Next Article in Journal
Sunitinib’s Effect on Bilateral Optic Nerve Damage in Rats Following the Unilateral Clamping and Unclamping of the Common Carotid Artery
Next Article in Special Issue
Understanding Causal Relationships Between Imaging-Derived Phenotypes and Parkinson’s Disease: A Mendelian Randomization and Observational Study
Previous Article in Journal
LncRNA RP11-297P16.4 Promotes the Invasion and Metastasis of Non-Small-Cell Lung Carcinoma by Targeting the miR-145-5p/MMP-2/9 Axis
Previous Article in Special Issue
Fibromyalgia, Depression, and Autoimmune Disorders: An Interconnected Web of Inflammation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Genomic Characterisation of the Relationship and Causal Links Between Vascular Calcification, Alzheimer’s Disease, and Cognitive Traits

by
Emmanuel O. Adewuyi
1,2,* and
Simon M. Laws
2,3
1
School of Medical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, WA 6027, Australia
2
Centre for Precision Health, School of Medical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, WA 6027, Australia
3
Collaborative Genomics and Translation Group, School of Medical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, WA 6027, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Biomedicines 2025, 13(3), 618; https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030618
Submission received: 13 January 2025 / Revised: 23 February 2025 / Accepted: 25 February 2025 / Published: 3 March 2025

Abstract

:
Background/Objectives: Observational studies suggest a link between vascular calcification and dementia or cognitive decline, but the evidence is conflicting, and the underlying mechanisms are unclear. Here, we investigate the shared genetic and causal relationships of vascular calcification—coronary artery calcification (CAC) and abdominal aortic calcification (AAC)—with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and five cognitive traits. Methods: We analyse large-scale genome-wide association studies (GWAS) summary statistics, using well-regarded methods, including linkage disequilibrium score regression (LDSC), Mendelian randomisation (MR), pairwise GWAS (GWAS-PW), and gene-based association analysis. Results: Our findings reveal a nominally significant positive genome-wide genetic correlation between CAC and AD, which becomes non-significant after excluding the APOE region. CAC and AAC demonstrate significant negative correlations with cognitive performance and educational attainment. MR found no causal association between CAC or AAC and AD or cognitive traits, except for a bidirectional borderline-significant association between AAC and fluid intelligence scores. Pairwise-GWAS analysis identifies no shared causal SNPs (posterior probability of association [PPA]3 < 0.5). However, we find pleiotropic loci (PPA4 > 0.9), particularly on chromosome 19, with gene association analyses revealing significant genes in shared regions, including APOE, TOMM40, NECTIN2, and APOC1. Moreover, we identify suggestively significant loci (PPA4 > 0.5) on chromosomes 1, 6, 7, 9 and 19, implicating pleiotropic genes, including NAV1, IPO9, PHACTR1, UFL1, FHL5, and FOCAD. Conclusions: Current findings reveal limited genetic correlation and no significant causal associations of CAC and AAC with AD or cognitive traits. However, significant pleiotropic loci, particularly at the APOE region, highlight the complex interplay between vascular calcification and neurodegenerative processes. Given APOE’s roles in lipid metabolism, neuroinflammation, and vascular integrity, its involvement may link vascular and neurodegenerative disorders, pointing to potential targets for further investigation.

1. Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disorder characterised by memory loss and progressive cognitive decline [1,2,3]. The growing burden of the disorder has become a major global public health concern, with over 10 million new dementia cases diagnosed annually, AD being the most prevalent form [1,2,3,4]. Although the exact causes of AD are not well understood, there is increasing appreciation of the likely role of vascular pathology in its onset and progression [5,6,7]. Emerging evidence highlights a link between vascular calcification and both AD and cognitive impairment [8,9,10]. Vascular calcification, including abdominal aortic calcification (AAC) and coronary artery calcification (CAC), refers to the abnormal deposition of calcium phosphate complexes within the walls of arteries, often serving as a marker of subclinical atherosclerosis [11].
Notably, AAC is widely prevalent, and evidence suggests that it strongly predicts several cardiovascular and respiratory conditions and independently increases the risk of myocardial infarction [12]. Similarly, CAC is commonly recognised as a critical indicator for predicting future atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease risk [13,14]. Atherosclerosis, the thickening and hardening of arterial walls due to cellular accumulation and calcification, contributes to the narrowing of blood vessels and is associated with cardiovascular diseases and neurodegenerative conditions, including dementia [6,15,16]. For instance, a study [16] recently highlighted the potential links between atherosclerosis and dementia, supporting the notion that systemic vascular health is closely tied to brain function. This observation reinforces the role of atherosclerosis as a potentially shared pathological factor in cardiovascular and neurodegenerative diseases.
More specifically, recent studies suggest an interaction between atherosclerosis-related traits and AD, potentially leading to additive effects on cognitive decline [7]. For example, an observational study reported that old women with AAC had an increased risk of developing all-cause late-life dementia [9]. The association between vascular calcification and cognitive impairment has also been observed in multiple populations [8,17,18,19]. Other studies, including cohorts from China and the Netherlands, observed associations between CAC and cognitive decline [8,10,17,18,19]. These findings suggest associations or co-occurring relationships between CAC or AAC and AD or cognitive impairment. Such relationships may accelerate disease progression and contribute to adverse outcomes, including more complex management plans [20]. On the other hand, the relationships may play a role in the pathogenesis of AD through shared genetic predisposition, common biological mechanisms, or direct causal relationships. Understanding the nature of these relationships and their underlying mechanisms could, thus, advance our knowledge of the aetiology of AD and provide opportunities for identifying therapeutic targets or precision preventative measures.
Despite growing evidence linking vascular calcification with AD and cognitive impairment, findings on this subject remain inconsistent. For instance, a systematic review and meta-analysis found limited evidence associating CAC scores with dementia risk [21], while a scoping review reported mixed results on the relationship between vascular calcification and AD or cognitive decline [22]. These inconsistencies, along with the limitations of traditional observational studies—such as measurement error, reverse causation, and residual confounding [23]—leave key questions unanswered. First, it remains unclear whether CAC or AAC is truly associated with AD or cognitive function, or if previous observational studies were biased or produced false-positive findings. Second, the nature of any observed relationships between AD and vascular calcification—whether causal or driven by shared genetic susceptibility—remains uncertain. Traditional observational studies are often limited in establishing causal relationships due to confounding influences from several factors, including lifestyles and environment [24,25,26,27]. In contrast, statistical genetic approaches (as in the current study) leveraging large-scale genome-wide association studies (GWAS) mitigate these limitations by using inherited genetic variants as instrumental variables. This genetic approach provides a more robust framework for investigating the genetic architecture underlying CAC, AAC, AD, and cognitive traits, with the potential to offer insights into their shared biological mechanisms [24,25,26,27]. Furthermore, genetic analyses can identify novel therapeutic targets by pinpointing variants and pathways contributing to disease risk, potentially guiding precision medicine strategies [24,25,26,27].
Hence, the current study systematically assesses the potential shared genetic and causal relationship of CAC and AAC with AD, cognitive performance, common executive function (cEF), and other cognitive-related traits. We analysed several large-scale genetic data employing well-regarded statistical genetics methods, including linkage disequilibrium score regression (LDSC), to examine genome-wide cross-traits genetic correlations. We also used the Mendelian randomisation (MR) method [28,29,30,31] to investigate whether CAC or AAC is causally associated with AD or cognitive traits, and vice versa. MR is a valuable and cost-effective method for assessing the relationship of CAC or AAC with AD or cognitive traits, as it uses genetic variants as instruments to infer causality while minimising the effects of confounding and reverse causation. Also, we utilised the pairwise GWAS (GWAS-PW) method to further investigate the relationship between CAC/AAC and AD/cognitive traits for insights into shared genetic variants and loci. Lastly, we performed gene-level analyses using a recently developed powerful gene-based association analysis method [32,33] to identify pleiotropic genes shared by CAC or AAC and AD or cognitive traits. The study approaches are especially important given the inconsistent evidence from traditional observational studies. By building evidence-based knowledge, and using a more robust genetic study, we can improve our understanding of the causes of AD and identify potential targets for further investigation.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Overview of Study Design

This study aimed to explore the genetic relationships of vascular calcification (CAC and AAC) with AD and cognitive traits. We employed four analytical approaches to achieve our objectives. First, we performed cross-traits genome-wide genetic correlation between CAC/AAC and AD/cognitive traits using the LDSC method. Given the strong evidence of the Apolipoprotein E (APOE) region’s significant impact on AD, we performed genetic correlation analyses excluding the region to determine if it drives any relationship between CAC or AAC and AD. Second, we assessed potential causal relationships through the bidirectional MR analysis method with sensitivity analyses and rigorous assessment of horizontal pleiotropy and heterogeneity. Third, we applied the GWAS-PW method to identify shared causal variants and pleiotropic loci for a pair of CAC or AAC and AD or cognitive traits, testing 1703 genomic regions.
The GWAS-PW method scans the genome to pinpoint pleiotropic loci by estimating the posterior probability that a genetic variant is causal for a pair of traits, or a genomic locus is associated with the two traits through distinct variants [34]. Lastly, we conducted gene-based association analyses to identify pleiotropic genes associated with CAC/AAC and AD/cognitive traits, prioritising pleiotropic loci identified in the GWAS-PW analysis. We used a recently developed and powerful gene-based analysis method, harnessing the strengths of three models of gene-based association analyses: fastBAT, mBAT, and mBAT-combo [32,33]. All analyses used well-powered GWAS summary data from individuals of European ancestry, with no evidence of significant sample overlap between the pairs of traits assessed.
During the preparation of this work, the authors used Grammarly and ChatGPT for grammar and language refinement. After using these tools, the authors reviewed and edited the content as needed and take full responsibility for the content of the publication.

2.2. Data Sources

We leveraged large-scale GWAS summary data from publicly available repositories and international research consortia. We sourced GWAS summary data for CAC from a recent study [35], which included 28,655 individuals. CAC was assessed using computed tomography imaging, quantified, and expressed using Agatston scores. This method is a standard non-invasive approach for evaluating the extent of calcified plaque in coronary arteries, which serves as a measure of subclinical atherosclerosis. For AAC, we used data from another recent publication, representing the largest-scale evaluation of AAC to date [12]. The study assessed AAC in 38,264 participants from the UK Biobank—a large population-based cohort of middle-aged to older adults (40–70 years) [12]. Lateral spine dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry scans, originally intended for bone mineral density assessment, were repurposed to evaluate AAC using manual scoring and machine learning methodologies [12]. Calcification in the lumbar spine (L1–L4) was graded using Kauppila’s scoring system [12,36]. Each vertebral segment was graded from 0 to 3: 0 for no calcification, 1 for calcification spanning less than one-third of the vertebra, 2 for one-third to two-thirds, and 3 for more than two-thirds. The scores for all four vertebrae were summed, yielding a total AAC score up to a maximum of 24 [12].
Additionally, we drew on one of the largest publicly available AD GWAS datasets, which comprised 71,880 cases and 383,378 controls, encompassing both clinically diagnosed AD and AD by proxy [37]. The clinically diagnosed AD dataset consisted of three case–control cohorts consisting of 79,145 individuals [37]. Participants were classified as cases or controls based on clinical diagnoses [37]. The ascertainment of AD cases was conducted through medical records or clinician-assessed diagnoses [37]. Control participants were screened to exclude neurodegenerative diseases, particularly dementia, ensuring they served as appropriate comparisons [37]. The ‘AD-by-proxy’ dataset was derived from the UK Biobank and included individuals whose biological parents had AD [37]. Information about the parents’ current age or age at death was included alongside the GWAS data. A high genetic correlation of 0.81 was observed between the ‘clinically diagnosed AD’ and ‘AD-by-proxy’ datasets, supporting their integration, as detailed in the original publication [37]. For potential (partial, due to data not being completely independent) replication, we used another AD GWAS data comprising only the clinically diagnosed cohorts [38]. AD was derived in the study through the meta-analyses of GWAS data from four consortia. The consortia included the Alzheimer’s Disease Genetic Consortium, Cohorts for Heart and Ageing Research in Genomic Epidemiology, European Alzheimer’s Disease Initiative, and the Genetic and Environmental Risk in Alzheimer’s Disease Consortium. The analysis included a large cohort of individuals of European ancestry, comprising 17,008 Alzheimer’s disease cases and 37,154 controls [38].
For cognitive traits, we included summary data from five studies: cEF (n = 427,037) [39], cognitive performance (n = 257,828) [40], intelligence (n = 269,867) [41], fluid intelligence scores (n = 125,935) [42], and educational attainment (n = 766,345) [40]. cEF refers to a unified cognitive factor that captures the shared variance across multiple executive function (EF) tasks, including response inhibition, interference control, working memory updating, and set shifting [39]. In the original publication, the authors conducted a GWAS analysis to derive a cEF factor score that represents the common executive control elements across various cognitive-related tasks [39]. Educational attainment in this study was measured as the number of years of schooling completed. This variable was uniformly defined across cohorts, with quality control applied to ensure consistency in the measurement. Cognitive performance was assessed using various tests designed to evaluate general cognitive abilities [40]. The specific tests and measures varied between cohorts but were standardised to enable meta-analysis across samples [40]. Intelligence was derived from various neurocognitive tests administered across multiple cohorts. These tests primarily measured fluid domains of cognitive functioning, such as reasoning, memory, and problem-solving [41].
In this study, we analysed quantitative variables from GWAS summary data as continuous traits, retaining their original scale as defined in the source studies. The summary statistics for each trait were derived from genome-wide association analyses, with effect sizes reported as beta coefficients. Detailed descriptions of the datasets, including participant demographics, study settings, measurements, and quality control procedures, are available in the referenced publications. Participants in all the data underlying our study were individuals of European ancestry. Supplementary Table S1 offers a comprehensive overview of the data utilised in this study, including sample sizes and, where applicable, links to their sources.

2.3. Cross-Trait Genome-Wide Genetic Correlation Analyses

We conducted cross-trait genetic correlation analyses between CAC and each of AD and cognitive traits using the LDSC method. We similarly assessed the genetic correlation of AAC with AD and cognitive traits. The LDSC method estimates SNP heritability and genetic correlations by regressing GWAS test statistics (e.g., Z-scores) on LD scores for each SNP [43]. The method can distinguish true genetic signals from confounding factors and provide robust estimates of genetic relationships between traits [43].
Initially, we estimated genetic correlations with an unconstrained genetic covariance intercept to assess the potential proportion of sample overlap between traits, similarly to the practice in previous studies [26,27,43,44,45,46]. Our findings indicated that the genetic covariance intercepts were not significantly different from zero, suggesting no substantial sample overlap between AAC or CAC and AD or cognitive traits. Consequently, we proceeded with genetic covariance intercept-constrained analyses. In all LDSC analyses, pre-computed LD scores from the 1000 Genomes European reference panel were applied, excluding SNPs that did not intersect with the reference panel or had a MAF < 1%. To explore the potential impact of the APOE region (on AD-related analyses), we conducted analyses both with and without this region. A Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold of 8.33 × 10−3 was applied for genetic correlations involving AD and five cognitive traits, with p-values below 0.05 considered nominally significant.

2.4. Causal Relationship Assessment Using Mendelian Randomisation

We used the two-sample Mendelian randomisation (2SMR) analysis method to explore the potential causality of CAC or AAC with AD and various cognitive traits. Several MR methods were utilised, and we conducted a bidirectional assessment for a clear insight into the likely causal relationships of these traits. To ensure the robustness of our findings, we carefully selected appropriate instrumental variables (IVs) for the analysis and rigorously addressed potential issues around horizontal pleiotropy and heterogeneity [28,29,30]. We analysed data of the same ancestry (Europeans) and followed the STROBE-MR guidelines in this study’s design and conduct of MR analysis [31]. Figure 1 provides a summary of the procedural framework of our MR analysis.

2.4.1. Selection of Instrumental Variables for MR Analysis

We selected IVs at the genome-wide significance (GWS) level (p < 5 × 10−8) from the relevant GWAS summary data used in our study. This stringent selection criterion ensures that the IVs are strongly associated with the exposure variables, with an F-statistic greater than 10 [28], thereby minimising the risk of weak instrument bias and fulfilling the first assumption of MR. Due to the limited availability of GWS IVs for CAC and AAC as exposure variables, we relaxed the selection threshold to the genome-wide suggestive level (p < 1 × 10−5). This adjustment was made with the understanding that using a smaller number of SNPs (<10) as IVs may introduce potential bias into our study. Although the second MR assumption—that IVs are not associated with confounders—is challenging or nearly impossible to fully validate, we rigorously evaluated our IVs and performed linkage disequilibrium clumping at a stringent threshold (r2 < 0.001, Figure 1) to enhance the independence of the selected instruments. Importantly, we ensured that our IVs were not linked to the outcome variables (p < 0.05), thereby contributing to adhering to the third MR assumption. To enhance the quality and reliability of our MR analysis, we excluded genetic variants with intermediate allele frequencies, as these can lead to ambiguous strand alignment. Additionally, we removed variants not present in the reference dataset and harmonised the exposure and outcome data, aligning alleles and ensuring consistency in effect directions. As highlighted in the sub-sections for the main MR and sensitivity analyses, we carried out other specific tests to ensure our IVs are robust.

2.4.2. Performing MR Analyses

We used the inverse variance weighted (IVW) as the main MR model in the current study. This approach combines the Wald ratios for each genetic instrument into a single, weighted average, where the weights are the inverse of the variance of each ratio. The IVW method assumes the absence of horizontal pleiotropy, and the model is reliable so long as this assumption holds. To address potential heterogeneity among the causal estimates derived from different variants, we employed multiplicative random effects of the IVW model. Also, to complement our IVW estimates, we used additional MR methods, including the weighted median (which can yield valid estimates with up to 50% invalid IVs) and the MR-Egger (which can provide valid estimates by correcting for pleiotropy) [28,29]. We considered MR results with p < 0.05 as nominally significant and implemented a Bonferroni correction to mitigate the risk of false positives due to multiple tests across the various outcomes. Based on this adjustment, we set the significance threshold at p < 0.008 (0.05/6), corresponding to the analysis of six outcome variables (where applicable). We utilised the R statistical packages and the Unix environment for data management and analyses, and used the 2SMR software (version 0.5.6) [48,49], and MR-PRESSO [50] for the MR analyses (implemented on the R packages [version 4.2.1]).

2.4.3. MR Sensitivity Analyses

Following the practice in related studies [28,51,52,53,54,55], and as part of the requirements for valid MR analysis [28,29,30,31], we conducted further tests to assess the reliability of our results. These tests include Cochran’s Q statistics to evaluate the heterogeneity of SNP effects, individual MR analyses, ’leave-one-out’ analyses to determine the impact of each IV on the overall results and examining the funnel plot for symmetry. We used the MR-Egger intercept to check for potential violations of the assumption of no unbalanced pleiotropy. Significant deviations of the MR-Egger intercept from zero indicate a possible violation of this assumption. Additionally, we implemented the MR pleiotropy residual sum and outlier (MR-PRESSO) method, which is known for identifying and addressing pleiotropy by excluding outliers [50]. Importantly, as an additional step, we meticulously reviewed our analyses and assessed each IV for their effect on the outcome variables. We excluded likely pleiotropic IVs associated with the outcome variables at a significance level of p < 0.05. This process involved scrutinising our LD clumped (clumping performed at r2  <  0.001) and properly harmonised data, excluding IVs with Poutcome-variable < 0.05, followed by MR analysis on the remaining instruments.

2.5. Assessing Shared Genetic Risk Loci: The Pairwise Gwas and Gene-Based Approach

We conducted colocalisation analysis using the GWAS-PW method, a tool designed to scan the genome for regions that likely share a causal variant or pleiotropic loci between traits [34]. This programme applies a Bayesian statistical model to estimate the probability (or PPA) and we modelled four potential scenarios: (1) a region contains a variant associated only with trait 1 (PPA1); (2) a variant associated only with trait 2 (PPA2); (3) a variant associated with both traits 1 and 2 (PPA3); or (4) independent variants are associated with each trait 1 and trait 2 (PPA4) but the region is shared by both traits [34]. In this study, we first conducted an analysis using CAC as trait 1 against AD and cognitive traits as trait 2. In the second analysis, we used AAC as trait 1 and, again, compared it with AD and cognitive traits as trait 2.
We applied GWAS-PW to assess potential shared causal variants and loci between CAC and AAC with AD and cognitive traits. The summary statistics for CAC and AAC were aligned with AD and cognitive trait data by rsID and alleles, ensuring consistent effect and non-effect alleles across traits. Standardised Z-scores and variances for each SNP were then used as the input to the GWAS-PW model [34]. The analysis covered 1703 predefined independent genetic regions based on LD patterns from the 1000 Genomes Project European reference data. There is no evidence for a significant overlap of samples between CAC or AAC and AD or cognitive traits, ruling out potential confounding or the need for adjustments in our analysis.
Our focus was on PPA3 and PPA4 results, thus, we considered regions with PPA3 > 0.9 to have a significant shared causal variant between the two traits (e.g., CAC and AD), while those with PPA3 > 0.5 were deemed suggestive. Similarly, we interpreted loci with PPA4 > 0.9 as harbouring distinct causal variants for each trait, influencing both independently, with the two traits sharing the loci. On the other hand, PPA4 > 0.5 was considered a suggestive association. The identification of risk variants or loci by GWAS-PW was further refined using three gene-based association analysis methods, including fastBAT, mBAT, and the mBAT-combo [32,33], to aggregate variant signals within genes and assess their overall contribution. Briefly, we aimed to detect shared genes in regions with strong evidence for pleiotropy based on PPA3/PPA4 > 0.9 (and at the suggestive level, PPA3/PPA4 > 0.5), and we used the gene-based method to ensure that the mapped genes were associated with the pairs of traits assessed. The mBAT-combo method combines mBAT and fastBAT statistics, and is superior to traditional sum-χ2 approaches, especially for identifying genes with masking effects [33]. The method has proven more powerful in simulations and real-world data [33]; hence, we prioritise using the method in the current study. For this analysis, SNPs were mapped within 50 kb of gene boundaries.

3. Results

3.1. Results of Genome-Wide Genetic Correlation Analyses

Table 1 presents the results of our genome-wide genetic correlation analyses. First, we observed a nominally significant positive genetic correlation (rg = 0.10, p = 3.14 × 10−2) between CAC and AD GWAS (from Jansen et al. [37]), but when excluding the APOE region, the signal weakens and becomes non-significant (p = 5.33 × 10−2, Table 1). We found no evidence of a significant genome-wide correlation between CAC and another AD GWAS (Lambert et al. [38]), with or without the exclusion of the APOE region. Conversely, we observed a negative correlation between CAC and cognitive traits, including cognitive performance (rg = −0.11, p = 1.59 × 10−6), fluid intelligence scores (rg = −0.11, p = 1.87 × 10−5), intelligence (rg = −0.11, p = 5.14 × 10−6), and educational attainment (rg = −0.10, p = 1.08 × 10−6), all surpassing the Bonferroni threshold. The correlation assessment between CAC and common executive function was not statistically significant (Table 1). Lastly, the genetic correlation between AAC and CAC was high (rg = 0.95, p = 4.70 × 10−3), surpassing the Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold, indicating a strong shared genetic basis between the two vascular calcification traits.
Second, we found no evidence of a significant genetic correlation between AAC and AD across different GWAS datasets, including the AD GWAS [37] (rg = 0.08, p = 2.32 × 10−1) and the AD GWAS [38] (rg = 0.01, p = 9.28 × 10−1). Excluding the APOE regions did not change these outcomes (Table 1). However, similar to CAC, we found significant negative genetic correlations of AAC with cognitive performance (rg = −0.10, p = 5.49 × 10−3) and educational attainment (rg = −0.13, p = 6.39 × 10−5), surpassing nominal and Bonferroni significance thresholds, respectively. The correlations with fluid intelligence (rg = −0.07, p = 5.24 × 10−2), intelligence (rg = −0.06, p = 1.12 × 10−1), and executive function (rg = −0.04, p = 2.53 × 10−1) were not statistically significant.

3.2. Results of MR-Based Causal Association Assessment

We conducted a 2SMR analysis to gain insights into the potential causal relationships between CAC or AAC and AD and cognitive traits. MR assesses causal effects under three specific assumptions: (1) the genetic variants used as IVs must have a strong association with the exposure, (2) these SNPs should be independent of any confounding factors, and (3) they should affect the outcome solely through their influence on the exposure [28,29,30,31]. We employed multiple MR approaches and performed a bidirectional analysis to gain a comprehensive understanding of these associations. To enhance the reliability of our results, we carefully selected suitable IVs and addressed concerns related to horizontal pleiotropy and heterogeneity. Figure 1 presents a detailed overview of our MR study design and highlights its specific assumptions.
The main MR results from the IVW model indicated no significant causal effect of CAC on AD (OR: 1.00, 95% CI: 0.99–1.01, p: 0.56) or AAC on AD (OR: 1.00, 95% CI: 0.98–1.03, p: 0.78) [Figure 2 and Figure 3, respectively]. Similarly, CAC or AAC had no significant causal effect on cognitive traits (Figure 2 and Figure 3). The only exception was the causal association of AAC on fluid intelligence scores. This effect was borderline nominally significant in the IVW model and nominally significant in both the weighted median and MR-PRESSO analyses (Figure 3 and Supplementary Table S2). However, the results did not survive the correction for multiple testing (Figure 3).
In the reverse analyses, our findings showed no significant causal effect of AD on CAC (OR: 1.29, 95% CI: 0.67–2.46, p: 0.44) or AAC (OR: 1.00, 95% CI: 0.86–1.16, p: 0.99) as summarised in Figure 4 and Figure 5, respectively. Likewise, none of the cognitive traits had a significant causal effect on CAC or AAC, except the borderline nominally significance of fluid intelligence scores’ effect on AAC (Figure 5), which did not survive correction for multiple testing. Current findings were consistent across other MR models, such as the weighted median and MR-Egger (Supplementary Tables S2–S5).
The MR-PRESSO method also found no significant relationship between AD and cognitive traits with CAC or AAC (except between fluid intelligence scores and AAC). Indeed, there were no outputs in the corrected analysis, suggesting the absence of outliers IVs. Supplementary Tables S2–S5 provide comprehensive results for our MR analysis. MR-Egger intercept and heterogeneity tests indicate these findings were not biased by pleiotropic instruments (Table 2, and more comprehensively in Supplementary Table S6). Detailed information on IVs utilised for analysis is presented in Supplementary Tables S7–S30.

Addressing Horizontal Pleiotropy in Our MR Analysis

Here, we present findings of how we further addressed horizontal pleiotropy in our study—demonstrating how seemingly non-significant MR pleiotropy tests can lead to erroneous claims of causal associations. These results emphasise the importance of addressing pleiotropy and heterogeneity for valid causal estimates. For example, our initial analysis showed a significant causal effect of AD on CAC (IVW model, OR: 3.06, 95% CI: 1.60–5.85, p: 7.51 × 10−4, Supplementary Table S31). This result was supported by a non-significant pleiotropy test (Egger intercept: −0.020, p: 0.122) and corroborated by other MR methods, including MR Egger, the weighted median, and the weighted mode (Supplementary Table S31). Additionally, the crude estimate from MR-PRESSO aligned with the IVW result, supporting a significant causal effect of AD on CAC. The corrected MR-PRESSO results indicated a nominally significant causal influence of AD on CAC (OR: 2.11, 95% CI: 1.20–3.73, p: 1.68 × 10−2). Using the same approach, we found a significant causal effect of AD on AAC (Supplementary Table S31).
However, these results do not reflect true causal relationships, as there was evidence of significant heterogeneity (Supplementary Table S31). Upon examining the effect of the selected instruments on the outcome variables, we found that heterogeneity arose from the association of IVs with the outcomes, violating the third assumption of MR, which requires influencing the outcome only through the exposure pathway. IVs associated with the outcome variable—even at a nominal level—can directly affect the outcome, violating the exclusion restriction assumption. When we addressed the heterogeneity in our study by excluding pleiotropic SNPs, the results, which were once significant, became non-significant.
Based on our experience with recent MR publications, we speculate that such false positive results occur frequently, leading to erroneous causal claims or conclusions. The example in our study, thus, underscores the importance of rigorously addressing heterogeneity in 2SMR studies to avoid false claims of causality or misleading conclusions.

3.3. Shared Genomic Loci Between CAC or AAC and AD or Cognitive Traits

To advance our understanding of the relationship between CAC and AAC with AD and cognitive traits, we applied the GWAS-PW method [34] towards identifying potential pleiotropic loci or variants (see Section 2). Our analysis found that none of the tested 1703 genomic regions had a PPA3(the model where a shared locus with the same causal variant influences both traits [34]) greater than 0.5, indicating no evidence of causal SNPs associated with both CAC/AAC and AD/cognitive traits. However, we identified pleiotropic loci shared by CAC or AAC and AD or cognitive traits (Table 3 and Table 4), particularly on chromosome 19, suggesting a shared locus but with separate causal variants influencing the pair of traits. The PPA4 estimates were greater than 0.9 in these regions, indicating strong evidence or a high likelihood of pleiotropy, with the regions associated with both traits through distinct SNPs [34].
For example, our findings revealed that the locus at chr19:44,744,370–46,102,289 (hg19) is pleiotropic for CAC and AD, with separate top SNPs (Table 3, PPA4 =1). Similarly, the locus at chr19:44,744,108–46,102,684 (hg19) showed strong evidence of association, with AAC and AD involving distinct SNPs (Table 3). Using the mBAT-combo method, we identified genes within these regions, many of which exhibited significant associations (Bonferroni-adjusted Pgene < 1.06 × 10−3) with CAC or AAC and AD (or at least a nominal level of significance) (Table 3, PPA4 = 1), and were also identified by at least an additional gene-based method, either fastBAT or mBAT. These genes include BCAM, TOMM40, NECTIN2, APOE, APOC1, CBLC, APOC4, APOC2, APOC4-APOC2, EXOC3L2, and CLPTM1 (Table 3).
Table 4 highlights the shared loci between CAC/AAC and cognitive traits and the pleiotropic genes associated with each trait pair. Notably, CAC and cEF share a region on chromosome 19: 44,744,370–46,102,547 (hg19, PPA4 = 1), where the implicated gene (PHLDB3) is only nominally significant for both traits. This locus was identified exclusively by the mBAT model. Similarly to AD, regions in chromosome 19 were significantly associated with CAC or AAC and some cognitive traits, including educational attainment, cEF, and fluid intelligence scores, as presented in Table 4.
Lastly, we identified additional loci with only suggestive association in the GWAS-PW analysis implicating regions in chromosomes one, six, seven, and nineteen, and their corresponding significant genes for the pair of traits assessed. Supplementary Table S32 provides information about these loci and the likely pleiotropic genes implicated.
To improve the readability of the GWAS-PW’s findings and the interpretation of implicated genes, we summarise the results. Table 5 presents key pleiotropic loci with functional annotations. This table provides a biological context for the identified associations, helping to simplify the study’s interpretability. The functional annotations were derived using publicly available bioinformatics resources to verify gene functions, biological pathways, and disease associations. Specifically, gene functions were obtained from GeneCards (www.genecards.org, accessed on 21 February 2025), highlighting roles in lipid metabolism (APOE, APOC1-APOC4), mitochondrial function (TOMM40), immune response and cell adhesion (NECTIN2), and protein degradation (CBLC). Additionally, the literature on AD and cardiovascular traits was consulted to contextualise the biological relevance of these genes in vascular calcification and neurodegeneration.

4. Discussion

Using well-established methods, we present findings from analyses assessing the potential shared genetic architecture and causal relationships of CAC, and AAC, with AD, and cognitive traits. Our genome-wide genetic correlation analysis revealed a nominally significant association between CAC and AD. However, this result was not replicated in another AD dataset. While differences in study power may partially explain the variability, the observed nominal significance disappeared after excluding the APOE region, suggesting that the correlation initially observed was primarily driven by pleiotropy at this locus. Furthermore, we found no significant genome-wide genetic correlation between AAC and AD. Conversely, we found a significant negative genetic correlation between CAC and several cognitive traits, surviving correction for multiple testing, including cognitive performance, educational attainment, and intelligence scores. AAC also demonstrated a significant negative correlation with cognitive performance and educational attainment.
The significant negative genetic correlations observed suggest that a higher genetic predisposition to CAC and AAC is associated with lower cognitive performance. This finding aligns with studies reporting associations between vascular calcification and cognitive decline [8,17,18,19]. However, given the nature of genetic correlation assessments, this inverse relationship could also indicate that a higher genetic predisposition to better cognitive performance is linked to a reduced risk of vascular calcification. It is important to note that genetic correlation does not imply causation, as the observed associations may arise due to pleiotropy, shared genetic susceptibility, or other confounding factors. Additionally, for traits such as educational attainment, non-genetic influences—including socioeconomic conditions, environmental exposures, and social factors—may further complicate the relationship. Thus, while our findings indicate an inverse genetic association between CAC/AAC and certain cognitive traits, alternative explanations involving interactions between genetics, cognition, environment, and life-course factors may also play a role.
We performed bidirectional MR analyses to examine the potential causal associations of CAC and AAC with AD and cognitive traits and gain further insights into the nature of the relationships. Our comprehensive and rigorous analyses indicate that CAC or AAC is not causally associated with AD or cognitive traits, regardless of the direction of analysis, whether CAC or AAC were exposure or outcome variables. The only exception was the causal effect of AAC on fluid intelligence scores and vice versa, which was borderline nominally significant in the IVW model. These results did not survive correction for multiple testing, making them less convincing. Our findings were consistent across several MR models, and tests for heterogeneity or pleiotropy did not indicate potential bias. Vascular calcification, specifically CAC, is a well-established marker for improved risk prediction of subsequent atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease [13,14]. Atherosclerosis (characterised by vascular calcification) is associated with the risk of AD [6]. Indeed, a recent study highlighted an additive interaction effect between atherosclerosis and AD on cognitive functions [7]. Hence, it is reasonable to hypothesise that vascular calcification might be related to AD or cognitive traits. Several conventional observational studies support this hypothesis, suggesting a positive association between vascular calcification and AD or cognitive decline [9,10,17,18,19,56], although mixed results have also been reported [22]. Our genetic-based assessment, however, did not confirm convincing significant causal associations. Current results provide new insights into the interplay between these phenotypes and improve our understanding beyond what is known through traditional observational evidence.
A recent observational study, for example, reported an association between AAC and an increased risk of all-cause late-life dementia among elderly women [9]. While this study highlighted a potential link [9], our genetic-based analysis does not support a causal relationship. Unlike genetic approaches, traditional observational studies are often susceptible to unmeasured confounders and biases related to lifestyle or environmental factors, which may explain the observed associations. Additionally, our null MR results could stem from several factors, including the limitations of genetic instruments, or the possibility that the relationship between vascular calcification and AD involves complex disease mechanisms, such as mediation by other cardiovascular risk factors. Moreover, it should be noted that our study specifically focuses on AD, whereas the observational study [9] did not distinguish between dementia subtypes and was restricted to late-life onset (after 80 years) in women only. Future research, thus, should further investigate specific dementia subtypes and consider additional methodological approaches to better understand the interplay between vascular calcification, dementia, and cognitive decline.
Importantly, our study emphasises the necessity of rigorously addressing pleiotropy and heterogeneity to enhance the reliability of MR findings. Despite the challenges in proving some of them, it is essential to uphold the core assumptions of MR to infer valid causal estimates [30]. Adhering to best practices is vital to prevent estimating spurious causal associations, including implementing various MR approaches and conducting sensitivity testing [28,29,30,57]. Notably, heterogeneity tests can reveal potential biases in MR studies, as illustrated in our results. For example, there was evidence for heterogeneity between AD (as exposure) and CAC (as outcome) based on Cochran’s Q p value (Supplementary Table S31) in our illustrated example. This evidence signals a potential violation of MR assumptions and ignoring this observation would have resulted in false positives in our study. This premise, thus, underscores the need for further assessment, such as checking that the IVs are valid. We often found cases where authors overlook such significant heterogeneity tests in recent MR studies, potentially leading to claims of causality that may not be true.
The limited genome-wide genetic correlation and non-causal associations results in our analyses do not preclude shared genetic predisposition through specific regions in the genome. Hence, we progressed our study to investigate the potential shared variants or loci between CAC/AAC and AD or cognitive traits using the GWAS-PW and gene-based association methods. Our findings revealed that none of the 1703 tested genomic regions exhibited a PPA3 greater than 0.5, indicating a lack of evidence for causal SNPs influencing CAC or AAC and AD or cognitive traits. However, we identified loci on chromosome 19 demonstrating a high likelihood of pleiotropy (PPA4 > 0.9), indicating distinct causal variants affecting CAC/AAC and AD/cognitive traits. These findings implicate pleiotropic genetic underpinnings, primarily driven by the identified loci, particularly regions in chromosome 19, partly highlighting consistency between our genetic correlation and GWAS-PW results.
Within the implicated loci on chromosome 19, we identified several pleiotropic genes, including BCAM, TOMM40, NECTIN2, APOE, APOC1, CBLC, APOC4, APOC2, APOC4-APOC2, EXOC3L2, and CLPTM1. These genes exhibited significant associations with both CAC or AAC and AD or cognitive traits, indicating their role in the genetic architecture of these conditions and the potential co-occurrence of CAC/AAC with AD and cognitive decline. Recent findings further support the pleiotropic nature of chromosome 19 loci in AD-related traits and vascular phenotypes. For example, a study [58] highlights the involvement of APOE and its neighbouring genes (TOMM40, NECTIN2, BCAM) in AD and cognitive decline, partly reinforcing our findings of shared genetic susceptibility between vascular calcification and AD-related traits. The study [58] also underscores the importance of this region in complex trait interactions, aligning with our observation of pleiotropic effects. Furthermore, our analysis revealed shared loci between CAC/AAC and cognitive traits, as detailed in Table 4, including a significant region on chr19: 44744370–46102547 associated with CAC and cEF via PHLDB3. Although PHLDB3 was only nominally significant (the only significant gene for both traits) and identified solely by the mBAT model of the gene-based analysis, the region exhibited a PPA4 = 1, indicating a high probability of pleiotropy (via distinct variants) for these traits.
Notably, the findings implicating APOE and its neighbouring genes as pleiotropic for AD and vascular calcification are intriguing. As the strongest genetic risk factor for late-onset AD [59], APOE ε4 exacerbates amyloid-beta accumulation, tau pathology, neuroinflammation, and age-related cognitive decline, contributing to neurodegeneration [60,61]. In contrast, APOE ε2 is considered protective in AD, enhancing amyloid-beta clearance and reducing amyloid plaque burden [62]. However, APOE ε2 is not entirely benign—it is linked to hypertriglyceridemia and type III hyperlipoproteinemia, which may increase cardiovascular risk in susceptible individuals [62]. Beyond its role in AD, APOE is a key regulator of lipid metabolism, though its cardiovascular effects vary by isoform. While APOE ε4 is associated with higher cardiovascular risk, exacerbating atherosclerosis and arterial calcification, APOE ε2’s role is more complex [62]. Although it may improve lipid transport and lower cholesterol levels in some cases, APOE ε2’s association with dyslipidaemia suggests a potential risk factor for cardiovascular disease under specific conditions [62,63,64].
Moreover, Apoe-knockout models indicate that APOE deficiency promotes vascular calcification through dysregulated lipid metabolism, chronic inflammation, and oxidative stress [61,65]. Apoe−/− mice further exhibit endothelial dysfunction, oxidative stress, and accelerated atherosclerotic plaque progression, reinforcing its role in vascular calcification [65]. Given APOE’s multiple roles, its shared genetic architecture may contribute to vascular calcification, AD pathology, and other metabolic or cerebrovascular conditions through distinct yet interconnected mechanisms. Such mechanisms may include dysregulated lipid metabolism, neuroinflammation, amyloid processing, impaired vascular integrity, and oxidative stress, collectively influencing both vascular and neurodegenerative disorders [61]. Additionally, APOE ε4 carriers experience increased cerebrovascular dysfunction, including blood–brain barrier disruption and impaired vascular integrity, which may link vascular calcification to neurodegeneration [66]. Future studies should further investigate APOE-mediated mechanisms to clarify their role in the interplay between vascular and neurodegenerative disorders.
In summary, our study provides new insights into the complex genetic relationships of CAC, and AAC, with AD, and cognitive traits. While we observed some genetic correlations—particularly a negative association between CAC/AAC and cognitive traits—our findings highlight the absence of causal relationships in the MR analyses. Further assessment using the GWAS-PW method suggests that potential associations do not result from shared causal SNPs but reflect genetic susceptibilities driven by distinct variants within implicated loci. Identifying pleiotropic loci and significant genes associated with vascular calcification, AD, and cognitive traits further supports this position.

Strengths and Limitations

A key strength of this study is its use of a genetic approach, which is less susceptible to confounding from environmental and lifestyle factors, thus enhancing the reliability of our findings beyond what is possible through the conventional observational study approach. To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the relationship between vascular calcification, AD, and cognitive traits using statistical genetic methods. However, some limitations should be considered when interpreting our results. Firstly, the data utilised are exclusively from the European population, limiting the generalisability of our findings to other ancestries. Secondly, due to a lack of sufficient genome-wide significant instruments for CAC and AAC, we relaxed the threshold for IVs selection in our MR analysis to the suggestive level. This observation may indicate less powerful GWAS results for CAC and AAC, which could affect the non-significant causal associations observed in our study. We note, however, that we have used the best available data for CAC and AAC (to the best of our knowledge), in our study. Future research, thus, needs to explore these relationships further as more robust CAC and AAC GWAS data become available. Thirdly, although sample overlap can bias analyses such as genetic correlation, MR, and GWAS-PW, our preliminary evaluation indicates no evidence of significant sample overlap between CAC/AAC and AD/cognitive traits, thus ruling out bias from this factor in our study. Finally, our findings of limited genetic overlap and no causal association of CAC and AAC with AD or cognitive traits do not eliminate the possibility of associations due to shared risk factors. Therefore, comprehensive prospective studies may be beneficial for further elucidating the nature of these associations. Moreover, we recommend an assessment of the relationships based on the various types of dementia.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, our study sheds new light on the intricate genetic relationships between CAC/AAC and AD/various cognitive traits. We observed notable genetic correlations, particularly a significant negative association between CAC/AAC and cognitive performance, which underscores the importance of genetic factors in these conditions. However, our findings from MR analyses highlight the absence of causal relationships between CAC or AAC and AD or cognitive traits, suggesting that the observed associations are not indicative of direct causal effects. Further assessments reinforce the notion that the genetic correlations observed are not due to shared causal SNPs, but rather reflect shared genetic susceptibilities influenced by distinct genetic variants within the implicated loci. The identification of pleiotropic loci and significant genes, such as those associated with the APOE region and other genes on chromosomes 1, 6, 7, 9, and 19, underscores the shared genetic susceptibility between vascular calcification, AD, and cognitive traits. These findings provide insights into the complex genetic interplay between the traits, revealing pleiotropic loci and significant genes that serve as essential targets for further investigation. Future research should focus on elucidating the mechanisms by which shared genetic factors influence both vascular and cognitive health. To build on our findings, future studies can integrate multi-omics approaches to uncover functional pathways linking vascular calcification and neurodegenerative processes. Experimental validation using cellular and animal models can be utilised to confirm the genetic associations and clarify their biological relevance. Additionally, given that genetic effects may vary by ancestry and sex, future research should investigate population-specific differences to improve the generalisability of findings.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/biomedicines13030618/s1, Table S1: Description of GWAS data analysed in this study with links to the source; Table S2: MR analysis of Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive traits with coronary artery calcification; Table S3: MR analysis of Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive traits with abdominal aortic calcification; Table S4: MR analysis of coronary artery calcification with Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive traits; Table S5: MR analysis of abdominal aortic calcification with Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive traits; Table S6: Comprehensive MR Sensitivity analyses of AD and cognitive traits with vascular calcification; Table S7: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of Alzheimer’s disease vs abdominal aortic calcification; Table S8: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of Alzheimer’s disease vs coronary artery calcification; Table S9: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of common executive function vs abdominal aortic calcification; Table S10: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of common executive function vs coronary artery calcification; Table S11: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of cognitive performance vs abdominal aortic calcification; Table S12: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of cognitive performance vs coronary artery calcification; Table S13: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of educational attainment vs abdominal aortic calcification; Table S14: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of educational attainment vs coronary artery calcification; Table S15: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of fluid intelligence score vs abdominal aortic calcification; Table S16: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of fluid intelligence score vs coronary artery calcification; Table S17: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of intelligence vs abdominal aortic calcification; Table S18: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of intelligence vs coronary artery calcification; Table S19: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of abdominal aortic calcification vs Alzheimer’s disease; Table S20: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of coronary artery calcification vs Alzheimer’s disease; Table S21: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of abdominal aortic calcification vs common executive function; Table S22: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of coronary artery calcification vs common executive function; Table S23: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of abdominal aortic calcification vs cognitive performance; Table S24: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of coronary artery calcification vs cognitive performance; Table S25: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of abdominal aortic calcification vs educational attainment; Table S26: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of coronary artery calcification vs educational attainment; Table S27: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of abdominal aortic calcification vs fluid intelligence score; Table S28: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of coronary artery calcification vs fluid intelligence score; Table S29: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of abdominal aortic calcification vs intelligence; Table S30: Instrumental variables for MR analysis of coronary artery calcification vs intelligence; Table S31: Illustrative MR analysis of Alzheimer’s disease and vascular calcification; Table S32: Shared genomic loci of CAC and AAC with AD and cognitive traits.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.O.A. and S.M.L.; methodology, E.O.A.; formal analysis, E.O.A.; writing—original draft preparation, E.O.A.; writing—review and editing, E.O.A. and S.M.L.; supervision, E.O.A. and S.M.L.; funding acquisition, E.O.A. and S.M.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Emmanuel O Adewuyi was funded by the Department of Health Western Australia through the Future Health Research and Innovation—WA Near-miss Awards: Emerging Leaders Programme (grant G1006599). In addition, Emmanuel O Adewuyi received support from an NHMRC Investigator Grant (GNT2025837). Simon M Laws was supported by funding from the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Australia (grant APP1161706 and APP1191535). Funders did not play a role in the design, analysis, or interpretation of the study findings.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted based on a secondary analysis of pre-existing, completely de-identified genetic data. Ethical approval was obtained for each of the primary studies that published the data used, as detailed in the original associated studies. No additional ethical clearance was necessary for this current investigation.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

For this study, we utilised publicly available software for our analyses. Below, we provide the URLs for these tools, some of which include online methods and access to their respective details, such as documentation and (where applicable) source codes: GWAS-PW: https://github.com/joepickrell/gwas-pw (accessed on 19 September 2024), LDSC: https://github.com/bulik/ldsc (accessed on 24 March 2024), Two-Sample MR: https://mrcieu.github.io/TwoSampleMR/articles/introduction.html (accessed on 25 March 2024). The GWAS data used in this study were sourced from publicly accessible repositories and research groups or consortia, as specified in the data sources section. Supplementary Table S1 contains further details about these datasets, including sample sizes and links to their sources. The published article and its Supplementary Materials include all data generated during this study.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge all the international research groups and consortia that generated the GWAS data analysed in this study. This research work has been supported by the Western Australian Future Health Research and Innovation Fund. The authors would like to acknowledge the use of Grammarly and ChatGPT (ChatGPT-4 Turbo) for grammar and language refinement during the preparation of this manuscript. After using these tools, the authors reviewed and edited the content as needed and take full responsibility for the content of the publication.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
AACAbdominal aortic calcification
ADAlzheimer’s disease
CACCoronary artery calcification
cEFCommon executive function
CIConfidence interval
CTCognitive traits
EAEffect allele
GWASGenome-wide association studies
GWAS-PWPair-wise GWAS
GWSGenome-wide significant
IVInstrumental variable
IVWInverse variance weighted
LDLinkage disequilibrium
LDSCLinkage disequilibrium score regression
MAFMinor allele frequency
MRMendelian randomisation
NEANon-effect allele
PPAPosterior probability of association
SNPSingle nucleotide polymorphism
2SMRTwo-sample Mendelian randomisation

References

  1. Knopman, D.S.; Amieva, H.; Petersen, R.C.; Chételat, G.; Holtzman, D.M.; Hyman, B.T.; Nixon, R.A.; Jones, D.T. Alzheimer disease. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 2021, 7, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Nandi, A.; Counts, N.; Bröker, J.; Malik, S.; Chen, S.; Han, R.; Klusty, J.; Seligman, B.; Tortorice, D.; Vigo, D.; et al. Cost of care for Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias in the United States: 2016 to 2060. Npj Aging 2024, 10, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Nandi, A.; Counts, N.; Chen, S.; Seligman, B.; Tortorice, D.; Vigo, D.; Bloom, D.E. Global and regional projections of the economic burden of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias from 2019 to 2050: A value of statistical life approach. EClinicalMedicine 2022, 51, 101580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Alzheimer’s Disease International. Dementia Statistics. Available online: https://www.alzint.org/about/dementia-facts-figures/dementia-statistics/ (accessed on 23 August 2023).
  5. Eisenmenger, L.B.; Peret, A.; Famakin, B.M.; Spahic, A.; Roberts, G.S.; Bockholt, J.H.; Johnson, K.M.; Paulsen, J.S. Vascular contributions to Alzheimer’s disease. Transl. Res. 2023, 254, 41–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Xie, B.; Shi, X.; Xing, Y.; Tang, Y. Association between atherosclerosis and Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Behav. 2020, 10, e01601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Frentz, I.; van Arendonk, J.; Leeuwis, A.E.; Vernooij, M.W.; van der Flier, W.M.; Bos, D.; De Deyn, P.P.; Wolters, F.J.; Ikram, M.A. Interaction Between Arteriosclerosis and Amyloid-β on Cognitive Function. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2024, 97, 953–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Bos, D.; Vernooij, M.W.; de Bruijn, R.F.A.G.; Koudstaal, P.J.; Hofman, A.; Franco, O.H.; van der Lugt, A.; Ikram, M.A. Atherosclerotic calcification is related to a higher risk of dementia and cognitive decline. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2015, 11, 639–647.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Porter, T.; Sim, M.; Prince, R.L.; Schousboe, J.T.; Bondonno, C.; Lim, W.H.; Zhu, K.; Kiel, D.P.; Hodgson, J.M.; Laws, S.M.; et al. Abdominal aortic calcification on lateral spine images captured during bone density testing and late-life dementia risk in older women: A prospective cohort study. Lancet Reg. Health-West. Pac. 2022, 26, 100502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Ma, Y.N.; Xie, W.X.; Hou, Z.H.; An, Y.Q.; Ren, X.S.; Ma, Y.J.; Li, C.L.; Wu, Y.F.; Lu, B. Association between coronary artery calcification and cognitive function in a Chinese community-based population. J. Geriatr. Cardiol. 2021, 18, 514–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Lee, S.J.; Lee, I.-K.; Jeon, J.-H. Vascular Calcification—New Insights into Its Mechanism. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2020, 21, 2685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Sethi, A.; Taylor, D.L.; Ruby, J.G.; Venkataraman, J.; Sorokin, E.; Cule, M.; Melamud, E. Calcification of the abdominal aorta is an under-appreciated cardiovascular disease risk factor in the general population. Front. Cardiovasc. Med. 2022, 9, 1003246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Johansen, M.C.; Blaha, M.J. Coronary Artery Calcium: A Risk Factor for Brain Aging? Circ. Cardiovasc. Imaging 2021, 14, e013062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Orringer, C.E.; Blaha, M.J.; Blankstein, R.; Budoff, M.J.; Goldberg, R.B.; Gill, E.A.; Maki, K.C.; Mehta, L.; Jacobson, T.A. The National Lipid Association scientific statement on coronary artery calcium scoring to guide preventive strategies for ASCVD risk reduction. J. Clin. Lipidol. 2021, 15, 33–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Vasan, R.S.; Pan, S.; Larson, M.G.; Mitchell, G.F.; Xanthakis, V. Arteriosclerosis, atherosclerosis, and cardiovascular health: Joint relations to the incidence of cardiovascular disease. Hypertension 2021, 78, 1232–1240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Zhang, Q.; Wu, G.; Zhang, X.; Wang, S.; Wang, Y. A two-sample Mendelian randomization study of atherosclerosis and dementia. iScience 2023, 26, 108325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Xia, C.; Vonder, M.; Sidorenkov, G.; Ma, R.; Oudkerk, M.; Harst, P.v.d.; Deyn, P.P.D.; Vliegenthart, R. Coronary Artery Calcium and Cognitive Function in Dutch Adults: Cross-Sectional Results of the Population-Based ImaLife Study. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2021, 10, e018172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Muhire, G.; Iulita, M.F.; Vallerand, D.; Youwakim, J.; Gratuze, M.; Petry, F.R.; Planel, E.; Ferland, G.; Girouard, H. Arterial stiffness due to carotid calcification disrupts cerebral blood flow regulation and leads to cognitive deficits. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2019, 8, e011630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Cheng, H.-M.; Wang, J.-J.; Chen, C.-H. The Role of Vascular Calcification in Heart Failure and Cognitive Decline. Pulse 2017, 5, 144–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Valderas, J.M.; Starfield, B.; Sibbald, B.; Salisbury, C.; Roland, M. Defining comorbidity: Implications for understanding health and health services. Ann. Fam. Med. 2009, 7, 357–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Xia, C.; Vonder, M.; Sidorenkov, G.; Oudkerk, M.; de Groot, J.C.; van der Harst, P.; de Bock, G.H.; De Deyn, P.P.; Vliegenthart, R. The relationship of coronary artery calcium and clinical coronary artery disease with cognitive function: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Atheroscler. Thromb. 2020, 27, 934–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Ngamdu, K.S.; Kalra, D.K. Risk of Stroke, Dementia, and Cognitive Decline with Coronary and Arterial Calcification. J. Clin. Med. 2024, 13, 4263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Hess, A.S.; Abd-Elsayed, A. Observational Studies: Uses and Limitations. In Pain: A Review Guide; Abd-Elsayed, A., Ed.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; pp. 123–125. [Google Scholar]
  24. Tam, V.; Patel, N.; Turcotte, M.; Bossé, Y.; Paré, G.; Meyre, D. Benefits and limitations of genome-wide association studies. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2019, 20, 467–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Buniello, A.; MacArthur, J.A.L.; Cerezo, M.; Harris, L.W.; Hayhurst, J.; Malangone, C.; McMahon, A.; Morales, J.; Mountjoy, E.; Sollis, E.; et al. The NHGRI-EBI GWAS Catalog of published genome-wide association studies, targeted arrays and summary statistics 2019. Nucleic Acids Res. 2018, 47, D1005–D1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Adewuyi, E.O.; O’Brien, E.K.; Nyholt, D.R.; Porter, T.; Laws, S.M. A large-scale genome-wide cross-trait analysis reveals shared genetic architecture between Alzheimer’s disease and gastrointestinal tract disorders. Commun. Biol. 2022, 5, 691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Adewuyi, E.O.; Porter, T.; O’Brien, E.K.; Olaniru, O.; Verdile, G.; Laws, S.M. Genome-wide cross-disease analyses highlight causality and shared biological pathways of type 2 diabetes with gastrointestinal disorders. Commun. Biol. 2024, 7, 643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Davies, N.M.; Holmes, M.V.; Davey Smith, G. Reading Mendelian randomisation studies: A guide, glossary, and checklist for clinicians. BMJ 2018, 362, k601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Sekula, P.; Fabiola Del Greco, M.; Pattaro, C.; Köttgen, A. Mendelian Randomization as an Approach to Assess Causality Using Observational Data. J. Am. Soc. Nephrol. 2016, 27, 3253–3265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Richmond, R.C.; Davey Smith, G. Mendelian Randomization: Concepts and Scope. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 2022, 12, a040501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Skrivankova, V.W.; Richmond, R.C.; Woolf, B.A.; Yarmolinsky, J.; Davies, N.M.; Swanson, S.A.; VanderWeele, T.J.; Higgins, J.P.; Timpson, N.J.; Dimou, N. Strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology using Mendelian randomization: The STROBE-MR statement. Jama 2021, 326, 1614–1621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Bakshi, A.; Zhu, Z.; Vinkhuyzen, A.A.E.; Hill, W.D.; McRae, A.F.; Visscher, P.M.; Yang, J. Fast set-based association analysis using summary data from GWAS identifies novel gene loci for human complex traits. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 32894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Li, A.; Liu, S.; Bakshi, A.; Jiang, L.; Chen, W.; Zheng, Z.; Sullivan, P.F.; Visscher, P.M.; Wray, N.R.; Yang, J.; et al. mBAT-combo: A more powerful test to detect gene-trait associations from GWAS data. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2023, 110, 30–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Pickrell, J.K.; Berisa, T.; Liu, J.Z.; Ségurel, L.; Tung, J.Y.; Hinds, D.A. Detection and interpretation of shared genetic influences on 42 human traits. Nat. Genet. 2016, 48, 709–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kavousi, M.; Bos, M.M.; Barnes, H.J.; Lino Cardenas, C.L.; Wong, D.; Lu, H.; Hodonsky, C.J.; Landsmeer, L.P.L.; Turner, A.W.; Kho, M.; et al. Multi-ancestry genome-wide study identifies effector genes and druggable pathways for coronary artery calcification. Nat. Genet. 2023, 55, 1651–1664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Kauppila, L.I.; Polak, J.F.; Cupples, L.A.; Hannan, M.T.; Kiel, D.P.; Wilson, P.W. New indices to classify location, severity and progression of calcific lesions in the abdominal aorta: A 25-year follow-up study. Atherosclerosis 1997, 132, 245–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Jansen, I.E.; Savage, J.E.; Watanabe, K.; Bryois, J.; Williams, D.M.; Steinberg, S.; Sealock, J.; Karlsson, I.K.; Hägg, S.; Athanasiu, L. Genome-wide meta-analysis identifies new loci and functional pathways influencing Alzheimer’s disease risk. Nat. Genet. 2019, 51, 404–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Lambert, J.C.; Ibrahim-Verbaas, C.A.; Harold, D.; Naj, A.C.; Sims, R.; Bellenguez, C.; DeStafano, A.L.; Bis, J.C.; Beecham, G.W.; Grenier-Boley, B.; et al. Meta-analysis of 74,046 individuals identifies 11 new susceptibility loci for Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Genet. 2013, 45, 1452–1458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Hatoum, A.S.; Morrison, C.L.; Mitchell, E.C.; Lam, M.; Benca-Bachman, C.E.; Reineberg, A.E.; Palmer, R.H.C.; Evans, L.M.; Keller, M.C.; Friedman, N.P. Genome-wide Association Study Shows That Executive Functioning Is Influenced by GABAergic Processes and Is a Neurocognitive Genetic Correlate of Psychiatric Disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 2023, 93, 59–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lee, J.J.; Wedow, R.; Okbay, A.; Kong, E.; Maghzian, O.; Zacher, M.; Nguyen-Viet, T.A.; Bowers, P.; Sidorenko, J.; Karlsson Linnér, R.; et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat. Genet. 2018, 50, 1112–1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Savage, J.E.; Jansen, P.R.; Stringer, S.; Watanabe, K.; Bryois, J.; de Leeuw, C.A.; Nagel, M.; Awasthi, S.; Barr, P.B.; Coleman, J.R.I.; et al. Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nat. Genet. 2018, 50, 912–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Watanabe, K.; Stringer, S.; Frei, O.; Umićević Mirkov, M.; de Leeuw, C.; Polderman, T.J.C.; van der Sluis, S.; Andreassen, O.A.; Neale, B.M.; Posthuma, D. A global overview of pleiotropy and genetic architecture in complex traits. Nat. Genet. 2019, 51, 1339–1348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Bulik-Sullivan, B.K.; Loh, P.-R.; Finucane, H.K.; Ripke, S.; Yang, J.; Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium; Patterson, N.; Daly, M.J.; Price, A.L.; Neale, B.M. LD Score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies. Nat. Genet. 2015, 47, 291–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Adewuyi, E.O.; Mehta, D.; Sapkota, Y.; Auta, A.; Yoshihara, K.; Nyegaard, M.; Griffiths, L.R.; Montgomery, G.W. Genetic analysis of endometriosis and depression identifies shared loci and implicates causal links with gastric mucosa abnormality. Hum. Genet. 2021, 140, 529–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Adewuyi, E.O.; Sapkota, Y.; International Endogene Consortium (IEC); 23andMe Research Team; International Headache Genetics Consortium (IHGC); Auta, A.; Yoshihara, K.; Nyegaard, M.; Griffiths, L.R.; Montgomery, G.W. Shared molecular genetic mechanisms underlie endometriosis and migraine comorbidity. Genes 2020, 11, 268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Tasnim, S.; Wilson, S.G.; Walsh, J.P.; Nyholt, D.R. Cross-Trait Genetic Analyses Indicate Pleiotropy and Complex Causal Relationships between Headache and Thyroid Function Traits. Genes 2022, 14, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Carter, A.R.; Anderson, E.L. Correct illustration of assumptions in Mendelian randomization. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2024, 53, dyae050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Davey Smith, G.; Hemani, G. Mendelian randomization: Genetic anchors for causal inference in epidemiological studies. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2014, 23, R89–R98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Bowden, J.; Davey Smith, G.; Burgess, S. Mendelian randomization with invalid instruments: Effect estimation and bias detection through Egger regression. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2015, 44, 512–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Verbanck, M.; Chen, C.-Y.; Neale, B.; Do, R. Detection of widespread horizontal pleiotropy in causal relationships inferred from Mendelian randomization between complex traits and diseases. Nat. Genet. 2018, 50, 693–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Adewuyi, E.O.; Mehta, D.; Nyholt, D. Genetic overlap analysis of endometriosis and asthma identifies shared loci implicating sex hormones and thyroid signalling pathways. Hum. Reprod. 2022, 37, 366–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Akosile, W.; Adewuyi, E. Genetic correlation and causality assessment between post-traumatic stress disorder and coronary artery disease-related traits. Gene 2022, 842, 146802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Islam, M.R.; Nyholt, D.R.; International Headache Genetics Consortium (IHGC). Cross-trait analyses identify shared genetics between migraine, headache, and glycemic traits, and a causal relationship with fasting proinsulin. Hum. Genet. 2023, 142, 1149–1172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Tasnim, S.; Wilson, S.G.; Walsh, J.P.; Nyholt, D.R. Shared genetics and causal relationships between migraine and thyroid function traits. Cephalalgia 2023, 43, 03331024221139253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Adewuyi, E.O.; O’Brien, E.K.; Porter, T.; Laws, S.M. Relationship of Cognition and Alzheimer’s Disease with Gastrointestinal Tract Disorders: A Large-Scale Genetic Overlap and Mendelian Randomisation Analysis. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2022, 23, 16199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Chu, Z.; Cheng, L.; Tong, Q. Carotid artery calcification score and its association with cognitive impairment. Clin. Interv. Aging 2019, 14, 167–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Haycock, P.C.; Burgess, S.; Wade, K.H.; Bowden, J.; Relton, C.; Davey Smith, G. Best (but oft-forgotten) practices: The design, analysis, and interpretation of Mendelian randomization studies1. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2016, 103, 965–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Tin, A.; Fohner, A.E.; Yang, Q.; Brody, J.A.; Davies, G.; Yao, J.; Liu, D.; Caro, I.; Lindbohm, J.V.; Duggan, M.R.; et al. Identification of circulating proteins associated with general cognitive function among middle-aged and older adults. Commun. Biol. 2023, 6, 1117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Raulin, A.-C.; Doss, S.V.; Trottier, Z.A.; Ikezu, T.C.; Bu, G.; Liu, C.-C. ApoE in Alzheimer’s disease: Pathophysiology and therapeutic strategies. Mol. Neurodegener. 2022, 17, 72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Liu, C.-C.; Kanekiyo, T.; Xu, H.; Bu, G. Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer disease: Risk, mechanisms and therapy. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2013, 9, 106–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Wan, Z.; Ma, T. The impact of apolipoprotein E, type∊ 4 allele on Alzheimer’s disease pathological biomarkers: A comprehensive post-mortem pilot-analysis. PLoS ONE 2025, 20, e0303486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Li, Z.; Shue, F.; Zhao, N.; Shinohara, M.; Bu, G. APOE2: Protective mechanism and therapeutic implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Mol. Neurodegener. 2020, 15, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Mahley, R.W. Apolipoprotein E: From cardiovascular disease to neurodegenerative disorders. J. Mol. Med. 2016, 94, 739–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Reiman, E.M.; Arboleda-Velasquez, J.F.; Quiroz, Y.T.; Huentelman, M.J.; Beach, T.G.; Caselli, R.J.; Chen, Y.; Su, Y.; Myers, A.J.; Hardy, J.; et al. Exceptionally low likelihood of Alzheimer’s dementia in APOE2 homozygotes from a 5,000-person neuropathological study. Nat. Commun. 2020, 11, 667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Sutton, N.R.; Malhotra, R.; St. Hilaire, C.; Aikawa, E.; Blumenthal, R.S.; Gackenbach, G.; Goyal, P.; Johnson, A.; Nigwekar, S.U.; Shanahan, C.M.; et al. Molecular Mechanisms of Vascular Health: Insights From Vascular Aging and Calcification. Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc. Biol. 2023, 43, 15–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Zlokovic, B.V. Cerebrovascular Effects of Apolipoprotein E: Implications for Alzheimer Disease. JAMA Neurol. 2013, 70, 440–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Schematic representation of MR assumptions and study design. The figure provides an overview of the MR analysis approach, emphasising its application and core assumptions in examining the potential causal relationship between exposure and outcome variables [28,29,30,31]. It highlights the three fundamental assumptions of MR: (1) the genetic variants (SNPs) used as instrumental variables must be robustly associated with the exposure, (2) these SNPs should not be associated with confounding factors (we note the direction of the arrow as recently illustrated [47]), and (3) they must influence the outcome exclusively through the exposure [28,29,30,31]. The figure also details the clumping parameters employed to ensure the independence and relevance of the genetic instruments. The analysis involves two rounds: first using CAC and AAC as exposures against AD and cognitive traits as outcomes, and the reverse analysis where AD and cognitive traits serve as exposures, with CAC and AAC as the outcome variables. AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, AD: Alzheimer’s disease, CAC: coronary artery calcification, EA: effect allele, IVW: inverse variance weighted, MR: Mendelian randomisation, MR-PRESSO: Mendelian randomization pleiotropy residual sum and outlier, NEA: non-effect allele, SNP: single nucleotide polymorphism.
Figure 1. Schematic representation of MR assumptions and study design. The figure provides an overview of the MR analysis approach, emphasising its application and core assumptions in examining the potential causal relationship between exposure and outcome variables [28,29,30,31]. It highlights the three fundamental assumptions of MR: (1) the genetic variants (SNPs) used as instrumental variables must be robustly associated with the exposure, (2) these SNPs should not be associated with confounding factors (we note the direction of the arrow as recently illustrated [47]), and (3) they must influence the outcome exclusively through the exposure [28,29,30,31]. The figure also details the clumping parameters employed to ensure the independence and relevance of the genetic instruments. The analysis involves two rounds: first using CAC and AAC as exposures against AD and cognitive traits as outcomes, and the reverse analysis where AD and cognitive traits serve as exposures, with CAC and AAC as the outcome variables. AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, AD: Alzheimer’s disease, CAC: coronary artery calcification, EA: effect allele, IVW: inverse variance weighted, MR: Mendelian randomisation, MR-PRESSO: Mendelian randomization pleiotropy residual sum and outlier, NEA: non-effect allele, SNP: single nucleotide polymorphism.
Biomedicines 13 00618 g001
Figure 2. Causal effect of CAC on AD and cognitive traits. CAC: coronary artery calcification, CI: confidence interval, IVW: inverse variance weighted, nIV: number of instrumental variables, OR: odds ratio, p: p-value, cExecutive function: common executive function.
Figure 2. Causal effect of CAC on AD and cognitive traits. CAC: coronary artery calcification, CI: confidence interval, IVW: inverse variance weighted, nIV: number of instrumental variables, OR: odds ratio, p: p-value, cExecutive function: common executive function.
Biomedicines 13 00618 g002
Figure 3. Causal effect of AAC onAD and cognitive traits. AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, CI: confidence interval, IVW: inverse variance weighted, nIV: number of instrumental variables, OR: odds ratio, p: p-value, cExecutive function: common executive function.
Figure 3. Causal effect of AAC onAD and cognitive traits. AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, CI: confidence interval, IVW: inverse variance weighted, nIV: number of instrumental variables, OR: odds ratio, p: p-value, cExecutive function: common executive function.
Biomedicines 13 00618 g003
Figure 4. Causal effect of AD on cognitive traits against CAC. CAC: coronary artery calcification, CI: confidence interval, IVW: inverse variance weighted, nIV: number of instrumental variables, OR: odds ratio, p: p-value, cExecutive function: common executive function.
Figure 4. Causal effect of AD on cognitive traits against CAC. CAC: coronary artery calcification, CI: confidence interval, IVW: inverse variance weighted, nIV: number of instrumental variables, OR: odds ratio, p: p-value, cExecutive function: common executive function.
Biomedicines 13 00618 g004
Figure 5. Causal effect of AD and cognitive traits on AAC. AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, CI: confidence interval, IVW: inverse variance weighted, nIV: number of instrumental variables, OR: odds ratio, p: p-value, cExecutive function: common executive function.
Figure 5. Causal effect of AD and cognitive traits on AAC. AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, CI: confidence interval, IVW: inverse variance weighted, nIV: number of instrumental variables, OR: odds ratio, p: p-value, cExecutive function: common executive function.
Biomedicines 13 00618 g005
Table 1. Genome-wide genetic correlation between AAC, CAC, AD, and cognitive traits.
Table 1. Genome-wide genetic correlation between AAC, CAC, AD, and cognitive traits.
Trait 1Trait 2rgsep
CACAD GWAS [37]0.10.053.14 × 10−2
AD GWAS [37] excluding APOE region0.10.055.33 × 10−2
AD GWAS [38]0.010.048.06 × 10−1
AD GWAS [38] excluding APOE region00.059.17 × 10−1
Cognitive performance−0.110.021.59 × 10−6
Fluid intelligence scores−0.110.031.87 × 10−5
Intelligence−0.110.025.14 × 10−6
Common executive function−0.040.027.55 × 10−2
Educational attainment−0.10.021.08 × 10−6
Abdominal aortic calcification0.95 *0.344.70 × 10−3
AACAD GWAS [37]0.080.072.32 × 10−1
AD GWAS [37] excluding APOE region0.070.073.49 × 10−1
AD GWAS [38]0.010.079.28 × 10−1
AD GWAS [38] excluding APOE region−0.010.079.19 × 10−1
Cognitive performance−0.10.035.49 × 10−3
Fluid intelligence scores−0.070.035.24 × 10−2
Intelligence −0.060.041.12 × 10−1
Common executive function−0.040.042.53 × 10−1
Educational attainment−0.130.036.39 × 10−5
AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, AD: Alzheimer’s disease, CAC: coronary artery calcification, GWAS: genome-wide association studies, APOE: Apolipoprotein E, rg: genetic correlation estimates, se: standard error, p: p-value. * estimated without constraining the genetic covariance intercept.
Table 2. MR sensitivity analyses of AD and cognitive traits with vascular calcification.
Table 2. MR sensitivity analyses of AD and cognitive traits with vascular calcification.
ExposureOutcomeHeterogeneity TestsHorizontal Pleiotropy Tests
MethodCochran’s Q p ValueMethodInterceptp Value
Coronary artery calcification vs. AD and cognitive traits
CACAlzheimer’s diseaseIVW0.72Egger intercept −0.00130.72
cExecutive function0.94−0.000420.59
Cognitive performance0.50.00130.43
Educational attainment 0.810.000410.61
Fluid intelligence scores0.65−0.00260.63
Intelligence0.390.000320.86
Abdominal aortic calcification vs. AD and cognitive traits
AACAlzheimer’s diseaseIVW0.88Egger intercept −0.00140.55
cExecutive function0.71−0.00190.11
Cognitive performance0.19−0.00690.024
Educational attainment 0.560.00130.47
Fluid intelligence scores0.69−0.00570.5
Intelligence0.72−0.00270.35
AD and cognitive traits vs. coronary artery calcification
Alzheimer’s diseaseCACIVW0.26Egger intercept −0.0210.11
cExecutive function0.960.0180.21
Cognitive performance0.950.00970.89
Educational attainment 10.00220.7
Fluid intelligence scores0.96−0.0190.45
Intelligence0.86−0.00140.88
AD and cognitive traits vs. abdominal aortic calcification
Alzheimer’s diseaseAACIVW0.99Egger intercept −0.000190.95
cExecutive function0.990.00470.55
Cognitive performance0.950.00360.26
Educational attainment 1−0.000960.63
Fluid intelligence scores0.780.00650.88
Intelligence0.98−0.000170.95
AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, CAC: coronary artery calcification, IVW, inverse variance weighted, MR: Mendelian randomisation, p: p-value, cExecutive function: common executive function.
Table 3. Shared genomic loci of CAC and AAC with AD.
Table 3. Shared genomic loci of CAC and AAC with AD.
CAC/
AAC
ADChr: BPPPA4Shared Genes
Gene
CAC/AACAD
Gene Pgene *Top SNPTop SNP pGene Pgene *Top SNPTop SNP p
CACAD19:
44,744,370–
46,102,289
1.00BCAM7.08 × 10−9rs1181478621.40 × 10−100rs412895121.5 × 10−278
TOMM403.15 × 10−8 rs412901201.57 × 10−110rs129721560
NECTIN24.29 × 10−8rs412901201.57 × 10−110rs129721560
APOE2.69 × 10−7rs412901201.57 × 10−110rs129721560
APOC18.13 × 10−7 rs412901201.57 × 10−110rs129721560
CBLC1.01 × 10−5rs1181478621.40 × 10−103.67 × 10−264rs412895121.5 × 10−278
APOC41.99 × 10−5rs74124.61 × 10−100rs20756500
APOC22.16 × 10−5rs74124.61 × 10−100rs101190
APOC4-APOC22.97 × 10−5rs74124.61 × 10−100rs20756500
EXOC3L24.56 × 10−5rs124611447.03 × 10−51.95 × 10−66rs104158502.17 × 10−33
TRAPPC6A5.08 × 10−5rs124611447.03 × 10−54.30 × 10−74rs284690951.07 × 10−38
BLOC1S38.06 × 10−5rs124611447.03 × 10−51.17 × 10−65rs284690951.07 × 10−38
NKPD13.92 × 10−4rs104212471.04 × 10−41.06 × 10−79rs284690951.07 × 10−38
CLPTM16.14 × 10−4rs74124.61 × 10−100rs7694490
PPP1R371.59 × 10−3rs104212471.04 × 10−45.47 × 10−84rs284690951.07 × 10−38
BCL33.23 × 10−3rs1489334451.26 × 10−77.93 × 10−127rs29651699.24 × 10−58
MARK45.66 × 10−3rs124611447.03 × 10−53.44 × 10−98rs284690951.07 × 10−38
CEACAM168.13 × 10−3rs621172041.58 × 10−67.75 × 10−121rs29651699.24 × 10−58
AACAD19: 44,744,108–
46,102,684
1.00TOMM402.36 × 10−11rs10658533.07 × 10−130rs129721560
NECTIN22.44 × 10−10rs10658533.07 × 10−130rs129721560
APOE2.55 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−130rs129721560
APOC13.97 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−130rs129721560
APOC26.12 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−130rs101190
APOC41.43 × 10−8rs10658533.07 × 10−130rs20756500
APOC4-APOC21.55 × 10−8rs10658533.07 × 10−130rs20756500
CLPTM12.55 × 10−8rs10658533.07 × 10−130rs7694490
BCAM1.06 × 10−5rs48037603.00 × 10−70rs412895121.46 × 10−278
CBLC4.26 × 10−4rs48037603.00 × 10−73.67 × 10−264rs412895121.46 × 10−278
AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, CAC: coronary artery calcification, AD: Alzheimer’s disease, SNP: single nucleotide polymorphism, p: p-value. PPA4: posterior probability of association for model 4—the probability that a genetic locus is associated with both traits, signifying pleiotropy, but independent variants are associated with each trait. For instance, a PPA4 value > 0.90 indicates a high probability that the locus is pleiotropic for both traits. * Gene-based analysis was computed using the mBAT-combo approach, with results consistent with at least one additional gene-based method, either mBAT or fastBAT.
Table 4. Shared genomic loci of CAC and AAC with cognitive traits.
Table 4. Shared genomic loci of CAC and AAC with cognitive traits.
CAC/AACCTChr: BPPPA4Shared GenesCAC/AACCT
Gene p *Top SNPTop SNP pGene-p *Top SNPTop SNP p
CACcEF19: 44,744,370–46,102,547 1.00** PHLDB34.38 × 10−2rs621157542.28 × 10−33.88 × 10−2rs116683851.10 × 10−2
CACEA19:
44,744,370–46,102,547
0.99TOMM403.15 × 10−8rs412901201.57 × 10−117.56 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
NECTIN24.29 × 10−8rs412901201.57 × 10−112.05 × 10−2rs4055091.07 × 10−5
APOE2.69 × 10−7rs412901201.57 × 10−117.88 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
APOC18.13 × 10−7rs412901201.57 × 10−116.22 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
APOC41.99 × 10−5rs74124.61 × 10−105.83 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
APOC22.16 × 10−5rs74124.61 × 10−101.01 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
APOC4-APOC22.97 × 10−5rs74124.61 × 10−105.72 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
EXOC3L24.56 × 10−5rs124611447.03 × 10−58.51 × 10−5rs3865698.22 × 10−6
TRAPPC6A5.08 × 10−5rs124611447.03 × 10−53.77 × 10−2rs129742003.56 × 10−3
BLOC1S38.06 × 10−5rs124611447.03 × 10−51.50 × 10−3rs1511652253.27 × 10−5
CLPTM16.14 × 10−4rs74124.61 × 10−104.07 × 10−2rs4055091.07 × 10−5
PPP1R371.59 × 10−3rs104212471.04 × 10−41.94 × 10−2rs1392901295.95 × 10−4
MARK45.66 × 10−3rs124611447.03 × 10−57.59 × 10−7rs104027471.71 × 10−8
AACcEF19: 44,744,147–46,101,600 1.00TOMM402.36 × 10−11rs10658533.07 × 10−131.57 × 10−15rs4293589.52 × 10−20
NECTIN22.44 × 10−10rs10658533.07 × 10−131.08 × 10−14rs4293589.52 × 10−20
APOE2.55 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−131.68 × 10−16rs4293589.52 × 10−20
APOC13.97 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−131.18 × 10−16rs4293589.52 × 10−20
APOC26.12 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−133.02 × 10−16rs4293589.52 × 10−20
APOC41.43 × 10−8rs10658533.07 × 10−131.02 × 10−15rs4293589.52 × 10−20
APOC4-APOC21.55 × 10−8rs10658533.07 × 10−137.48 × 10−16rs4293589.52 × 10−20
CLPTM12.55 × 10−8rs10658533.07 × 10−132.71 × 10−15rs4293589.52 × 10−20
BCAM1.06 × 10−5rs48037603.00 × 10−72.63 × 10−4rs48037644.24 × 10−4
CBLC4.26 × 10−4rs48037603.00 × 10−71.78 × 10−6rs121622226.16 × 10−4
AACEA19: 44,744,147–46,101,600 0.96TOMM402.36 × 10−11rs10658533.07 × 10−137.56 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
NECTIN22.44 × 10−10rs10658533.07 × 10−132.05 × 10−2rs4055091.07 × 10−5
APOE2.55 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−137.88 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
APOC13.97 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−136.22 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
APOC26.12 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−131.01 × 10−2rs4055091.07 × 10−5
APOC41.43 × 10−8rs10658533.07 × 10−135.83 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
APOC4-APOC21.55 × 10−8rs10658533.07 × 10−135.72 × 10−3rs4055091.07 × 10−5
CLPTM12.55 × 10−8rs10658533.07 × 10−134.07 × 10−2rs4055091.07 × 10−5
AACFIS19: 44,744,147–46,101,600 0.96TOMM402.36 × 10−11rs10658533.07 × 10−131.54 × 10−3rs116688611.23 × 10−3
NECTIN22.44 × 10−10rs10658533.07 × 10−132.19 × 10−3rs81133117.09 × 10−4
APOE2.55 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−133.20 × 10−3rs116688611.23 × 10−3
APOC13.97 × 10−9rs10658533.07 × 10−136.93 × 10−3rs116688611.23 × 10−3
BCAM1.06 × 10−5rs48037603.00 × 10−73.39 × 10−3rs81133117.09 × 10−4
CBLC4.26 × 10−4rs48037603.00 × 10−72.41 × 10−2rs81133117.09 × 10−4
AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, CAC: coronary artery calcification, cEF: common executive function, CT: cognitive traits, EA: educational attainment, FIS: fluid intelligence scores, SNP: single nucleotide polymorphism, p: p-value. PPA4: posterior probability of association for model 4—the probability that a genetic locus is associated with both traits, signifying pleiotropy, but independent variants are associated with each trait. For instance, a PPA4 value > 0.90 indicates a high probability that the locus is pleiotropic for both traits. * Gene-based analysis was computed using the mBAT-combo approach, with results consistent with at least one additional gene-based method, either mBAT or fastBAT. ** gene identified by mBAT gene-based model only.
Table 5. Key pleiotropic loci with functional annotations.
Table 5. Key pleiotropic loci with functional annotations.
Locus (Chr:BP)Key GenesAssociated TraitsFunctional AnnotationBiological Implications
19: 44,744,370–46,102,289APOE, TOMM40, NECTIN2, APOC1, APOC2, APOC4CAC, AAC, AD, Cognitive TraitsAPOE is a major lipid transport protein linked to AD risk; TOMM40 is involved in mitochondrial protein transport; NECTIN2 plays a role in cell adhesion and immune signalling Associated with neurodegeneration, vascular health, and AD
19: 44,744,370–46,102,547PHLDB3CAC, Cognitive Traits (cEF)Plays a role in cell signalling, potential involvement in neuronal functionMay influence cognitive function and neurodevelopment
19: 44,744,108–46,102,684BCAM, CBLCAAC, ADBCAM encodes a laminin-binding protein, implicated in cell adhesion; CBLC is involved in ubiquitin signalling and protein degradationSuggests vascular contributions to AD risk through endothelial interactions
19: 44,744,147–46,101,600TOMM40, APOE, APOC1, NECTIN2AAC, Cognitive Traits (cEF, EA, FIS)Overlaps with well-established AD risk loci, involved in lipid metabolism, mitochondrial function, and immune responseSupports shared genetic architecture between vascular calcification and cognition
AAC: abdominal aortic calcification, CAC: coronary artery calcification, cEF: common executive function, EA: educational attainment, FIS: fluid intelligence scores. Chr: chromosome, BP: base pair position.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Adewuyi, E.O.; Laws, S.M. Genomic Characterisation of the Relationship and Causal Links Between Vascular Calcification, Alzheimer’s Disease, and Cognitive Traits. Biomedicines 2025, 13, 618. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030618

AMA Style

Adewuyi EO, Laws SM. Genomic Characterisation of the Relationship and Causal Links Between Vascular Calcification, Alzheimer’s Disease, and Cognitive Traits. Biomedicines. 2025; 13(3):618. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030618

Chicago/Turabian Style

Adewuyi, Emmanuel O., and Simon M. Laws. 2025. "Genomic Characterisation of the Relationship and Causal Links Between Vascular Calcification, Alzheimer’s Disease, and Cognitive Traits" Biomedicines 13, no. 3: 618. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030618

APA Style

Adewuyi, E. O., & Laws, S. M. (2025). Genomic Characterisation of the Relationship and Causal Links Between Vascular Calcification, Alzheimer’s Disease, and Cognitive Traits. Biomedicines, 13(3), 618. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030618

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop