Next Article in Journal
Nature Inspired Engineering: Biomimetic Sensors
Previous Article in Journal
Integrated Explainable Diagnosis of Gear Wear Faults Based on Dynamic Modeling and Data-Driven Representation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Toward Wearable MagnetoCardioGraphy (MCG) for Cognitive Workload Monitoring: Advancements in Sensor and Study Design

1
Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
2
Center for Research Injury Research and Policy, Abigail Wexner Research Institute at Nationwide Children’s Hospital, Columbus, OH 43205, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sensors 2025, 25(15), 4806; https://doi.org/10.3390/s25154806
Submission received: 1 July 2025 / Revised: 29 July 2025 / Accepted: 1 August 2025 / Published: 5 August 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Wearables)

Abstract

Despite cognitive workload (CW) being a critical metric in several applications, no technology exists to seamlessly and reliably quantify CW. Previously, we demonstrated the feasibility of a wearable MagnetoCardioGraphy (MCG) sensor to classify high vs. low CW based on MCG-derived heart rate variability (mHRV). However, our sensor was unable to address certain critical operational requirements, resulting in noisy signals, often to the point of being unusable. In addition, test conditions for the participants were not decoupled from motion (i.e., physical activity (PA)), raising questions as to whether the noted changes in mHRV were attributed to CW, PA, or both. This study reports software and hardware advancements to optimize the MCG data quality, and investigates whether changes in CW (in the absence of PA) can be reliably detected. Performance is validated for healthy adults (n = 10) performing three types of CW tasks (one for low CW and two for high CW to eliminate the memory effect). Results demonstrate the ability to retrieve MCG R-peaks throughout the recordings, as well as the ability to differentiate high vs. low CW in all cases, confirming that CW does modulate the mHRV. A paired Bonferroni t-test with significance α = 0.01 confirms the hypothesis that an increase in CW decreases mHRV. Our findings lay the groundwork toward a seamless, practical, and low-cost sensor for monitoring CW.

1. Introduction

Cognitive workload (CW) is defined as the mental effort exerted during a mental task [1,2]. Regulating CW is widely recognized as a means of enhancing human performance, reducing the likelihood of errors, and supporting the optimal physical and mental well-being of individuals [3,4]. Monitoring CW is especially crucial in safety-critical settings like cockpits, air traffic control, and driving, where even minor errors can have severe consequences [5]. Similarly, CW assessment is critical in a variety of clinical and healthcare contexts, where mental effort, attention, or fatigue can significantly affect safety, performance, and therapeutic outcomes [6,7,8].
Today’s gold-standard for monitoring CW relies on self-reporting methods that are subjective/biased and, hence, unreliable [9]. Objective physiological measures have been explored for assessing CW. ElectroEncephaloGraphy (EEG), MagnetoEncephaloGraphy (MEG), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positron emission tomography (PET), and eye tracking/pupillometry are the most common physiological measures of CW. EEG directly captures the brain’s electrical activity through electrodes positioned on the scalp. This method tracks variations in CW over time and delivers results with high temporal precision [10]. MEG measures the magnetic fields generated by the brain’s electrical activity. It has shown to achieve similar accuracies as EEG [11]. fMRI detects changes in blood flow related to neural activity. It measures the signal that is dependent on blood oxygenation level and has shown to monitor brain activity with high spatial resolution [12,13]. PET observes metabolic processes in the brain. It can monitor dopamine receptors at rest and while performing a cognitive task [14]. Task-evoked pupillary responses (TEPR) have also shown to effectively estimate the cognitive effort involved in completing a task [15]. However, although the aforementioned techniques have shown to be effective, they can be very complicated to construct and operate or very expensive to obtain.
To address such challenges, we recently introduced a wearable MagnetoCardioGraphy (MCG) sensor with the ability to differentiate high vs. low CW using MCG-derived heart rate variability (mHRV) metrics [16]. Here, we refer to the MCG-derived HRV as mHRV to distinguish it from ElectroCardioGraphy (ECG)-based HRV, noting that while MCG and ECG originate from the same source, they are not identical (much as pulse rate variability, or PRV, is not identical to HRV [17]). As is well known, a healthy heart does not beat at a constant rhythm; instead, its natural fluctuations enable the cardiovascular system to quickly respond to sudden physical or mental demands that disrupt the body’s balance. Accordingly, the autonomic nervous system regulates heart function in response to CW: high CW tends to increase sympathetic activity and suppress parasympathetic activity, leading to reduced beat-to-beat variability. To this end, numerous studies have utilized HRV as an indirect measure of CW [18,19,20,21,22]. However, unlike ECG, MCG has numerous advantages, including a non-contact operation, insensitivity to hair and sweat, and robustness to changes in biological tissue properties (since tissues are non-magnetic) [23]. Notably, we have been the first to explore the classification of CW using mHRV acquired from wearable low-cost MCG sensors [16]. In brief, our first such sensors consisted of an array of seven (7) miniature coils placed upon the chest to passively capture the naturally emanated cardiac magnetic fields. To visualize such signals, the raw data collected by the sensor were amplified and post-processed, as detailed in [16].
Despite the promising results, our study [16]—as the first of its kind—suffered from several limitations. First, the MCG sensor hardware exhibited structural fragility, making it susceptible to frequent malfunctions and noisy data collection. Specifically, the outputs of each of the sensing coils were soldered to jumper wires that were, in turn, soldered to an amplifier board. The presence of these 14 wires (2 for each coil, with a total of 7 coils) not only limited the mobility of the sensor but also increased the system noise. Second, the coils were not embedded in appropriate mounting fixtures, causing them to jitter, further contributing to noise. Third, the MCG sensor was secured to the subject’s chest with gauze tape, causing unwanted vibrations during use and further increasing the noise. Fourth, the employed signal processing (simply averaging across the coils and filtering) lacked the sophistication required to de-noise the data. As a result, the MCG data from some subjects had to be completely discarded, while even the `good’ recordings had to be reduced to 0.6 times the original duration (not necessarily continuous in time) to ensure that the signal R-peaks were visible. Finally, the test conditions used for the participants were not decoupled from motion (i.e., physical activity, PA), raising concerns as to whether the noted changes in mHRV were attributed to CW, PA, or both. Specifically, participants were recording their answers for the high CW task manually on a cell phone, whereas the literature (and our own studies) have shown that even subtle movements or stress responses can influence the mHRV [16,24,25].
In a major step forward, we herewith introduce significant advancements in hardware architecture, signal processing methodologies, and study design to overcome the above-mentioned limitations. The approach was validated for (n = 10) healthy adult participants performing three types of CW tasks (one for low CW and two for high CW to eliminate the memory effect). Our work resulted in a robust and accurate MCG sensor with wearable form-factor and low-cost fabrication, demonstrated to retrieve the MCG R-peaks for the entire duration of the recordings and classify high vs. low CW in 100% of the cases. Our findings also confirm that CW alone does modulate mHRV, regardless of the presence of PA and, thus, lays the groundwork for future work in the area of MCG-based CW classification. In summary, the key advantage of our work is leveraging MCG as a completely passive cardiac measure for CW. Unlike previous technologies for CW monitoring, our approach requires no skin contact; it senses the heart’s magnetic field directly. This enables continuous monitoring without electrode setup or skin irritation. Furthermore, we operate in normal ambient conditions (no shielding), which is novel. In addition, the setup is very cheap to build and operate, as opposed to PET and MEG. Aside from the hardware aspects, another key contribution of this work lies in the separation of changes in HRV from CW and PA. Although the literature has confirmed the relationship between changes in CW and HRV, no published work exists that tracks the changes back to CW.
The rest of the paper is organized as follows: Section 2 provides information on the MCG sensor hardware, signal processing methodology, study scenarios, and study participants. Experimental results are reported in Section 3. We discuss the results, practical/clinical implications of our findings, and future work in Section 4. The paper concludes in Section 5.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. MCG Sensor Hardware

The MCG sensor employed in this work is shown in Figure 1a and was intended to capture the naturally emanated magnetic fields of the heart, namely the MCG signal. One potential concern that may arise with the wearable MCG sensor is the possibility of the resulting signal being attributed to heart vibrations or sounds as opposed to MCG. However, as addressed in our previous research [26], the recorded signal is solely attributed to the heart’s magnetic field, and the sensor does not measure acoustic heart sounds. Indeed, the coil design has been specifically optimized to pick up MCG signals, while the accompanying signal processing further helps suppress noise while preserving the cardiac signal. The sensor consisted of an array of eight (8) coils (each 12 mm in height and 16.6 mm in outer diameter), embedded within a 3D-printed fixture of 95 mm in diameter. The coils were the same as those used in [16]; however, their total number was increased from 7 (in [16]) to 8 (in this work), to enhance uncorrelated noise suppression. Specifically, averaging the same MCG signal over N number of coils is expected to reduce the noise by N . The number N = 8 was optimized for a maximum number of coils fitting within the 95 mm diameter of the sensor, with the latter diameter selected for an optimal fit upon the average human heart.
The initial optimization parameters for the coil design were previously established in [27], where the induction coil sensor was designed based on the model of a tightly wound air core coil with an inner diameter D i , outer diameter D, length L, and wire diameter d. In this case, the sensitivity can be written as
S D 2.5 = M 1 + D D i 1 D i D 5 1 D i D 3 1 4 ( 1 D i D 2
and obtains maximum values if D i D 0.6 and L D 0.7 . Therefore, by selecting a value for D, optimal values for D i and L can be chosen. As for the array holding these coils, the design specifications were constrained by two main criteria:
  • An array that can properly hold the coils in place,
  • A sensor that can be properly fixed on the chest with no jittering.
To address the first criterion, we 3D-printed a design with holes that are equivalent to the abovementioned length, L. By doing this, we made sure that the coils are held in place without moving. To address the second criterion, we added hooks and introduced ratchet straps so that the sensor will remain in place.
Contrary to [16] where the coils where simply positioned on a plastic base and secured on this base, as well as upon the wearer, with tape (see Figure 1b), a new 3D-printed fixture with a 3-layer structure was introduced. The following explains in details the different layers of the sensor as shown in Figure 2:
  • The bottom layer included a dedicated pocket (each 12 mm deep) to keep each of the coils in place and avoid jittering. This step is critical, as jittering can misalign the waveforms of each of the 8 coils: any misalignment between heart beats results in imperfect constructive averaging, reducing the effectiveness of noise cancellation. In fact, even small temporal jitters can blur sharp features (e.g., QRS complexes), limiting the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) gain. The bottom layer also included hooks that accommodated 3 ratchet straps to hold the sensor in place on the subject’s chest (see Figure 3). There were 8 hooks in total with 6 serving to hold the ratchet straps attachments (2 hooks per strap) and 2 serving as latches for the top layer of the sensor. At the end of each ratchet strap, we placed a tightening buckle as needed to better adjust the size and enhance the participant’s comfort (see Figure 3).
  • The middle layer served as a platform that held the top layer (namely the array connector in Figure 2) on top of the coils. Inside this layer were small holes that allowed the output of each of the coils (i.e., two wires for every coil), to be soldered to the base of the array connector. In other words, the input to the array connector was the output of the coils. In doing this, the wires were properly organized within the sensor to avoid tangling.
  • The top layer (namely, the array connector) served to convert the outputs of the 8 coils into a two-ethernet cable signal, as shown in Figure 1a. This was a major improvement compared to Figure 1b where the coil outputs were coming directly out of the sensor in a tangled way, increasing the noise.
The structure was 3D-printed using a Bambu Lab X1-Carbon 3D-printer (Bambu Lab, Shenzhen, China). We selected Polyethylene Terephthalate Glycol-modified (PETG) material that offered a balance between ease of printing and mechanical strength. Notably, PETG is particularly suitable for functional parts that require durability, impact resistance, and water resistance. The material is non-magnetic and, hence, transparent to the collected MCG signals.
The complete MCG sensor setup is shown in Figure 4. Specifically, the two ethernet cables from the output of the array connector were connected to an amplifier board to improve the noise figure of the system and make it more robust. Similarly to [27], the amplifiers incorporated an input network to suppress oscillations in the input port, were placed a distance away from the sensor coils to reduce noise, and were all mounted on a single board to reduce relative motion/vibrations. Once the MCG signals from each of the coils were amplified, they passed through a 24-bit multi-channel Analog-to-Digital Converter (ADC) from National Instruments (Austin, TX, USA). The ADC operated with a ±10 V power supply and was configured to sample at 5 kHz. The digitized signals were then set as input to a laptop computer for post-processing, as discussed next.

2.2. MCG Signal Processing

Referring to Figure 5, the first step in signal processing was to filter each of the 8 MCG signals and remove high-frequency noise, such as electrical interference. To do so, a digital band-pass filter in the range [6–36] Hz was applied. The second step was to apply averaging across the 8 band-pass filtered signals. The reasoning behind this averaging was discussed in Section 2.1. To further de-noise the signal and detect the location of each of the heart beats (as needed to retrieve the mHRV), we applied our recently reported algorithm, beat estimation [28]. beat estimation leverages signal averaging and template matching to robustly identify heart beats from noisy MCG signals, achieving a dramatic improvement in the R-peak detection accuracy over the state of the art and nearly perfect mHRV estimation. As a last step, the mHRV was computed. Here, we calculated the mHRV as the mean of the difference in duration between R-peaks, denoted as M e a n R R , which was calculated according to
M e a n R R = 1 N 1 i = 1 N 1 R i + 1 R i ,
where R i is the index of an R-peak obtained through beat estimation. Other metrics, like the standard deviation (SD), were not calculated because over a short period of time (e.g., 5 to 10 min of recordings considered in this study), the duration between heart beats is constantly changing: while this does not significantly affect the mean, it does render using a constant mean model to calculate SD invalid. Therefore, to calculate the SD, assumptions on the variations of the mean over a short period of time should be taken, and then, from the derived model for the time-varying mean, a time-varying SD can be calculated.

2.3. Study Design

Human subjects were enrolled to participate in three (3) different scenarios of exerted CW. These scenarios were designed to validate the following hypotheses: (a) the MCG sensor setup reported in this study can classify high vs. low CW, validating the results of [16] for a different hardware and algorithmic setup, and (b) CW modulates the mHRV, even in the absence of any PA. Specifically, the testing scenarios were (see Figure 6) as follows:
1.
Scenario 1: Low CW: The subject was sitting on an office chair while watching a relaxing video. The subject was asked to refrain from speaking and performing any type of motion. The testing duration was 7.5 min.
2.
Scenario 2: High CW with PA: The subject was sitting on an office chair, performing N-back tasks while also recording their answer (True or False) on a phone held on their dominant hand. For this experiment, the subject only answered `true’ when the current stimulus matched the stimulus from 2 steps earlier. To increase the level of difficulty, a mix of numbers (1 and 2), letters (A and B), and shapes (triangle and circle) were used as the stimulus. The subject was asked to refrain from speaking, but motion was allowed. The testing duration was 7.5 min.
3.
Scenario 3: High CW without PA: The subject was sitting on an office chair while mentally answering `true’ or `false’ for two-digit addition and subtraction math equations. The subject was asked to refrain from speaking and performing any type of motion. The testing duration was 7.5 min.
Our first hypothesis was to reproduce the results in [16] for the new sensor (hardware and signal processing) and relied on Scenario 1 and Scenario 2. Our second hypothesis was to clarify the uncertainty raised in [16] as to whether the mHRV changes are due to CW, PA, or both and relied on Scenario 1 and Scenario 3. The high CW tasks in Scenario 2 and 3 were intentionally selected as different to eliminate the memory effect. The selection of the tasks themselves relied on previous studies for the exertion of low and high CW [24,25].
For all scenarios, the following sensors were placed on the human subjects and used to record data simultaneously (see Figure 7):
  • MCG sensor: The MCG sensor was described in Section 2.1 and Section 2.2. For proper placement on the chest, we counted from the clavicle and down to the space between the third and fourth ribs to identify the location of the heart and aligned the MCG sensor with this location.
  • ECG sensor: A three-lead off-the-shelf Arduino UNO R3 micro-controller board (Arduino S.r.l, Ivrea, Italy) was used. The ECG signal served as a `gold standard’ comparison vs. the results obtained through our MCG sensor. The ECG sensor output was connected to one of the ADC channels, and the signal processing followed the steps in Figure 5 (except the averaging).
  • Inertial Measurement Unit (IMU): A Witmotion WT9011DCL MPU9250 Bluetooth accelerometer (WitMotion Shenzhen Co., Ltd., Shenzhen, China) was placed on the opposite side of the palm of the subject’s dominant hand, i.e., the one used to hold the phone in Scenario 2. The sensor had dimensions of 32.5 mm × 23.5 mm × 11.4 mm and was used to monitor the presence/lack of PA.
  • Finger Pulse Oximeter: A fingertip pulse oximeter SM-1100S (Gurin Products, LLC, Tustin, CA, USA) was used to measure oxygen saturation. Although data from the oxygen sensor was not used in the post-processing, it was utilized as assurance that the participants were not in distress.

2.4. Study Participants

For this study, 10 adults between the ages of 19 and 33 ( μ = 23.5 years; σ = 4.11 years) were recruited, as shown in Table 1. For this proof-of-concept study, participants were selected as healthy (i.e., no cardiac conditions) to ensure that changes in mHRV were specifically attributed to CW. Our inclusion criteria (approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB)) required “healthy” status and normal BMI. No screening for subclinical conditions (like undiagnosed arrhythmias) was performed, but none of the volunteers reported any cardiovascular disease or took cardiac medications. This is important because pre-existing cardiac conditions can significantly affect the HRV. For example, pathological arrhythmias or heart disease often reduce HRV and alter its pattern [29]. By restricting to healthy adults, we ensured our HRV changes reflected cognitive load rather than underlying pathology. For future studies involving participants with known or suspected cardiac conditions, we can leverage the partnership with Nationwide Children’s Hospital to recruit patients who have already been appropriately screened. This approach helps ensure that our findings are not confounded by underlying cardiac issues. Additionally, we can use a three-lead ECG system to aid in detecting arrhythmias, as different types of cardiac irregularities are known to alter the morphology of the heartbeat [30,31]. To determine the sample size of human subjects for validation, we based our decision on previous studies of CW monitoring, which included 10 [32], 16 [33], 12 [34], 9 [35,36], and 11 [29] participants. All participants were males due to the garment being more comfortable in the absence of breast tissue (note the tightening of the ratchet straps in the chest area in Figure 3). The protocol was approved by The Ohio State University Institutional Review Board (protocol # 2019H0259). Though exact measurements of the heart-to-coil distance were not possible to acquire, all subjects had a healthy body mass index (BMI < 30 kg/m2).

3. Results

3.1. Efficacy of the MCG Sensor Hardware and Signal Processing Advancements

To emphasize the improvements that resulted from the advancements in the MCG sensor hardware, Figure 8a shows a zoom-in on an example MCG signal after band-pass filtering and averaging, but before applying beat estimation. For comparison, Figure 8b shows the equivalent signal using the hardware setup in [16]. `Gold standard’ ECG signals are also super-imposed with a goal to pinpoint the accurate retrieval of the R-peaks. As seen, the signal in Figure 8a is much clearer, and the R-peaks can be seen even in the absence of advanced signal processing. To quantify the performance improvement, we calculated the total number of detected R-peaks in the MCG signal and divided that by the total number of detected R-peaks in the ECG signal (i.e., the ground truth) as shown in Equation (3). For the full recording (duration = 3 min) in Figure 8b, the detection accuracy was only 70.92%, whereas for the one in Figure 8a (duration = 7.5 min), it was a perfect 100%. That is, the hardware advancements proposed in this paper drastically improve the detection of the R-peaks.
Detection Accuracy = # of detected R-peaks # of true R-peaks × 100
When beat estimation is also taken into account, the performance improvement of the proposed MCG sensor is highlighted, as shown in Figure 9a. A clear and accurate detection of the R-peaks is taking place. Although this is also true for Figure 9b that shows the corresponding data for the sensor in [16], the noise level is much lower in Figure 9a. Table 2 provides a summary of the detection accuracy of all subjects in each of the scenarios after applying beat estimation. Scenario 3 had the highest average accuracy of 99.3%, Scenario 2 had the lowest average accuracy of 98.2 %, and Scenario 1 had an average accuracy of 98.4 %. This shows that our obtained R-peak data are accurate, valid, and comparable to those obtained from the ECG sensor.

3.2. Efficacy of the Testing Scenarios

For Scenario 1, the subjects were monitored throughout the duration of the testing to ensure that they were fully engaged with the relaxing video being played in front of them. For Scenario 2, we calculated the subjects’ accuracy in performing the N-back tasks using their answers recorded on the phone. All 10 subjects achieved an accuracy of over 80%. This confirms that the intended CW was actually exerted. For Scenario 3, since no physical recording of the answers was available, the subjects verbally confirmed their engagement in the task once the recording was over.
As for the PA engagement, Figure 10a–c show the IMU recordings in both X and Y directions in units of gravitational acceleration g (m/s2) for Scenarios 1 through 3 for one of the subjects. The results confirm that there was minimal to no motion in Scenarios 1 and 3, unlike Scenario 2 where motion was recorded due to the subjects noting the answers of the N-back tasks on the phone. Indeed, Figure 10b shows multiple spikes throughout the presented duration of 20 s, whereas the other Figures do not. Accordingly, the variance was very low in both Figure 10a,c, unlike that of Scenario 2. This behavior was observed across all 10 subjects.

3.3. HRV Results

Figure 11 plots the maximum, mean, and minimum values of the M e a n R R obtained through MCG and ECG for each of the testing scenarios. To obtain this plot, we considered the average over the 10 subjects for each scenario. By calculating the percentage error for the mean between the ECG and MCG, we found out that a maximum error of 0.16% occurred in Scenario 1, the lowest error of 0.02% occurred for Scenario 3, while Scenario 2 had an error of 0.15%. As for the maximum, the highest error of 0.55% occurred in Scenario 2, the lowest error of 0.27% occurred in Scenario 3, while Scenario 1 achieved an error of 0.48%. Finally, the error values for the minimum ranged from 0.19% as the highest error in Scenario 3, to almost 0% in Scenario 1, while Scenario 2 had an error of only 0.16%. That is, our mHRV results are almost the same results as those obtained from ECG, which has a very high SNR and is known as the ground truth, with errors not exceeding 0.6% on average when it comes to the minimum, maximum, and mean values of calculated M e a n R R . These results further confirm the adequacy of our hardware and algorithmic setup.
To prove our hypotheses, Figure 12a,c show box plots for the MCG for Scenario 1 vs. Scenario 2 and Scenario 1 vs. Scenario 3, respectively. For comparison and validation against the `gold standard’, Figure 12b,d represent the equivalent box plots for the ECG. In all figures, the red box represents the M e a n R R calculated for Scenario 1, with a red circle indicating the median of all participants in that scenario. The green box in Figure 12a,b represents the M e a n R R calculated for Scenario 2, with a green circle indicating the median of all participants in that scenario. Lastly, the green box in Figure 12c,d represents the M e a n R R calculated for Scenario 3, with a blue circle indicating the median of all participants in that scenario. The blue line connecting all boxes together is the mean of each scenario taken across all participants.
Figure 12a,b show that the mHRV and HRV drop from baseline when both CW and PA activity are exerted. This part of the experiment was conducted to compare to the one in [16] and confirm the results. However, in [16] and in Figure 12a,b, it is not evident whether the drop occurs due to CW, PA, or both. To confirm that the changes in CW indeed cause drops in the mHRV and HRV, we compared Scenario 1 to Scenario 3 (noting the difference in the high CW task as compared to Scenario 2 to avoid the memory effect). Figure 12c,d show the obtained results. Since only CW changes occur between these two scenarios, as also confirmed by the IMU readings, we validate our hypothesis that the mHRV and HRV drop with increasing CW levels, regardless of the presence of PA.
To confirm the significance of HRV differences across scenarios, we performed a paired Bonferroni t-test with significance α = 0.01 that confirmed our hypothesis. Let μ i denote the sample mean of the ith scenario for i { 1 , 2 , 3 } . We obtained a p-value of p ( 1 ) = 2.2 × 10 3 for H 0 ( 1 ) : μ 1 < μ 2 and a p-value of p ( 2 ) = 4.28 × 10 4 for H 0 ( 2 ) : μ 1 < μ 3 .

4. Discussion

In this study, we observed a clear decrease in mHRV during periods of high CW compared to a low CW baseline, even when the participants remained motionless. Indeed, tasks that imposed greater mental demands—such as an N-back working memory challenge or complex arithmetic problem solving—elicited significantly lower mHRV values relative to the resting baseline condition (in our case, watching a relaxing video). This drop in mHRV with increasing CW, despite the absence of any PA, indicates that the autonomic nervous system shifts toward sympathetic dominance (and/or vagal withdrawal) purely due to mental effort [37]. Our findings are the first to decouple the effects of mental workload on mHRV from any physical influences. This interpretation aligns with established physiological responses: mental strain is known to suppress the high-frequency (parasympathetic) component of HRV [38], leading to an overall reduction in variability. Our results bolster the evidence that mHRV is a sensitive marker of cognitive strain. Furthermore, we observed this mHRV decline consistently across different types of cognitive tasks, suggesting a robust autonomic response to increased mental effort.
Notably, the literature suggests that CW can alter HRV even when the mean heart rate or other physiological measures (such as blood pressure) remain relatively unchanged [37,39]. This underscores the unique benefits of our approach: continuous monitoring of mHRV can capture subtle shifts in autonomic balance that might be missed by looking only at average heart rate or other coarse vital signs [38]. Importantly, the MCG sensor sidesteps some challenges faced by ECG-based methods. Because MCG detects magnetic fields, it is immune to issues like electrode placement variability, skin impedance changes (due to sweat or motion), and the need for skin contact, conductive gels, or adhesive patches. This can translate to more seamless operation and improved comfort for day-to-day use of the sensor.
When comparing our results to prior ECG- and MCG-based studies of CW, we find strong agreement. In laboratory tasks such as the N-back working memory challenge, others have reported pronounced decreases in time-domain ECG-based HRV indices as the task difficulty increases [40]. The authors concluded that HRV metrics provide a valid index of cognitive load, correlating with subjective workload and driver performance measures. In our own previous work [16], we demonstrated that a wearable non-contact MCG sensor could distinguish high vs. low CW by analyzing standard mHRV metrics. However, such prior ECG and MCG works still involve minimal movements, leaving some room for doubt about subtle physical influences. Our findings in a controlled motionless setting complement such prior studies by confirming that the reduction in mHRV is truly due to CW itself. This adds credence to the idea that HRV could serve as a general-purpose indicator of cognitive strain across a spectrum of contexts—from a person quietly solving math problems (as in our study) to a multitasking driver in motion—as long as one accounts for or isolates the confounding influences.
The potential for a future wearable implementation of the system is confirmed by our ability to identify inter-beat intervals even when the raw magnetic signal was barely distinguishable by eye. In essence, the hardware design and accompanying signal processing (particularly beat estimation) boost the effective SNR by aggregating information over time and intelligently rejecting noise, thus enabling accurate mHRV computation where a naive approach would fail. The reported technical enhancements in this work greatly improve the signal reliability and confidence in using MCG recorded from wearable sensors for HRV analysis. By demonstrating that we can derive stable mHRV measurements from such a challenging signal environment, we have mitigated one of the primary concerns regarding wearable MCG deployment. Previous work had suggested the promise of using un-cooled portable magnetometer sensors for bio-signal capture, but issues of noise and motion artifacts were major barriers [16,27]. In our implementation, not only did the beat estimation handle intrinsic sensor noise, it also dealt effectively with any minor disturbances (such as environmental electromagnetic fluctuations or the slight shifts in sensor position relative to the heart).
As is expected, CW can manifest in many contexts beyond the scenarios explored in this pilot study. The selection of the subject scenarios relied on paradigms that have been widely used in the literature to reliably modulate the level of mental effort [32], while the choice of having a controlled laboratory setting arises from the fact that we want to track changes in mHRV solely due to CW. Other memory/cognitive tasks in controlled environments are expected to yield similar results: even with different intensities, mHRV is expected to drop whenever CW increases. Beyond these scenarios, our MCG approach should, in principle, apply to any task that triggers an autonomic response. For instance, demanding cognitive tasks in safety-critical domains, such as air-traffic control or surgical training, have been shown to tax cognitive resources and mHRV [41]. Likewise, tasks in healthcare (e.g., concussion recovery exercises), education (exam-taking, tutoring), gaming, and immersive training could all benefit from workload monitoring [42,43]. However, applying the proposed approach directly in real-world scenarios would introduce extra variables that might impact the mHRV, i.e., mHRV is influenced by several factors beyond CW, such as stress or fitness levels [44]. As such, for this proof-of-concept study, the experimental scenarios were carefully chosen to mimic real-life activities while at the same time limiting external variables that might hinder the mHRV. By contrast, in uncontrolled environments, cognitive load changes might be confounded by other stressors. Any real-world system must account for these. In summary, even though the proposed MCG sensor can be used wherever continuous unobtrusive monitoring of CW is needed, such as applications that include safety monitoring (e.g., warning drowsy or cognitively overloaded drivers/pilots), workplace optimization (e.g., measuring staff workload in manufacturing or control-room environments), and consumer devices (e.g., games or virtual reality systems that adapt to user effort), it is still in the testing stage. Optimizing the sensor for daily life activities presents several challenges, particularly on the hardware front. While the current amplifier is portable, it is not yet small enough to be considered wearable. Further development is needed to refine and downsize the amplifier so it can match the dimensions of the MCG sensor and be mounted directly on top. Additionally, the ADC, which digitizes the recorded signals for further processing, should ideally be integrated into the same board as the amplifier. Future research should explore the use of an external electromagnetic transducer designed to suppress common-mode noise. On the algorithmic side, while techniques like adaptive filtering [45], which utilizes data from an IMU, and Independent Component Analysis (ICA) [46] are being considered, incorporating an IMU alone is not sufficient. It is equally important to understand the origins of these motion artifacts in order to model and effectively eliminate them.
Though females were not included in this study, we expect the results obtained in this study to be generalizable. That is, a similar response in mHRV is anticipated with increasing CW, regardless of the participant’s sex. Specifically, the basic autonomic mechanisms (i.e., how CW affects HRV) are qualitatively similar across sexes, even if baseline values differ. Known sex differences do exist (for example, women tend to have a slightly higher resting heart rate), but no evidence suggests opposite directions of the CW effect. This was shown in several similar works that address CW issues and used only males [47,48], or only females [49] in their testing. This was also demonstrated in our previous work [16], where it was proven that increasing CW will lead to a drop in HRV, among both males and females. Thus, although baseline variations might occur, i.e., variations in the significance of the drop in HRV, HRV is still expected to drop whenever CW increases in both males and females. As for concerns regarding the sensitivity of the sensor due to variations in breast tissue size between males and females, this was proved to have little to not effect during our experiments, as our subjects had varying chest sizes, especially since most of them work out. In our future work, we will aim to modify the garment to make it suitable for female participants as well. This can be achieved by improving the bottom layer of the sensor, i.e., the layer in direct contact with the chest, to a new material that can adjust with different chest formations and relying on stretchable wires to hold the sensor in place. To make our results more statistically valid, a larger pool of subjects, including children, will be tested on in the future. Though the participant sample size is relatively small, and the population is not widely representative, all the subjects revealed a decreasing trend in HRV between Scenarios 1 and Scenarios 2. These results are consistent with those observed for different participants and different CW tasks in [16]. Hence, adding more subjects is not expected to affect this result and should be generalizable. The addition of box plots and statistical tests, rather than just individual results, will also make our results more statistically valid and generalizable. A more extensive study with more patients (including BMI, sex, and health status considerations) will be performed in the future. In particular, in clinical populations (e.g., with cardiac pathology), HRV-based CW inference may require additional validation and possibly different analysis techniques.

5. Conclusions

We presented a comprehensive study showing that CW alone can drive significant changes in mHRV, as captured by a wearable non-contact MCG sensor. Using our newly designed sensor that is robust against unwanted noise, and utilizing our previously established beat estimation algorithm, we achieved accurate beat detection and mHRV measurements. Our results obtained for n = 10 participants across three scenarios of low and high CW, with and without PA, enabled us to observe a clear decrease in mHRV M e a n R R under high CW compared to low CW, regardless of the presence of PA. These findings confirm that, even in the absence of any PA, the human heart’s rhythm reflects the level of cognitive strain. In comparison with prior work and addressing its limitations, we demonstrated improved reliability and understanding of this phenomenon. The conclusion drawn is that mHRV-based metrics, obtainable via unobtrusive MCG technology, are valid indicators of CW. This work advances the state of the art in cognitive monitoring by (i) isolating cognitive effects on a cardiac signal and (ii) providing a path toward practical implementation of wearable MCG as enabled by hardware and algorithmic advancements. In summary, our study establishes a foundation for the future development of smart wearable systems to monitor CW in daily life, with broad implications for personal health, safety, and performance optimization.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.K. (Ali Kaiss), J.Y. and A.K. (Asimina Kiourti); Methodology, A.K. (Ali Kaiss) and A.K. (Asimina Kiourti); Software, A.K. (Ali Kaiss); Validation, A.K. (Ali Kaiss); Investigation, A.K. (Ali Kaiss); Resources, A.K. (Ali Kaiss); Data curation, A.K. (Ali Kaiss); Writing—original draft, A.K. (Ali Kaiss) and A.K. (Asimina Kiourti); Writing—review & editing, J.Y. and A.K. (Asimina Kiourti); Supervision, A.K. (Asimina Kiourti); Project administration, A.K. (Asimina Kiourti); Funding acquisition, A.K. (Asimina Kiourti). All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by The Ohio State University Chronic Brain Injury (CBI) Discovery Theme and the National Science Foundation (NSF) under grant no. 2320490.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of The Ohio State University Institutional Review Board (protocol #2019H0259).

Informed Consent Statement

We provided an explanation to each subject and obtained written informed consent.

Data Availability Statement

Any inquiries regarding the data presented in this article should be directed to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Shubham Jain for his help with brainstorming and implementing the ethernet connections, the technical details of which will be reported in a future publication. The authors would also like to thank Brandon Wang for his help in designing the 3D model for the new sensor.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
CWCognitive Workload
ECGElectroCardioGraphy
HRVHeart Rate Variability
MCGMagnetoCardioGraphy
PAPhysical Activity

References

  1. Kahneman, D. Attention and Effort; Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ranchet, M.; Morgan, J.C.; Akinwuntan, A.E.; Devos, H. Cognitive workload across the spectrum of cognitive impairments: A systematic review of physiological measures. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 80, 516–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Vidulich, M.A.; Tsang, P.S. Mental workload and situation awareness. In Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012; pp. 243–273. [Google Scholar]
  4. Desmond, P.A.; Hoyes, T.W. Workload variation, intrinsic risk and utility in a simulated air traffic control task: Evidence for compensatory effects. Saf. Sci. 1996, 22, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Jóhannsdóttir, K.R.; Magnúsdóttir, E.H.; Sigurjónsdóttir, S.; Guðnason, J. The role of working memory capacity in cardiovascular monitoring of cognitive workload. Biol. Psychol. 2018, 132, 154–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Almukhtar, A.; Caddick, V.; Naik, R.; Goble, M.; Mylonas, G.; Darzi, A.; Orihuela-Espina, F.; Leff, D.R. Objective assessment of cognitive workload in surgery: A systematic review. Ann. Surg. 2025, 281, 942–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Mark, J.A.; Curtin, A.; Kraft, A.E.; Ziegler, M.D.; Ayaz, H. Mental workload assessment by monitoring brain, heart, and eye with six biomedical modalities during six cognitive tasks. Front. Neuroergonomics 2024, 5, 1345507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kosch, T.; Karolus, J.; Zagermann, J.; Reiterer, H.; Schmidt, A.; Woźniak, P.W. A survey on measuring cognitive workload in human-computer interaction. ACM Comput. Surv. 2023, 55, 1–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Moreno, R. Cognitive load theory: More food for thought. Instr. Sci. 2010, 38, 135–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Volf, N.; Gluhih, A. Background cerebral electrical activity in healthy mental aging. Hum. Physiol. 2011, 37, 559–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zhu, K.; Kiourti, A. A review of magnetic field emissions from the human body: Sources, sensors, and uses. IEEE Open J. Antennas Propag. 2022, 3, 732–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cappell, K.A.; Gmeindl, L.; Reuter-Lorenz, P.A. Age differences in prefontal recruitment during verbal working memory maintenance depend on memory load. Cortex 2010, 46, 462–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Fischer, H.; Nyberg, L.; Karlsson, S.; Karlsson, P.; Brehmer, Y.; Rieckmann, A.; MacDonald, S.W.; Farde, L.; Bäckman, L. Simulating neurocognitive aging: Effects of a dopaminergic antagonist on brain activity during working memory. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 575–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Karlsson, S.; Nyberg, L.; Karlsson, P.; Fischer, H.; Thilers, P.; MacDonald, S.; Brehmer, Y.; Rieckmann, A.; Halldin, C.; Farde, L.; et al. Modulation of striatal dopamine D1 binding by cognitive processing. Neuroimage 2009, 48, 398–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kahneman, D.; Beatty, J. Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science 1966, 154, 1583–1585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Wang, Z.; Zhu, K.; Kaur, A.; Recker, R.; Yang, J.; Kiourti, A. Quantifying cognitive workload using a non-contact magnetocardiography (MCG) wearable sensor. Sensors 2022, 22, 9115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Mejía-Mejía, E.; Budidha, K.; Abay, T.Y.; May, J.M.; Kyriacou, P.A. Heart rate variability (HRV) and pulse rate variability (PRV) for the assessment of autonomic responses. Front. Physiol. 2020, 11, 779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hughes, A.M.; Hancock, G.M.; Marlow, S.L.; Stowers, K.; Salas, E. Cardiac measures of cognitive workload: A meta-analysis. Hum. Factors 2019, 61, 393–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Mansikka, H.; Virtanen, K.; Harris, D. Comparison of NASA-TLX scale, modified Cooper–Harper scale and mean inter-beat interval as measures of pilot mental workload during simulated flight tasks. Ergonomics 2019, 62, 246–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Luque-Casado, A.; Perales, J.C.; Cárdenas, D.; Sanabria, D. Heart rate variability and cognitive processing: The autonomic response to task demands. Biol. Psychol. 2016, 113, 83–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Wilson, G.F. An analysis of mental workload in pilots during flight using multiple psychophysiological measures. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 2002, 12, 3–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Landi, C.T.; Villani, V.; Ferraguti, F.; Sabattini, L.; Secchi, C.; Fantuzzi, C. Relieving operators’ workload: Towards affective robotics in industrial scenarios. Mechatronics 2018, 54, 144–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Zhu, K.; Kiourti, A. Detection of extremely weak and wideband bio-magnetic signals in non-shielded environments using passive coil sensors. IEEE J. Electromagn. RF Microwaves Med. Biol. 2022, 6, 501–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Zhou, Y.; Masoumi Shahrbabak, S.; Bahrami, R.; Rahman, F.N.; Sanchez-Perez, J.A.; Gazi, A.H.; Inan, O.T.; Hahn, J.O. Non-Pharmacological Mitigation of Acute Mental Stress-Induced Sympathetic Arousal: Comparison Between Median Nerve Stimulation and Auricular Vagus Nerve Stimulation. Sensors 2025, 25, 1371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Parreira, J.D.; Chalumuri, Y.R.; Mousavi, A.S.; Modak, M.; Zhou, Y.; Sanchez-Perez, J.A.; Gazi, A.H.; Harrison, A.B.; Inan, O.T.; Hahn, J.O. A proof-of-concept investigation of multi-modal physiological signal responses to acute mental stress. Biomed. Signal Process. Control 2023, 85, 105001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Zhu, K.; Kiourti, A. Real-Time Magnetocardiography with Passive Miniaturized Coil Array in Earth Ambient Field. Sensors 2023, 23, 5567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Zhu, K.; Shah, A.M.; Berkow, J.; Kiourti, A. Miniature coil array for passive magnetocardiography in non-shielded environments. IEEE J. Electromagn. RF Microwaves Med. Biol. 2020, 5, 124–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kaiss, A.; Islam, M.A.; Kiourti, A. Estimating Heart Rate Variability in Challenging Low SNR Regimes Using Wearable Magnetocardiography Sensors. IEEE J. Electromagn. RF Microwaves Med. Biol. 2024, 9, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Schulz, C.M.; Schneider, E.; Fritz, L.; Vockeroth, J.; Hapfelmeier, A.; Wasmaier, M.; Kochs, E.; Schneider, G. Eye tracking for assessment of workload: A pilot study in an anaesthesia simulator environment. Br. J. Anaesth. 2011, 106, 44–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Couderc, J.P. Measurement and regulation of cardiac ventricular repolarization: From the QT interval to repolarization morphology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 2009, 367, 1283–1299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Xue, J.; Chen, Y.; Han, X.; Gao, W. Electrocardiographic morphology changes with different type of repolarization dispersions. J. Electrocardiol. 2010, 43, 553–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Herff, C.; Heger, D.; Fortmann, O.; Hennrich, J.; Putze, F.; Schultz, T. Mental workload during n-back task—quantified in the prefrontal cortex using fNIRS. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 7, 935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Knoll, A.; Wang, Y.; Chen, F.; Xu, J.; Ruiz, N.; Epps, J.; Zarjam, P. Measuring cognitive workload with low-cost electroencephalograph. In Proceedings of the IFIP Conference on Human-Computer Interaction, Lisbon, Portugal, 5–9 September 2011; pp. 568–571. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hirachan, N.; Mathews, A.; Romero, J.; Rojas, R.F. Measuring cognitive workload using multimodal sensors. In Proceedings of the 2022 44th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine & Biology Society (EMBC), Glasgow, UK, 11–15 July 2022; pp. 4921–4924. [Google Scholar]
  35. Mazur, L.M.; Mosaly, P.R.; Hoyle, L.M.; Jones, E.L.; Marks, L.B. Subjective and objective quantification of physician’s workload and performance during radiation therapy planning tasks. Pract. Radiat. Oncol. 2013, 3, e171–e177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Merkle, F.; Kurtovic, D.; Starck, C.; Pawelke, C.; Gierig, S.; Falk, V. Evaluation of attention, perception, and stress levels of clinical cardiovascular perfusionists during cardiac operations: A pilot study. Perfusion 2019, 34, 544–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Delliaux, S.; Delaforge, A.; Deharo, J.C.; Chaumet, G. Mental workload alters heart rate variability, lowering non-linear dynamics. Front. Physiol. 2019, 10, 565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hjortskov, N.; Rissén, D.; Blangsted, A.K.; Fallentin, N.; Lundberg, U.; Søgaard, K. The effect of mental stress on heart rate variability and blood pressure during computer work. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2004, 92, 84–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Digiesi, S.; Manghisi, V.M.; Facchini, F.; Klose, E.M.; Foglia, M.M.; Mummolo, C. Heart rate variability based assessment of cognitive workload in smart operators. Manag. Prod. Eng. Rev. 2020, 11, 56–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Arutyunova, K.R.; Bakhchina, A.V.; Konovalov, D.I.; Margaryan, M.; Filimonov, A.V.; Shishalov, I.S. Heart rate dynamics for cognitive load estimation in a driving simulation task. Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 31656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Von Janczewski, N.; Wittmann, J.; Engeln, A.; Baumann, M.; Krauß, L. A meta-analysis of the n-back task while driving and its effects on cognitive workload. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2021, 76, 269–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Wilbanks, B.A.; Aroke, E.; Dudding, K.M. Using eye tracking for measuring cognitive workload during clinical simulations: Literature review and synthesis. CIN Comput. Inform. Nurs. 2021, 39, 499–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Wilbanks, B.A.; McMullan, S.P. A review of measuring the cognitive workload of electronic health records. CIN Comput. Inform. Nurs. 2018, 36, 579–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Kim, J.s.; Lee, K.y. A comparative study on the optimal model for abnormal detection event of heart rate time series data based on the correlation between PPG and ECG. J. Internet Comput. Serv. 2019, 20, 137–142. [Google Scholar]
  45. Ram, M.R.; Madhav, K.V.; Krishna, E.H.; Komalla, N.R.; Reddy, K.A. A novel approach for motion artifact reduction in PPG signals based on AS-LMS adaptive filter. IEEE Trans. Instrum. Meas. 2011, 61, 1445–1457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Kim, B.S.; Yoo, S.K. Motion artifact reduction in photoplethysmography using independent component analysis. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 2006, 53, 566–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Safari, M.; Shalbaf, R.; Bagherzadeh, S.; Shalbaf, A. Classification of mental workload using brain connectivity and machine learning on electroencephalogram data. Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 9153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Martínez-Díaz, I.C.; Carrasco, L. Neurophysiological stress response and mood changes induced by high-intensity interval training: A pilot study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 7320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Ismail, L.; Karwowski, W. The brain networks indices associated with the human perception of comfort in static force exertion tasks. Front. Neuroergonomics 2025, 6, 1542393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. (a) Proposed MCG sensor with 3-layer 3D-printed fixture, ethernet cables, and straps. (b) Previous MCG sensor with jumper wires kept in place with tape [16].
Figure 1. (a) Proposed MCG sensor with 3-layer 3D-printed fixture, ethernet cables, and straps. (b) Previous MCG sensor with jumper wires kept in place with tape [16].
Sensors 25 04806 g001
Figure 2. Exploded 3D view of the MCG sensor design.
Figure 2. Exploded 3D view of the MCG sensor design.
Sensors 25 04806 g002
Figure 3. (a) Front and (b) back view showing how the ratchet straps hold the MCG sensor in place upon a human subject.
Figure 3. (a) Front and (b) back view showing how the ratchet straps hold the MCG sensor in place upon a human subject.
Sensors 25 04806 g003
Figure 4. Visualization of the complete MCG sensor setup showing how the ethernet cables connect to the amplifier board, subsequent ADC, and laptop.
Figure 4. Visualization of the complete MCG sensor setup showing how the ethernet cables connect to the amplifier board, subsequent ADC, and laptop.
Sensors 25 04806 g004
Figure 5. Block diagram of beat estimation.
Figure 5. Block diagram of beat estimation.
Sensors 25 04806 g005
Figure 6. Testing scenarios: (a) Scenario 1, (b) Scenario 2, and (c) Scenario 3.
Figure 6. Testing scenarios: (a) Scenario 1, (b) Scenario 2, and (c) Scenario 3.
Sensors 25 04806 g006
Figure 7. Experimental setup for data collection with visualization on employed sensors.
Figure 7. Experimental setup for data collection with visualization on employed sensors.
Sensors 25 04806 g007
Figure 8. A zoom-in on pre-processed ECG (blue) and MCG (red) data obtained through (a) the advanced MCG sensor reported in this paper and (b) the MCG sensor reported in [16].
Figure 8. A zoom-in on pre-processed ECG (blue) and MCG (red) data obtained through (a) the advanced MCG sensor reported in this paper and (b) the MCG sensor reported in [16].
Sensors 25 04806 g008
Figure 9. A zoom-in on post-processed ECG (blue) and MCG (red) data obtained through (a) the advanced MCG sensor reported in this paper and (b) the MCG sensor reported in [16].
Figure 9. A zoom-in on post-processed ECG (blue) and MCG (red) data obtained through (a) the advanced MCG sensor reported in this paper and (b) the MCG sensor reported in [16].
Sensors 25 04806 g009
Figure 10. IMU data in X and Y axis in units of g (m/s2) for (a) Scenario 1, (b) Scenario 2, and (c) Scenario 3.
Figure 10. IMU data in X and Y axis in units of g (m/s2) for (a) Scenario 1, (b) Scenario 2, and (c) Scenario 3.
Sensors 25 04806 g010
Figure 11. Validation results for MCG: this plot shows how the HRV values obtained through our MCG sensor match those obtained from the ECG sensor.
Figure 11. Validation results for MCG: this plot shows how the HRV values obtained through our MCG sensor match those obtained from the ECG sensor.
Sensors 25 04806 g011
Figure 12. (a) Box plot for MCG for Scenarios 1 vs. 2, (b) box plot for ECG for Scenarios 1 vs.2, (c) box plot for MCG for Scenarios 1 vs. 3, and (d) box plot for ECG for Scenarios 1 vs. 3. The blue line that connects the box plots represents the mean of the calculated HRV in each scenario.
Figure 12. (a) Box plot for MCG for Scenarios 1 vs. 2, (b) box plot for ECG for Scenarios 1 vs.2, (c) box plot for MCG for Scenarios 1 vs. 3, and (d) box plot for ECG for Scenarios 1 vs. 3. The blue line that connects the box plots represents the mean of the calculated HRV in each scenario.
Sensors 25 04806 g012
Table 1. Summary of study participants.
Table 1. Summary of study participants.
Subject IDAgeSexHeight (m)Weight (Kg)BMI (kg/m2)
Subject 123Male1.756019.6
Subject 223Male1.788526.8
Subject 333Male1.77124.6
Subject 420Male1.77626.3
Subject 525Male1.726722.6
Subject 623Male1.7872.723.0
Subject 724Male1.829829.5
Subject 826Male1.8277.224.4
Subject 919Male1.789028.4
Subject 1019Male1.76121.1
Table 2. Summary of detection accuracy.
Table 2. Summary of detection accuracy.
Detection Accuracy (%)
Subject IDScenario 1Scenario 2Scenario 3
Subject 196.0696.6799.35
Subject 2100100100
Subject 396.498.6197.5
Subject 497.210099.2
Subject 599.698.6100
Subject 698.790.298.3
Subject 799.199.299.7
Subject 810099.699.7
Subject 996.899.699.3
Subject 1099.999.599.9
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kaiss, A.; Yang, J.; Kiourti, A. Toward Wearable MagnetoCardioGraphy (MCG) for Cognitive Workload Monitoring: Advancements in Sensor and Study Design. Sensors 2025, 25, 4806. https://doi.org/10.3390/s25154806

AMA Style

Kaiss A, Yang J, Kiourti A. Toward Wearable MagnetoCardioGraphy (MCG) for Cognitive Workload Monitoring: Advancements in Sensor and Study Design. Sensors. 2025; 25(15):4806. https://doi.org/10.3390/s25154806

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kaiss, Ali, Jingzhen Yang, and Asimina Kiourti. 2025. "Toward Wearable MagnetoCardioGraphy (MCG) for Cognitive Workload Monitoring: Advancements in Sensor and Study Design" Sensors 25, no. 15: 4806. https://doi.org/10.3390/s25154806

APA Style

Kaiss, A., Yang, J., & Kiourti, A. (2025). Toward Wearable MagnetoCardioGraphy (MCG) for Cognitive Workload Monitoring: Advancements in Sensor and Study Design. Sensors, 25(15), 4806. https://doi.org/10.3390/s25154806

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop