
Military service during GW χ2 = 1.71
Active duty155 (77%)18 (72%)78 (80%)59 (75%)
Reserves38 (19%)6 (24%)15 (15%)17 (22%)
National guard9 (5%)1 (4%)5 (5%)3 (4%)
Branch of Service during GW χ2 = 0.72
Army126 (62%)17 (68%)61 (62%)48 (61%)
Marines39 (19%)4 (16%)20 (20%)15 (19%)
Navy20 (10%)2 (8%)9 (9%)9 (11%)
Air force17 (8%)2 (8%)8 (8%)7 (9%)
Current PTSD28 (14%)10 (40%)12 (12%)6 (8%)χ2 = 17.12 ***
Current MDD19 (9%)6 (24%)8 (8%)5 (6%)χ2 = 7.30 *
Hx ETOH abuse/depend.48 (24%)11 (44%)19 (19%)18 (23%)χ2 = 6.73 *
Hx substance abuse/depend.16 (8%)4 (16%)5 (5%)7 (9%)χ2 = 3.40
Psychotropic medication use40 (20%)9 (36%)18 (18%)13 (16%)χ2 = 4.81
TBI Hx χ2 = 9.02
Improbable105 (52%)13 (52%)50 (51%)42 (53%)
Possible49 (24%)4 (16%)20 (31%)15 (19%)
Mild46 (23%)8 (32%)16 (16%)22 (28%)
Moderate2 (1%)0 (0%)2 (2%)0 (0%)
APOE genotype a χ2 = 6.11
ε2/ε321 (11%)3 (12%)12 (13%)6 (8%)
ε2/ε48 (4%)2 (8%)3 (3%)3 (4%)
ε3/ε3129 (65%)15 (60%)58 (60%)56 (73%)
ε3/ε433 (16%)5 (20%)18 (19%)10 (13%)
ε4/ε47 (4%)0 (0%)5 (5%)2 (3%)
Mild–moderate CDC CMI cases126 (62%)20 (80%)64 (65%)43 (63%)χ2 = 6.52 *
Severe CDC CMI cases36 (18%)10 (40%)19 (19%)7 (9%)χ2 = 12.89 **
Kansas GWI cases83 (41%)14 (56%)41 (42%)28 (35%)χ2 = 3.36
Kansas exclusionary condition59 (29%)7 (28%)31 (32%)21 (27%)χ2 = 0.56
Predicted Khamisiyah exposure88 (44%)11 (44%)44 (45%)33 (42%)χ2 = 0.18
* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001. a data unavailable for two intermediate and two CN (cognitively normal) veterans.
Table 4. Neuroimaging results.
Table 4. Neuroimaging results.
MCIIntermediateCNStatistics
Hippocampal volume (cc)8.05 (0.98)8.59 (0.80)8.68 (0.79)F = 6.76 a
Cortical Thickness (mm)
Frontal lobe2.54 (0.09)2.59 (0.10)2.59 (0.10)F = 2.28
Parietal lobe2.33 (0.11)2.38 (0.09)2.40 (0.09)F = 4.10 b
Temporal lobe2.87 (0.11)2.95 (0.12)2.95 (0.11)F = 3.42
Occipital lobe1.90 (0.11)1.92 (0.10)1.95 (0.10)F = 2.07
Insula2.98 (0.15)3.03 (0.13)3.06 (0.14)F = 2.13
a ANCOVA with ICV, age, sex, education, rank, CMI case status, Kansas GWI exclusionary status, predicted Khamisiyah exposure status, current PTSD, current MDD, history of ETOH abuse/dependence, and APOE ε2/ε4 status as covariates. b ANCOVA with age, sex, education, rank, CMI case status, Kansas GWI exclusionary status, predicted Khamisiyah exposure status, current PTSD, current MDD, history of ETOH abuse/dependence, and APOE ε2/ε4 status as covariates. p = 0.04, not significant after correction for multiple comparisons.
Table 5. Deployment-related exposure or experience as a function of cognitive group.
Table 5. Deployment-related exposure or experience as a function of cognitive group.
Did Not ExperienceExperienced for 1–6 DaysExperienced for 7–30 DaysExperienced for >30 Daysχ2p
MCIIntCNMCIIntCNMCIIntCNMCIIntCN
Smoke from oil well fires4142916201420342960322816.510.01
Heard chemical alarms1225272837392427283611616.130.01
Within 1 mild of SCUD missile explosion2955565833338894436.980.32
Contact with POWs48455228283012191112863.390.76
Contact with dead animals3263624416328145166122.620.001
Contact with destroyed enemy vehicles20353944303420272216956.780.34
Contact with vehicles destroyed by friendly fire6076822818164318316.430.38
Cream/liquid pesticides2836518591621134838278.720.19
Powdered pesticide608689831444286616.220.01
Pesticide-treated uniform58676402813982922215.690.46
Wore flea collar88959601002312219.650.14
Living area sprayed with pesticides52787689131288285418.860.004
Took PB pills2026233622262425282028232.90.82
Contact with CARC paint4477721611101249288913.370.04
Lived in tent with fuel burning heater243946859127105649357.040.32
Int: Intermediate cognitive impairment; POWs: prisoners of war; PB: pyridostigmine. Values are %; Bold type indicates significant after adjustment for multiple comparisons.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chao, L.L. The Prevalence of Mild Cognitive Impairment in a Convenience Sample of 202 Gulf War Veterans. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7158. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17197158

AMA Style

Chao LL. The Prevalence of Mild Cognitive Impairment in a Convenience Sample of 202 Gulf War Veterans. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(19):7158. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17197158

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chao, Linda L. 2020. "The Prevalence of Mild Cognitive Impairment in a Convenience Sample of 202 Gulf War Veterans" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 19: 7158. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17197158

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop