Next Article in Journal
Effect of a Comprehensive Health Care Program on Blood Pressure, Blood Glucose, Body Composition, and Depression in Older Adults Living Alone: A Quasi-Experimental Pretest–Posttest Study
Previous Article in Journal
A Software for Calculating the Economic Aspects of Floating Offshore Renewable Energies
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Temperament and Sense of Coherence: Emotional Intelligence as a Mediator

by
Małgorzata Szcześniak
* and
Klaudia Strochalska
Institute of Psychology, University of Szczecin, 71-017 Szczecin, Poland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(1), 219; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17010219
Submission received: 3 December 2019 / Revised: 23 December 2019 / Accepted: 25 December 2019 / Published: 27 December 2019

Abstract

:
Sense of coherence (SOC) reflects an individual’s capacity and available resources to deal with stressful situations. For some time now studies have revealed that people differ in their experience of SOC; yet, very little is known about how and through which mechanisms a high level of SOC is formed. In order to increase our understanding about the paths to a high SOC in the stage of adulthood, we focused on exploring the role both of temperament, as it has been confirmed as a potential component in the development of more complex traits that emerge later in life, and of emotional intelligence (EI) as it has been found to increase SOC. The sample consisted of 173 participants between 18 and 49. We used the Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa and San Diego Autoquestionnaire (TEMPS-A), Orientation to Life Questionnaire (OLQ), and Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (INTE). Results showed a negative correlation between the depressive, cyclothymic, irritable, and anxious dimensions of temperament and SOC, and EI. There was also a positive correlation between hyperthymic temperament and SOC, and EI. EI correlated positively with a general sense of coherence and its three dimensions. The PROCESS macro for SPSS showed that emotional intelligence mediates the relationship between depressive, cyclothymic, hyperthymic, irritable and anxious temperament, comprehensibility, manageability, meaningfulness, and global orientation to life. On the basis of the obtained outcomes, it can be stated that emotional intelligence mediates the relationship between dimensions of temperament and dimensions of SOC.

1. Introduction

The sense of coherence (SOC) construct was introduced by Antonovsky [1] and defined as a global life orientation that perceives the internal and external world as comprehensible, manageable, and meaningful. SOC reflects an individual’s capacity and available resources to deal with stressful situations [2]. For some time now studies have revealed [3,4] that people differ in their experience of SOC. Systematic review has indicated that neuroticism inversely predicts SOC, while extraversion, conscientiousness, and agreeableness are its positive predictors [5]. Likewise, a diffuse/avoidant style reduces SOC as well as its components: comprehensibility and meaningfulness [6]. Moreover, SOC is influenced by resistance resources [7], processes of identity development [8], and family background [9]. Yet, very little is known about how and through which mechanisms a high level of SOC is formed [9,10]. In order to increase our understanding about the paths to a high SOC in the stage of adulthood, we focused on exploring the role both of temperament, as it has been confirmed as a potential component in the development of more complex traits that emerge later in life [11], and of emotional intelligence (EI) as it has been found to increase SOC [12].
Temperament has been defined as a set of individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation, affected by heredity, maturity, and experience [13]. Conceptual and empirical work highlight that temperament traits regulate numerous areas of everyday functioning [14]. For example, the sociability and activity subscales of temperament are positively correlated with the sense of coherence construct, while impulsive sensation-seeking, neuroticism–anxiety, and aggression–hostility are negatively associated with SOC [15]. Negative significant correlations are also found between the degree of depressiveness and SOC [16].
The EI construct has been broadly conceptualized as an individual manifestation of adaptive emotional functioning that encompasses the ability to perceive, comprehend, and regulate emotions in the self and in others [17,18,19]. Higher levels of EI, understood as a trait and as an ability, have been found to be associated with indices of well-being [19,20,21] and life satisfaction [22,23,24], as well as higher scores for empathy [25], subjective happiness [22], psychological resilience [26], self-control [27], self-efficacy [28], and sociability [27].
Despite the many studies conducted with respect to temperament, not much attention has been paid to its role in the development of SOC. Although, Fried et al. [29] revealed a negative correlation between a difficult temperament and SOC. Moreover, a temperamental predisposition to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) constituted a moderator variable of the relationship between SOC and PTSD [30]. Hutchinson and colleagues [31] found negative correlations between SOC and temperamental subscales of neuroticism–anxiety and aggression–hostility, and positive correlations between SOC and activity, and sociability. With regard to temperament and EI, Gardner et al. [32] observed moderate correlations between the trait of EI and three dimensions of temperament (effortful control, extraversion, and orienting sensitivity). Their findings also suggested that EI is largely a biological-based tendency as the overall model was significant for EI and more than 40% of the variance was accounted for by temperament. Finally, the intensity of an individual’s SOC may be determined [33] both by their genetic strengths (temperament) and emotional resources (emotional intelligence). Based on the literature review presented so far, we hypothesized that:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Depressive, cyclothymic, irritable, and anxious dimensions of temperament correlate negatively with SOC and EI, while hyperthymic temperament correlates positively with SOC and EI.
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
EI correlates positively with SOC.
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
EI mediates the effect of five dimensions of temperament on global SOC and its dimensions (comprehensibility, manageability, and meaningfulness).
Since our study is a cross-sectional design, it is crucial to provide a convincing rationale for the specified directions of causal effects [34,35] between temperament and SOC. In regard to temporal precedence, there is both theory-driven and data-driven evidence that SOC may be influenced by temperament. According to Antonovsky’s theory [1,2], although SOC originates from early childhood, it only becomes and remains relatively stable throughout adult life after the age of 30. Moreover, there is a general consensus that temperament has a broad impact on individual experience and interactions [36]. The regulative theory of temperament developed by Strelau [37] highlights the role of temperament in the development of personality and in the regulation of people’s relations with their environment. Empirical data [38] have shown that symptoms of anxiety and depression explain a great part of SOC variance in nonclinical and clinical samples.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The sample consisted of 173 participants between 18 and 49 (88% women). The average age was approx. 24 (M = 23.62; SD = 5.25). The biggest group was formed by studying respondents (61%), followed by studying and working (24%), and working (15%). In terms of place of residence, 24% of participants declared living in rural areas, 16% in a town with fewer than 50,000 inhabitants, 27% between 51,000 and 499,000, and 33% in a city with over 500,000 inhabitants.

2.2. Data Collection

The participants in the present study were recruited through Internet platforms, such as Facebook and Google. A general description of the research project included a link to the anonymous online survey. Prior to filling in the questionnaires, all of the respondents interested in taking part in the study were prompted with a web-based informed consent. Only after giving their agreement, the participants had access to the battery of questionnaires. They were not offered any rewards for participating. Respondents under the age of 18 were excluded from participation. The protocol was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology at the University of Szczecin and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.3. Assessment of the Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa and San Diego Autoquestionnaire (TEMPS-A)

The Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa and San Diego Autoquestionnaire (TEMPS-A) is a self-report measure designed to assess temperament in healthy subjects and psychiatric patients through the depressive, cyclothymic, hyperthymic, irritable, and anxious subscales [39,40]. Individuals with a depressive temperament tend toward rigid thinking, self-accusation, and shyness. Cyclothymic temperament refers to mood lability, superficial thinking, and intense emotions. People with a hyperthymic temperament characterize themselves with the highest number of “positive” qualities: optimistic view of life, sociability, and self-assurance. An irritable temperament implies higher energy and anger, a lower level of empathy, and dissatisfaction. An anxious temperament implies a tendency to worry, ruminate, and continuous tension. In the women’s version, the questionnaire contains 110 items (109 in the version for men) and shows substantial internal consistency of the scales. The TEMPS-A has a yes/no response format, and 20 items per scale. In the present study, the Cronbach alpha coefficients were as follows: total score (α = 0.91), depressive (α = 0.76), cyclothymic (α = 0.85), hyperthymic (α = 0.80), irritable (α = 0.84), and anxious (α = 0.88).

2.4. Assessment of the Orientation to Life Questionnaire (OLQ)

The Orientation to Life Questionnaire (OLQ), developed by Antonovsky [1], measures the sense of coherence and consists of three interrelated components. Comprehensibility refers to the degree to which individuals recognize internal and external stimuli as comprehensible, coherent, and clear [41]. Thus, this cognitive dimension of SOC helps in the management of stressful situations. Manageability implies the extent to which people dispose of resources that can be used to meet the necessities of the stimuli. It is the instrumental or behavioral dimension of SOC. Meaningfulness indicates the degree to which one believes that existence has meaning and that life challenges are opportunities and not only burdens. It is the motivational dimension of SOC. The Orientation to Life Questionnaire has 29 items and the responses are ranged on a seven-point Likert scale (from 1 = never to 7 = always). The higher SOC scores mean a stronger sense of coherence. Previous reported alpha coefficients varied between 0.85 and 0.91. In the current study, the Cronbach alpha coefficients were as follows: comprehensibility (α = 0.71), manageability (α = 0.79), meaningfulness (α = 0.80), and global orientation to life (α = 0.88).

2.5. Assessment of the Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (INTE)

The Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (INTE), a Polish version [42] of a questionnaire developed by Schutte et al. [17], is a well-known 33-item self-report measure of emotional intelligence. It is based on the Salovey and Mayer model: the appraisal and expression of emotion, the regulation of emotion, and the use of emotions in thinking and acting [43]. Respondents use a 5-point scale, where 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree, to designate to what extent each item illustrates their agreement. The scores are from 33 to 165. The higher the score, the higher the EI. The internal consistency in the original studies [17] showed an alpha of 0.90. In the present study, the Cronbach alpha coefficient was α = 0.88.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics software version 23 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). There were no missing values since the data were gathered through an Internet platform that did not allow respondents to proceed unless they answered all questions. Descriptive statistics of means, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis were computed with no violations of the normality assumption found on any of the variables.
The internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) was calculated for all instruments applied. The correlation analysis was done using Pearson’s r coefficient. Linear regression analyses were used to test the normality of residuals and constant variance (homoscedasticity), detect multicollinearity, and outliers, and examine if and how much sex, age, background, and place of residence, as potential confounders in the model, would distort the relationship between the exposure (temperament and its dimensions) and outcome (sense of coherence) variables.
The PROCESS macro for SPSS (version 3.2, Heinrich-Heine-Universität, Düsseldorf, Germany) was run to establish the extent to which temperament influences sense of coherence through emotional intelligence. Temperament was the independent variable and the factor of SOC was the dependent variable. EI was included as a mediating variable. Thus, there were 20 single-level no. 4 mediation models [44], involving three-variable systems (Figure 1). For the present analysis, the 95% confidence interval of the indirect effects was calculated using 5000 bootstrapped resamples. This method appears to be superior to traditional mediation analyses because it does not require the data to adhere to assumptions of normality [45].

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

The depressive, cyclothymic, hyperthymic, irritable, and anxious subscales of temperament, global SOC and its three dimensions, and global EI were screened for skewness and kurtosis to evaluate the normality of the scale’s distribution. We assumed values less than ±2 commonly considered acceptable as a normal distribution [45]. No variables exceeded the cutoffs of ±2 (Table 1).

3.2. Testing for the Normality of Residuals, Multicollinearity, and Confounders

In terms of the assumptions associated with the error term (residuals), homoscedasticity and autocorrelation were tested. A scatterplot of standardized residuals against the predicted values (Figure 1) shows that the plotted points had no obvious pattern nor systematic structure, being evenly divided above and below their mean value of zero [46]. Thus, the assumptions of normality, linearity, and homoscedasticity were met since the plotted values formed a rough rectangle.
The graphical presentation was confirmed by the Shapiro–Wilk test, which checks how closely residuals follow a normal distribution. The results of p = 0.95 for both unstandardized and standardized residuals suggested no deviations from normality. The Durbin–Watson statistic was 2.22, confirming that residuals were independent of each other, and indicating that the independence of the error terms was not a severe problem. In fact, values between 1.5 and 2.5 suggest that the assumption of non-autocorrelation was not violated [47].
Moreover, the regression model was tested to detect multicollinearity [48] because of correlations among all of the dimensions of temperament and the remaining variables. In the whole sample, the tolerance values ranged from 0.427 to 0.920, notably above the critical value of 0.1. The variance inflation factor (VIF) values ranged from 1.087 to 2.341 respectively, not exceeding the recommended threshold of 4.0 [49]. Both results suggest that multicollinearity was unlikely to be an issue in our study. The Mahalanobis distance procedure was evaluated, using Chi-squared distribution with a very conservative probability estimate for a case being an outlier (p < 0.001) [46]. Only four out of 173 cases were detected as possible multivariate outliers. Nonetheless, because the analyses run with and without the cases named as outliers showed that they did not have a large effect on the correlations, regressions, or mediations, and did not change the results [50], they were taken into consideration. With respect to age, sex, background, and place of residence as potentially confounding variables, they did not make a significant contribution to the model, as the change in the regression coefficient was very small between measurement with age (R2 = 0.689) and without (R2 = 0.674). However, in different equations and with different sets of independent or confounding variables, this outcome could potentially change.

3.3. Correlations between the Study Variables

In the next step, we verified the relationship between the dimensions of temperament, SOC, and EI (H1), and between SOC and EI (H2). Pearson’s r correlation (Table 2) showed a negative correlation between the depressive, cyclothymic, irritable, and anxious dimensions of temperament and SOC, and EI. Concomitantly, there was a positive correlation between hyperthymic temperament and SOC, and EI. Emotional intelligence correlated positively with a general sense of coherence and its three dimensions (H2).

3.4. Mediations

In the following part of the analyses, EI was introduced as a potential mediator which could weaken, strengthen or have no influence on the existing correlation between the independent variable (dimensions of temperament) and the dependent variable (dimensions of SOC) (Figure 2).
The PROCESS macro for SPSS showed (Table 3) that the c path (the direct effect) decreased after the introduction of EI and its dimensions in 20 out of 20 models (c’ path). On the basis of the obtained outcomes, it can be stated that emotional intelligence mediates the relationship between depressive, cyclothymic, hyperthymic, irritable, and anxious temperament, comprehensibility, manageability, meaningfulness, and global orientation to life.

4. Discussion

This research aimed at investigating the association between temperament, SOC, and EI (H1 and H2) as well as the possible mediatory effect of EI on the relationship between temperament and SOC (H3). Specifically, we expected a negative correlation between the depressive, cyclothymic, irritable, and anxious dimensions of temperament and SOC, and EI. We also assumed a positive correlation between hyperthymic temperament and SOC, and EI. Concurrently, we posited that EI would correlate positively with a general sense of coherence and its three dimensions, and would mediate the relationship between depressive, cyclothymic, hyperthymic, irritable and anxious temperament, comprehensibility, manageability, meaningfulness, and global orientation to life. The outcomes supported our three hypotheses and are in line with previous research.
Firstly, a negative correlation between depressive temperament and SOC confirmed the results obtained by Blom and colleagues [38], in which they observed an inverse association between generalized symptoms of depression and sense of coherence both in nonclinical and clinical samples. In the aforementioned study, depression, measured with Beck’s Depression Inventory, was also the strongest predictor of SOC. Similarly, Kurowska and Ciesielska [51] found that the level of depression grows with a lower level of sense of coherence. In respect to EI, our results refer to earlier studies. For example, in a recent study [52], it was noticed that adolescents and adults who exhibit lower levels of depression report higher EI as well. Correspondingly, individuals who display a higher ability to discriminate among feelings and regulate emotional states show less depression [53].
Since a close relationship between depression and anxiety has been acknowledged by many researchers [54], it is comprehensible that anxious temperament is negatively correlated with SOC. It has been established that people suffering from anxiety disorders have a lower level of sense of coherence [51]. Both before and after adjustment for potential confounders, a significant negative association between the presence of anxiety and SOC was observed [55]. Likewise, a longitudinal study by Carmel and Bernstein [56] showed strong and significant correlations between the A-trait and sense of coherence in three different measurements, although the associations became weaker over time. In terms of anxiety and EI, it is well documented that individuals with high self-reported emotional intelligence better tend to regulate the negative affect [53]. In the first reported investigation of the relationship between indices of social and performance anxiety and EI [57], the authors found high negative correlations between both types of anxious behavior and emotional intelligence.
Our study also showed positive correlations between hyperthymic temperament (characterized by optimism, sociability, self-esteem, resilience), SOC, and EI. The results confirm previous findings in which the propensity to perceive a greater sense of one’s own value played a positive role in the sense of coherence [58,59] and in EI, known as the ability to notice, understand, and manage emotions in oneself and others [60]. Moreover, since SOC is an inclusive concept [61] and has much in common with resilience, our results reflect the outcomes of Kesebir et al. [62] who observed a strong relationship between hyperthymic temperament and resilience both in depressive and healthy individuals.
With respect to a negative correlation between a cyclothymic temperament and SOC, there is some evidence [63] that mood lability seems to affect the development of a cohesive sense of self. In other studies [64], in the general population sample, two different measures of affective lability correlated negatively with identity integrity and the SOC total score, and comprehensibility, manageability, and meaningfulness subscales. Furthermore, affective instability was negatively associated with clarity of emotion, which refers to how clearly one recognizes and discriminates among one’s own emotions [65]. Since irritable temperament is conceptually linked to cyclothymic temperament [66], its negative correlation with SOC and EI is fully justified.
Finally, as we expected, H3 found its confirmation since in 20 out of 20 models, emotional intelligence acted as a mediator in the relationship between different dimensions of temperaments and the sense of coherence. These results are consistent with earlier studies [67] that reported a mediatory role of EI between some personality traits and positive resources. Such a mediatory effect may be because EI allows monitoring and management of one’s own emotions to guide one’s thinking and actions [68]. Previous researchers [69] have shown that EI mediated the relationship between emotional instability and happiness. Such outcomes have confirmed that awareness of internal sources of dissatisfaction might help people to overcome less functional tendencies that contribute to unhappiness. By analogy, successful affective information processing and management of emotions in the context of typical features of different types of temperaments might facilitate individuals in finding a sense of coherence in their life experiences.

5. Limitations

Our findings highlight the mediatory influence of emotional intelligence on the relationship between temperament and sense of coherence. However, a few limitations are important to note. The first limitation is that the cross-sectional nature of the data does not allow for the establishment of causal associations among the variables. Longitudinal and experimental studies would provide a more accurate representation of the relationships examined. Secondly, because of the unequal ratio of female to male respondents in this study, a cautious interpretation is suggested when applying the outcomes beyond the gender difference. We recommend that in future research, the same study should be performed with a greater participation of men. Thirdly, the sample was constrained to Internet users who could be unrepresentative of the population. Hence, researchers should investigate more diverse online groups. Fourthly, since the data were gathered through self-reports, they could be affected by the tendency to give responses that make the participant look good (socially desirable responding; SDR) [70]. Although the respondents were assured of anonymity, which is a common practice that allows bias to be reduced [71], the results obtained in our study could be affected by two response styles that include claiming socially desirable traits for the self (attribution responses) or disclaiming unwanted characteristics (denial responses) [72]. We suggest that in future studies, researchers concerned about social desirability bias might administer scales intended to screen each individual’s propensity toward SDR [70].

6. Conclusions

In terms of theoretical implications, the current research extends our insight into the interplay among temperament, EI, and SOC as the present study is one of few to have investigated this topic. Moreover, it provides meaningful evidence about the mediatory role of EI between temperament and SOC, shedding further light on how different types of temperament might affect global life orientation. The outcomes confirm Antonovsky’s theory that individuals’ SOC is in large part influenced by their general resources (temperament) and their ability to perceive, comprehend, and regulate emotions in the self (emotional intelligence). With respect to practical implications, the current findings suggest the importance of soft skills training (EI) for dealing with less functional temperamental tendencies in order to develop a deeper, clearer, and more coherent meaning of internal and external challenges.

Author Contributions

M.S. designed the conception of the work, prepared the statistical analysis, interpreted the results, and wrote the original draft. K.S. designed the conception of the work, carried out the research, and wrote the original draft. The data sets used during the current study are available from the corresponding author. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the study participants.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Antonovsky, A. Unraveling the Mystery of Health: How People Manage Stress and Stay Well; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  2. Eriksson, M.; Lindström, B. Validity of Antonovsky’s sense of coherence scale: A systematic review. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2005, 59, 460–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Nilsson, B.; Holmgren, L.; Westman, G. Sense of coherence in different stages of health and disease in northern Sweden: Gender ad psychosocial differences. Scand. J. Prim. Health Care 2000, 18, 14–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Thoma, M.V.; Mc Gee, S.L.; Fegert, J.M.; Glaesmer, H.; Brähler, E.; Maercker, A. Evaluation of the revised sense of coherence scale in a representative German sample. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0209550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kerksieck, P.; Büssing, A.; Frick, E.; Jacobs, C.; Baumann, K. Reduced sense of coherence due to neuroticism: Are transcendent beliefs protective among catholic pastoral workers? J. Relig. Health 2017, 56, 1956–1970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Czyżowska, D.; Gurba, E. Poczucie koherencji a religijność i sposób konstruowania tożsamości osobistej w okresie wyłaniającej się dorosłości. Psychol. Rozwoj. 2015, 20, 59–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Read, S.; Aunola, K.; Feldt, T.; Leinonen, R.; Ruoppila, I. The relationship between generalized resistance resources, sense of coherence, and health among Finnish people aged 65–69. Eur. Psychol. 2005, 10, 244–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Luyckx, K.; Schwartz, S.J.; Goossens, S. Employment, sense of coherence, and identity formation: Contextual and psychological processes on the pathway to sense of adulthood. J. Adolesc. Res. 2008, 23, 566–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Feldt, T.; Kokko, K.; Kinnunen, U.; Pulkkinen, L. The role of family background, school success, and career orientation in the development of sense of coherence. Eur. Psychol. 2005, 10, 298–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Slootjes, J.; Keuzenkamp, S.; Saharso, S. The mechanisms behind the formation of a strong Sense of Coherence (SOC): The role of migration and integration. Scand. J. Psychol. 2017, 58, 571–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Shiner, R.L.; Caspi, A. Temperament and the development of personality traits, adaptations and narratives. In Handbook of Temperament; Zentner, M., Shiner, R.L., Eds.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 497–516. [Google Scholar]
  12. Miturska, E. Predictors of therapy effectiveness for women subjected to violence. In Trauma Treatment: Factors Contributing to Efficiency; Widera-Wysoczańska, A., Ed.; Cambridge Scholars Publishing: Newcastle upon Tyne, UK, 2016; pp. 107–118. [Google Scholar]
  13. Derryberry, M.K.; Ahadi, S.A.; Evans, D.E. Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 78, 122–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bojanowska, A.; Zalewska, A.M. Happy temperament? Four types of stimulation control linked to four types to subjective well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 2017, 18, 1403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Hutchinson, A.K.; Stuart, A.D.; Pretorius, H.G. Biological contributions to well-being: The relationships amongst temperament, character strengths and resilience. SA J. Ind. Psychol. 2010, 36, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Erim, Y.; Morawa, E.; Atay, H.; Aygün, S.; Gökalp, P.; Senf, W. Sense of coherence and depression in the framework of immigration: Turkish patients in Germany and in Turkey. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2011, 23, 542–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Schutte, N.S.; Malouff, J.M.; Hall, L.E.; Haggerty, D.J.; Cooper, J.T.; Golden, C.J.; Dornheim, L. Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 1998, 25, 167–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Schutte, N.S.; Thorsteinsson, E.B.; Hine, D.W.; Foster, R.; Cauchi, A.; Binns, C. Experiential and rational processing styles, emotional intelligence and wellbeing. Aust. J. Psychol. 2010, 62, 14–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Schutte, N.S.; Malouff, J.M.; Simunek, M.; McKenley, J.; Hollander, S. Characteristic emotional intelligence and emotional well-being. Cogn. Emot. 2002, 16, 769–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Di Fabio, A.; Kenny, M.E. Promoting well-being: The contribution of emotional intelligence. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Schutte, N.S.; Malouff, J.M.; Bobik, C.; Coston, T.D.; Greeson, C.; Jedlicka, C.; Rhodes, E.; Wendorf, G. Emotional intelligence and interpersonal relations. J. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 141, 523–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Extremera Pacheco, N.; Rey, L.; Sánchez-Álvarez, N. Validation of the Spanish version of the Wong Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (WLEIS-S). Psicothema 2019, 31, 94–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Hajihasani, M.; Sim, T. Marital satisfaction among girls with early marriage in Iran: Emotional intelligence and religious orientation. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 2019, 24, 297–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Huang, N.T.; Lee, H.L. Ability emotional intelligence and life satisfaction: Humor style as a mediator. Soc. Behav. Pers. 2019, 47, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Mayer, J.D.; Caruso, D.R.; Salovey, P. Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for an intelligence. Intelligence 1999, 27, 267–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Armstrong, A.R.; Galligan, R.; Critchley, C.R. Emotional intelligence and psychological resilience to negative life events. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2011, 51, 331–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Feher, A.; Yan, G.; Saklfske, D.H.; Plouffe, R.A.; Gao, Y. An investigation of the psychometric properties of the Chinese Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire Short Form (Chinese TEIQue-SF). Front. Psychol. 2019, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Wu, Y.; Lian, K.; Hong, P.; Liu, S.; Lin, R.M.; Lian, R. Teachers’ emotional intelligence and self-efficacy: Mediating role of teaching performance. Soc. Behav. Pers. 2019, 43, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Fried, B.G.; Teichamn, M.; Rahav, G. Adolescent gambling: Temperament, sense of coherence and exposure to advertising. Addict. Res. Theory 2010, 18, 586–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Kaźmierczak, I.E.; Strelau, J.; Zawadzki, B. Search for a curvilinear relationship between the sense of coherence and the intensity of PTSD in MVA survivors. Anxiety Stress Coping 2012, 25, 443–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hutchinson, A.M.K.; Suart, A.D.; Pretorius, H.G. The relationships between temperament, character strengths, and resilience. In The Human Pursuit of Well-Being: A Cultural Approach; Brdar, I., Ed.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2011; pp. 133–144. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gardner, K.J.; Qualter, P.; Whiteley, H. Developmental correlates of emotional intelligence: Family environment, and childhood trauma. Aust. J. Psychol. 2011, 63, 75–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Griffiths, C.A.; Ryan, P.; Foster, J.H. Thematic analysis of Antonovsky’s sense of coherence theory. Scand. J. Psychol. 2011, 52, 168–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kline, R.B. Mediation analysis in leadership studies: New developments and perspectives. In Handbook of Methods in Leadership Research; Schyns, B., Hall, R.J., Neves, P., Eds.; Edward Elgar: Nrothampton, MA, USA, 2017; pp. 173–194. [Google Scholar]
  35. Kraemer, H.C.; Kiernan, M.; Essex, M.; Kupfer, D.J. How and why criteria defining moderators and mediators differ between the Byron & Kenny and MacArthur approaches. Health Psychol. 2008, 27, S101–S108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Blatný, M.; Milová, K.; Jelínek, M.; Osecka, T. Personality predicotrs of successful development: Toddler temperament and adolescent personality traits predict well-being and career stability in middle adulthood. PLoS ONE. 2015, 10, e0126032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Strelau, J. Temperament: A Psychological Perspective; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  38. Henje Blom, E.C.; Serlachius, E.; Larsson, J.O.; Theorell, T.; Ingvar, M. Low Sense of Coherence (SOC) is a mirror of general anxiety and persistent depressive symptoms in adolescent girls: A cross-sectional study of a clinical and a non-clinical cohort. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 2010, 10, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Akiskal, H.S.; Akiskal, K.K. Special issue: TEMPS: Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa, Paris and San Diego. J. Affect. Disord. 2005, 85, 1–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Dembińska-Krajewska, D.; Rybakowski, J. The Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa and San Diego Autoquestionnaire (TEMPS-A)—An important tool to study affective temperament. Psychiatr. Pol. 2014, 48, 261–276. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  41. Eriksson, M.; Mittelmark, M.B. The sense of coherence and its measurement. In Handbook of Salutogenesis; Mittelmrak, M.B., Sagy, S., Eriksson, M., Bauer, G.F., Pelikan, J.M., Lindström, B., Espnes, G.A., Eds.; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2017; pp. 97–106. [Google Scholar]
  42. Jaworowska, A.; Matczak, A. Kwestionariusz Inteligencji Emocjonalnej; Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych Polskiego Towarzystwa Psychologicznego: Warszawa, Poland, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  43. Szczygieł, D.; Jasielska, A.; Wytrykowska, A. Psychometric properties of the Polish version of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form. Pol. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 46, 447–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  45. George, D.; Mallery, P. IBM SPSS Statistics 23 Step by Step: A Simple Guide and Reference; Routledge: Oxon, MA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  46. Fidell, L.S.; Tabachnick, B.G. Preparatory data analysis. In Handbook of Psychology: Research Methods in Psychology; Schinka, J.A., Velicer, W.F., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2003; pp. 115–141. [Google Scholar]
  47. Hair, J.F.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.M.; Sarstedt, M. A Primer on Partial Less Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM); SAGE Publications: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  48. Kim, H.W.; Chan, H.C.; Gupta, S. Value-based adoption of mobile internet: An empirical investigation. Decis. Support Syst. 2007, 43, 111–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Dattalo, P. Analysis of Multiple Dependent Variables; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  50. Stevens, J.P. Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  51. Kurowska, K.; Ciesielska, L. Depression and a sense of coherence in case of people with anxiety disorders. Psychiatr. Psychol. Klin. 2013, 13, 40–49. [Google Scholar]
  52. Kim, Y.J.; Park, E.H.; Jon, D.I.; Jung, M.H.; Hong, N. Does emotional intelligence fluctuate within clinical depressive condition? Psychiatry Investig. 2017, 14, 532–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Fernández-Berrocal, P.; Alcaide, R.; Extremera, N.; Pizarro, D. The role of emotional intelligence in anxiety and depression among adolescents. Individ. Differ. Res. 2006, 4, 16–27. [Google Scholar]
  54. Hranov, L.G. Comorbid anxiety and depression: Illumination of a controversy. Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 2007, 113, 171–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Uchida, H.; Tsujino, D.; Muguruma, T.; Hino, N.; Sasaki, K.; Miyoshi, M.; Koyama, Y.; Hirao, K. Low sense of coherence is associated with anxiety among adults: Results based on data from all 47 prefectures of Japan. Compr. Psychiatry 2018, 86, 131–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Carmel, S.; Bernstein, J. Trait-anxiety and sense of coherence: A longitudinal study. Psychol. Rep. 1989, 65, 221–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Summerfeldt, L.J.; Kloosterman, P.H.; Antony, M.M.; Parker, J.D. Social anxiety, emotional intelligence, and interpersonal adjustment. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2006, 28, 57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Johnson, M. Approaching the salutogenesis of sense of coherence: The role of ‘active’ self-esteem and coping. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2004, 9, 419–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Moksnes, U.K.; Eilertsen, M.E.B.; Lazarewicz, M. The association between stress, self-esteem and depressive symptoms in adolescents. Scand. J. Psychol. 2016, 57, 22–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Kong, F.; Zhao, J.; You, X. Emotional intelligence and life satisfaction in Chinese University students: The mediating role of self-esteem and social support. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2012, 53, 1039–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Almedom, A.M. Resilience, hardiness, sense of coherence, and posttraumatic growth: All paths leading to “light at the end of the tunnel”. J. Loss Trauma 2005, 10, 253–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Kesebir, S.; Gündoğar, D.; Küçüksubaşi, Y.; Yaylacı, E.T. The relations between affective temperament and resilience in depression: A controlled study. J. Affect. Disord. 2013, 148, 352–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Bleiberg, E. Borderline disorders in children and adolescents: The concept, the diagnosis, and the controversies. Bull. Menn. Clin. 1994, 58, 169–196. [Google Scholar]
  64. Pollock, P.H.; Broadbent, M.; Clarke, S.; Dorrian, A.; Ryle, A. The Personality Structure Questionnaire (PSQ): A measure of the multiple self-states model of identity disturbance in cognitive analytic therapy. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2001, 8, 59–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Thompson, R.J.; Dizén, M.; Berenbaum, H. The unique relations between emotional awareness and facets of affective instability. J. Res. Pers. 2009, 43, 875–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Akiskal, H.S.; Mallya, G. Criteria for the “soft” bipolar spectrum: Treatment implications. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 1987, 23, 68–73. [Google Scholar]
  67. Di Fabio, A.; Palazzeschi, L.; Bucci, O.; Guazzini, A.; Burgassi, C.; Pesce, E. Personality traits and positive resources of workers for sustainable development: Is emotional intelligence a mediator for optimism and hope? Sustainability 2018, 10, 3422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Mayer, J.D.; Salovey, P. The intelligence of emotional intelligence. Intelligence 1993, 17, 433–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Hafen, C.A.; Singh, K.; Laursen, B. The happy personality in India: The role of emotional intelligence. J. Happiness Stud. 2011, 12, 807–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Paulhus, D.L. Measurement and control of response bias. In Measures of Social Psychological Attitudes; Robinson, J.P., Shaver, P.R., Wrightsman, L.S., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1991; pp. 17–59. [Google Scholar]
  71. Woszczynski, A.B.; Whitman, M.E. The problem of common method variance in IS research. In The Handbook of Information Systems Research; Whitman, M.E., Woszczynski, A.B., Eds.; Idea Group Publishing: Hershey, PA, USA, 2004; pp. 66–78. [Google Scholar]
  72. Paulhus, D.L. Two-component models of socially desirable responding. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1984, 46, 598–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The scatterplot of the standardized residuals against standardized predicted values.
Figure 1. The scatterplot of the standardized residuals against standardized predicted values.
Ijerph 17 00219 g001
Figure 2. Theoretical model of the role of the mediator in the relationship between dimensions of temperament and dimensions of SOC; ** p < 0.01.
Figure 2. Theoretical model of the role of the mediator in the relationship between dimensions of temperament and dimensions of SOC; ** p < 0.01.
Ijerph 17 00219 g002
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the Assessment of the Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa and San Diego Autoquestionnaire (TEMPS-A), Orientation to Life Questionnaire (OLQ), and Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (INTE) (n = 173).
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the Assessment of the Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa and San Diego Autoquestionnaire (TEMPS-A), Orientation to Life Questionnaire (OLQ), and Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (INTE) (n = 173).
ScalesMSDSkewnessKurtosisα
1. DE0.460.190.28−0.180.75
2. CY0.470.240.06−0.880.85
3. HY0.450.200.01−0.440.78
4. IR0.310.210.62−0.310.82
5. AN0.420.230.14−0.780.86
6. CO32.638.550.480.640.71
7. MA50.299.600.01−0.600.79
8. ME38.029.33−0.36−0.300.80
9. OL121.2222.510.03−0.290.88
10. EI123.0316.82−0.590.680.88
Abbreviations: depressive temperament (DE); cyclothymic temperament (CY); hyperthymic temperament (HY); irritable temperament (IR); anxious temperament (AN); comprehensibility (CO); manageability (MA); meaningfulness (ME); global orientation to life (OL); emotional intelligence (EI).
Table 2. Correlation between dimensions of temperament, dimensions of sense of coherence (SOC), and EI (N = 173).
Table 2. Correlation between dimensions of temperament, dimensions of sense of coherence (SOC), and EI (N = 173).
Dimensions of TemperamentCOMAMEOLEI
Depressive−0.43 ***−0.64 ***−0.54 ***−0.66 ***−0.32 ***
Cyclothymic−0.49 ***−0.56 ***−0.46 ***−0.62 ***−0.29 ***
Hyperthymic0.34 ***0.41 ***0.53 ***0.53 ***0.39 ***
Irritable−0.33 ***−0.54 ***−0.40 ***−0.52 ***−0.33 ***
Anxious−0.50 ***−0.58 ***−0.31 ***−0.57 ***−0.19 *
EI0.30 ***0.39 ***0.53 ***0.50 ***
* p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001; comprehensibility (CO); manageability (MA); meaningfulness (ME); global orientation to life (OL); emotional intelligence (EI).
Table 3. The role of emotional intelligence in the relationship between dimensions of temperament and sense of coherence.
Table 3. The role of emotional intelligence in the relationship between dimensions of temperament and sense of coherence.
a Pathb Pathc Pathc’ PathIndirect EffectB(SE)Lower CIUpper CI
1. DE–EI–CO−28.18 ***0.42 ***−76.95 ***−64.83 ***−12.11604.2749−21.3393−4.9238
2. DE–EI–MA−28.18 ***0.11 ***−31.99 ***−28.71 ***−3.28241.2654−5.9397−1.0964
3. DE–EI–ME−28.18 ***0.22 ***−25.93 ***−19.70 ***−6.23712.3791−11.6542−2.3373
4. DE–EI–OL−28.18 ***0.42 ***−76.95 ***−64.83 ***−12.11604.2951−21.6365−4.8290
5. CY–EI–CO−20.67 ***0.08−17.44 ***−15.65 ***−1.79050.9721−3.9663−0.2523
6. CY–EI–MA−20.67 ***0.14 ***−22.21 ***−19.29 ***−2.92701.0793−5.2600−1.1168
7. CY–EI–ME−20.67 ***0.23 ***−17.88 ***−12.92 ***−4.95961.6969−8.6323−2.0151
8. CY–EI–OL−20.67 ***0.46 ***−57.55 ***−47.87 ***−9.67713.3532−17.1733−4.0022
9. HY–EI–CO32.45 ***0.10 (ni)14.39***11.14 ***3.25091.63950.16106.4865
10. HY–EI–MA32.45 ***0.15 ***19.36***14.31 ***5.04531.62072.23678.5635
11. HY–EI–ME32.45 ***0.21 ***24.29***17.43 ***6.85712.31513.007011.8999
12. HY–EI–OL32.45 ***0.51 ***61.55***42.22 ***19.33454.004211.636927.2898
13. IR–EI–CO−25.55 ***0.10 **−13.08 ***−10.28 ***−2.80291.3590−6.0434−0.6856
14. IR–EI–MA−25.55 ***0.13 ***−23.89 ***−20.40 ***−3.49291.2400−6.2208−1.4522
15. IR–EI–ME−25.55 ***0.24 ***−17.43 ***−11.10 ***−6.32842.0159−10.7930−2.9083
16. IR–EI–OL−25.55 ***0.49 ***−54.42 ***−41.79 ***−12.62424.0136−21.2732−5.8575
17. AN–EI–CO−14.23 **0.10 **−18.61 ***−17.08 ***−1.52610.9311−3.6849−0.0765
18. AN–EI–MA−14.23 **0.16 ***−23.87 ***−21.51 ***−2.35861.1373−4.6953−0.2421
19. AN–EI–ME−14.23 **0.27 ***−12.48 ***−8.60 **−3.87102.0551−8.4428−0.3687
20. AN–EI–OL−14.23 **0.54 ***−54.97 ***−47.21 ***−7.75573.8812−16.0240−0.7991
** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001; confidence interval (CI); nonsignificant (ni); depressive temperament (DE); cyclothymic temperament (CY); hyperthymic temperament (HY); irritable temperament (IR); anxious temperament (AN); comprehensibility (CO); manageability (MA); meaningfulness (ME); global orientation to life (OL); emotional intelligence (EI).

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Szcześniak, M.; Strochalska, K. Temperament and Sense of Coherence: Emotional Intelligence as a Mediator. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 219. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17010219

AMA Style

Szcześniak M, Strochalska K. Temperament and Sense of Coherence: Emotional Intelligence as a Mediator. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(1):219. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17010219

Chicago/Turabian Style

Szcześniak, Małgorzata, and Klaudia Strochalska. 2020. "Temperament and Sense of Coherence: Emotional Intelligence as a Mediator" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 1: 219. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17010219

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop