Next Article in Journal
The Role of Psychosocial Stress on a Family-Based Treatment for Adolescents with Problematic Behaviors
Previous Article in Journal
Loneliness at Universities: Determinants of Emotional and Social Loneliness among Students
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

An Explanatory Model for the Relationship between Motivation in Sport, Victimization, and Video Game Use in Schoolchildren

1
Department of Education, University of Almería, 04120 Almería, Spain
2
Department of Integrated Didactics, University of Huelva, 21007 Huelva, Spain
3
Department of Didactics of Musical, Plastic and Corporal Expression, University of Granada, 18071 Granada, Spain
4
Kinesiology School, University Santo Tomas, 837003 Santiago, Chile
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15(9), 1866; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15091866
Submission received: 4 August 2018 / Revised: 23 August 2018 / Accepted: 24 August 2018 / Published: 29 August 2018

Abstract

:
(1) Background: Society is changing amazingly fast, and this is bringing about changes in the way that people spend their free time. In the 21st century, free time is increasingly spent using technological devices such as video games, thus increasing levels of sedentariness. The aim of the present study was to define an explanatory model for the problematic use of video games, physical activity, motivational climate in sports, and victimization in schoolchildren, and to analyze the relationships between these variables according to gender; (2) Methods: A total of 734 schoolchildren, of both sexes, participated in this research study. They were aged from 10 to 12 and lived in the province of Granada (Spain). The main instruments used were the questionnaires PMCSQ-2, PAQ-C, QERV, and SVS. A multigroup structural equation model was used, which had an excellent fit (χ2 = 319.472; df = 72; p < 0.001; CFI = 0.962; NFI = 0.952; IFI = 0.962; RMSEA = 0.048); (3) Results: The practice of physical activity was related negatively and indirectly to the problematic use of video games ((r = −0.085, boys); (r = −0.081, girls)), and this in turn was related positively and directly to victimization ((r = 0.094, boys); (r = 0.174, girls)). Additionally, task climate was inversely related to the problematic use of video games for girls (r = −0.133), and ego climate was directly related to the use of these devices only with regard to boys (r = 0.250). (4) Conclusions: It must be noted that schoolchildren’s pathological use of video games is closely related to lower levels of physical activity. In addition, those motivational climates in sports that are oriented towards performance exacerbate this pathological behavior, which accentuates the importance of promoting motivational climates that are oriented towards tasks in schoolchildren.

1. Introduction

Society is changing at an unprecedented rate, which is affecting the way in which people spend their free time. The youngest in society were born in the middle of the “technological era”, their growing up coinciding with an unstoppable surge in technological advancement and the development of electronic entertainment devices never seen before [1,2]. Therefore, in the 21st century, society is facing the problem of the pathological use of digital entertainment devices, which is especially prevalent in the youngest sectors of the population, such as schoolchildren [3].
Authors such as Chacón-Cuberos et al. [4] or Verheijen et al. [5], indicate that parents have little control over their children’s use of this kind of technology, due to its rapid spread throughout society. The problematic use of video games in schoolchildren is also a risk factor for diverse health problems at the cognitive, social, and physical levels [6,7,8]. A great deal of research has analyzed the negative consequences of the problematic use of video games. Some of these are physical, such as hormonal changes and various eye problems; some are social, such as the deterioration and loss of socio-emotional skills; and others are cognitive, such as increasing anxiety, which can lead to depression in serious cases [9,10]. In this regard, a great deal of research has analyzed the negative behavioral consequences that stem from the abuse of this kind of technology, revealing a reduction in physical activity that leads to a rise in sedentary lifestyles and obesity [11], all of which are associated with poorer health in general [12]. Another of the negative consequences of video games abuse is increased aggressiveness, a direct link having been identified between these two factors [13,14]. This is attributed to the violent nature of many video games, whose use is considered a risk factor for violent behavior [15,16].
In this sense, schoolchildren are very easily influenced and readily modify their behavior patterns. Video games are seen as the ultimate audiovisual medium, but they disseminate imitable negative behaviors that adversely affect anyone who has not undergone an appropriate socialization process [17,18]. By playing video games with a high content of aggressiveness, young people come to associate violence with positive emotions such as the enjoyment the game provides, which results in the young person becoming desensitized to such violent behavior [19,20]. For that reason, as indicated by Ditrrick et al. [21] and Kimmig et al. [22], constant use of these kind of video games by young people causes a fall in their levels of anxiety and fear in the face of violent situations, which in some cases causes them to lose their ability to feel empathy for victims and to identify instead with the aggressors, which in turn may bring about a greater tolerance of aggression and even engender violent behavior towards peers.
According to this basis, recent studies such as those by Boxer et al. [23] or DeCamp & Ferguson [24] connect the abuse of screen-based entertainment such as video games and TV with an increase in victimization, a deterioration of academic performance, and the proliferation of symptoms related to depression that can lead to intrafamilial violence. In the face of all this, physical activity becomes a fundamental tool for counteracting the spread of unhealthy habits induced by greater sedentariness related to the use of video games. It can also prevent addiction problems; perhaps even the use of harmful substances such as alcohol, tobacco and other drugs [25,26,27].
For these reasons, studying the process of motivating schoolchildren to take up physical activity is considered essential, given that this is ultimately what will determine whether they continue with sporting activities, or either abandon them or replace them with sedentary habits related to the use of digital entertainment and the consumption of harmful substances [28]. The study of motivation towards sport is vital since, depending on the type of sport practiced, it can act both as a risk factor and as a factor preventing the adoption of unhealthy behaviors [29,30]. With regard to the study of motivation, in the context of physical activity and sport, the Achievement Goal Theory constitutes one of the most popular theoretical frameworks. This theory is based on the fundamental idea that motivation towards the practice of physical activity depends on the objectives that the person wants to achieve, which are known as achievement goals [31,32].
On the basis of their work on motivational orientations, authors such as Lochbaum et al. [33] indicate the existence of two different motivational climates: one is oriented towards the task and is characterized by more self-determined or intrinsic motivations, where the person practices physical activity for pleasure or fun, which promotes greater adherence to the practice of sport; the other is oriented towards ego and is characterized by a less self-determined or extrinsic type of motivation, usually associated with competition and external rewards, and thus promoting rivalry between peers because of their fear of failure, which is associated more with the abandonment of physical activity [34,35]. In this line, ego-oriented climates could be related to non-adaptive behaviors such as aggressiveness and the problematic use of video games, while task-oriented climates could be associated with adaptive behaviors and healthy habits.
Therefore, the present research study has the following objectives: (a) to define and contrast an explanatory model for the motivational climate, the practice of physical activity, the use of video games, and victimization among schoolchildren, and (b) to analyze the relationships between motivational climate, the practice of physical activity, the use of video games, and victimization by gender (boys/girls), by the means of a multigroup structural equation analysis.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects and Design

This descriptive and cross-sectional study was conducted on a sample of 734 schoolchildren, both boys and girls (45.2% boys and 54.8% girls), aged between 10 and 12 (M = 10.88; SD = 0.69), in fifth and sixth primary grade in the city of Granada. The sample was chosen by convenience sampling, with the requirement being that the participants be at the third stage of primary education. The children in the sample were from 11 eleven different schools in the city of Granada, and all the schools that agreed to participate were asked to do so on a voluntary basis. It should be pointed out that in order to avoid data duplication, the non-repetition of subjects was guaranteed by individualized monitoring during data collection.

2.2. Measures

Problematic use of video games: the instrument CERV (Questionnaire of Experiences Related to Video Games) originally developed by Chamarro et al. [36] was used. This instrument, which was validated on a sample of 7168 Spanish adolescents by Chamarro et al. [36], is made up of 17 negatively formulated items, graded by the means of a Likert scale with four options (1 = Never; 2 = Sometimes; 3 = Often; 4 = Always). The items are totaled up to describe the person’s behavior in relation to the use of video games, classifying them in tertiles to categorize the variables into: “No problems”, “Potential problems” and “Serious problems”. The reliability of the original instrument by Chamarro et al. [36] was α = 0.87, and in the present study it is α = 0.91.
Practice of physical activity: the questionnaire “Physical Activity Questionnaire for older Children (PAQ-C)” was used in its validated and adapted version in Spanish [37]. The questionnaire assesses moderate to vigorous physical activity practiced during the last seven days, by the means of 10 questions about the type and frequency of the activities carried out. The scores for each question ranged from 1 to 5, and calculation of the final score indicated a higher or lower frequency of the practice of physical activity. This instrument achieved a consistency of α = 0.77, which was acceptable and very similar to the value obtained by Kowalski et al. [37] in the original study (α = 0.79).
Motivational climate in sport (PMCSQ-2). This questionnaire derives from the original version by Newton et al. [38], adapted to Spanish by González-Cutre et al. [39]. It is made up of 33 items graded by means of a Likert scale with five options ranging from 1 = Totally Disagree to 5 = Totally Agree. The questionnaire establishes two categories: Task Climate, with its categories Cooperative Learning, Effort/Improvement and Important Role, and Ego Climate, with its categories Punishment for Mistakes, Unequal Recognition and Team Member Rivalry. The internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of the instrument obtained by González-Cutre et al. [39] in its Spanish version was α = 0.90 for Ego Climate (α = 0.77 for punishment for mistakes, α = 0.87 for unequal recognition, α = 0.61 for team member rivalry) and α = 0.84 for Task Climate (α = 0.65 for cooperative learning, α = 0.70 for effort/improvement, α = 0.70 for important role). The present research study obtained α = 0.89 for Ego Climate (α = 0.92 for punishment for mistakes, α = 0.91 for unequal recognition, α = 0.68 for member rivalry) and α = 0.93 for Task Climate (α = 0.83 for cooperative learning, α = 0.84 for effort/improvement, α = 0.86 for important role).
School Victimization Scale: This questionnaire was created by Mynard & Joseph [40] and adapted to Spanish by Cava et al. [41]. It is made up of 20 items graded by the means of a Likert scale (1 = Never; 4 = Always), addressing three types of victimization; Physical Victimization, Verbal Victimization, and Relational Victimization. In the original study, Mynard & Joseph [40] obtained an internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of α = 0.77. In this study, the coefficient was higher, at α = 0.93 (α = 0.88 for Relational Victimization; α = 0.86 for Physical Victimization; α = 0.84 for Verbal Victimization).

2.3. Procedure

First, through the Faculty of Education of the University of Granada, with the cooperation of the Department of Education of the regional government of Andalucia, a request for collaboration was made to the participating schools in the city of Granada, which had been selected by convenience sampling. These schools were informed of the nature of the research study, and collaboration of the students was requested. Secondly, a consent form was sent to the legal guardians of the teenagers requesting their informed consent, since the participants were minors.
The anonymity of the data collected was guaranteed throughout the process, it being made clear that the information would be used for scientific purposes only. The research team was constantly available in order to solve any doubts. The questionnaires were all completed without any problems or abnormalities to report. Finally, teachers, school counselors, and responsible staff were thanked for their collaboration and told that they would soon be sent a report on the data obtained, with confidentiality preserved at all times.
After data collection, a total of 52 questionnaires had to be rejected because they were not correctly completed. The present research study followed the guidelines set out in the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2008) for research projects, as well as the national legislation regulating clinical drug trials (Royal Decree 223/2004 of 6 February), biomedical research (Law 14/2007 of 3 July), and the protection of personal data (Organic Law 15/1999 of 13 December).

2.4. Data Analysis

The statistical software used was IBM SPSS® version 22.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA) for Windows, for the analysis of basic descriptors. The program IBM AMOS® 23 (IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA) was used for analysis of the relationships between constructs in the structural model. Once the theoretical model had been developed, a path analysis was carried out, considering matrix relationships derived from a multigroup analysis classifying participants by gender as a grouping variable. Therefore, two different structural models were established in order to check whether relationships between variables vary according to gender.
Path routes are created using 11 observable variables and three latent variables in order to determine indicators (Figure 1). These indicators give causal explanations of the latent variables by observing relationships between the indicators, considering the reliability of the measurements. Additionally, measurement errors are included in the observable variables in order to control them directly. One-way arrows are influence lines between latent and observable variables, and they are interpreted as multivariate regression coefficients. Two-way arrows show the relationship between latent variables, also representing the regression coefficients.
Motivational climate oriented towards task (TC) and motivational climate oriented towards ego (EC) act as exogenous variables, and each of them is inferred by three indicators. For task climate, the indicators are IR (Important Role), E/I (Effort/Improvement), and CL (Cooperative Learning). For ego climate, they are PM (Punishment for Mistakes), UR (Unequal Recognition), and MR (Team Member Rivalry). The use of video games (VIDEO GAMES) acts as an endogenous variable, receiving the effect of task climate (TC) and ego climate (EC). The practice of physical activity (PHYSICAL ACTIVITY) also acts as an endogenous variable, receiving the effect of task climate (TC), ego climate (EC), and the use of video games (VIDEO GAMES). Likewise, victimization (VICTIMIZATION) acts as an endogenous variable, receiving the effect of the use of video games (VIDEO GAMES), the practice of physical activity (PHYSICAL ACTIVITY), task climate (TC), and ego climate (EC).
The model fit was checked in order to verify its compatibility and the empirical information obtained. Fit reliability was based on the fit indexes established by Marsh [42]. In relation to Chi-square, non-significant values associated with p denote a good model fit. The Comparative Fit Index (CFI) will be acceptable if it is higher than 0.90, and excellent if it is higher than 0.95. The Normed Fit Index (NFI) will be acceptable if it is higher than 0.90. The Incremental Fit Index (IFI) will be acceptable if it is higher than 0.90, and excellent if it is higher than 0.95. Finally, the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) will be excellent if it is lower than 0.05 and acceptable if it is lower than 0.08.

3. Results

The proposed structural equation model according to schoolchildren’s gender obtained a good fit in all assessment indices. The Chi-square had a significant value of p (χ2 = 319,472; DF = 72; p < 0.001). Nevertheless, this index cannot be interpreted in a standardized way, which poses an additional problem by its sensitivity to sample size [42]. Therefore, other standardized fit indices that were less sensitive to sample size were used. The comparative fit index (CFI) had a value of 0.962, which was excellent. The normed fit index (NFI) had a value of 0.952, and the incremental fit index (IFI) a value of 0.962, both also being acceptable. The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) had an excellent value of 0.048.
Figure 2 and Table 1 show the estimated values of the parameters in the structural model for school boys. These must be of a suitable magnitude, and the effects must be significantly different from zero. No improper estimations such as negative variances should be found.
Statistically significant relations were found at the p < 0.005 level in all associations between motivational climate and its dimensions, all of these being positive and direct. The relationship between Task Climate and Ego Climate was significant at the p < 0.005 level, being negative and indirect (r = −0.339). Likewise, statistically significant relations were found at the p < 0.005 level in all associations between Victimization and its indicators, all of these being positive and direct.
Analyzing the influence of the indicators in the latent variables, it could be seen that all of them had statistically significant differences at the p < 0.005 level, all of these being positive and direct. In Task Climate, the indicator with the highest correlation coefficient was Effort/Improvement (r = 0.919), followed by Important Role (r = 0.826) and Cooperative Learning (r = 0.806). In Ego Climate, the strongest association was found in Punishment for Mistakes (r = 0.809), followed by Unequal Recognition (r = 0.774) and Member Rivalry (r = 0.516). With regard to Victimization, the indicator with the highest correlation was Overt Verbal Victimization (r = 0.938), followed by Relational Victimization (r = 0.878), and Overt Physical Victimization (r = 0.775).
Likewise, significant associations (p < 0.005) were found between Task Climate and Victimization—being positive and direct (r = 0.208)—and between Task Climate and Physical Activity—also being positive and direct (r = 0.294). Statistically significant differences were also found at a level of p < 0.005 between Ego Climate and Victimization, revealing a direct association (r = 0.396), with an average correlation strength. With regard to the relationship between Ego Climate and the use of video games, the differences were also statistically significant (r = 0.250).
The use of video games and the practice of physical activity revealed a negative and indirect relationship (r = 0.085; p = −0.027), the correlation being week. However, the relationship between the use of video games and victimization was positive and direct, but correlation was also week (r = 0.094; p = 0.020). Task Climate and the use of video games did not reveal any statistically significant associations in school boys. The same occurred in the relationship between Ego Climate and the practice of physical activity, and the relationship between victimization and the practice of physical activity.
Figure 3 and Table 2 show estimated values of the parameters in the structural model for school girls. These must be of a suitable magnitude and the effects must be significantly different from zero. No improper estimations such as negative variances should be found.
Statistically significant relationships were found at the p < 0.005 level in all associations between motivational climate and its dimensions, all of these being positive and direct. The relationship between Task Climate and Ego Climate was significant at the p < 0.005 level, being negative and indirect (r = −0.360). Likewise, statistically significant relationships at the p < 0.005 level were found between victimization and its indicators, all of these being positive and direct.
Analyzing the influence of the indicators on each of the latent variables, statistically significant differences were found at the p < 0.005 level in all of them, all relationships being positive and direct. In Task Climate, the indicator with the highest correlation coefficient was Effort/Improvement (r = 0.871), followed by Important Role (r = 0.731) and Cooperative Learning (r = 0.728). In Ego Climate, the strongest association was found in Unequal Recognition (r = 0.839), followed by Punishment for Mistakes (r = 0.786), and Member Rivalry (r = 0.627). With regard to Victimization, the indicator showing the strongest correlation was Overt Verbal Victimization (r = 0.968), followed by Relational Victimization (r = 0.801), and Overt Physical Victimization (r = 0.722).
In the same way, significant associations (p < 0.005) were found between Ego Climate and Victimization -being positive and direct (r = 0.265)- and between Ego Climate and Physical Activity—being also positive and direct (r = 0.146), both having a weak correlation. Statistically significant differences were also found at the p < 0.005 level between Task Climate and the practice of physical activity, revealing a direct association (r = 0.393). With regard to the relationship between Task Climate and the use of video games, a negative and indirect association was found (r = −0.133), also having a weak correlation.
The use of video games and the practice of physical activity revealed a negative and indirect relationship (r = −0.081; p = 0.015), having a weak correlation. However, in the association between the use of video games and Victimization, the relationship was positive and direct, also having a weak correlation, although higher than the previous one (r = 0.174; p = 0.015).
Ego Climate and the use of video games did not reveal any statistically significant associations in school girls. The same occurred in the relationship between Task Climate and victimization, and between the practice of physical activity and Victimization.

4. Discussion

This research study conducted a multigroup structural equation analysis aimed at contrasting associations between the motivational climate in sport, victimization, the problematic use of video games, and the practice of physical activity. The path model developed had excellent fit indices and created a valid explanatory model enabling the understanding of the relationships that exist between motivational factors, victimization, the use of video games, and the practice of physical activity by schoolchildren, both boys and girls, in the same way as do other national and international studies [43,44,45,46].
Analyzing motivational climate, the proposed structural model reveals a significant and inverse relationship between task climate and ego climate for both boys and girls, which is stronger and more differentiated for girls. It seems evident that subjects strongly oriented towards the task show little motivation towards ego, and vice versa [47,48,49]. This is because individuals adopt a predominant orientation, either towards the task, which rewards effort and personal development, or towards ego, which fosters team member rivalry and the display of personal skill [50]. With regard to girls, this inverse relationship is stronger because boys are more ego-oriented and less task-oriented than girls, this being explained by the fact that girls tend to work towards intrinsic goals while boys focus on the achievement of extrinsic goals [51,52].
With regard to the categories that make up task climate dimensions, it can be seen that the influence of the indicators is proportional, with effort/improvement being the indicator with the strongest correlation, in both boys and girls, followed by important role and cooperative learning, the three indicators showing a stronger correlation in boys. In the case of ego climate, the most influential indicator for boys is punishment for mistakes, whereas for girls it is unequal recognition. These data are in line with the results of studies by Bandeira et al. [47] and Xiang et al. [53], which indicate that effort and improvement constitute the fundamental factor characterizing the type of motivation that is more self-determined or intrinsic, its focus being on improving and developing skills rather than thinking about external rewards. Since boys are more ego-oriented than girls, they are more concerned about punishment, whereas girls are influenced to a greater extent by unequal recognition, worrying less than boys about punishment and focusing more on intrinsic goals [49,51].
As for victimization, the influence of the indicators follows the same pattern in both boys and girls. For girls, however, correlation is slightly stronger in overt verbal victimization, while overt physical victimization and relational victimization have a stronger correlation for boys. The results indicate that girls suffer more verbal victimization than boys, while boys suffer more physical and relational victimization than girls [54,55]. In line with the findings of Devries et al. [56], it is clear that boys are more likely to victimize or be victimized physically, whereas in girls there tends to be a greater bias towards verbal aggression and victimization. The reason for this may be that boys are less fearful of physical aggression, as a result of social and cultural factors [57].
In terms of the relationship between motivational climate and victimization, a positive and direct relationship was found between task climate and victimization among boys, there being no such relationship in girls. This can be explained by the fact that task climate promotes cooperation, whereby individuals are obliged to work in a collective manner, interacting with others, which can easily give rise to incidents of bullying when the coach is absent or in unattended areas such as in hallways and changing rooms [58,59]. In terms of ego climate and victimization, a positive and direct relationship is found between them, for both boys and girls, but it is stronger for boys. The reason for this is that boys are more strongly oriented towards tasks than are girls. Therefore, when schoolchildren engage in physical activity, whether collectively or not, if that an activity is centered around competition, they may find themselves faced with defeat or disappointing results that cause them to experience frustration, which may lead to violent situations within the peer group [59,60].
A positive and direct relationship was found between the two motivational climates and the practice of physical activity, this being stronger in the case of task climate. Analyzed research indicates that the intention to be physically active is directly related to intrinsic motivation, which is connected to the more self-determined type of motivation, focusing on enjoyment and personal development [61,62]. The two types of motivational climate are not mutually exclusive; they are complementary and thus positively and directly related to the practice of sports. However, task climate is more beneficial in terms of commitment to participating in sports, ego climate being more oriented towards achieving results and showing off one’s skills [63,64].
Analyzing the connection between task climate and the problematic use of video games, a negative and indirect relationship was found for girls, showing an average strength of correlation, whereas this association is not significant for boys. In the case of ego climate and the problematic use of video games, they are directly and positively related for boys, but there is no such association for girls. Studies carried out by Gentile et al. [12] and Chacón et al. [65] show similar relationships, attributing them to the need for rivalry and the external reinforcement, provided by motivational climates oriented towards ego, this applying in virtually the same way to both sporting competitions and video games. In contrast, task climate can act as a protective factor against the pathological use of video games, since it promotes intrinsic motivation to engage in physical activity, increasing adherence to it and reducing sedentary use of digital entertainment [66,67].
The relationship between the problematic use of video games and the practice of physical activity is negative and indirect for boys and girls alike, its correlation being slightly stronger for boys. Coinciding with the results of research by Stubblefield et al. [68] and Krossbakken et al. [69], these findings are explainable by the fact that schoolchildren’s free time is usually devoted mainly to physical activity; however, since a couple of decades ago, this free time has been devoted increasingly to digital media such as video games. Therefore, these results indicate the need to encourage adherence to the practice of physical activity, to the detriment of sedentariness related to digital entertainment, in order to improve children’s health in general [70,71,72,73]. Additionally, boys tend to use video game consoles and video games more than girls, so their correlation is stronger [74].
A positive and direct relationship was found between the problematic use of video games and victimization in schoolchildren, the correlation being stronger in girls than boys. These data agree with other research studies consulted [75,76,77] in finding that young people who play video games with violent content tend to suffer more victimization than those who do not exhibit problematic use of video games [78,79,80]. Since most current video games do have violent content, constant exposure to them can affect children’s cognitive processes, leading to their perceiving violent behavior as normal and losing their sensitivity and empathy in the face of violent acts [23]. From this it can be deduced that the problematic use of video games becomes a risk factor in terms of school harassment or bullying and levels of sedentariness, negatively influencing both factors [81,82].
The present research study has some limitations, one of them being its cross-sectional nature, which precludes the establishment of cause and effect relationships. For such reasons, and in line with the data derived from the study, there is a need to implement intervention programs aimed at promoting adherence to the practice of physical activity instead of sedentary digital entertainment, in an attempt to replace time devoted to video games with healthy behavior related to the practice of physical activity and sport. It would also be interesting to promote motivational climates oriented towards task in the school context, in view of the inverse relationship that has been found with the problematic use of video games.

5. Conclusions

The findings reveal a positive relationship between task climate and the practice of physical activity for both genders, but it is stronger for girls. The association between task climate and the use of video games is negative and indirect for girls, whereas for boys it is not significant. Additionally, task climate and victimization is positively related for boys. Furthermore, ego climate and the use of video games are positively related for boys, but no association is found for girls. The relationship between the use of video games and the practice of physical activity is negative for both boys and girls, being stronger in the case of boys. The same tendency is followed between ego climate and victimization. Finally, there is a direct relationship between the use of video games and victimization for both genders, but the correlation is much stronger for girls. No relationship was found between victimization and the practice of physical activity, for either boys or girls.

Author Contributions

M.C.-S., E.Z.-S. and R.C.-C. conceived the hypothesis of this study. M.C.-S., F.Z.-O. and J.L.U.-J. participated in data collection. M.C.-S. and R.C.C. analysed the data. All authors contributed to data interpretation of statistical analysis. M.C.-S., R.C.-C. and F.Z.-O. wrote the paper with significant input from M.C.-S. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by “Precompetitive Research Projects for Young Researchers 2017”—Modality B under de Grant PPJI_B-05, the project “DISPERSA” under the Grant TIN2015-67149-C3-R and European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) under the Grant P11-TIC-7486.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Eichenbaum, A.; Kattner, F.; Bradford, D.; Gentile, D.A.; Choo, H.; Chen, V.; Khoo, A.; Green, C.S. The role of game genres and the development of internet gaming disorder in school-aged children. J. Addict. Behav. Ther. Rehab. 2017, 4, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Engel, A.; Salvador, C.C.; Membrive, A.; Badenas, J.O. Information and communication technologies and students’ out-of-school learning experiences. Digit. Ed. Rev. 2018, 33, 130–149. [Google Scholar]
  3. Hinkley, T.; Cliff, D.; Lum, J.; Hesketh, K. Active minds happy kids: A stealth-based pilot intervention targeting preschoolers’ screen-time, outdoor play and social skills. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2017, 20, e3–e4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Chacón-Cuberos, R.; Castro-Sánchez, M.; González-Campos, G.; Zurita-Ortega, F. Victimization in school, digital leisure and irritability: Analysis using structural equations. RELIEVE 2018, 24, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Verheijen, G.P.; Burk, W.J.; Stoltz, S.E.; van den Berg, Y.H.; Cillessen, A.H. Friendly fire: Longitudinal effects of exposure to violent video games on aggressive behavior in adolescent friendship dyads. Aggress. Behav. 2018, 44, 257–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Smohai, M.; Urbán, R.; Griffiths, M.D.; Király, O.; Mirnics, Z.; Vargha, A.; Demetrovics, Z. Online and offline video game use in adolescents: Measurement invariance and problem severity. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abus. 2017, 43, 111–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Shi, J.; Boak, A.; Mann, R.; Turner, N.E. Adolescent Problem Video Gaming in Urban and Non-urban Regions. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2018, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Tian, M.; Tao, R.; Zheng, Y.; Zhang, H.; Yang, G.; Li, Q.; Liu, X. Internet gaming disorder in adolescents is linked to delay discounting but not probability discounting. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 80, 59–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Anderson, E.L.; Steen, E.; Stavropoulos, V. Internet use and Problematic Internet Use: A systematic review of longitudinal research trends in adolescence and emergent adulthood. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 2017, 22, 430–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Van Rooij, A.J.; Schoenmakers, T.M.; Van de Mheen, D. Clinical validation of the C-VAT 2.0 assessment tool for gaming disorder: A sensitivity analysis of the proposed DSM-5 criteria and the clinical characteristics of young patients with ‘video game addiction’. Addict. Behav. 2017, 64, 269–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Jashinsky, J.; Gay, J.; Hansen, N.; Muilenburg, J. Differences in TV Viewing and Computer Game Playing’s Relationships with Physical Activity and Eating Behaviors among Adolescents: An NHANES Study. Am. J. Health Ed. 2017, 48, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Gentile, D.A.; Bender, P.K.; Anderson, C.A. Violent video game effects on salivary cortisol, arousal, and aggressive thoughts in children. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 70, 39–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Mielgo-Ayuso, J.; Aparicio-Ugarriza, R.; Castillo, A.; Ruiz, E.; Avila, J.M.; Aranceta-Bartrina, J.; Gil, A.; Ortega, R.M.; Serra-Majem, L.; Varela-Moreiras, G.; et al. Sedentary behavior among Spanish children and adolescents: Findings from the ANIBES study. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 94–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Cernikova, M.; Smahel, D.; Wright, M.F. Children’s Experiences and Awareness about Impact of Digital Media on Health. Health Commun. 2018, 33, 664–673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Ceranoglu, T.A.; Weigle, P.E. Understanding Video Games: A Child Psychiatrist’s Call of Duty. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2017, 56, S353–S354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Jamnik, M.R.; DiLalla, L.F. A multimethodological study of preschoolers’ preferences for aggressive television and video games. J. Genet. Psychol. 2018, 179, 156–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Adachi, P.J.; Willoughby, T. The link between playing video games and positive youth outcomes. Child Dev. Perspect. 2017, 11, 202–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Anderson, C.A.; Bushman, B.J.; Bartholow, B.D.; Cantor, J.; Christakis, D.; Coyne, S.M.; Donnerstein, E.; Brockmyer, J.F.; Gentile, D.A.; Green, C.S.; et al. Screen violence and youth behavior. Pediatrics 2017, 140, S142–S147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Delisi, M.; Vaighn, M.G.; Gentile, D.A.; Anderson, C.A.; Shook, J.J. Violent video games, delinquency, and youth violence new evidence. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 2013, 11, 132–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Calvert, S.L.; Appelbaum, M.; Dodge, K.A.; Graham, S.; Nagayama-Hall, G.C.; Hamby, S. The American Psychological Association Task Force assessment of violent video games: Science in the service of public interest. Am. Psychol. 2017, 72, 126–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Ditrrick, C.; Beran, T.; Mishna, F.; Hetherington, R.; Shariff, S. Do children who bully their peers also play violent video games? A Canadian national study. J. Sch. Violence 2013, 12, 297–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Kimmig, A.C.; Andringa, G.; Derntl, B. Potential adverse effects of violent video gaming: Interpersonal-affective traits are rather impaired than disinhibition in young adults. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 736–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Boxer, P.; Groves, C.; Docherty, M. Video games do indeed influence children and adolescents’ aggression, prosocial behaviour, and academic performance. A clearer reading of Ferguson. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 10, 671–673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. DeCamp, W.; Ferguson, C.J. The impact of degree of exposure to violent video games, family background, and other factors on youth violence. J. Youth Adolesc. 2017, 46, 388–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Burns, R.; Brusseau, T.; Hannon, J. Multivariate associations among health-related fitness, physical activity, and TGMD-3 test items in disadvantaged children from low-income families. Percept. Motor Skill 2017, 124, 86–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Lee, B.Y.; Adam, A.; Zenkov, E.; Hertenstein, D.; Ferguson, M.C.; Wang, P.I.; Wong, M.S.; Wedlock, P.; Nyathi, S.; Gittelsohn, J.; et al. Modeling the economic and health impact of increasing children’s physical activity in the United States. Health Aff. 2017, 36, 902–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Pakarinen, A.; Parisod, H.; Smed, J.; Salanterä, S. Health game interventions to enhance physical activity self-efficacy of children: A quantitative systematic review. J. Adv. Nurs. 2017, 73, 794–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Solomon-Moore, E.; Sebire, S.J.; Thompson, J.L.; Zahra, J.; Lawlor, D.A.; Jago, R. Are parents’ motivations to exercise and intention to engage in regular family-based activity associated with both adult and child physical activity? BMJ Open Sport Exerc. Med. 2017, 2, e000137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Clark, H.; Camiré, M.; Wade, T.J.; Cairney, J. Sport participation and its association with social and psychological factors known to predict substance use and abuse among youth: A scoping review of the literature. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2015, 8, 224–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Atkins, M.; Johnson, D.; Force, E.; Petrie, T. Peers, parents and coaches, oh my! The relation of the motivational climate to boys’ intention to continue in sport. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2015, 16, 170–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Sommet, N.; Elliot, A.J. Achievement goals, reasons for goal pursuit, and achievement goal complexes as predictors of beneficial outcomes: Is the influence of goals reducible to reasons? J. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 109, 1141–1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Laninga-Wijnen, L.; Ryan, A.M.; Harakeh, Z.; Shin, H.; Vollebergh, W.A. The moderating role of popular peers’ achievement goals in 5th-and 6th-graders’ achievement-related friendships: A social network analysis. J. Educ. Psychol. 2018, 110, 289–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Lochbaum, M.; Kallinen, V.; Konttinen, N. Task and Ego Goal Orientations across the Youth Sports Experience. Stud. Sport 2017, 11, 99–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Gjesdal, S.; Haug, E.M.; Ommundsen, Y. A Conditional Process Analysis of the Coach-Created Mastery Climate, Task Goal Orientation, and Competence Satisfaction in Youth Soccer: The Moderating Role of Controlling Coach Behavior. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 2018, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Ring, C.; Kavussanu, M. The impact of achievement goals on cheating in sport. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2018, 35, 98–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Chamarro, A.; Carbonell, X.; Manresa, J.M.; Muñoz-Mirallles, R.; Ortega-González, R.; López-Morrón, M.R.; Batalla-Martinez, C.; Toran-Monserrat, P. The Video Game Experiences Questionnaire (CERV): An instrument to detect the problematic use of video games in Spanish teenagers. Adicciones 2014, 26, 303–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Kowalski, K.C.; Crocker, P.R.; Donen, R.M. The physical activity questionnaire for older children (PAQ-C) and adolescents (PAQ-A) manual. Coll. Kinesiol. Univ. Sask. 2014, 87, 1–38. [Google Scholar]
  38. Newton, M.; Duda, J.L.; Yin, Z. Examination of the psychometric properties of the Perceived Motivational Climate in Sport Questionnaire-2 in a sample of female athletes. J. Sports Sci. 2000, 18, 275–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. González-Cutre, D.; Sicilia, A.; Moreno, J.A. Modelo cognitivo-social de la motivación de logro en educación física. Psicothema 2008, 20, 642–651. [Google Scholar]
  40. Mynard, H.; Joseph, S. Development of the Multidimensional Peer-Victimization Scale. Aggress. Behav. 2000, 26, 169–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Cava, M.J.; Musitu, G.; Murgui, S. Individual and social risk factors related to overt victimization in a sample of Spanish adolescents. Psychol. Rep. 2007, 101, 275–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Marsh, H.W. Handbook of Sport Psychology, 3nd ed.; Tenenbaum, G., Eklund, R.C., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  43. Espelage, D.L.; Low, S.K.; Jimerson, S.R. Understanding school climate, aggression, peer victimization, and bully perpetration: Contemporary science, practice, and policy. Sch. Psychol. Q. 2014, 29, 233–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Llewellyn, N.; Rudolph, K.D. Individual and sex differences in the consequences of peer victimization: Moderation by approach and avoidance motivation. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 50, 2210–2220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Mitchell, T.B.; Cooley, J.L.; Evans, S.C.; Fite, P.J. The moderating effect of physical activity on the association between ADHD symptoms and peer victimization in middle childhood. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2016, 47, 871–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Brown, C.F.; Demaray, M.K.; Tennant, J.E.; Jenkins, L.N. Cyber Victimization in High School: Measurement, Overlap with Face-to-Face Victimization, and Associations with Social-Emotional Outcomes. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 46, 288–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Bandeira, P.F.; Souza, M.S.; Zanella, L.W.; Valentini, N.C. Impact of motor interventions oriented by mastery motivational climate in fundamental motor skills of children: A systematic review. Motricidade 2017, 13, 50–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Johnson, C.E.; Erwin, H.E.; Kipp, L.; Beighle, A. Student Perceived Motivational Climate, Enjoyment, and Physical Activity in Middle School Physical Education. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 2017, 36, 398–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Masci, I.; Schmidt, M.; Marchetti, R.; Vannozzi, G.; Pesce, C. When Children’s Perceived and Actual Motor Competence Mismatch: Sport Participation and Gender Differences. J. Motor Learn. Dev. 2017, 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Gano-Overway, L.A.; Steele, E.; Boyce, B.A.; Whaley, D. Exploring relationships between the coach-initiated motivational climate and psychological coping skills over the high school American football season. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 2017, 12, 790–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Menges, J.I.; Tussing, D.V.; Wihler, A.; Grant, A.M. When job performance is all relative: How family motivation energizes effort and compensates for intrinsic motivation. Acad. Manag. J. 2017, 60, 695–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Knowles, A.; Wallhead, T.L.; Readdy, T. Exploring the Synergy between Sport Education and In-School Sport Participation. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 2018, 37, 113–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Xiang, P.; Ağbuğa, B.; Liu, J.; McBride, R.E. Relatedness need satisfaction, intrinsic motivation, and engagement in secondary school physical education. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 2017, 36, 340–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Bouffard, L.A.; Koeppel, M.D. Sex differences in the health risk behavior outcomes of childhood bullying victimization. Vict. Offender 2017, 12, 549–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Williams, S.G.; Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J.; Wornell, C.; Finnegan, H. Adolescents transitioning to high school: Sex differences in bullying victimization associated with depressive symptoms, suicide ideation, and suicide attempts. J. School. Nurs. 2017, 33, 467–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Devries, K.M.; Knight, L.; Allen, E.; Parkes, J.; Kyegombe, N.; Naker, D. Does the Good Schools Toolkit reduce physical, sexual and emotional violence, and injuries, in girls and boys equally? A cluster-randomised controlled trial. Prevent. Sci. 2017, 18, 839–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Barrense-Dias, Y.; Berchtold, A.; Akre, C.; Suris, J.C. Youth Dating Violence in Switzerland. J. Adolesc. Health 2017, 60, S79–S80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Gråstén, A.; Watt, A. A motivational model of physical education and links to enjoyment, knowledge, performance, total physical activity and body mass index. J. Sports Sci. Med. 2017, 16, 318–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Cornell, D.; Shukla, K.; Konold, T. Peer victimization and authoritative school climate: A multilevel approach. J. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 107, 1186–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Blakely-McClure, S.J.; Ostrov, J.M. Relational aggression, victimization and self-concept: Testing pathways from middle childhood to adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 2016, 45, 376–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Guerra, N.G.; Williams, K.R.; Sadek, S. Understanding bullying and victimization during childhood and adolescence: A mixed methods study. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 295–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Bekiari, A.; Nikolaidou, Z.A.; Hasanagas, N. Typology of Motivation and Aggression on the Basis of Social Network Variables: Examples of Complementary and Nested Behavioral Types through Conventional Statistics. Soc. Netw. 2017, 6, 135–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Jaakkola, T.; Yli-Piipari, S.; Barkoukis, V.; Liukkonen, J. Relationships among perceived motivational climate, motivational regulations, enjoyment, and PA participation among Finnish physical education students. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2017, 15, 273–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Wadsworth, D.D.; Rudisill, M.E.; Hastie, P.A.; Irwin, J.M.; Rodriguez-Hernandez, M.G. Preschoolers’ Physical Activity Participation Across a Yearlong Mastery-Motivational Climate Intervention. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2017, 88, 339–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Chacón, R.; Zurita, F.; Castro, M.; Espejo, T.; Martínez, A.; Pérez, A.J. Motivational climate in sport and its relationship with digital sedentary leisure habits in university students. Saúde Soc. 2017, 26, 29–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Chacón, R.; Zurita, F.; Puertas, P.; Knox, E.; Cofré, C.; Viciana, V.; Muros, J.J. Relationship between Healthy Habits and Perceived Motivational Climate in Sport among University Students: A Structural Equation Model. Sustainability 2018, 10, 938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Foley, L.; Jiang, Y.; Mhurchu, C.N.; Jull, A.; Prapavessis, H.; Rodgers, A.; Maddison, R. The effect of active video games by ethnicity, sex and fitness: Subgroup analysis from a randomised controlled trial. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2015, 11, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Stubblefield, S.; Datto, G.; Phan, T.L.; Werk, L.N.; Stackpole, K.; Siegel, R.; Stratbucker, W.; Tucker, J.M.; Christison, A.L.; Hossain, J.; et al. Problem video gaming among children enrolled in tertiary weight management programs. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2017, 20, 109–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Krossbakken, E.; Torsheim, T.; Mentzoni, R.A.; King, D.L.; Bjorvatn, B.; Lorvik, I.M.; Pallesen, S. The effectiveness of a parental guide for prevention of problematic video gaming in children: A public health randomized controlled intervention study. J. Behav. Addict. 2018, 7, 52–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Chastin, S.F.; Palarea-Albaladejo, J.; Dontje, M.L.; Skelton, D.A. Combined effects of time spent in physical activity, sedentary behaviors and sleep on obesity and cardio-metabolic health markers: A novel compositional data analysis approach. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0139984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Lewis, B.A.; Napolitano, M.A.; Buman, M.P.; Williams, D.M.; Nigg, C.R. Future directions in physical activity intervention research: Expanding our focus to sedentary behaviors, technology, and dissemination. J. Behav. Med. 2017, 40, 112–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Timperio, A.; Crawford, D.; Ball, K.; Salmon, J. Typologies of neighbourhood environments and children’s physical activity, sedentary time and television viewing. Health Place 2017, 43, 121–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Sallis, J.F.; Conway, T.L.; Cain, K.L.; Carlson, J.A.; Frank, L.D.; Kerr, J. Neighborhood built environment and socioeconomic status in relation to physical activity, sedentary behavior, and weight status of adolescents. Prev. Med. 2018, 110, 47–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Ohannessian, C.M. Video game play and anxiety during late adolescence: The moderating effects of gender and social context. J. Affect. Disord. 2018, 226, 216–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Mishna, F.; Khoury-Kassabri, M.; Gadalla, T.; Daciuk, J. Risk factors for involvement in cyber bullying: Victims, bullies and bully-victims. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 2012, 34, 63–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Bascón-Seda, A.; Ramírez-Macías, G. Physical Education and its teachers in cases of bullying from the victim´s perspective. Educ. Sport Health Phys. Act. 2018, 2, 75–91. [Google Scholar]
  77. Lam, L.; Cheng, Z.; Liu, X. Violent online games exposure and cyberbullying/victimization among adolescents. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2013, 16, 159–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Ferguson, C.J. Do angry birds make for angry children? A meta-analysis of video game influences on children’s and adolescents’ aggression, mental health, prosocial behavior, and academic performance. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 10, 646–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Griffiths, M.D.; Nuyens, F. An overview of structural characteristics in problematic video game playing. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2017, 4, 272–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Lee, H.J.; Tran, D.D.; Morrell, H.E. Smoking, ADHD, and Problematic Video Game Use: A Structural Modeling Approach. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2018, 21, 281–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Smith, S.; Ferguson, C.; Beaver, K. A longitudinal analysis of shooter games and their relationship with conduct disorder and cself-reported delinquency. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 2018, 58, 48–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Ramírez-Granizo, I.A.; Fernández-Revelles, A.B.; Padial-Ruz, R.; Espejo-Garcés, T.; García-Martínez, I. The level of physical activity and its relation to problematic use with video games. A narrative review. J. Sport. Health Res. 2018, 10, 117–124. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Model theories. Note1: TC, Task Climate; CL, Cooperative Learning; E/I, Effort/Improvement; IR, Important Role; EC, Ego Climate; MR, Member Rivalry; PM, Punishment for Mistakes; UR, Unequal Recognition; VIDEO GAMES, Use of video games; PA, Physical Activity; VICTIMIZATION, Victimization; RV, Relational Victimization; OPV, Overt Physical Victimization; OVV, Overt Verbal Victimization.
Figure 1. Model theories. Note1: TC, Task Climate; CL, Cooperative Learning; E/I, Effort/Improvement; IR, Important Role; EC, Ego Climate; MR, Member Rivalry; PM, Punishment for Mistakes; UR, Unequal Recognition; VIDEO GAMES, Use of video games; PA, Physical Activity; VICTIMIZATION, Victimization; RV, Relational Victimization; OPV, Overt Physical Victimization; OVV, Overt Verbal Victimization.
Ijerph 15 01866 g001
Figure 2. Structural equation model for boys. Note: TC, Task Climate; CL, Cooperative Learning; E/I, Effort/Improvement; IR, Important Role; EC, Ego Climate; MR, Member Rivalry; PM, Punishment for Mistakes; UR, Unequal Recognition; VIDEO GAMES, Use of video games; PA, Physical Activity; VICTIMIZATION, Victimization; RV, Relational Victimization; OPV, Overt Physical Victimization; OVV, Overt Verbal Victimization.
Figure 2. Structural equation model for boys. Note: TC, Task Climate; CL, Cooperative Learning; E/I, Effort/Improvement; IR, Important Role; EC, Ego Climate; MR, Member Rivalry; PM, Punishment for Mistakes; UR, Unequal Recognition; VIDEO GAMES, Use of video games; PA, Physical Activity; VICTIMIZATION, Victimization; RV, Relational Victimization; OPV, Overt Physical Victimization; OVV, Overt Verbal Victimization.
Ijerph 15 01866 g002
Figure 3. Structural equation model for girls. Note: TC, Task Climate; CL, Cooperative Learning; E/I, Effort/Improvement; IR, Important Role; EC, Ego Climate; MR, Member Rivalry; PM, Punishment for Mistakes; UR, Unequal Recognition; VIDEO GAMES, Use of video games; PA, Physical Activity; VICTIMIZATION, Victimization; RV, Relational Victimization; OPV, Overt Physical Victimization; OVV, Overt Verbal Victimization.
Figure 3. Structural equation model for girls. Note: TC, Task Climate; CL, Cooperative Learning; E/I, Effort/Improvement; IR, Important Role; EC, Ego Climate; MR, Member Rivalry; PM, Punishment for Mistakes; UR, Unequal Recognition; VIDEO GAMES, Use of video games; PA, Physical Activity; VICTIMIZATION, Victimization; RV, Relational Victimization; OPV, Overt Physical Victimization; OVV, Overt Verbal Victimization.
Ijerph 15 01866 g003
Table 1. Weights and standardized regression weights for boys.
Table 1. Weights and standardized regression weights for boys.
Relationships between VariablesR.W.S.R.W.
EstimatesE.E.C.R.PEstimates
VIDEO GAMESTC−0.5610.430−1.3040.192−0.056
VIDEO GAMESEC2.7780.5235.313***0.250
PATC0.3010.0446.880***0.294
PAEC−0.0390.053−0.7400.459−0.034
PAVIDEO GAMES−0.0090.004−2.208*−0.085
VICTIMIZATIONTC0.3040.0674.522***0.208
VICTIMIZATIONEC0.6440.0837.760***0.396
VICTIMIZATIONPA0.0090.0580.1470.8830.006
VICTIMIZATIONVIDEO GAMES0.0140.0062.330*0.094
IRTC1.000---0.826
E/ITC1.1390.04326.325***0.919
CLTC0.9630.04123.662***0.806
PMEC1.000---0.809
UREC0.4670.03115.203***0.774
MREC0.7260.06211.730***0.516
OVVVICTIMIZATION1.000---0.938
OPVVICTIMIZATION0.6790.02725.495***0.775
RVVICTIMIZATION0.8110.02630.927***0.878
ECTC−0.1800.026−6.815***−0.339
Note 1: TC, Task Climate; CL, Cooperative Learning; E/I, Effort/Improvement; IR, Important Role; EC, Ego Climate; MR, Member Rivalry; PM, Punishment for Mistakes; UR, Unequal Recognition; VIDEO GAMES, Use of video games; PA, Physical Activity; VICTIMIZATION, Victimization; RV, Relational Victimization; OPV, Overt Physical Victimization; OVV, Overt Verbal Victimization. Note: R.W., Regression Weights; S.R.W., Standardized Regression Weights; E.E., Error Estimation; C.R., Critical Ratio; *** Statistically significant relationship between variables at a level of 0.005; * Statistically significant relationship between variables at a level of 0.05.
Table 2. Weights and standardized regression weights for girls.
Table 2. Weights and standardized regression weights for girls.
Relationships between VariablesR.W.S.R.W.
EstimatesE.E.C.R.PEstimates
VIDEO GAMESTC−1.4060.445−3.159***−0.133
VIDEO GAMESEC−0.5460.377−1.4480.148−0.062
PATC0.4800.0529.283***0.393
PAEC0.1500.0423.585***0.146
PAVIDEO GAMES−0.0090.004−2.422*−0.081
VICTIMIZATIONTC0.0000.0780.0040.9970.000
VICTIMIZATIONEC0.3790.0636.063***0.265
VICTIMIZATIONPA0.0050.0530.1030.9180.004
VICTIMIZATIONVIDEO GAMES0.0280.0064.939***0.174
IRTC1.000---0.731
E/ITC1.2110.06020.110***0.871
CLTC1.0450.05618.806***0.728
PMEC1.000---0.786
UREC0.4970.02618.942***0.839
MREC0.9320.05716.428***0.627
OVVVICTIMIZATION1.000---0.968
OPVVICTIMIZATION0.5300.02323.296***0.722
RVVICTIMIZATION0.7900.03026.585***0.801
ECTC−0.1730.023−7.581***−0.360
Note: TC, Task Climate; CL, Cooperative Learning; E/I, Effort/Improvement; IR, Important Role; EC, Ego Climate; MR, Member Rivalry; PM, Punishment for Mistakes; UR, Unequal Recognition; VIDEO GAMES, Use of video games; PA, Physical Activity; VICTIMIZATION, Victimization; RV, Relational Victimization; OPV, Overt Physical Victimization; OVV, Overt Verbal Victimization; R.W., Regression Weights; S.R.W., Standardized Regression Weights; E.E., Error Estimation; C.R., Critical Ratio; *** Statistically significant relationship between variables at a level of 0.005; * Statistically significant relationship between variables at a level of 0.05.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Castro-Sánchez, M.; Chacón-Cuberos, R.; Ubago-Jiménez, J.L.; Zafra-Santos, E.; Zurita-Ortega, F. An Explanatory Model for the Relationship between Motivation in Sport, Victimization, and Video Game Use in Schoolchildren. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 1866. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15091866

AMA Style

Castro-Sánchez M, Chacón-Cuberos R, Ubago-Jiménez JL, Zafra-Santos E, Zurita-Ortega F. An Explanatory Model for the Relationship between Motivation in Sport, Victimization, and Video Game Use in Schoolchildren. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018; 15(9):1866. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15091866

Chicago/Turabian Style

Castro-Sánchez, Manuel, Ramón Chacón-Cuberos, José Luis Ubago-Jiménez, Edson Zafra-Santos, and Félix Zurita-Ortega. 2018. "An Explanatory Model for the Relationship between Motivation in Sport, Victimization, and Video Game Use in Schoolchildren" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 15, no. 9: 1866. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15091866

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop