Next Article in Journal
Lifestyle Factors and Metabolic Syndrome among Workers: The Role of Interactions between Smoking and Alcohol to Nutrition and Exercise
Next Article in Special Issue
Simultaneous Training for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Their Parents with a Focus on Social Skills Enhancement
Previous Article in Journal
Virulence Profiles of Vibrio vulnificus in German Coastal Waters, a Comparison of North Sea and Baltic Sea Isolates
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

An Autistic Endophenotype and Testosterone Are Involved in an Atypical Decline in Selective Attention and Visuospatial Processing in Middle-Aged Women

by
Ángel Romero-Martínez
* and
Luis Moya-Albiol
Department of Psychobiology, University of Valencia, Avenida Blasco Ibañez, 21, 46010 Valencia, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12(12), 15960-15966; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121215033
Submission received: 21 October 2015 / Revised: 3 December 2015 / Accepted: 11 December 2015 / Published: 15 December 2015
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Mental Healthcare and Autism)

Abstract

:
Mothers of offspring with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) could present mild forms of their children’s cognitive characteristics, resulting from prenatal brain exposure and sensitivity to testosterone (T). Indeed, their cognition is frequently characterized by hyper-systemizing, outperforming in tests that assess cognitive domains such as selective attention, and fine motor and visuospatial skills. In the general population, all these start to decline around the mid-forties. This study aimed to characterize whether middle-aged women who are biological mothers of individuals with ASD had better performance in the aforementioned cognitive skills than mothers of normative children (in both groups n = 22; mean age = 45), using the standardized Stroop and mirror-drawing tests. We also examined the role of T in their performance in the aforementioned tests. ASD mothers outperformed controls in both tests, giving more correct answers and making fewer mistakes. In addition, they presented higher T levels, which have been associated with better cognitive performance. Cognitive decline in specific skills with aging could be delayed in these middle-aged women, corresponding to a cognitive endophenotype, T playing an important role in this process.

1. Introduction

The existence of an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) endophenotype and the high heritability of this disorder could explain why ASD parents present mild forms of cognitive characteristics that are fully developed in their offspring. Like their offspring, ASD parents tend to present communication and cognitive flexibility deficits. Nevertheless, due to their highly systemizing minds they outperform or at least have similar scores to parents of non-affected children in certain cognitive abilities, such as selective attention and visuospatial processing [1,2,3,4]. These abilities and also psychomotor skills become impaired with aging [5,6,7], the decline starting around the mid-forties [8]. Nevertheless, the effect of aging on these cognitive skills has not been analyzed in first-degree relatives of people with ASD such as their biological mothers.
In parallel with cognitive decline, testosterone (T) levels drop with aging [9]. Moreover, the administration of exogenous T and consequent increase in T levels in young and elderly women improves processing speed, selective attention, and fine motor and visuospatial skills [10,11,12]. Several authors have indicated that chronically stressed middle-aged women have higher T levels, including mothers of people with ASD compared to non-stressed women [13,14]. There is, however, a gap in the literature concerning the role of T in cognition in ASD mothers.
Given the complexity of this issue, as a first approach, we decided to investigate the performance of mothers of offspring with ASD on selective attention and fine motor and visuospatial processing speed tests. We aimed to investigate the existence of the ASD endophenotype and the effects of T levels on the performance of middle-aged ASD mothers in these cognitive domains. For this purpose, we tested ASD mothers and matched control women using two tests, the Stroop test and a mirror-drawing test. Based on the fact that ASD mothers present highly systemizing minds, we hypothesized that they would not present the decline in cognitive function in these domains that typically occurs with aging, showing better performance than controls [1,2,3,4,15]. We also examined the effects of T on these cognitive tasks. We expected to find that a higher baseline T and stronger T response to the tasks would be associated with better performance in these tasks, due to the positive association between T and these abilities in women [10,11,12].

2. Method

2.1. Participants

The final sample was composed of 46 participants, including 22 mothers of individuals with ASD and 22 mothers of individuals with typical developing (with no neurodevelopmental or psychiatric conditions).
ASD mothers were recruited from an association of parents of people with autism located in Valencia, Spain (Asociación Valenciana de Padres de Autistas). They participated voluntarily in the study and gave written informed consent in accordance with ethical guidelines for human research (Declaration of Helsinki). The ages of the offspring with ASD ranged from five to 30 years (14.62 ± 6.90), with a sex ratio of 20 males to two females, which is attributable to the high prevalence of this disorder in males. Offspring were clinically diagnosed with ASD by clinical staff following the DSM-5 criteria. Moreover, the majorities of the ASD offspring are highly dependent on their parents and live at home with their parents. Furthermore, those individuals with ASD never work and they also require lifelong care. The inclusion criterion for participating in the study was being a biological mother of an individual with a clinical diagnosis of an ASD. An adequate control group was selected: this was composed of mothers in the same age-range who had offspring without autistic traits and no chronic illness. Furthermore, in the control group, the number of offspring per mother and the mean age of the children were similar to that in the other group.

2.2. Procedure

Participants were instructed to abstain from eating, drinking stimulants (such as tea, coffee, or alcohol), brushing their teeth, and smoking during the 2-h period before arriving at the laboratory. The experimental procedure was performed between 4:00 p.m. and 7:00 p.m. to minimize hormonal variations attributable to the circadian rhythm, and each session lasted approximately 2.5 h. On arrival, participants’ anthropometric data (age, weight, height) were collected, compliance with the instructions was checked, and an initial saliva sample was collected to measure baseline testosterone levels. Participants were then taken to a different room that was soundproofed and kept at a constant temperature (22 ± 1 °C) for the neuropsychological tests. After a 5-min habituation period and a brief introduction to the tests, the participants remained silent for 3 min and then the next saliva sample was collected (to assess preparatory period T levels). All participants spent around the same period of time completing the Stroop and mirror-drawing tests (25–30 min). Immediately after completing these tests, a further saliva sample was collected (for post-test T levels). The participants then returned to the first room where additional saliva samples were collected at 20 and 30 min (for assessing T during recovery).

2.3. Neuropsychological Tests

2.3.1. The Stroop Test

The Spanish version of the Stroop Color-Word Test (Stroop test) [16] was used to assess the participants’ ability to “resist interference”. They were instructed to name the ink color of the printed words while ignoring distracting stimuli (color name). The number of correct responses given in 45 s in the color-word condition was recorded as the dependent variable, as recommended in recent research [17,18]. Errors were indicated by the examiner, and participants were asked to correct them before continuing as quickly and as accurately as possible.

2.3.2. The Mirror-Drawing Test

Mirror-drawing tests consist of tracing a shape that can only be seen inverted in a mirror. Specifically, in the test used, a mirror is mounted on a vertically orientated metallic support, while another support prevents participants from having a direct view of the model, the outline of a star. Every time there is a divergence from the line of the star, a short beep sounds indicating that the participant has made a mistake. Participants were allowed one practice circuit and instructed to prioritize accuracy over speed. The test needs multi-process agreement because the reversed visual feedback occurs concurrently with the process of visual-proprioceptive recalibration [19], and, hence, high levels of attention are necessary [20]. We used the number of traces (to the nearest ¼ of a circuit) completed in 120 s and errors as dependent variables.

2.4. Hormone Measurements

Saliva was directly collected from the mouth to a glass tube for T measurements. Participants were informed about the need to closely follow the instructions for saliva sampling in order to obtain valuable data. The samples were frozen at −20 °C until analysis by enzyme immunoassay using a T saliva Elisa kit (Diagnostics Biochem Canada Inc.: Dorchester, ON, Canada). The assay sensitivity was 1 pg/mL and results were expressed in pmol/L. Good precision was obtained, with intra- and inter-assay variation coefficients of 3.98% and 7.98%, respectively.

2.5. Data Analysis

After confirming the normality of the data using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, t-tests were performed with “group” as the between-subject factor for anthropometric data (age and body mass index) and cognitive test scores. Chi-square statistics were calculated for analyzing the frequencies of the demographic variables.
Magnitude of T response was estimated by the area under the curve with respect to the increase (AUCi), using the trapezoidal rule [21]. AUCi is calculated with reference to the baseline measurement, ignoring the distance from zero for all measurements and emphasizing the changes over time.
Spearman’s or Pearson’s correlations were calculated to assess relationships between variables as appropriate for each group (ASD mothers and controls).
Data analyses were carried out using IBM SPSS (Version 22.0, IBM: New York, NY, USA). Statistical significance was defined as p values ≤ 0.05; while there was considered to be a tendency to significance with p values from > 0.05 to p ≤ 0.07. Average values are reported in the tables as mean ± SD.

3. Results

Descriptive characteristics and cognitive performance on the neuropsychological tests of both ASD mothers and controls are summarized in Table 1. The groups did not differ in age, body mass index, menstrual cycle phase, marital status, educational level, number of children, smoking status or drug use. Moreover, a significant “group” effect was found in the AUCi for T (F1,44 = 4.20, p = 0.04, gp 2 = 0.10). In fact, though baseline T levels were not significantly different, T levels increased in ASD mothers but fell in controls.
A significant “group” effect was found in the “Stroop test correct responses”, “mirror-drawing test number of traces” and “mirror-drawing test number of mistakes” t44 = 2.39, p = 0.021, d = 0.72; t44 = 2.16, p = 0.036, d = 0.65, and t44 = −2.50, p = 0.016, d = 0.75, respectively. ASD mothers obtained more correct responses and traces in the Stroop test and mirror-drawing test and fewer errors in the mirror-drawing test than controls.
Relationships of T with Stroop test and mirror-drawing test performance for ASD mothers and controls are summarized in Table 2.
Table 1. Mean ± SD of age and body mass index (BMI) and demographic variables for mothers with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) offspring (ASD mothers) and controls.
Table 1. Mean ± SD of age and body mass index (BMI) and demographic variables for mothers with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) offspring (ASD mothers) and controls.
Demographic VariablesASD MothersControls
Age45.27 ± 1.7145.00 ± 0.90
BMI26.69 ± 0.9925.37 ± 1.13
Number of children1.92 ± 0.822.18 ± 0.90
Number of children at home1.71 ± 0.752.14 ± 0.88
Phases of menstrual cycle
Luteal (Day 1–14)45.4%33.4%
Follicular (Day 15-menstrual period)36.4%45.8%
Amenorrhea (>6 months)18.2%20.8%
Educational level
≤12 years32%23%
University degree(s)68%77%
Marital status
Married/Cohabiting82%86%
Divorced/Widowed/Single18%14%
Employment status
Employed95%95%
Unemployed0%0%
Retired/Other5%5%
Smoker
Yes21%40%
No79%60%
Drug use (medical)
No54%36%
Yes46%64%
Care recipient characteristics and caregiving situation
Autism-spectrum quotient (AQ adolescent)—degree of autism31.75 ± 4.73
Time caregiving since definitive Diagnosis (years)9.73 ± 1.03
Global activity49.23 ± 5.37
Independence (Barthel Index)84.17 ± 3.61
Disability percentage38.85 ± 8.27
Time caring per week (h/week)62.13 ± 45.34
Table 2. Pearson correlations between testosterone (T) and cognitive performance for mothers of ASD offspring and controls. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 2. Pearson correlations between testosterone (T) and cognitive performance for mothers of ASD offspring and controls. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Stroop TestMirror Drawing Test Number of TracesMirror Drawing Test Errors
ASDControlsASDControlsASDControls
Baseline0.0360.0190.2880.1960.0560.022
AUCi total0.0880.0210.0310.0980.0330.131
AUCi preparatory0.558 **0.0440.469 *0.0580.2390.055

4. Discussion

ASD mothers gave more correct responses in the Stroop test and completed more traces in the mirror-drawing test, and further, they committed fewer mistakes in the mirror-drawing test than controls. In addition, an analysis of the relationship between T and cognitive performance revealed that a large increase in T levels in the preparatory period was associated with better performance in both tests.
Studies with ASD mothers have suggested that they present highly systemizing minds, outperforming their peers in certain tasks, particularly those which need good fine motor and visuospatial skills [14,22,23,24]. Our results support this hypothesis, in that, ASD women outperformed controls in the Stroop and mirror-drawing tests, demonstrating higher selective attention, better fine motor and visuospatial skills and processing speed than controls.
On the other hand, we should underline that ASD mothers of this study are caregivers of their offspring for a long period of time. That is, they are under chronic stress, and this has been associated with an acceleration of cognitive decline in processing speed and attention [25,26,27]. However, the healthy caregiver hypothesis suggests that caregivers may maintain their cognitive abilities, because caregiving requires engagement in tasks that are cognitively complex such as managing medications, arranging appointments and transportation, and juggling conflicting demands and this is consistent with our results; that is, exercising and challenging cognitive skills seem to slow the rate of cognitive decline [28]. Further research is needed, however, to clarify the specific components of cognition that are impaired or preserved.
It has also been suggested that high T levels may help maintain cognitive skills in both young and elderly women [10,11,12]. Our data is congruent with this hypothesis, in that, large increases in T before the test were associated with better performance in ASD mothers. Moreover, ASD mothers had higher overall T levels than controls and this could explain why they outperformed controls in these specific skills. These findings imply that the caregiving role may reduce the cognitive decline with aging in this population by triggering higher T levels, and this could be understood as a mechanism to safeguard the wellbeing of the care recipient. As chronic stress tends to be associated with high cortisol (C) levels, which tends to show deleterious effects on cognition, it would be also interesting to analyze the role of C in this kind of population. Moreover, the T maintains an inverse relationship with the C. Hence, future studies should consider the relationship between those hormones and their role on human cognition.
The main limitation of this study is that it is cross-sectional, and individual differences may mask other effects assessed at a single point in life. Moreover, we should recognize that it is exploratory in nature with the small sample size. It is, however, extremely difficult to conduct longitudinal studies of caregivers, given their caregiving commitments. Nonetheless, our data are relevant given the growing need for caregiving, and they are novel, as no laboratory studies have analyzed the T response to selective attention and visuospatial skill tests in middle-aged female caregivers of individuals with ASD.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, this study demonstrates that cognitive decline with aging in middle-aged women who have children with ASD could be delayed or reduced, specifically in processing speed, selective attention, and fine motor and visuospatial skills by the ASD cognitive endophenotype and/or their systemizing minds. Moreover, it seems that T may play an important role in this process. Hence, it is essential to highlight the existence of this cognitive endophenotype characterized by high cognitive sensitivity to T effects and to develop a neuropsychological general assessment to check which domains should be targeted by interventions in this population.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Education (PSI2008-04408/PSIC) and the Science Policy Unit at the Department of Education of the Valencian regional government (PROMETEOII/2015/020). These funders had no further role in the study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of the data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Author Contributions

Ángel Romero-Martínez, and Luis Moya-Albiol conceived and designed the experiments; Ángel Romero-Martínez, and Luis Moya-Albiol performed the experiments; Ángel Romero-Martínez analyzed the data; Ángel Romero-Martínez and Luis Moya-Albiol contributed with reagents/materials/analysis tools; Ángel Romero-Martínez, and Luis Moya-Albiol wrote the paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Gokcen, S.; Bora, E.; Erermis, S.; Kesikci, H.; Aydin, C. Theory of mind and verbal working memory deficits in parents of autistic children. Psychiatry Res. 2009, 166, 46–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Ropar, D.; Mitchell, P. Susceptibility to illusions and performance on visuospatial tasks in individuals with autism. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 2001, 42, 539–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Wheelwright, S.; Auyeung, B.; Allison, C.; Baron-Cohen, S. Defining the broader, medium and narrow autism phenotype among parents using the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Mol. Autism. 2010, 1, 2–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Wong, D.; Maybery, M.; Bishop, D.V.; Maley, A.; Hallmayer, J. Profiles of executive function in parents and siblings of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Genes Brain Behav. 2006, 5, 561–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Inzitari, M.; Newman, A.B.; Yaffe, K.; Boudreau, R.; de Rekeneire, N.; Shorr, R.; Harris, T.B.; Rosano, C. Gait speed predicts decline in attention and psychomotor speed in older adults: The health aging and body composition study. Neuroepidemiology 2007, 29, 156–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Jenkins, L.; Myerson, J.; Joerding, J.A.; Hale, S. Converging evidence that visuospatial cognition is more age-sensitive than verbal cognition. Psychol. Aging 2000, 15, 157–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Klencklen, G.; Despres, O.; Dufour, A. What do we know about aging and spatial cognition? Reviews and perspectives. Aging Res. Rev. 2012, 11, 123–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Deary, I.J. Intelligence. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2012, 63, 453–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Ravdin, L.D.; Katzen, H.L. Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Aging and Dementia; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  10. Aleman, A.; Bronk, E.; Kessels, R.P.; Koppeschaar, H.P.; van Honk, J. A single administration of testosterone improves visuospatial ability in young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004, 29, 612–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Dettenborn, L.; Hinkelmann, K.; Muhtz, C.; Gao, W.; Wingenfeld, K.; Spitzer, C.; Moritz, S.; Kirschbaum, C.; Otte, C. Hair testosterone and visuospatial memory in middle-aged men and women with and without depressive symptoms. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013, 38, 2373–2377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Pike, C.J.; Carroll, J.C.; Rosario, E.R.; Barron, A.M. Protective actions of sex steroid hormones in Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2009, 30, 239–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Powell, L.H.; Lovallo, W.R.; Matthews, K.A.; Meyer, P.; Midgley, A.R.; Baum, A.; Stone, A.A.; Underwood, L.; McCann, J.J.; Janikula Herro, K.; et al. Physiologic markers of chronic stress in premenopausal, middle-aged women. Psychosom. Med. 2002, 64, 502–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Romero-Martínez, A.; de Andrés-García, S.; Ruiz-Robledillo, N.; González-Bono, E.; Moya-Albiol, L. High cognitive sensitivity to activational effects of testosterone in parents of offspring with autism spectrum disorders. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2014, 71, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Cruz, L.P.; Camargos-Júnior, W.; Rocha, F.L. The broad autism phenotype in parents of individuals with autism: A systematic review of the literature. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2013, 35, 252–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Golden, C.J. Test de Colores y Palabras Stroop; TEA Ediciones: Manual, Madrid, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  17. López-Villalobos, J.A.; Serrano-Pintado, I.; Andrés-De Llano, J.M.; Sánchez-Mateos, J.D.; Alberola-López, S.; Sánchez-Azón, M.I. Usefulness of the Stroop test in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Rev. Neurol. 2010, 50, 333–40. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  18. Sánchez-Cubillo, I.; Periáñez, J.A.; Adrover-Roig, D.; Rodríguez-Sanchez, J.M.; Ríos-Lago, M.; Tirapu, J. Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: Role of task switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2009, 15, 438–450. [Google Scholar]
  19. Miall, R.C.; Cole, J. Evidence for stronger visuo-motor than visuo-proprioceptive conflict during mirror drawing performed by a deafferented subject and control subjects. Exp. Brain Res. 2007, 176, 432–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Mizuki, Y.; Takii, O.; Tanaka, T.; Tanaka, M.; Inanaga, K. Periodic appearance of frontal midline theta activity during performance of a sensory-motor task. Folia Psychiatr. Neurol. Jpn. 1982, 36, 375–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Pruessner, J.C.; Kirschbaum, C.; Meinlschmid, G.; Hellhammer, D.H. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2003, 28, 916–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Baron-Cohen, S. The hyper-systemizing, assortative mating theory of autism. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2009, 30, 865–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Grove, R.; Baillie, A.; Allison, C.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Hoekstra, R.A. Empathizing, systemizing, and autistic traits: Latent structure in individuals with autism, their parents, and general population controls. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2013, 122, 600–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Romero-Martínez, A.; de Andrés-García, S.; Sariñana-González, P.; Sanchis-Calatayud, M.V.; Roa, J.M.; González-Bono, E.; Moya-Albiol, L. The 2D:4D ratio and its relationship with other androgenisation parameters in parents of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Ann. Psychol. 2013, 29, 264–271. [Google Scholar]
  25. Miller, L.S.; Lewis, M.S.; Williamson, G.M.; Lance, C.E.; Dooley, W.K.; Schulz, R.; Weiner, M.F. Caregiver cognitive status and potentially harmful caregiver behavior. Aging Ment. Health 2006, 10, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Oken, B.S.; Fonareva, I.; Wahbeh, H. Stress-related cognitive dysfunction in dementia caregivers. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 2011, 24, 191–198. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  27. Mackenzie, C.S.; Smith, M.C.; Hasher, L.; Leach, L.; Behl, P. Cognitive functioning under stress: Evidence from informal caregivers of palliative patients. J. Palliat. Med. 2007, 10, 749–758. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  28. Bertrand, R.M.; Saczynski, J.S.; Mezzacappa, C.; Hulse, M.; Ensrud, K.; Fredman, L. Caregiving and cognitive function in older women: Evidence for the healthy caregiver hypothesis. J. Aging Health 2012, 24, 48–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Romero-Martínez, Á.; Moya-Albiol, L. An Autistic Endophenotype and Testosterone Are Involved in an Atypical Decline in Selective Attention and Visuospatial Processing in Middle-Aged Women. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 15960-15966. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121215033

AMA Style

Romero-Martínez Á, Moya-Albiol L. An Autistic Endophenotype and Testosterone Are Involved in an Atypical Decline in Selective Attention and Visuospatial Processing in Middle-Aged Women. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2015; 12(12):15960-15966. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121215033

Chicago/Turabian Style

Romero-Martínez, Ángel, and Luis Moya-Albiol. 2015. "An Autistic Endophenotype and Testosterone Are Involved in an Atypical Decline in Selective Attention and Visuospatial Processing in Middle-Aged Women" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 12, no. 12: 15960-15966. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121215033

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop