Next Article in Journal
Patterns Associated with Adult Mandibular Fractures in Southern Taiwan—A Cross-Sectional Retrospective Study
Next Article in Special Issue
A Diagnostic Post-Occupancy Evaluation of the Nacadia® Therapy Garden
Previous Article in Journal
Synthesis and Ion-Exchange Properties of Graphene Th(IV) Phosphate Composite Cation Exchanger: Its Applications in the Selective Separation of Lead Metal Ions
Previous Article in Special Issue
Modeling the Effects of Urban Design on Emergency Medical Response Calls during Extreme Heat Events in Toronto, Canada
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Experimental Study on the Health Benefits of Garden Landscape

Department of Landscape Architecture, Hankyong National University, 327 Chungang-ro, Anseong-si, Gyeonggi-do 17579, Korea
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14(7), 829; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14070829
Submission received: 23 June 2017 / Revised: 17 July 2017 / Accepted: 21 July 2017 / Published: 24 July 2017
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Landscapes and Human Health)

Abstract

:
To mitigate the negative effects of modern cities on health, scientists are focusing on the diverse benefits of natural environments; a conceptual approach to use gardens for promoting human health is being attempted. In this study, the effects of the visual landscape of a traditional garden on psychological and physiological activities were investigated. Eighteen male and female adults participated in this indoor experiment (mean age, 26.7 years). Twelve different landscape images for city and garden were presented continuously for 90 s. In the time series changes of oxygenated hemoglobin (O2Hb), different patterns of changes were observed between the city and garden. The mean O2Hb values increased for the city landscapes, whereas they decreased for the garden landscapes both in the left and right prefrontal cortices. Significant differences in the negative psychological states of tension, fatigue, confusion, and anxiety were observed between the city and garden landscapes. Important differences in the physiological and psychological responses to the two different landscapes were also detected between male and female participants, providing valuable clues to individual differences in the health benefits of natural landscapes. To validate the use of gardens as a resource for promoting health in urban dwellers, further scientific evidence, active communication, and collaboration among experts in the relevant field are necessary.

1. Introduction

Over the past several decades, the level of urbanization in Asian countries has increased from 27% in 1950 to 57% in 2014 [1]. Currently, Korea is considered one of the most urbanized countries (82%). However, urbanization does not necessarily improve the quality of life [2]. Despite the many advantages of developed infrastructures, artificialized urban environments, in general, might be closely related to the negative health outcomes in modern people, which is supported by an increasing body of research [3,4,5,6].
To mitigate the negative effects of modern city environments, scientists are currently focusing on the benefits of natural environments [7,8,9]. In recent years, quantified scientific evidence has been reported on the health benefits of natural environments with the development of biological markers [10,11,12,13,14,15]. The epidemiological approach supports the positive relationship between nature and health-related parameters [16,17]. On the basis of these findings, evidence-based policy movement is also undertaken. The Korean government passed legislation to use forests for promoting health, which included national funding support from the government, a specialist production system, and facility construction on forest therapy [18].
The visible landscape is believed to affect human beings in many ways, including aesthetic appreciation, health, and well-being [19]. Since the landmark study of Ulrich [20] that demonstrated that window landscape views affect patient recovery after surgery, increasing attention has been paid to the health-related effects of visible landscape in the fields of landscape architecture and public health [19,21,22,23].
A new conceptual approach is being developed to use gardens for promoting human health [24,25,26,27,28]. Gardens are private or public green spaces ornamented with vegetation and beautiful landscapes that are used for recreation, restoration, and healing. In recent years, there has been a growing awareness of the need for gardens for stress reduction and healing [29,30,31]. Landscape features in gardens vary with scales and styles. Among these, traditional gardens often represent local identities [32] by revealing common landscape features of local areas and providing activity spaces for urban dwellers [33]. However, despite increasing social needs, scientific evidence on the health benefits of garden landscapes is still limited to support this notion. Therefore, this study aimed to investigate the effects of visual landscapes of traditional gardens on psychological and physiological activities by quantifying health-related benefits.

2. Methodology

2.1. Participants and Landscape Images

The voluntary participants were 18 young adults consisting of 9 male and 9 female with a mean age (±standard error (SE)) of 26.7 ± 0.7 years (Table 1). All participants were physically and psychologically healthy. Those with past and/or current mental disorders, cardiovascular or allergic diseases, and smoking or drinking habits were screened in the process of recruiting. Prior to the experiment, written informed consent was obtained from all the participants after the study aims and protocol were provided and explained. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee (08-10) of the Graduate School of Horticulture, Chiba University.
City and garden landscape images were prepared by taking pictures in Gyeongju city in Korea, which has some of the most famous historic places with a vast number of archaeological sites and traditional properties. Some parts of the city have even been designated as world heritage sites by the UNESCO. City landscape images were taken at the commercial area of the city center, which exhibits typical characteristics of the local cities in Korea. Garden landscape images were taken at the Anapji, one of the typical traditional gardens in Korea. Twelve different views in each area were used in the experiment and were taken at three different distances, i.e., distant, medium, and near. Pictures were taken in the winter season to evaluate the health-related benefits of winter landscapes (Figure 1). Each city or garden session lasted for 90 s with 12 images shown in total. Each person viewed both the city and garden images and the order of the sessions was counterbalanced for each participant. Before the experimental sessions, dummy images were also provided. Images were presented using a high definition display placed 3 m from the participant. The room temperature was set at 25 °C and relative humidity at 60%.

2.2. Data Collection

As an index of the physiological response, blood flow in the brain was monitored using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) to investigate the changes in brain activity when observing two different landscapes. Hemoglobin concentrations in the left and right prefrontal cortices were continually measured using NIRO-300 (Hamamatsu Photonics, Shizuoka, Japan) with 1-s segments during the experiment. Increased brain activity was accompanied by increased blood flow, because brain activity requires oxygen consumption. It has been known that the blood flow changes are consistent with changes of oxy-hemoglobin (O2Hb) [34], and that decreases in O2Hb concentrations are related to physiological relaxation [35]. Prior to the viewing session, the O2Hb concentration in the left and right prefrontal cortices was confirmed to have become constant for 15 s. To investigate the reactivity of the autonomic nervous system, systolic and diastolic blood pressures and pulse rate were measured with an oscillometric method (HEM-1010, Omron, Kyoto, Japan) following the presentation of each landscape image.
The Profile of Mood States (POMS) [36] was used to assess the following six mood dimensions on a five-point scale: “tension-anxiety (T-A)”, “depression (D)”, “anger-hostility (A-H)”, “confusion (C)”, “vigor (V)”, and “fatigue (F)”. The Total Mood Disturbance (TMD) score was also calculated for the POMS data. A semantic differential (SD) method [37] was used to evaluate the emotional response to the city and garden landscapes. The SD rating test was conducted on seven scales for eight different pairs of adjectives, including natural-artificial, open-closed, attractive for walking-unattractive for walking, comfortable–uncomfortable, familiar-unfamiliar, clean-dirty, beautiful-ugly, and warm-cold. To compare the changes in the level of state anxiety for each landscape, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [38] test was also conducted. Three types of psychological tests were administered to the participants after they watched the landscape images.

2.3. Data Analysis

For each 1-s segment of NIRS data, time series changes were investigated and differences at each time segment between city and garden landscapes were analyzed. Hemoglobin concentrations were also calculated on three 30-s periods—early, mid, and late—to compare male and female hemodynamic responses to the two landscapes. Systolic and diastolic blood pressures were also measured to investigate the autonomic nervous reactivity to the landscape images. The physiological results were analyzed using the paired t-test and the psychological data were compared by the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. Statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS 21.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). All data were presented as means ± standard error (SE) and the statistical differences were considered to be significant at p < 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Cerebral Blood Flow

The NIRS data revealed different hemodymanic reactivity to the city and garden landscapes. At first, there were no significant differences between the two stimuli in the left and right hemispheres before viewing the images of each landscape. Mean values of O2Hb for 90 s of city stimuli were 0.43 nmol/L (±0.03) and 0.40 nmol/L (±0.03) in the left and right hemispheres, respectively. In contrast, the mean values of O2Hb for 90 s of garden stimuli were decreased to −1.63 nmol/L (±0.07) and −0.97 nmol/L (±0.02) in the left and right hemispheres, respectively. In the time series changes of O2Hb, different patterns of changes were found between the city and garden (Figure 2). The mean O2Hb values increased for the city landscape, whereas they decreased for the garden both in the left and right prefrontal cortices. Significant differences were detected during 8 to 90 s in the left hemisphere and during 5 to 90 s in the right hemisphere for the garden images, compared with the city landscape images. The O2Hb values of the garden landscape were significantly lower from 8 to 90 s in the left hemisphere and 5 to 90 s in the right hemisphere, compared with the city landscape images. In the analysis of the three 30-s values, significant differences were detected between the city and garden landscapes for the early, mid, and late time periods. Interesting differences were also found in the mean values of the O2Hb concentrations between male and female participants (Figure 3). Although the female participants exhibited a decrease for both landscapes, the male participants displayed an apparent increase for the city landscape and a decrease for the garden, indicating more distinguished differences for the city than the garden.

3.2. Autonomic Nervous Activity

No significant differences between the city and garden landscapes were found in the analysis of systolic (101.1 ± 2.9 mmHg for city landscape; 101.7 ± 2.6 mmHg for garden landscape) and diastolic blood pressures (61.3 ± 2.2 mmHg for city landscape; 61.2 ± 2.0 mmHg for garden landscape); the pulse rate (67.4 ± 1.6 bpm for city landscape; 66.7 ± 1.7 bpm for garden landscape) was also similar for both landscapes. However, when comparing between the male and female participants, significant differences were observed in the diastolic blood pressure (Figure 4). Male participants had a significantly higher value for the city (65.8 ± 3.1 mmHg) than for the garden (63.1 ± 3.3 mmHg; p < 0.05) landscape, while the female participants exhibited a significantly lower value for the city (56.8 ± 2.3 mmHg) than for the garden (59.2 ± 2.3 mmHg; p < 0.05) landscape. For the systolic blood pressure and pulse rate, both male and female participants displayed similar responses to the city and garden landscapes with no significant differences.

3.3. Psychological Response

In the analysis of the psychological responses to the images of the two landscapes, significant differences were detected. In the POMS analysis (Figure 5), significant differences were found between the city and garden landscapes for negative mood states of T-A (city, 37.2 ± 1.4; garden, 32.1 ± 0.4; p < 0.01), A-H (city, 40.7 ± 1.1; garden, 37.3 ± 0.2; p < 0.01), F (city, 42.2 ± 1.7; garden, 36.9 ± 1.4; p < 0.01), C (city, 45.8 ± 1.5; garden, 39.1 ± 0.7; p < 0.01), and TMD (city, 172.1 ± 5.8; garden, 152.9 ± 3.2; p < 0.01). Based on the SD data for the city and garden landscapes (Figure 6), the garden was evaluated as significantly more natural (city, −1.94 ± 0.37; garden, 0.78 ± 0.34; p < 0.01), more open (city, −0.67 ± 0.41; garden, 1.11 ± 0.35; p < 0.01), more attractive for walking (city, −1.22 ± 0.35; garden, 0.94 ± 0.37; p < 0.01), more comfortable (city, −0.89 ± 0.35; garden, 1.17 ± 0.28; p < 0.01), cleaner (city, −0.94 ± 0.33; garden, 1.89 ± 0.21; p < 0.01), and more beautiful (city, −0.72 ± 0.31; garden, 1.44 ± 0.29; p < 0.01), compared with the city landscape. A significant difference was also observed in the state anxiety level (Figure 7) between the city (45.7 ± 3.2) and garden (36.5 ± 2.1; p < 0.01) landscapes.
When comparing the POMS scores between male and female participants, a significant difference was found in the T-A subscale. The male group, but not the female, exhibited a significant difference in the T-A scores between the city and garden. The scores of the negative subscales in the female group (T-A, 36.1 ± 2.0; D, 40.9 ± 0.8; A-H, 40.2 ± 1.6; F, 40.0 ± 2.2; C, 44.1 ± 1.6; TMD, 164.4 ± 7.3) were lower than in the male group (T-A, 38.3 ± 2.0; D, 42.4 ± 1.2; A-H, 41.2 ± 1.7; F, 44.4 ± 2.5; C, 47.4 ± 2.5; TMD, 179.8 ± 8.7) for the city landscape.

4. Discussion

In the present study, the physiological and psychological responses to the city and garden landscapes were investigated in healthy male and female adults. There were significant differences in the cerebral activity between the two landscapes. For the garden landscape, the cerebral activity was slowly decreased as evidenced by the declining O2Hb concentrations in the prefrontal areas, compared with the city landscape. This result can be related to decreased physiological stress. In the analysis of psychological responses, negative mood states—including tension, anger, fatigue, confusion, and anxiety—were significantly reduced when viewing the garden compared with the city scenes. Although similar results have been shown in previous studies on natural environments, including forest [11,12,39], farmland [40], urban park [41], and neighborhood green space [42], only a few studies have reported the health-related value of traditional gardens. The traditional garden has unique landscape features different from other green spaces such as forests or parks, because it includes historical and cultural elements in its view. Therefore, the evidence derived from this study might support the new function of traditional gardens for the health and well-being of urban dwellers.
Although an emerging body of research is focused on the diverse benefits of natural landscapes, very little has been considered on the effects of winter landscapes. Song et al. [43] investigated the effects of walking in urban parks in winter and found positive outcomes on the cardiovascular activity and psychological states. These outcomes reflected not only the effects of environmental stimuli but also the results of physical activity, which has limitations in considering the benefits of visual winter landscapes. Therefore, the present results may support the fact that these health benefits arise from the natural landscape itself even in the absence of lush green vegetation.
Important differences in the physiological and psychological responses to the two different landscapes were also detected between male and female groups. In 30-s segment NIRS data, the female group exhibited different patterns of O2Hb changes than the male group. When viewing the city landscape, the mean O2Hb values increased in the male group and slightly decreased in the female group, displaying a more significant difference in the left prefrontal area than in the right area. Similar trends were also observed in the diastolic blood pressure results. After viewing the two landscape images, the female group exhibited significantly lower values for the city, while the male group displayed higher values for the city. The women might have had a less negative perception of the city landscape than the male participants, because self-reported psychological tests indicated that the female group had lower levels of negative mood states related to the city than males. On the basis of these sex differences in multiple parameters, it is suggested that sex might be a factor for characterizing the individual differences for landscape preferences, as previously suggested by Lyons [44].
Despite the importance of individual differences in association with health benefits of natural environments, there is still not enough research-based evidence. Individual differences can diverge in the participants’ attributes, such as age, sex, socioeconomic status, and environmental experience [44,45]. Indeed, the results of this study revealed different health outcomes of two different landscapes between male and female participants. The data from the female participants might be partly inconsistent with previous studies showing the negative effects of city environments in young adults. Recent studies dealt with this issue from the view point of Type A personality or law of initial value [46,47]. In addition to these previous studies, the present experimental study provided valuable clues to individual differences in the health benefits of natural landscapes, despite the limitation of the small sample size. Solid evidence on this issue is still lacking and further studies are needed to advance natural environment as a health promoting agent.
Because of the increasing interest in their health benefits, there are many initiatives advocating the use of natural environments for promoting health, such as the Green Gym project [48]. The Natural Environment Initiative [49] was launched by Harvard University to investigate how natural environments can support human health and well-being through the participation of many specialists including public health scientists, landscape architects, and environmental psychologists. In Japan, the forest therapy project has been carried out to utilize the forest environment as a health promotion agent throughout the country. National movement is also undertaken in Korea through the reform of public policy and legislation to deal with the health problems of modern people, including depression and lifestyle-related diseases. From this perspective, traditional gardens may need to be considered as one of the health promoting resources for urban dwellers as well as historic and cultural resources. To materialize this idea, scientifically rigorous evidence is still required through further studies and active communication and collaboration among experts in the fields of public health, cultural properties, landscape architecture, and policymaking.

5. Conclusions

In the present study, the physiological and psychological responses to the city and garden landscapes were investigated in healthy male and female adults. For the garden landscape, the cerebral activity was decreased as evidenced by the declining O2Hb concentrations in the prefrontal areas. Negative mood states—including tension, anger, fatigue, confusion, and anxiety—were significantly reduced when viewing the garden compared with the city scenes. Differences in the physiological and psychological responses to the two different landscapes were detected between male and female groups.
When viewing the city landscape, the mean O2Hb values increased in the male group and slightly decreased in the female group. In the analysis of the diastolic blood pressure for the city landscape, the female group exhibited significantly lower values, while the male group displayed higher values. In multiple parameters, the female group exhibited less negative or slightly positive responses to the city landscape than the male group, suggesting that sex might be a factor for characterizing the individual differences for landscape preferences. Despite an emerging body of research in this field, the effects of winter landscapes have been rarely considered. The present study revealed a part of health-related values of winter landscape, supporting that these health benefits stem from the natural element itself even in the absence of lush green vegetation. The evidence derived from this study might support the new function of traditional gardens for the health and well-being of urban dwellers.

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by Association of Graduate Schools of Science and Technology (AGSST) program of Chiba University.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. United Nations. World Urbanization Prospects: 2014 Revision; United Nations: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  2. Park, C.M. The quality of life in South Korea. Soc. Indic. Res. 2009, 92, 263–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Danaei, G.; Singh, G.M.; Paciorek, C.J.; Lin, J.K.; Cowan, M.J.; Finucane, M.M.; Farzadfar, F.; Stevens, G.A.; Riley, L.M.; Lu, Y.; et al. The global burden of cardiovascular risk transition: Associations of four metabolic risk factors with national income, urbanization, and Western diet in 1980 and 2008. Circulation 2013, 127, 1493–1502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Galea, S.; Uddin, M.; Koenen, K. The urban environment and mental disorders: Epigenetic links. Epigenetics 2011, 6, 400–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Gong, P.; Liang, S.; Carlton, E.J.; Jiang, Q.; Wu, J.; Wang, L.; Remais, J.V. Urbanisation and health in China. Lancet 2012, 379, 843–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wagner, K.H.; Brath, H. A global view on the development of non communicable diseases. Prev. Med. 2012, 54, S38–S41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Li, Q.; Morimoto, K.; Nakadai, A.; Inagaki, H.; Katsumata, M.; Shimizu, T.; Hirata, Y.; Hirata, K.; Suzuki, H.; Miyazaki, Y.; et al. Forest bathing enhances human natural killer activity and expression of anti-cancer proteins. Int. J. Immunopathol. Pharmacol. 2007, 20, 3–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Selhub, E.; Logan, A.C. Your Brain on Nature; John Wiley and Sons: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  9. Takayama, N.; Korpela, K.; Lee, J.; Morikawa, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Park, B.J.; Li, Q.; Tyrväinen, L.; Miyazaki, Y.; Kagawa, T. Emotional, restorative and vitalizing effects of forest and urban environments at four sites in Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 7207–7230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lederbogen, F.; Kirsch, P.; Haddad, L.; Streit, F.; Tost, H.; Schuch, P.; Wüst, S.; Pruessner, J.C.; Rietschel, M.; Deuschle, M.; et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature 2011, 474, 498–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Lee, J.; Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Ohira, T.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Effect of forest bathing on physiological and psychological responses in young Japanese male subjects. Public Health 2011, 125, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Lee, J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Takayama, N.; Park, B.J.; Li, Q.; Song, C.; Komatsu, M.; Ikei, H.; Tyrväinen, L.; Kagawa, T.; et al. Influence of forest therapy on cardiovascular relaxation in young adults. Evid.-Based Complement. Altern. Med. 2014, 2014, 834360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kasetani, T.; Hirano, H.; Kagawa, T.; Sato, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the atmosphere of the forest)—Using salivary cortisol and cerebral activity as indicators. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2007, 26, 123–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Thompson, C.W.; Roe, J.; Aspinall, P.; Mitchell, R.; Clow, A.; Miller, D. More green space is linked to less stress in deprived communities: Evidence from salivary cortisol patterns. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2012, 105, 221–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Tsunetsugu, Y.; Lee, J.; Park, B.J.; Tyrväinen, L.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological effects of viewing urban forest landscapes assessed by multiple measurements. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013, 113, 90–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Mitchell, R.; Popham, F. Effect of exposure to natural environment on health inequalities: An observational population study. Lancet 2008, 372, 1655–1660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Takano, T.; Nakamura, K.; Watanabe, M. Urban residential environments and senior citizens’ longevity in megacity areas: The importance of walkable green spaces. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2002, 56, 913–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Korea Forest Service. Forestry Culture and Recreation Act; Ministry of Government Legislation: Daejeon, Korea, 2016.
  19. Velarde, M.D.; Fry, G.; Tveit, M. Health effects of viewing landscapes—Landscape types in environmental psychology. Urban For. Urban Green 2007, 6, 199–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Ulrich, R.S. View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science 1984, 224, 420–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Abraham, A.; Sommerhalder, K.; Abel, T. Landscape and well-being: A scoping study on the health-promoting impact of outdoor environments. Int. J. Public Health 2010, 55, 59–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Bowler, D.E.; Buyung-Ali, L.M.; Knight, T.M.; Pullin, A.S. A systematic review of evidence for the added benefits to health of exposure to natural environments. BMC Public Health 2010, 10, 456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Tzoulas, K.; Korpela, K.; Yli-Pelkonen, V.; Kaźmierczak, A.; Niemela, J.; Jamese, P. Promoting ecosystem and human health in urban areas using Green Infrastructure: A literature review. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2007, 81, 167–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Armstrong, D. A survey of community gardens in upstate New York: Implications for health promotion and community development. Health Place 2000, 6, 319–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hartig, T.; Marcus, C.C. Healing gardens-places for nature in health care. Lancet 2006, 368, 536–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kingsley, J.Y.; Townsend, M.; Henderson-Wilson, C. Cultivating health and wellbeing: Members’ perceptions of the health benefits of a Port Melbourne community garden. Leisure Stud. 2009, 28, 207–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Marcus, C.C. Healing gardens in hospitals. Interdiscip. Des. Res. 2007, 1, 1–27. [Google Scholar]
  28. Said, I. Therapeutic effects of garden: Preference of ill children towards garden over ward in Malaysian hospital environment. J. Teknol. 2003, 38, 55–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Sherman, S.A.; Varni, J.W.; Ulrich, R.S.; Malcarne, V.L. Post-occupancy evaluation of healing gardens in a pediatric cancer center. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2005, 73, 167–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ulrich, R.S. Effects of gardens on health outcomes: Theory and research. In Healing Gardens: Therapeutic Benefits and Design Recommendations; Cooper-Marcus, C., Barnes, M., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 27–85. [Google Scholar]
  31. Whitehouse, S.; Varni, J.W.; Seid, M.; Cooper-Marcus, C.; Ensberg, M.J.; Jacobs, J.R.; Mehlenbeck, R.S. Evaluating a children’s hospital garden environment: Utilization and consumer satisfaction. J. Environ. Psychol. 2001, 21, 301–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Abendroth, S.; Kowarik, I.; Müller, N.; von der Lippe, M. The green colonial heritage: Woody plants in parks of Bandung, Indonesia. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2012, 106, 12–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Feng, S.; Xu, K. The elaboration of five “Perspectives”: Application to traditional landscape idea in modern landscape design. Appl. Mech. Mater. 2012, 226–228, 2458–2461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hoshi, Y.; Kobayashi, N.; Tamura, M. Interpretation of near infrared spectroscopy signals: A study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. J. Appl. Physiol. 2001, 90, 1657–1662. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  35. Igarashi, M.; Yamamoto, T.; Lee, J.; Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Miyazaki, Y. Effects of stimulation by three-dimensional natural images on prefrontal cortex and autonomic nerve activity: A comparison with stimulation using two-dimensional images. Cogn. Process. 2014, 15, 551–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. McNair, D.M.; Lorr, M.; Droppleman, L.F. Revised Manual for the Profile of Mood States; Educational and Industrial Testing Service: San Diego, CA, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  37. Osgood, C.E.; Suci, G.J.; Tannenbaum, P.H. The Measurement of Meaning; University of Illinois Press: Urbana, IL, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
  38. Spielberger, C.D. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  39. Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kasetani, T.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. The physiological effects of shinrin-yoku (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing): Evidence from field experiments in 24 forests across Japan. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2010, 15, 18–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Lee, J.; Park, B.J.; Ohira, T.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Acute effects of exposure to a traditional rural environment on urban dwellers: A crossover field study in terraced farmland. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 1874–1893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Igarashi, M.; Takagaki, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological effects of a walk in urban parks in fall. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 14216–14228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Kuo, F.E. Coping with poverty: Impacts of environment and attention in the inner city. Environ. Behav. 2001, 33, 5–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Song, C.R.; Joung, D.; Ikei, H.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological effects of walking on young males in urban parks in winter. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2013, 32, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Lyons, E. Demographic correlates of landscape preference. Environ. Behav. 1983, 15, 487–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Jiang, B.; Chang, C.Y.; Sullivan, W.C. A dose of nature: Tree cover, stress reduction, and gender differences. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2014, 132, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Miyazaki, Y.; Tsunetsugu, Y. A tentative proposal on physiological polymorphism and its experimental approaches. J. Physiol. Anthropol. Appl. Hum. Sci. 2005, 24, 297–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Miyazaki, Y. Elucidation of a physiological adjustment effect in a forest environment: A pilot study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 4247–4255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Pretty, J.; Peacock, J.; Sellens, M.; Griffin, M. The mental and physical health outcomes of green exercise. Int. J. Environ. Health Res. 2005, 15, 319–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Africa, J.; Logan, A.; Mitchell, R.; Korpela, K.; Allen, D.; Tyrväinen, L.; Nisbet, E.; Li, Q.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Miyazaki, Y.; et al. The Natural Environment Initiative: Illustrative Review and Workshop Statement; Center for Health and the Global Environment at the Harvard School of Public Health: Boston, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Landscape images of city and garden used in the experiment.
Figure 1. Landscape images of city and garden used in the experiment.
Ijerph 14 00829 g001
Figure 2. Changes in oxy-hemoglobin (O2Hb) in the left (top) and right (bottom) prefrontal cortex areas when observing city and garden landscapes. Mean ± standard error; N = 18; bar means significant difference.
Figure 2. Changes in oxy-hemoglobin (O2Hb) in the left (top) and right (bottom) prefrontal cortex areas when observing city and garden landscapes. Mean ± standard error; N = 18; bar means significant difference.
Ijerph 14 00829 g002
Figure 3. Comparison of oxy-hemoglobin (O2Hb) concentrations between male and female participants when observing city and garden landscapes in early, mid, and late sessions. Mean ± standard error; Male N = 9; Female N = 9; * p < 0.05.
Figure 3. Comparison of oxy-hemoglobin (O2Hb) concentrations between male and female participants when observing city and garden landscapes in early, mid, and late sessions. Mean ± standard error; Male N = 9; Female N = 9; * p < 0.05.
Ijerph 14 00829 g003
Figure 4. Comparison of the diastolic blood pressure (DBP) between city and garden landscapes in all (left), male (middle), and female (right) participants. Mean ± standard error; Total N = 18; Male N = 9, Female N = 9; * p < 0.05.
Figure 4. Comparison of the diastolic blood pressure (DBP) between city and garden landscapes in all (left), male (middle), and female (right) participants. Mean ± standard error; Total N = 18; Male N = 9, Female N = 9; * p < 0.05.
Ijerph 14 00829 g004
Figure 5. Comparison of the profile of mood states (POMS) scores between city and garden landscapes. T-A, tension-anxiety; D, depression; A-H, anger-hostility; F, fatigue; C, confusion; V, vigor; Mean ± standard error; N = 18; ** p < 0.01.
Figure 5. Comparison of the profile of mood states (POMS) scores between city and garden landscapes. T-A, tension-anxiety; D, depression; A-H, anger-hostility; F, fatigue; C, confusion; V, vigor; Mean ± standard error; N = 18; ** p < 0.01.
Ijerph 14 00829 g005
Figure 6. Comparison of the semantic differential (SD) scores for eight different feelings between city and garden landscapes. Subfigures indicate the feelings of natural-artificial (A), open-closed (B), attractive for walking-unattractive for walking (C), comfortable–uncomfortable (D), familiar-unfamiliar (E), clean-dirty (F), beautiful-ugly (G) and warm-cold (H). Mean ± standard error; N = 18; ** p < 0.01.
Figure 6. Comparison of the semantic differential (SD) scores for eight different feelings between city and garden landscapes. Subfigures indicate the feelings of natural-artificial (A), open-closed (B), attractive for walking-unattractive for walking (C), comfortable–uncomfortable (D), familiar-unfamiliar (E), clean-dirty (F), beautiful-ugly (G) and warm-cold (H). Mean ± standard error; N = 18; ** p < 0.01.
Ijerph 14 00829 g006
Figure 7. Comparison of the state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI) scores between city and garden landscapes. Mean ± standard error; N = 18; ** p < 0.01.
Figure 7. Comparison of the state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI) scores between city and garden landscapes. Mean ± standard error; N = 18; ** p < 0.01.
Ijerph 14 00829 g007
Table 1. Baseline values of the participants.
Table 1. Baseline values of the participants.
ParametersTotal aMale aFemale a
MeanSEMeanSEMeanSE
Age26.70.727.81.125.60.8
Height (cm)167.41.3171.21.0163.71.5
Weight (kg)57.71.762.81.452.61.9
BMI (kg/m2) b20.50.421.40.519.60.5
Pulse rate (bpm)67.61.769.13.166.21.5
SBP (mmHg) b107.92.7115.83.4100.11.9
DBP (mmHg) b64.41.966.03.362.82.0
a Total sample N = 18; Male sample N = 9; Female sample N = 9; b BMI, body mass index; SBP, systolic blood pressure; DBP, diastolic blood pressure.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lee, J. Experimental Study on the Health Benefits of Garden Landscape. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 829. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14070829

AMA Style

Lee J. Experimental Study on the Health Benefits of Garden Landscape. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2017; 14(7):829. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14070829

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lee, Juyoung. 2017. "Experimental Study on the Health Benefits of Garden Landscape" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 14, no. 7: 829. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14070829

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop