Previous Article in Journal
SPEED: A Graphical User Interface Software for Processing Eye Tracking Data
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Systematic Review

The Neuroanatomical Correlates of Dyspnea: An Activation Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis

1
Department of Internal Medicine, Lahn-Dill-Kliniken, 35578 Wetzlar, Germany
2
Department of Internal Medicine, University of Marburg, 35037 Marburg, Germany
3
Sleep Laboratory, Fachkrankenhaus Kloster Grafschaft, 57392 Schmallenberg, Germany
4
Department of Pulmonology, Pius-Hospital Carl von Ossietzky University of Oldenburg, 26121 Oldenburg, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
NeuroSci 2025, 6(2), 36; https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci6020036
Submission received: 4 January 2025 / Revised: 7 April 2025 / Accepted: 15 April 2025 / Published: 17 April 2025

Abstract

:
The sensation of dyspnea is related to various cardiopulmonary and neuromuscular diseases and is characterized by its sensory and affective qualities. Although there is a vast number of studies investigating its pathophysiology, less is known about the neuroanatomy of dyspnea perception. An activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis of 13 studies investigating different breathing challenges using either PET or fMRI was performed to demonstrate the neuroanatomical correlates of dyspnea perception. The ALE meta-analysis was performed using the GingerAle software 3.0.2 and was displayed with the Mango software 4.1. Synthesizing the results of all included studies, clusters involving the insula and cingulated cortex in both hemispheres were observed. Subgroup analysis for the restrained breathing condition revealed activation involving the right and left cingulate cortex and left anterior cingulate cortex. For the loaded breathing condition, statistically significant activation was found for the postcentral gyrus, the superior temporal gyrus, and the right thalamus. The combined ALE map for both conditions showed activity patterns in the right cingulate cortex, the right insula, and the right thalamus. This ALE meta-analysis demonstrates that two separate neuronal pathways related to either the affective or intensity domain are involved in the central processing of dyspnea perception.

1. Introduction

Dyspnea is a multi-dimensional sensation which occurs as the result of an imbalance between the demand and the ability to breath [1]. It is a common symptom affecting about 50% of patients in the acute hospital setting and can be due to cardiac, pulmonary, neurological, and muscular disease [2]. The terminology of dyspnea meaning disordered (gr. dys) breathing (gr. pnea) does not account for its sensory, cognitive, and affective dimensions and neglects the social consequences for patients during everyday life. A more comprehensive definition is provided by the American Thoracic Society which describes dyspnea as a “subjective experience of breathing discomfort that consists of qualitatively distinct sensations that vary in intensity (…) and derives from interactions among multiple physiological, psychological, social, and environmental factors, and may induce secondary physiological and behavioral responses” [3]. While the sensory domain of dyspnea is related to the intensity of the perception, the affective dimension is associated with the feeling of unpleasantness and fear. Different challenges can induce dyspnea including breathing against a resistive airway, chest wall constriction, or breathing of hypercapnic gas mixtures [4]. These conditions may result in specific sensory qualities including chest tightness (constriction of airways), effort (afferents from respiratory muscles), or air hunger (not getting enough air). While conditions with resistive loads may cause a sensation of increased effort (work of breathing) by stimulating respiratory motor afferents, restricted breathing may rather lead to the sensation of air hunger sensed by chemoreceptive or pulmonary afferents [5]. Both the sensory and affective dimension of dyspnea are related to distinct neuroanatomical pathways. The sensory or intensity domain of dyspnea corresponds to the somatosensory information which is sent to the brainstem and is then relayed from the ventroposterior thalamus to the primary and secondary somatosensory cortex. The affective processing of dyspnea begins with afferents of airway and lung receptors sent to the brainstem via the vagal nerve from where information is sent to the amygdala and dorsal areas of the thalamus. These send projections to the insula and cingulate cortex where an awareness of the affective domain of dyspnea emerges [6]. While breathing is usually unconscious, its aware perception is considered to be a warning signal which prompts the individual to adapt its behavioral response. The awareness of inner states can, therefore, be considered as a basic form of consciousness which enables the organism to correct for somatovisceral imbalances and to regain homeostasis [7].
The neuroanatomical correlates of inner perceptions like dyspnea are most commonly assessed using the imaging modalities of either PET (positron emission tomography) or fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) which give a three-dimensional impression on the activity pattern derived from task dependent neuronal vascularization. For each voxel, the activity value is reported with the corresponding (x, y, z) coordinate which is used to compare the task with the baseline condition [8]. Oftentimes studies with PET and fMRI include only a limited sample size leading to low statistical power and reduced generalizability of the results. The method of ALE (activation likelihood estimation) meta-analysis is based on synthesizing the results of different studies investigating a comparable condition by combining the coordinates of activated voxels. Statistical significance is tested for the combined ALE map and clusters of activity are generated [9,10,11]. Since evidence on the functional neuroanatomy of dyspnea is limited, this work intends to synthesize the data of the existing literature by performing an ALE meta-analysis using the GingerAle software version 3.0.2.

2. Methods

The literature search was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA-P 2015) checklist as illustrated in Figure 1 [12].

2.1. Data Sources and Search Strategy

A comprehensive literature search was conducted across the databases PubMed, EMBASE and PsycNet until November 30 2024. The search strategy included the combination of each term with the Boolean operators AND and OR and search by proximity. The keywords and Medical Subject Heading (MeSH) terms “dyspnea” OR “air hunger” OR “breathlessness” AND “fMRI” OR “PET” in title and/or abstract. References were exported with the Citavi© software version 7.0 (Lumivero, Denver, CO, USA) and duplicates were removed.

2.2. Eligibility Criteria

The final meta-analysis only included prospective interventional studies which investigated the neuroanatomical correlates of dyspnea perception induced by different laboratory stimuli including restrained ventilation, loaded breathing, and induced hypercapnia. Articles were excluded if they were reviews, letters or comments, editorials, case reports or case series and lacked information about the above-mentioned eligibility criteria or did not report the foci coordinates. No restrictions considering publication date or language of the published articles were applied.

2.3. Study Election and Data Extraction

The identified records were screened for eligibility by reading titles and abstracts of each article. If the eligibility criteria were met, the full text was obtained and investigated for inclusion in the meta-analysis. The x, y, z coordinates of each foci were extracted manually into a text file including the reference space, study name, condition and sample size. All foci data were converted to the MNI (Montreal Neurological Imaging) coordinate space before entering the calculation of the ALE meta-analysis. The within-condition analysis included all studies investigating the effect of different stimuli on brain activity using fMRI or PET scans. Subgroup analysis was performed for studies investigating restrained breathing and mechanical loading on neural correlates of dyspnea perception.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

The ALE meta-analysis was conducted using the software GingerALE version 3.0.2 (http://brainmap.org/ale/index.html) in an automated four step procedure. (1) At first, the foci data were entered into the GingerALE software which calculates the ALE values of each voxel using a 2 × 2 × 2 mm3 matrix. The ALE technique models the uncertainty of locations in a three-dimensional space as a Gaussian probability distribution centered about a peak at the reported coordinate. Using a random-effects model, a full-width half maximum (FWHM) depending on the subject size is applied to reflect the average smoothness of the input data. A wider FWHM is applied in data sets with a smaller subject size. Thereby a statistical map in which each voxel represents the probability that activation occurs is created. (2) The second step involves a permutation test to determine the statistical significance of each ALE value for which a random data set representing the null distribution at each voxel is calculated using a Monto Carlo simulation. The permutation test was run with 1000 permutations and an ALE map with a p-value for each voxel was determined. (3) A cluster-level inference was used to find clusters of data above a defined threshold. The ALE map was thresholded with a p < 0.05. (4) The cluster-forming threshold was p < 0.001 for a minimum cluster size of 100 mm3. The calculation of ALE maps was performed for all included studies to find the within-condition effect for the different conditions. In addition, ALE values were calculated for studies with a restrained breathing condition and those with a loaded breathing condition. The ALE maps were plotted on an anatomical MNI template using the Mango software version 4.1.

3. Results

3.1. Study Selection

The study selection process is illustrated in the flow chart in Figure 1. There was a total count of 673 records identified after the initial search including from 447 PubMed, from 199 EMBASE, and from 27 PsycNet. After removal of all duplicates, the remaining 673 records were then screened for eligibility. Finally, 13 studies were included in the meta-analysis. An overview on the characteristics of all included studies is provided in Table 1 and Table 2.

3.2. Within-Condition Analysis

The meta-analysis of all included studies investigating the neuroanatomical correlates of dyspnea resulted in four statistically significant clusters of activity as shown in Table 3 and Figure 2. On the right hemisphere a cluster of 1760 mm3 with coordinates for the weighted center in (44, 9, −4) involving the insula in Brodmann area (BA) 13 was observed. The right cingulate cortex showed a statistically significant activation with coordinates for the weighted center in (5, 6, 49) in BA 24. Correspondingly on the left hemisphere, clusters in the insula (744 mm3, −32, 23, 0) and in the cingulate cortex (888 mm3, −6, 23, 26) were observed.

3.3. Subgroup Analysis

In order to the assess the brain activity related to either restrained or loaded breathing and to account for the heterogeneity among studies, separate ALE maps for both conditions were calculated as illustrated in Figure 3 and Table 4. For restrained breathing, statistically significant activity in the right and left cingulate cortex with peak coordinates in BA 24 (2448 mm3, 2, 20, 48) and in BA 32 (1296 mm3, −3, 28, 23) were observed. The loaded breathing condition led to clusters in the right postcentral gyrus (BA 1, 560 mm3, 63, −7, 14), the left superior temporal gyrus (BA 41, 512 mm3, −47, −30, 15) and the right thalamus (504 mm3, 5, −17, 7). The combined activity pattern of both conditions showed clusters in the right cingulate cortex (BA 24, 1800 mm3, 4, 8, 48), the right insula (BA 13, 1376 mm3, 10, −17, 8), and the right thalamus (688 mm3, 10, −17, 8).

4. Discussion

This ALE meta-analysis demonstrates the neuroanatomical correlates of dyspnea by synthesizing studies using either PET or fMRI during different breathing challenges. The cluster analysis for all included studies showed statistically significant activation for the insula and ACC in both hemispheres. Subgroup analysis for the restrained and loaded breathing condition revealed a divergent activity pattern involving the sensory and affective pathway of dyspnea perception [26]. While the loaded breathing condition showed clusters involving the postcentral gyrus, the superior temporal gyrus, and the thalamus, restrained breathing challenges led to statistically significant activation in the right and left cingulate cortex.
These findings substantiate the idea of two distinct pathways underlying the sensory and affective domain of dyspnea perception [27]. The intensity or sensory quality of breathlessness is related to afferents from mechanoreceptors of respiratory muscles which project to the brainstem medulla and are relayed in the ventroposterior thalamus to the primary and secondary somatosensory cortex. This discriminative pathway of respiratory proprioception encodes the intensity level of dyspnea or work of breathing and involves projections to motor areas from where efferent commands are sent to the respiratory muscles.
The affective pathway includes parts of the limbic system and represents the qualitative domain of dyspnea perception which is often referred to as air hunger [26,27]. It begins with respiratory vagal afferents which project to the brainstem medulla from where projections are sent to the amygdala and mediodorsal areas of the thalamus. This pathway further includes the insula and cingulate cortex which are interconnected and represent the emotional or qualitative domain of dyspnea. Both the insula and cingulate cortex are subdivided into areas which are characterized by specific functions of sensorimotor integration. The insula shows a posterior to mid-anterior pattern of information integration and is functionally subdivided into four distinct regions [28]. The anterior-ventral part of the insula is connected to the ACC and represents the socio-emotional domain of dyspnea perception. The anterior-dorsal insula is involved in cognitive tasks and connected with the anterior cingulate and the parietal cortex. While the central insula is engaged in olfactogustatory perception, the mid-posterior region of the insula is related to sensorimotor integration and derived reactions to the perceived sensory input. Both the insula and the cingulate cortex are not only part of the limbic system, but represent visceromotor areas (VMA) which are involved in assessing interoceptive states to regulate autonomic homeostasis [29,30].
The cingulate cortex is divided into four posterior to mid-anterior regions with specific functional organization. The ACC is related to emotion salience and visceral integration and is connected to structures of the ventral or “what” stream of sensorimotor integration [31]. It represents interoceptive states which guide actions towards or away from a perceived goal [32]. The medial cingulate cortex is involved in response selection, is connected with the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and is related to visuospatial orientation. The retrosplenial cortex is connected to hippocampal areas and contributes to memory formation.
In general, the ACC and PCC are associated with distinguishable tasks of interoceptive or spatio-temporal processing and are related to either the ventral “what” or dorsal “where stream. The ACC is connected with structures of the limbic system including the orbitofrontal cortex, the amygdala and the anterior insula which encode the emotional quality of perceived stimuli. As shown for the within-condition ALE map, a co-activation of the anterior insular cortex and the ACC is regularly observed. In addition, a linear activation of both the AIC and the ACC with the (un-) pleasantness of the perception has been demonstrated [33].
Regarding the results of the ALE meta-analysis, the bi-hemispheric activation of the insula and cingulate cortex for all included studies, therefore, substantiates the idea of a qualitative affective pathway of dyspnea perception. This finding may be clinically most evident with fear being the most commonly reported emotion associated with breathlessness [34]. In contrast, the posterior part of the cingulate cortex which is connected with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and parietal areas engaged in executive control. It is part of the dorsal “where stream of sensorimotor integration and is less activated during perceived breathlessness”.
While the affective and sensory pathway encode distinguishable qualities of dyspnea which underly distinct anatomical substrates, both share the common goal of correcting for autonomic disbalances to maintain visceral homeostasis. The awareness of regularly unconscious states is referred to as interoception and can be embedded within the broader concepts of predictive coding and active inference [35,36]. Accordingly, VMA (e.g., the ACC) generate predictions about future sensory events which are compared with the actual sensory feedback. A mismatch between these predictions and the feedback from vagal and somatosensory afferents causes a prediction error which leads to the emerging sensation, for example, dyspnea. This is structurally organized within visceromotor afferents which not only send motor commands to respiratory muscles but provide an efference copy for viscerosensory areas [37]. In case of a mismatch, predictions generated by VMA are updated to match the actual sensory feedback. This process of neuronal adaptation referred to as predictive coding may explain the activation in the insula und cingulate cortex which provide homeostatic set-points against which the feedback of vagal afferents is compared [38]. In addition to the emergence of perceived dyspnea, the prediction error induces a response of the effector system to correct for the detected disbalance. This control and regulation system includes an autonomic reflex circuit for the affective pathway and projections to higher motor areas for the somatosensory system. Theoretically, the emergence of a behavioral response is related to the framework of active inference which states that prediction errors cause the effector system to seek for a resolution of the occurred mismatch [39]. Therefore, the awareness of usually unconscious inner states (interoception) can be regarded as a basic from of consciousness which prompts the individual to adapt its behavior towards regaining autonomic homeostasis. Accordingly, the activation of the insula is related to the mental experience of inner states (feelings), to which the ACC attributes a motivational value thereby influencing the individual’s behavior on an emotional level and inducing an adaptive response [40].
Besides the immediate consequences on perception and behavior, the affective pathway may also contribute to memory formation by sending input into the hippocampal memory system via afferents from the anterior cingulate to the peri-/entorhinal cortex [41]. This may explain the subjectivity of dyspnea perception which is cognitively and emotionally modulated based on previous experiences. In addition, the concept of gain control in interoceptive inference relates to the finding that the emergence of sensations is influenced by the current emotional and attentional state which may or may not contribute to the awareness of a mismatch between predictions and sensory feedback [38]. The present work, therefore, contributes to an improved understanding of the neuroanatomical correlates underlying the interindividual differences in dyspnea perception.

5. Conclusions

This ALE meta-analysis demonstrates that the sensation of dyspnea can be described on an emotional and intensity level depending on the applied breathing challenge. These perceptual qualities are related to distinct neuroanatomical substrates that allow for the interoception of usually unconscious states and thereby enable the individual to adapt its behavior towards regaining autonomic homoeostasis. Both the conscious perception of dyspnea and the behavioral response can be explained with the framework of predictive coding and active inference. In addition, the observed neuroanatomical correlates of breathlessness involving parts of the limbic and the somatosensory system substantiate the clinically observed individual differences in dyspnea perception which is based on previous experiences and the current emotional and attentional state.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.M., J.K. and D.D.; methodology, C.M., J.K. and D.D.; data curation and formal analysis, C.M.; writing-original draft preparation, C.M., J.K. and D.D.; visualization, C.M.; review and editing, C.M., J.K. and D.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Open Access funding provided by the Open Access Publishing Fund of Philipps-Universität Marburg.

Data Availability Statement

All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this article. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

All authors certify that they have no affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with any financial interest (such as honoraria; educational grants; participation in speakers’ bureaus; membership, employment, consultancies, stock ownership, or other equity interest; and expert testimony or patent-licensing arrangements), or non-financial interest (such as personal or professional relationships, affiliations, knowledge or beliefs) in the subject matter or materials discussed in this manuscript.

Abbreviations

ACCAnterior cingulate cortex
ALEActivation likelihood estimation
BABrodmann area
fMRIFunctional magnetic resonance imaging
FWHMFull width half maximum
MNIMontreal Neurological Imaging
PCCPosterior cingulate cortex
PRISMAPreferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses
VMAVisceromotor areas

References

  1. Mahler, D.A.; O’Donnell, D.E. Neurobiology of dyspnea: An overview. In Dyspnea: Mechanisms, Measurement, and Management, 3rd ed.; CRC Press: London, UK, 2014; Volume 20, pp. 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Desbiens, N.A.; Mueller-Rizner, N.; Connors, A.F.; Wenger, N.S. The relationship of nausea and dyspnea to pain in seriously ill patients. Pain 1997, 71, 149–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Parshall, M.B.; Schwartzstein, R.M.; Adams, L.; Banzett, R.B.; Manning, H.L.; Bourbeau, J.; Calverley, P.M.; Gift, A.G.; Harver, A.; Lareau, S.C.; et al. An official American Thoracic Society statement: Update on the mechanisms, assessment, and management of dyspnea. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 2012, 185, 435–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Nikolova, N.; Harrison, O.; Toohey, S.; Brændholt, M.; Legrand, N.; Correa, C.; Vejlø, M.; Jensen, M.S.; Fardo, F.; Allen, M. The respiratory resistance sensitivity task: An automated method for quantifying respiratory interoception and metacognition. Biol. Psychol. 2022, 170, 108325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Lansing, R.W.; Gracely, R.H.; Banzett, R.B. The multiple dimensions of dyspnea: Review and hypotheses. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 2008, 167, 53–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Vogt, B.A. Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005, 6, 533–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Denton, D.A.; McKinley, M.J.; Farrell, M.; Egan, G.F. The role of primordial emotions in the evolutionary origin of consciousness. Conscious. Cogn. 2009, 18, 500–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Penny, W.D.; Friston, K.J.; Ashburner, J.T.; Kiebel, S.J.; Nichols, T.E. Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2012; pp. 101–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Eickhoff, S.B.; Laird, A.R.; Grefkes, C.; Wang, L.E.; Zilles, K.; Fox, P.T. Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: A random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 30, 2907–2926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Eickhoff, S.B.; Bzdok, D.; Laird, A.R.; Kurth, F.; Fox, P. Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. Neuroimage 2012, 59, 2349–2361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Turkeltaub, P.E.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Laird, A.R.; Fox, M.; Wiener, M.; Fox, P. Minimizing within-experiment and within-group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012, 33, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Moher, D.; Shamseer, L.; Clarke, M.; Ghersi, D.; Liberati, A.; Petticrew, M.; Shekelle, P.; Stewart, L.A.; Prisma-P Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst. Rev. 2015, 4, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Banzett, R.B.; Mulnier, H.E.; Murphy, K.; Rosen, S.D.; Wise, R.J.; Adams, L. Breathlessness in humans activates insular cortex. Neuroreport 2000, 11, 2117–2120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Liotti, M.; Brannan, S.; Egan, G.; Shade, R.; Madden, L.; Abplanalp, B.; Robillard, R.; Lancaster, J.; Zamarripa, F.E.; Fox, P.T.; et al. Brain responses associated with consciousness of breathlessness (air hunger). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 2035–2040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Peiffer, C.; Poline, J.B.; Thivard, L.; Aubier, M.; Samson, Y. Neural substrates for the perception of acutely induced dyspnea. Am J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 2001, 163, 951–957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Evans, K.C.; Banzett, R.B.; Adams, L.; Mckay, L.; Frackowiak, R.S.; Corfield, D.R. BOLD fMRI identifies limbic, paralimbic, and cerebellar activation during air hunger. J. Neurophysiol. 2002, 88, 1500–1511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Macey, K.E.; Macey, P.M.; Woo, M.A.; Henderson, L.A.; Frysinger, R.C.; Harper, R.K.; Alger, J.R.; Yan-Go, F.; Harper, R.M. Inspiratory loading elicits aberrant fMRI signal changes in obstructive sleep apnea. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 2006, 151, 44–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. McKay, L.C.; Adam, L.; Frackowiak, R.S.; Corfield, D.R. A bilateral cortico-bulbar network associated with breath holding in humans, determined by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 2008, 40, 1824–1832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. von Leupoldt, A.; Sommer, T.; Kegat, S.; Baumann, H.J.; Klose, H.; Dahme, B.; Büchel, C. The unpleasantness of perceived dyspnea is processed in the anterior insula and amygdala. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 2008, 177, 1026–1032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Binks, A.P.; Evans, K.C.; Reed, J.D.; Moosavi, S.H.; Banzett, R.B. The time-course of cortico-limbic neural responses to air hunger. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 2014, 204, 78–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Herigstad, M.; Hayen, A.; Evans, E.; Hardringe, F.M.; Davie, R.J.; Wiech, K.; Pattinson, K.T.S. Dyspnea-Related Cues Engage the Prefrontal Cortex Evidence From Functional Brain Imaging in COPD. Chest 2015, 148, 953–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Esser, R.W.; Stoeckel, M.C.; Kirsten, A.; Watz, H.; Taube, K.; Lehmann, K.; Magnussen, H.; Büchel, C.; von Leupoldt, A. Brain activation during perception and anticipation of dyspnea in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Front. Physiol. 2017, 8, 617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Chan, P.Y.; Cheng, C.H.; Wu, Y.T.; Wu, C.W.; Liu, H.L.; Shaw, F.Z.; Liu, C.Y.; Davenport, P.W. Cortical and subcortical neural correlates for respiratory sensation in response to transient inspiratory occlusions in humans. Front. Physiol. 2018, 9, 1804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Hassanpour, M.S.; Simmons, W.K.; Feinstein, J.S.; Luo, Q.; Lapidus, R.C.; Bodurka, J.; Paulus, M.P.; Khalsa, S.S. The insular cortex dynamically maps changes in cardiorespiratory interoception. Neuropsychopharmacology 2018, 43, 426–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Chan, P.Y.; Wu, Y.T.; Hsu, A.L.; Li, C.W.; Wu, C.W.; von Leupoldt, A.; Hsu, S.C. The effect of anxiety on brain activation patterns in response to inspiratory occlusions: An fMRI study. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 15045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. von Leupoldt, A.; Dahme, B. Cortical substrates for the perception of dyspnea. Chest 2005, 128, 345–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Davenport, P.W.; Vovk, A. Cortical and subcortical central neural pathways in respiratory sensations. Respir. Physiol. Neeurobiol. 2009, 167, 72–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Uddin, L.Q.; Nomi, J.S.; Hébert-Seropian, B.; Ghaziri, J.; Boucher, O. Structure and function of the human insula. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017, 34, 300–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Craig, A.D. How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 3, 655–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Flynn, F.G. Anatomy of the insula functional and clinical correlates. Aphasiology 1999, 13, 55–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Rolls, E.T. The cingulate cortex and limbic systems for emotion, action, and memory. Brain Struct. Funct. 2019, 224, 3001–3018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Ungerleider, L.G.; Haxby, J.V. ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1994, 4, 157–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Medford, N.; Critchley, H.D. Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: Awareness and response. Brain Struct. Funct. 2010, 214, 535–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Bailey, P.H. The dyspnea-anxiety-dyspnea cycle-COPD patients’ stories of breathlessness: “It’s scary/when you can’t breathe”. Qual. Health Res. 2004, 14, 760–778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Critchley, H.D.; Wiens, S.; Rotshtein, P.; Öhman, A.; Dolan, R.J. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 2004, 7, 189–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Craig, A.D. Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2003, 13, 500–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Barrett, L.F.; Simmons, W.K. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015, 16, 419–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Friston, K.J.; Daunizeau, J.; Kilner, J.; Kiebel, S.J. Action and behavior: A free-energy formulation. Biol. Cybern. 2010, 102, 227–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Pezzulo, G.; Rigoli, F.; Friston, K.J. Active inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural control. Prog. Neurobiol. 2015, 134, 17–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Damasio, A.; Carvalho, G.B. The nature of feelings: Evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2013, 14, 143–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Nieuwenhuys, R.; Voogd, J.; van Huijzen, C. Telencephalon: Hippocampus and related structures. In The Human Central Nervous System; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2008; pp. 361–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study selection according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA).
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study selection according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA).
Neurosci 06 00036 g001
Figure 2. Clusters of the ALE meta-analysis for the within-condition in sagittal, coronal and transversal view. Statistically significant activation was found in both the right (1) and left (4) insula as well as for the right (2) and left (3) cingulate cortex. The ALE value of each voxel is color-coded according to the bar on the right.
Figure 2. Clusters of the ALE meta-analysis for the within-condition in sagittal, coronal and transversal view. Statistically significant activation was found in both the right (1) and left (4) insula as well as for the right (2) and left (3) cingulate cortex. The ALE value of each voxel is color-coded according to the bar on the right.
Neurosci 06 00036 g002
Figure 3. Clusters of the ALE meta-analysis for restrained breathing, loaded breathing and combined conditions in sagittal, coronal and transversal view. Statistically significant activation was found in both the right (1) and left (2) cingulate cortex for the restrained breathing condition. Clusters in the right postcentral gyrus (3), the left superior temporal gyrus (4) and the right thalamus (5) were observed for the loaded breathing condition. Combining ALE maps of both conditions, clusters in the right cingulate cortex (6), the right insula (7), and the right thalamus (8) were observed. The ALE value of each voxel is color-coded according to the bar below the images of each condition.
Figure 3. Clusters of the ALE meta-analysis for restrained breathing, loaded breathing and combined conditions in sagittal, coronal and transversal view. Statistically significant activation was found in both the right (1) and left (2) cingulate cortex for the restrained breathing condition. Clusters in the right postcentral gyrus (3), the left superior temporal gyrus (4) and the right thalamus (5) were observed for the loaded breathing condition. Combining ALE maps of both conditions, clusters in the right cingulate cortex (6), the right insula (7), and the right thalamus (8) were observed. The ALE value of each voxel is color-coded according to the bar below the images of each condition.
Neurosci 06 00036 g003
Table 1. Studies included in the meta-analysis.
Table 1. Studies included in the meta-analysis.
Study IDPopulationIntervention
Author (Year)JournalStudy DesignImaging ModalitySample Size
(% Female)
Age (Years)ConditionsFoci
Banzett et al. (2000) [13]Brain Imaging NeuroReportProspective interventional studyPET8 (0)n.a.Ventilation via nose mask or mouthpiece by positive pressure ventilation controlling for pCO2 constant at 40 Torr, 14 breaths/minute13
Liotti et al. (2000) [14]PNASProspective interventional studyPET8 (n.a.)n.a.Administration of 8% CO2/92% O2 via face mask or mouth piece, dyspnea rating scale from 0 to 10014
Pfeiffer et al. (2001) [15]Am J Respir Crit Care MedProspective interventional studyPET8 (0)33 ± 3
(mean ± SD)
Two loads that induce a sensation of either slight/moderate or severe dyspnea in more than 50% of runs, 10 s before data acquisition the load was added10
Evans et al. (2002) [16]Journal of NeurophysiologyProspective interventional studyfMRI (2 T)6 (4)25–32 (range)13.2 breaths/min, pCO2 constant by raising inspired CO2, low Vt = 0.75 ± 0.21 L, high Vt = 1.38 ± 0.06 L23
Macey et al. (2006) [17]Resp Physiol
Neurobiol
Prospective interventional studyfMRI (1.5 T)7 (0) OSAS, 12 (0) controls46 ± 5
(mean ± SD)
47 ± 3
(mean ± SD)
Nose clip and breathing via mouthpiece, each trial of 60 s unrestricted breathing, 90 s of inspiratory loading (−6 to 15 mmHg)11
McKay et al. (2008) [18]NeuroimageProspective interventional studyfMRI (2 T)8 (6)20–35Breath holds for 15 s at resting expiratory lung volume, raised inspiratory CO2 for 15 s to increase petCO2 by 7–8 mmHg19
von Leupoldt et al. (2008) [19]Am J Respir Crit Care MedProspective interventional studyfMRI (3 T)14 (7)26.6 ± 6.2
(mean ± SD)
11 conditions of inspiratory loaded and unloaded breathing continuous for 24 s with a mean load of 8.22 kPa/L/s, loaded condition with dyspnea pictures9
Abbreviations: Am J Respir Crit Care Med, American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; n.a., not available; OSAS, obstructive sleep apnea syndrome; PET, positron emission tomography; PNAS; Proceeding of the National Academy of Science, Resp Physiol Neurobiol; Respiratory Physiology & Neurobiology; SD, standard deviation, T, Tesla, Vt, tidal volume.
Table 2. Studies included in the meta-analysis.
Table 2. Studies included in the meta-analysis.
Study IDPopulationIntervention
Author (Year)JournalStudy DesignImaging ModalitySample Size
(% Female)
Age (Years)ConditionsFoci
Binks et al. (2014) [20]Resp Physiol
and Neurobiol
Prospective interventional studyfMRI (1.5 T)8 (3)23–31 (range)Mechanical ventilation with constant mild hypercapnia (~45 mmHg), Vt alternating between 0.96 ± 0.23 L and 0.48 ± 0.08 L, air hunger on transient and steady state9
Herigstad et al. (2015) [21]ChestProspective interventional studyfMRI (3 T)41 (15) COPD
40 (16) controls
68.0 ± 8.2
(mean ± SD)
Dyspnea related cues, rating on a scale from 0 to 10013
Esser et al. (2017) [22]Frontiers in PhysiologyProspective interventional studyfMRI (3 T)17 (8) in COPD
21 (11) in controls
65.6 ± 9.3 (mean ± SD) in COPD
63.4 ± 8.8 (mean ± SD) in controls
Inspiratory resistive loads of increasing magnitude, 10 blocks of mild dyspnea (smallest load), 10 blocks of severe dyspnea (≥5 of 10 on Borg Scale) each lasting for 24 s13
Chan et al. (2018) [23]Frontiers in PhysiologyProspective interventional studyfMRI (3 T)23 (13)23.7 ± 3.1
(mean ± SD)
Inspiratory occlusion of 150 ms at the onset of inspiration every 2–4 breaths, 12 min of acquisition time, at least 32 successfully occluded breaths, rating of level of breathlessness based on VAS from 0 to 1008
Hassanpour et al. (2018) [24]NeuropharmacologyProspective interventional studyMRI (3 T)22 (11)26 ± 6
(mean ± SD)
Intravenous infusion of isoproterenol hydrochlorid and normal saline as control, each dose (1 μg, 2 μg and normal saline) was repeated twice, intensity of breathing sensation (0 = none, 10 most ever) was reduced7
Chan et al. (2019) [25]Scientific Reports nature researchProspective interventional studyMRI (3 T)34 (20)23 ± 3.4
(mean ± SD)
Breathing through face mask while respiration was repeatedly interrupted by inspiratory occlusion of 150 ms delivered every 2–4 breaths, at least 32 breaths were collected14
Abbreviations: COPD, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; Resp Physiol Neurobiol; Respiratory Physiology & Neurobiology; SD, standard deviation; T, Tesla, Vt, tidal volume.
Table 3. Location of clusters with statistically significant brain activation and coordinates for the weighted center for all included studies.
Table 3. Location of clusters with statistically significant brain activation and coordinates for the weighted center for all included studies.
ClusterBrain RegionBAxyzVolume (mm3)ALE (×10−3)
1Right Insula13449−41760 18.7
2Right Cingulate Cortex245649976 17.4
3Left Cingulate Cortex24−62326888 17.4
4Left Insula13−32230744 15.2
Abbreviations: ALE, activation likelihood estimation; BA, Brodmann area.
Table 4. Location of clusters with statistically significant brain activation and weighted center for the restrained and loaded breathing condition.
Table 4. Location of clusters with statistically significant brain activation and weighted center for the restrained and loaded breathing condition.
ClusterBrain RegionBAxyzVolume (mm3)ALE (×10−3)
Restrained breathing
1Left/Right Cingulate Cortex2421048244816.9
2Left Cingulate Cortex32−32823129611.0
Loaded Breathing
3Right Postcentral gyrus163−71456013.4
4Left Superior Temporal Gyrus/Transverse Temporal Gyrus 41−47−301551212.0
5Right Thalamus-5−1775049.9
Restrained and Loaded Breathing
6Right Cingulate Cortex244848180017.4
7Right Insula134361137618.5
8Right Thalamus 10−17868811.3
Abbreviations. ALE, activation likelihood estimation; BA, Brodmann area.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Müller, C.; Kerl, J.; Dellweg, D. The Neuroanatomical Correlates of Dyspnea: An Activation Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis. NeuroSci 2025, 6, 36. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci6020036

AMA Style

Müller C, Kerl J, Dellweg D. The Neuroanatomical Correlates of Dyspnea: An Activation Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis. NeuroSci. 2025; 6(2):36. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci6020036

Chicago/Turabian Style

Müller, Christoph, Jens Kerl, and Dominic Dellweg. 2025. "The Neuroanatomical Correlates of Dyspnea: An Activation Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis" NeuroSci 6, no. 2: 36. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci6020036

APA Style

Müller, C., Kerl, J., & Dellweg, D. (2025). The Neuroanatomical Correlates of Dyspnea: An Activation Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis. NeuroSci, 6(2), 36. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci6020036

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop