Chess Practice as a Protective Factor in Dementia
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- -
- Prevention: “Disease prevention is a procedure through which individuals, particularly those with risk factors for a disease, are treated in order to prevent a disease from occurring” [2]. In this case, the disease has not started or at least has not been detected yet.
- -
- Early diagnosis: “Methods to determine in patients the nature of a disease or disorder at its early stage of progression. Generally, early diagnosis improves prognosis and treatment outcome” [3].
- -
- Protective factor: “An aspect of personal behavior or lifestyle, environmental exposure, or inborn or inherited characteristic, which, on the basis of epidemiologic evidence, is known to be associated with prevention or mitigation of a health-related condition considered important to prevent” [4]. This concept has a narrow relation with the previous ones and, therefore, we are assuming that a protective factor is an aspect that could appear either linked with prevention or even when the diagnosis has been established (enhancing the mitigation of such condition) and also in early detection.
Aim
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Design
2.2. Literature Search
2.3. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
2.4. Data Extraction
3. Results
3.1. High Mental Activities such as Chess to Prevent Dementia
3.2. Chess to Postpone the Development of Dementia
3.3. Cultivating Intellectual Activities since Youth Seems to Protect against Dementia
3.4. Leisure Activities such as Chess or Similar Ones and Cognitive Decline
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- World Health Organization. Available online: http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs362/es/ (accessed on 1 October 2016).
- Springer Nature. Disease Prevention-Nature. Available online: https://www.nature.com/subjects/disease-prevention (accessed on 26 February 2019).
- Medline. Early Diagnosis. MeSH–NCBI. Available online: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/mesh/68042241 (accessed on 26 February 2019).
- Medline. Protective Factor. MeSH-NCBI. Available online: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/mesh/68042241 (accessed on 26 February 2019).
- Coyle, J.T. Use it or lose it—Do effortful mental activities protect against dementia? N. Engl. J. Med. 2003, 348, 2489–2490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kienja, A.; Frydecka, D.; Adamowski, T.; Bickel, H.; Reynish, E.; Prince, M. Epidemiological studies of cognitive impairment and dementia across Eastern and Middle European countries (epidemiology of dementia in Eastern and Middle European Countries). Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2013, 26, 111–117. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2012, 11, 1006–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Armstrong, R.; Hall, B.J.; Doyle, J.; Waters, E. Scoping the scope of a Cochrane review. J. Public Health 2011, 33, 147–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Colquhoun, H.L.; Levac, D.; O’Brien, K.K.; Straus, S.; Tricco, A.C.; Perrier, L.; Kastner, M.; Moher, D. Scoping reviews: Time for clarity in definition, methods, and reporting. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2014, 67, 1291–1294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scarmeas, N.; Levy, G.; Tang, M.X.; Manly, J.; Stern, Y. Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. 2001, 57, 2236–2242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fabrigoule, C.; Letteneur, L.; Dartigues, J.F.; Zarrouk, M.; Commenges, D.; Barber-Gateau, P. Social and leisure activities and risk of dementia: A prospective longitudinal study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 1995, 43, 485–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dowd, S.B.; Davidhizar, R. Can mental and physical activities such as chess and gardening help in the prevention and treatment of Alzheimer’s. J. Pract. Nurs. 2003, 53, 11–13. [Google Scholar]
- Derby, C.A.; Katz, M.J.; Lipton, R.B.; Hall, C.B. Trends in dementia incidence in a birth cohort analysis of the Einstein Aging Study. JAMA Neurol. 2017, 74, 1345–1351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, R.S.; Scherr, P.A.; Schneider, J.A.; Tang, Y.; Bennet, D.A. Relation of cognitive activity to risk of developing Alzheimer disease. J. Neurol. 2007, 69, 1911–1920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spector, A.; Woods, B.; Orrell, M. Cognitive stimulation for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Exp. Rev. Neurother. 2008, 8, 751–757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dartigues, J.F.; Foubert-Samier, A.; Le-Goff, M.; Vittard, M.; Amieva, H.; Orgogozo, J.M. Playing board games, cognitive decline and dementia: A French population-based cohort study. BMJ Open 2013, 3, e002998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Selkoe, D.J. Translating cell biology into therapeutic advances in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Nat. 1999, 399, 23–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friedland, R.P.; Fritsch, T.; Smyth, K.A.; Koss, E.; Lerner, A.J.; Chen, C.H. Patients with Alzheimer’s disease have reduced activities in midlife compared with healthy control-group members. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 3440–3445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Archer, H.A.; Schott, J.M.; Barnes, J.; Fox, N.C.; Holton, J.L.; Revesz, T.; Cipolotti, L. Knight’s move thinking? Mild cognitive impairment in a chess player. Neurocase 2006, 11, 26–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Li, K.; Juang, J.; Qiu, L.; Yang, X.; Huang, X.; Lui, S. A multimodal MRI dataset of professional chess players. Sci. Data 2009, 2, 150044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lin, Q.; Cao, Y.; Gao, J. The impacts of a GO-game (Chinese chess) intervention on Alzheimer disease in a Northeast Chinese population. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2015, 7, 163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lee, B.; Park, Y.; Jung, W.H.; Kim, H.S.; Oh, J.S.; Choi, C.H. White matter neuroplastic changes in long-term trained players of the game of “Baduk” (GO): A voxel-based diffusion-tensor imaging study. Neuroimage 2010, 52, 9–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gatz, M. Educating the brain to avoid dementia: Can mental exercise prevent Alzheimer’s disease? PLoS Med. 2005, 2, e7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, R.S.; Mendes-de-Leon, C.F.; Barnes, L.L.; Schneider, J.A.; Bienias, J.L.; Evans, D.A. Participation in cognitively stimulation activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA 2002, 287, 742–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fabrigoule, C. Do leisure activities protect against Alzheimer’s disease? Neurology 2002, 1, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, H.X.; Xu, W.; Pei, J.J. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. BBA 2012, 1822, 482–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Blazer, D.G. Brain-stimulating habits linked to lower Alzheimer’s protein levels. Duke Med. Health News 2012, 18, 3. [Google Scholar]
- Akbaraly, T.N.; Portet, F.; Fustinoni, S.; Dartigues, J.F.; Artero, S. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. Neurology 2009, 73, 854–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Helzner, E.; Scarmeas, N.; Cosentino, S.; Portet, F.; Stern, Y. Leisure activity and cognitive decline in incident Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 2007, 64, 1749–1754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fritsch, T.; Smyth, K.A.; Debanne, S.M.; Petot, G.J.; Friedland, R.P. Participation in novelty-seeking leisure activities and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 2005, 18, 134–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dowd, S.B.; Root, A. What chess has given us? Acad. Erch. Quart. 2003, 7, 1. [Google Scholar]
- Chiu, Y.C.; Huang, C.Y.; Kolanowski, A.M.; Huang, H.L.; Shyu, Y.; Lee, S.H.; Lin, C.R.; Hsu, W.C. The effects of participation in leisure activities on neuropsychiatric symptoms of persons with cognitive impairment: A cross-sectional study. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 2013, 50, 1314–1325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gilbert, P.; McEwan, K.; Catarino, F.; Baiao, R.; Palmeira, L. Fears and happiness and compassion in relationship with depression, alexithymia and attachment security in a depressed sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2014, 53, 228–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, L.; Chang, H.J.; Yeh, H.; Hou, C.J.Y.; Tsai, C.H.; Tsai, J.P. Factors associated with leisure participation among the elderly living in longterm care facilities. Int. J. Gerontol. 2010, 4, 69–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ball, Q.; Berch, D.B.; Helmers, K.F.; Jobe, J.B.; Leveck, M.D. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomize controlled trial. JAMA 2002, 288, 2271–2281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stern, C.; Munn, Z. Cognitive leisure activities and their role in preventing dementia: A systematic review. Int. J. Evid. Based Health 2010, 8, 2–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Verghese, J.; Lipton, R.B.; Katz, M.J.; Hall, C.B.; Derby, C.A.; Kulansky, G. Leisure activities and the risk off dementia in the elderly. N. Engl. J. Med. 2003, 348, 2508–2516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rajan, K.B.; Wilson, R.S.; Weuve, J.; Barnes, L.L.; Evans, D.A. Cognitive impairment 18 years before clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease dementia. Neurology 2015, 85, 898–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Database | Search Equation | Articles Found | Articles Selected |
---|---|---|---|
PubMed | Dementia AND chess | 6 | 4 |
Alzheimer disease OR dementia AND chess OR chess game AND protective factor | 2 | 2 | |
Chess AND dementia OR Alzheimer’s disease AND memory AND perception AND attention | 216 | 4 | |
Chess OR chess game AND dementia OR Alzheimer’s disease | 123 | 4 | |
Scopus | Dementia AND chess | 5 | 3 |
Dementia AND chess AND Alzheimer’s disease | 2 | 2 | |
Dementia OR Alzheimer’s disease AND chess OR chess game | 4 | 0 | |
Chess AND dementia OR Alzheimer’s disease AND memory AND perception AND attention | 0 | 0 | |
Chess AND dementia OR Alzheimer’s disease | 15 | 2 | |
CINALH | Chess AND dementia AND Alzheimer’s disease | 28 | 0 |
Google Academic | Chess AND dementia | 0 | 0 |
Chess AND dementia | 4 | 0 | |
LILACS | Chess AND dementia | 8 | 0 |
CUIDEN | Ajedrez AND demencia | 0 | 0 |
ARTICLE | DESIGN | SAMPLE | OBJECTIVES | RESULTS |
---|---|---|---|---|
[10] | Cohort study. | 1772 old people. New York. | To determinate whether leisure activities modify the risk for the incident of dementia. | The risk of dementia decreased in subjects with high leisure activities (RR: 0.62; 95% CI: 0.46 to 0.83). |
[11] | Cohort study. | 2040 old people. France. | To study the relationship between social and leisure activities, and risk of subsequent dementia in older community residents. | All but one of the social and leisure activities noted were significantly associated with a lower risk of dementia. Travelling (RR: 0.48, 95% CI: 0.24–0.94), odd Jobs or knitting (RR: 0.46, 95% CI: 0.26–0.85), and gardening (RR: 0.53, 95% CI: 0.28–0.99) remained significant. |
[12] | Literature review. | - | To review studies on AD and mental activity to see whether the second can be helpful in preventing the first. | It appears that engaging the elderly in activities that use the brain can be helpful in stemming the epidemic that is AD. |
[13] | Cohort study. | 1348 participants. New York. | To examine trends in dementia incidence and concomitant trends in cardiovascular comorbidities among individuals aged 70 years or older, who were enrolled in the Einstein Aging Study between 1993 and 2015. | 150 incidents of dementia cases developed during 5932 person-years. Dementia incidence decreased in successive birth cohorts. Incidence per 100 person-years was 5.09 in birth cohorts before 1920, 3.11 in the 1920 through 1924 birth cohorts, 1.73 in the 1925 through 1929 birth cohorts, and 0.23 in cohorts born after 1929. |
[14] | Longitudinal cohort study. | 700 old people. | To examine the relationship between cognitive activity and development of AD. | More frequent participation in cognitive activity was associated with reduced incidence of AD (HD: 0.58; 95% CI: 0.44, 0.77). |
[15] | Perspective. | - | To describe cognitive stimulation therapy development and evaluation, its use in clinical setting and issues for future investigation. | - |
[16] | Prospective population-based study. | 3675 old people. France. | To study the relationship between board game playing and risk of subsequent dementia in the Paquid cohort. | The risk of dementia was 15% lower in board game players compared to non-players (HR: 0.85, 95% CI: 0.74 to 0.99; p = 0.04). |
[17] | Review. | 1 case 100 studies | To review and attempt to integrate the disparate elements of the disease into a coherent whole, perhaps helping to focus future investigative efforts on developing rational treatments. | An integrated diagnostic and therapeutic approach to this complex and tragic disorder may still seem remote, the current rate of scientific progress indicates that some level of practical success may come sooner than one might think. |
[18] | Case-control study | 193 case people and 358 control people | To evaluated the relationships between non-occupational activities and AD in a case-control study. | The odds ratio for AD in those performing less than the mean value of activities was 3.85 (95% CI: 2.65–5.58, p < 0.001). |
[19] | N = 1 study | 1 case | Report a case of a chess player who presents cognitive disorders. | Cognitive function appeared and AD was diagnosticated. |
[20] | Parallel group. | 29 participants. | Report dates to provide research with new materials to further explore the human brain. | - |
[21] | Case-control study. | 147 old people. China. | To explore the functions of GO game in AD patients. | GO game intervention ameliorates AD manifestations. |
[22] | Case-control study. | 16 experienced Baduk players. Korea. | To elucidate the brain’s structural development as it relates to the cognitive components needed to play Baduk, based on White matter neuroplastic changes. | Long-term trained Baduk players developed larger regions of white matter with increased fractional anisotropy values in different areas that are related to attention control, working memory, executive regulation, and problem solving. |
[23] | Literature review. | - | To determine what literature states about mental practice as a protective factor against dementia and AD. | There is no convincing evidence of memory practice and other cognitively stimulating activities be sufficient to prevent AD. |
[24] | Longitudinal cohort study. | 801 old people. USA. | To test the hypothesis that frequent participation in cognitive activities is associated with a reduced risk of AD. | During an average of 4.5 years of follow-up, 111 persons developed AD. In a proportional hazards model that controlled for age, sex, and education, a 1-point increase in cognitive activity score was associated with a 33% reduction in risk of AD (HR: 0.67; 95% CI: 0.49–0.92) |
[25] | Opinion article. | - | - | - |
[26] | Systematic review. | 55 studies. | To present evidence from observational studies concerning the impact of leisure activities on the risk of dementia and cognitive decline and provide evidence from intervention studies on the topic. | A protective effect of mental activity on cognitive function has been consistently reported in both observational and interventional studies. The association of mental activity with the risk of dementia was robust in observational studies but inconsistent in clinical trials. Current evidence concerning the beneficial effect of other types of leisure activities on the risk of dementia is still limited and results are inconsistent. |
[27] | Divulgative study review. | - | - | - |
[28] | Cohort study- | 5.698 old people. France. | To examine the association between leisure activities and risk of incident dementia and its subtypes. | Stimulating leisure activities were found to be significantly associated with a reduced risk of dementia (HR: 0.49, 95% CI: 0.31; 0.79) and AD (HR: 0.39, 95% CI: 0.21; 0.721). |
[29] | Cohort study. | 283 old people. New York. | To determine whether pre-diagnosis leisure activities modify the rate of cognitive decline in patients with AD. | Each leisure activity was associated with an additional yearly decline of 0.005 of a z-score unit in cognitive score (p = 0.17). |
[30] | Case-control Study. | 264 cases and 545 controls (809 participants). Ohio. | To study the association between participation in different types of mentally stimulating leisure activities and status as AD case or normal control. | Logistic regression analysis indicating that adjusting for control variables, greater participation in novelty-seeking, and exchange-of-ideas activities were significantly associated with decreased odds of AD. |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lillo-Crespo, M.; Forner-Ruiz, M.; Riquelme-Galindo, J.; Ruiz-Fernández, D.; García-Sanjuan, S. Chess Practice as a Protective Factor in Dementia. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2116. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122116
Lillo-Crespo M, Forner-Ruiz M, Riquelme-Galindo J, Ruiz-Fernández D, García-Sanjuan S. Chess Practice as a Protective Factor in Dementia. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019; 16(12):2116. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122116
Chicago/Turabian StyleLillo-Crespo, Manuel, Mar Forner-Ruiz, Jorge Riquelme-Galindo, Daniel Ruiz-Fernández, and Sofía García-Sanjuan. 2019. "Chess Practice as a Protective Factor in Dementia" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16, no. 12: 2116. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122116
APA StyleLillo-Crespo, M., Forner-Ruiz, M., Riquelme-Galindo, J., Ruiz-Fernández, D., & García-Sanjuan, S. (2019). Chess Practice as a Protective Factor in Dementia. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(12), 2116. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122116