Next Article in Journal
Dabrafenib-Trametinib and Radiotherapy for Oligoprogressive BRAF Mutant Advanced Melanoma
Next Article in Special Issue
The Role of the Left Inferior Parietal Cortex in Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome—An rTMS Study
Previous Article in Journal
Decreased Maternal Morbidity and Improved Perinatal Results of Magnesium-Free Tocolysis and Classical Hysterotomy in Fetal Open Surgery for Myelomeningocele Repair: A Single-Center Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Co-Occurrence of Tic Disorders and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder—Does It Reflect a Common Neurobiological Background?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Theta Activity Dynamics during Embedded Response Plan Processing in Tourette Syndrome

1
Cognitive Neurophysiology, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, TU Dresden, 01309 Dresden, Germany
2
Institute of Systems Motor Science, University of Lübeck, 23562 Lübeck, Germany
3
Doctoral School of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, 1053 Budapest, Hungary
4
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, 1053 Budapest, Hungary
5
Cognitive Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Trier, 54286 Trier, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Biomedicines 2023, 11(2), 393; https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines11020393
Submission received: 17 December 2022 / Revised: 25 January 2023 / Accepted: 27 January 2023 / Published: 28 January 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Neurobiology of Tourette Syndrome along the Lifespan)

Abstract

:
Gilles de la Tourette syndrome (GTS) is a neuropsychiatric disorder. Because motor signs are the defining feature of GTS, addressing the neurophysiology of motor processes is central to understanding GTS. The integration of voluntary motor processes is subject to so-called “binding problems”, i.e., how different aspects of an action are integrated. This was conceptualized in the theory of event coding, in which ‘action files’ accomplish the integration of motor features. We examined the functional neuroanatomical architecture of EEG theta band activity related to action file processing in GTS patients and healthy controls. Whereas, in keeping with previous data, behavioral performance during action file processing did not differ between GTS and controls, underlying patterns of neural activity were profoundly different. Superior parietal regions (BA7) were predominantly engaged in healthy controls, but superior frontal regions (BA9, BA10) in GTS indicated that the processing of different motor feature codes was central for action file processing in healthy controls, whereas episodic processing was more relevant in GTS. The data suggests a cascade of cognitive branching in fronto-polar areas followed by episodic processing in superior frontal regions in GTS. Patients with GTS accomplish the integration of motor plans via qualitatively different neurophysiological processes.

1. Introduction

Gilles de la Tourette syndrome (GTS) is a neurodevelopmental, multi-faceted neuropsychiatric disorder [1] with motor tics as its most characteristic sign [2]. Given the prominence of motor signs, GTS has often been viewed as a movement disorder. Recent years have witnessed a number of studies showing that this is unlikely to be the case [3]. Rather, it appears that the integration of perception and actions plays a critical role in the pathophysiology of GTS [4,5,6]. This notwithstanding, because tics as a motor sign are the defining feature of GTS, addressing motor processing in these patients, particularly regarding neurophysiological mechanisms, is a prerequisite for the understanding of this disorder.
As is the case for the integration of perception and action, the integration of motor subprocesses is also subject to so-called “binding problems” [7,8,9]. Such “binding problems” appear very relevant for the understanding of GTS, particularly with respect to perception–action integration [3]. In fact, within the Theory of Event Coding (TEC) [7], it has recently been demonstrated that perception–action binding is increased in GTS. Within the TEC, however, not only is perception–action integration detailed, but also how different features define a motor response are integrated to allow for the smooth unfolding of actions. In TEC, this is conceptualized as “action files” [10,11]. Action files can be examined in experimental procedures in which an action (A) is planned, but its execution has to be postponed until another action (B) is planned and performed. When there is strong binding of motor features constituting action A, it is demanding to execute another action B that shares some features with action A [10,11]. Very recent research in GTS, however, provided the first evidence that action file binding processes are not altered in adult GTS [12]. However, that study does not provide detailed insights into the functional neuroanatomical architecture associated with neural activity during action file coding and embedded action planning in patients with GTS and in healthy controls. This, however, is critical for various reasons:
In fact, it is likely that the functional neuroanatomical architecture associated with neurophysiological processes is different between patients with GTS and healthy controls because there are numerous reports that structural and functional brain organization is altered in these patients [13,14,15,16]. Particularly with respect to brain networks implicated in motor control, such alterations were reported [15,16]. Moreover, critically, the role of oscillatory brain activity and frequency band-specific processes as a major property of neural information processing [17,18,19,20] has not been evaluated in GTS. This is critical in the context of GTS because evidence is accumulating that this, especially theta band activity in fronto-striatal-thalamic loops, is relevant for the understanding of the pathophysiology of GTS [21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30]. Moreover, theta oscillations are important in action and motor control [31,32] because their biophysical properties are the basis for the integration of information across distant neural assemblies [17,31,33]. In the current study, we thus focused on theta frequency band activity (TBA) and examined the functional neuroanatomical architecture associated with TBA during action file coding in GTS. Based on the above considerations, we hypothesized that the functional neuroanatomical pattern of brain activation associated with action file binding effects in the theta band differs between patients with GTS and healthy controls. We assume that differences in the pattern of activities associated with action file binding effects in patients with GTS and controls are most likely evident in parietal and frontal regions because fMRI-based functional connectivity analyses revealed changes in GTS structures, particularly in fronto-parietal networks [34,35].
Importantly, considering the role of theta oscillations for the above-mentioned processes in GTS, we did not restrict our analyses to the period where action files are actually processed, but extended them to in-between trial/pre-trial periods in an action file coding experiment. The rationale is that previous data [36] suggest that TBA in particular in the pre-trial period is predictive for the strength/modulation of TBA within a trial in which motor response control processes are required. Such pre-trial TBA readings were interpreted to reflect some form of proactive control processes, i.e., processes needed to prepare the cognitive system for upcoming demands [37,38,39]. Theoretical approaches on prefrontal cortex functioning indicate that particularly fronto-polar regions play a role in “branching control” [40,41,42]. Such a branching of control enables the maintenance of a state/information that may be useful in the future to revert to a pending task following the completion of an ongoing one [41]. Such branching processes are assumed to precede processes of episodic control [41], comprising signals for guiding action selection. This episodic control is of central relevance for binding processes in the TEC-framework because these depend on the retrieval of information from episodic memory traces [43]. Based on previous findings of theta band activity in pre-trial intervals and above-mentioned theoretical approaches on prefrontal cortex functioning and information theory considerations [40,41,42], anterior orbito-frontal and anterior ventro-medial cortical areas are likely activated in the pre-trial interval. The fronto-polar region is closely connected to regions in the temporal cortex (temporal pole and superior temporal cortex) as well as more posterior regions in the ventro-lateral prefrontal cortex, including inferior frontal cortices [42,44,45]. Therefore, these regions may also be activated in the pre-trial phase. No clear hypotheses can be stated regarding the direction of pre-trial/within-trial correlations of TBA and whether there are differences between patients with GTS and healthy controls. However, given that structural neuroanatomical connections show widespread changes in patients with GTS [13,15] differences are likely. To calculate these correlations, we used the source-reconstructed activity as was done in previous work [36]. This is because the considerations outlined above suggest that the functional neuroanatomical structure is also of importance for a theoretically meaningful interpretation of possible correlations between pre-trial and within-trial theta band activity. Moreover, this approach confers the advantage that beamforming reduces residual variance in EEG data and, thus, increases the reliability of correlation analyses using neurophysiological data.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Patients and Controls

All patients and healthy controls provided written informed consent to participate in the study. The study was conducted in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki (1964). From child or adolescent patients, a written informed consent of their legal guardians was obtained. The local ethics committee approved the study.
We investigated a group of N = 30 patients with GTS (N = 19 male patients, N = 11 female patients, mean age 20.67 ± 6.63 standard deviation (SD), range 12–35 years); part of these patients were also investigated in a previous study [12]. Adult patients (N = 18) were recruited from the specialized GTS outpatient clinic in the Department for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy of the University Medical Center Schleswig-Holstein, Campus Lübeck, Germany. Child and adolescent patients (N = 12) were recruited from the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry at the University Hospital Dresden, Germany, and the Vadaskert Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Hospital and Outpatient Clinic in Budapest, Hungary. To assess the lifetime clinical information of each patient, we used a standardized clinical assessment, including a clinical neuropsychiatric interview, IQ testing and scoring of tic severity and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. To screen psychiatric co-morbidities, i.e., mood disorders and obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), we used the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.) [46] for participants over 17 years and the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview Kid (M.I.N.I. KID) [47] for participants from 9 to 17 years. The M.I.N.I. KID also evaluates the presence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The severity of ADHD of the adult patients was determined using the ADHD-Index and the DSM-ADHD-Scale of the German version of the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scale [48]. Lifetime tics were evaluated by the Diagnostic Confidence Index [49]. Tic severity was identified using the Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (YGTSS) [50]. Premonitory urges were measured using the Premonitory Urge for Tic Scale (PUTS) [51]. We recorded a standardized video from each participant, which was independently scored by two experienced examiners using the Modified Rush Videotape Rating Scale [52] with a total tic score ranging from 0 to 20. When scores of the two examiners differed, a consensus was reached after discussing all relevant segments of the video. In addition to a Rush consensus score, we determined the tic frequency (tics/min). Depending on the participant’s age, OCD symptoms were evaluated using the Yale Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (YBOCS) [53] or the Children´s Yale Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (CY-BOCS) [54]. For IQ testing, we used either the short German version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children (HAWIK-IV) for participants from 9 to 16 years [55] or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) for participants over 16 years [56]. Handedness was determined using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [57].
We also investigated N = 30 healthy control participants (21 male patients, 9 female patients, mean age 20.93 ± 6.74 SD, range 12–35 years). Comparable to the patients, healthy controls also underwent clinical assessment that included a clinical neuropsychiatric interview, IQ testing, the M.I.N.I. or M.I.N.I. KID and the YBOCS or the CY-BOCS, and for adult participants, the ADHD-Index and the DSM-ADHD-Scale of the German version of the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scale. Further, we also recorded a standardized video to determine total tic score and detect handedness. According to the interview assessment, six healthy controls had comorbidities. Three had depression in the past, one had panic attacks in the past and one had recurrent depression and panic attacks in the past. They had no clinically relevant psychiatric symptomatology during their study participation.
The clinical data of the Tourette patients are shown in Table 1. Eight GTS patients had depression in the past, and only one of them had a depressive episode at the time of testing. One had an anxiety disorder, and two had panic attacks in the past. Further, six GTS patients had a diagnosis of ADHD. One patient consumed cannabis on a regular basis. At the time of study participation, 7 of 30 patients were on medication, including aripiprazole (N = 5), tiapride (N = 1) and methylphenidate (N = 2). The mean IQ of the patient group was 102.9 (±13.4), and it was 110.9 (±9.22) for the control group (t(51) = −2.69, p < 0.01). Two patients and three healthy control participants were left-handed.

2.2. Task

The “R-R task” paradigm [11,58] was previously used to investigate action file binding processes [59]. The experiment was programmed in Presentation (Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., Berkeley, CA, USA) and is visualized in Figure 1.
Stimuli were shown in white on a black background on a 17-inch CRT screen at a viewing distance of 60 cm from the participants. Each trial consisted of two tasks nested within each other (stimulus–response pattern ABBA). A fixation cross was displayed for 50 ms at the beginning of each trial. Then, Stimulus A was presented for 2000 ms. The response for Stimulus A (S1) had to be planned and withheld until the response for Stimulus B (S2). After the presentation of Stimulus A, a fixation cross was displayed for 50 ms and followed by Stimulus B, which was presented for 200 ms. Stimulus A consisted of a left-pointing or right-pointing arrowhead and the symbol “~” either above or below the arrowhead. The arrowhead indicated the response hand (i.e., left or right hand), whereas the symbol “~” indicated the response direction (i.e., up or down). A series of three consecutive button presses was required for Stimulus A: first, the home key on the side described by the arrowhead (i.e., left or right), followed by a press on the key above or below in the direction of the asterisk, and lastly, the home key had to be pressed for a second time (see Figure 1). Stimulus B was either the symbol “&” or “#”, which indicated a left or right button press, respectively. Stimulus B required a single button press on the response side corresponding to the stimulus. The numerical pad on the keyboard was used for the participant’s responses. The buttons 1, 4 and 7 (below, home and above) were used for the left response direction, and the buttons 3, 6 and 9 were used for the right response direction. The participants were instructed to leave their index fingers on the corresponding sides of the numerical pad. If Stimulus A consisted of a right-pointing arrowhead with the symbol “~” below it and Stimulus B was the symbol “&”, the correct response sequence was 4 (left home key)—6 (right home key)—3 (below)—6 (right home key). Participants performed a practice round of 40 trials before a total of 256 trials were presented in four blocks. Of these 256 trials, half of the trials revealed a feature overlap in the response side (same hand used for the response for Stimulus A and B), and the other half of the trials revealed no feature overlap (different hands used for Stimulus A and B).

2.3. EEG Recording and Analysis

EEG data were recorded using 60 Ag/AgCl electrodes with an equidistant cap layout (EasyCap, Wörthsee, Germany) at a sampling rate of 500 Hz. A “QuickAmp” amplifier and the “Brain Vision Recorder” software (Brain Products, Gilching, Germany) were used for the recording. The ground and reference electrodes were placed at the coordinates of θ = 58, φ = 78 and θ = 90, φ = 90, respectively. Electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ. The EEG data were pre-processed using the Brain Vision Analyzer (Brain Products, Gilching, Germany). The data were down-sampled to 256 Hz and filtered between 0.5 Hz and 40 Hz at an order of 8. Additionally, a 50 Hz notch filter was applied. Consecutively, the data were re-referenced to the average activity of all electrodes. EEG data were manually inspected, and technical artefacts were removed. Eye blinks, lateral eye movements and periodical artefacts (i.e., cardiovascular activity) were removed using an independent component analysis (ICA, Infomax algorithm) after visual inspection of the component topographies and time-courses. The cleaned data sets were transferred to the Fieldtrip Toolbox [60] running in Matlab R2019a (Mathworks, Inc., Massachusetts, United States). The data were segmented based on the presentation of Stimulus B (−5 to 3 s) for both overlap levels (feature overlap vs. no-feature overlap). For further analysis, a pre-trial interval (−2850 to −2050 ms) prior to the presentation of Stimulus A and a within-trial (0 to 800 ms) after the presentation of Stimulus B were chosen. The duration of these intervals was set to 800 ms, as this period allowed for at least four theta cycles at a central frequency of 5.5 Hz (see Beamforming section). Automated artefact rejection was applied to all segments with amplitudes higher than 100 μV and lower than −100 μV. Furthermore, segments with activity below 0.5 μV over a time period of 100 ms were removed from the data.
For the within-trial interval, segments were divided into a feature overlap and no-feature overlap condition. For the pre-trial interval, both the feature overlap and no-feature overlap trials were selected, as the participant could not be aware of the trial type before the presentation of the stimuli. For the pre- and within-trial periods, time-frequency (TF) analyses on the segments were performed using Morlet wavelets (𝑤) as presented below:
w ( t , f ) = A exp ( t 2 δ t 2 ) exp ( 2 i π f t ) ,
with t = time, A = ( δ t π ) 1 / 2 , δ t = wavelet duration, and i = 1 . A ratio of between central frequency ( f 0 ) and the width of the Gaussian shape ( δ f ) = 5   was used. The average power over the theta frequency band was calculated for each electrode and time point. To examine the binding effect, the TBA in the within-trial period was compared between the feature overlap and no-feature overlap trials. T-tests between the feature overlap and no-feature overlap conditions were computed for every time point between 0 and 800 ms relative to Stimulus B onset separately for controls and GTS patients.

2.4. Beamforming Analysis

Source-level activity was reconstructed from the sensor-level EEG data using a multi-step beamforming approach as applied in previous studies [36]. In the first step, a Dynamic Imaging of Coherent Sources (DICS) [61] beamformer was applied. For this, a Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) was calculated at the central frequency of 5.5 Hz with a taper of 1.5 Hz. From the cross-frequency spectrum, common spatial filters were computed for the feature overlap condition and the no-feature overlap conditions for both the control and the patient group. The data were projected into the source space onto an equally spaced 1 cm grid created from an MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) space. The projection of the data was based on the forward model included in the FieldTrip toolbox. For the control and the patient group, theta power values were extracted for the pre-trial and the within-trial interval. The Neural Activity Index (NAI) was calculated for the pre-trial period by dividing the source values by the local noise estimates [62]. For the within-trial interval, a contrast between the feature overlap and the no-feature overlap condition was calculated and normalized on the total power of both conditions.
After DICS-beamforming, the clusters of theta activity in the reconstructed source data were determined by applying a Density-Based Spatial Clustering of Applications with Noise (DBSCAN) [63] in Matlab. Using the DBSCAN algorithm, further analyses could be restricted to voxels within functional neuroanatomical regions with high theta activity. For this, power values were thresholded at the top 1% of the power distribution within regions labeled in the Automatic Anatomical Labelling (AAL) [64] atlas. For the DBSCAN, the minimum cluster size was set to seven voxels, and the epsilon value was set to twice the edge length of each voxel in order to detect neighboring voxels. The clusters were visually inspected and selected for further analysis based on anatomical label and cluster size (voxels included in the cluster). Subsequent beamforming analysis was restricted on the selected voxels.
In the last step, a Linear Constraint Minimum Variance (LCMV) [62] beamformer was applied to retrieve the TBA time course in the selected voxels. The pre-processed data were multiplied with the spatial filter computed on the covariance data of the averaged data for both the control and the patient group. Time-frequency analyses were calculated using Morlet wavelets with the same parameters as before (see Section 2.3). Power values were averaged over all voxels included in each cluster, resulting in a single time-frequency course for each TBA cluster. As a measure of the binding effect, the difference between the feature overlap and no-feature overlap condition was calculated. Pearson correlations were calculated for each time point between clusters of the pre-trial and within-trial periods. The resulting correlation matrices visualize the time course of the two clusters on the axes, and thus, each value in the matrix represents the correlation between the respective time points in the two clusters. To reduce the likelihood of false-positive findings due to the large number of calculated correlations, the Benjamini-Hochberg [65] method was applied to control the false discovery rate (FDR). In addition to the FDR correction, only q-values (p-values corrected for FDR) smaller than 0.01 were regarded as significant.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

The statistical analyses on the behavioral data were conducted using R [66]. The mean accuracy (percentage of correct responses) and mean reaction times (RTs) for correct responses were calculated for Stimulus B (immediate response). For Stimulus A (planned response), mean RTs and mean accuracy were calculated over all three required responses cumulatively. Mixed effects ANOVAs were calculated for RT and accuracy on both stimuli with the within-factor task (feature overlap vs. no-feature overlap) and between factor groups (GTS vs. HC). Bayesian linear models (BF10) analogous to the mixed effects ANOVAs were calculated with the “BayesFactor” package [67]. The Bayes Factor (BF) provides the marginal likelihood of the model compared to the null model. Below a Bayes Factor of 1, both models are equally likely, whereas a Bayes Factor < 1 favors the null hypothesis, and a Bayes Factor > 1 favors the alternative hypothesis [68]. A Bayes Factor < 1/3 or > 3 provides moderate evidence for H0 and H1, respectively, whereas a Bayes Factor of 10 can be interpreted as strong evidence for H1 (analogously, a BF of 1/10 provides strong evidence for H0).

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral Data

An ANOVA including the within-factor task (feature overlap vs. no-feature overlap) and between-factor group (GTS vs. HC) on the reaction time to Stimulus B revealed the significant main effect of task condition (F1,58 = 9.38, p = 0.003, η2g < 0.01). A BF10 = 10.28 ± 1.3% can be interpreted as strong evidence for this model. RTs were higher in the feature overlap (M = 575 ms ± SE = 25) compared to the no-feature overlap condition (M = 558 ms ± SE = 26), indicating a binding effect for the response times. There was no significant main effect of group (F1,58 = 0.57, p = 0.45, η2g = 0.01, BF10 = 0.78 ± 0.5%) and no significant interaction effect of task and group (F1,58 = 0.34, p = 0.56, η2g < 0.001, BF10 = 0.29 ± 0.8%). An ANOVA on the accuracy in response B with the within-factor task and the between-factor group showed no significant main effects for group (F1,58 = 1.18, p = 0.28, η2g = 0.01, BF10 = 0.44 ± 0.8%) or task (F1,58 = 0.11, p = 0.74, η2g < 0.001, BF10 = 0.20 ± 1.1%). There was no significant interaction effect of group and task (F1,58 = 0.20, p = 0.66, η2g = 0.001, BF10 = 0.28 ± 1.5%). An ANOVA on the cumulated response times for Stimulus A with the within-factor task and between-factor group did not reveal significant main effects of group (F1,58 = 1.13, p = 0.29, η2g = 0.02, BF10 = 0.77 ± 3.1%) and task (F1,58 = 1.54, p = 0.22, η2g < 0.001, BF10 = 0.38 ± 0.8%). Additionally, there was no significant interaction effect of group and task (F1,58 < 0.01, p = 0.95, η2g < 0.001, BF10 = 0.25 ± 0.8%). In the ANOVA on the overall accuracy for Stimulus A, there were no significant main effects of group (F1,58 = 0.64, p = 0.43, η2g = 0.01, BF10 = 0.64 ± 2.7%) and task (F1,58 = 2.03, p = 0.16, η2g < 0.01, BF10 = 0.47 ± 1.2%). There was no significant interaction effect of group and task (F1,58 = 0.13, p = 0.71, η2g < 0.001, BF10 = 0.27 ± 1.6%).

3.2. Neurophysiology

For the within-trial interval, significant differences in TBA between the feature overlap and no-feature overlap conditions were found for multiple electrodes for both the HC and GTS group (Figure 2A). The time-frequency representations over the electrodes showing significant differences can be found in Figure 2B. For the source-level data and the within-trial interval, theta activity clusters in the right inferior postcentral cortex and the right superior parietal cortex were found in the HC group (Figure 2C). In the GTS group, a cluster in the left fronto-dorsal cortex was established as well as a smaller cluster in the right fronto-dorsal cortex.
For the pre-trial interval, the TBA cannot be compared to another condition or interval, as the pre-trial interval encompasses the period that is typically used as a baseline. Hence, it is not possible to establish whether the TBA activity in the pre-trial interval differs from random signals on the electrode level. As the clusters identified on the source-level show significant correlations even after FDR-correction was applied, we conclude that the activation in the identified regions is meaningful, as the correlations between random/non-meaningful signals multiplied with a filter created from the signals (see Section 2.3—LCMV beamforming) cannot be significant. In the GTS group, clusters of theta activity were revealed within the left medial frontal cortex and the left fronto-ventral cortex in the pre-trial interval (Figure 3).
The correlation map between the pre-trial ventral frontal theta cluster and the within-trial fronto-dorsal theta cluster revealed a significant negative correlation in the GTS group (Figure 4; please note that only significant correlation maps are shown). In the correlational analyses, strong correlations between the fronto-ventral theta activity at all time points during the pre-trial and the theta activity in the fronto-dorsal cluster in the time frame from 0 to approximately 200 ms after the presentation of Stimulus B were evident (rmax = −0.53; rmin = −0.67 in q < 0.01). This indicates that higher pre-trial TBA in the ventral frontal cortex is associated with lower TBA in the fronto-dorsal cortex during the presentation of Stimulus B.
For the control group, the DBSCAN analysis of the beamformed data (contrast feature overlap vs. no-feature overlap) revealed clusters of TBA in the orbitofrontal cortex, the insula and the temporal cortex in the pre-trial interval (Figure 3). However, no significant correlations between the pre-trial and within-trial clusters were found.

4. Discussion

In the current study, we examined the functional neuroanatomical architecture of theta band activity related to action file processing in patients with GTS and healthy controls. Other than taking more conventional approaches to EEG data analysis, we did not restrict the data analysis to the period, in which action files are processed, but extended the analysis to the pre-trial period.
In keeping with a previous study [12], behavioral data (reaction times and accuracy) did not show differences in action file binding effects between patients with GTS and controls. However, neurophysiological dynamics in GTS and healthy controls revealed qualitative differences in brain organization and/or differences in the strategies applied rather than differences in the efficacy of action file processing. The data show that in the within-trial phase, action file binding effects in TBA were associated with superior parietal regions (BA7) and the precuneus (BA7) in the control group. In the GTS group, superior frontal regions (medial and lateral) (BA9, BA10) were found. This group-specific pattern of brain regions associated with action file binding effects, together with a lack of group differences in the behavioral data, suggests that patients with GTS and healthy controls accomplish action file binding via qualitatively distinct neural processes using different strategies.
Thus, in healthy controls, the superior parietal cortex (BA7) was associated with action file binding effects in the theta band. BA7 was shown to be important for visuo-motor coordination [69,70,71,72,73,74], especially in bimanual tasks [75], which is also the case in the applied action file binding experiment. In addition, BA7 was found to comprise separate, interconnected functional systems representing motor features [69,71,73]. The used action file experiment incorporates an ABBA design for response execution, in which an action (A) is planned, but its execution has to be postponed until another action (B) is planned and performed. Feature overlap of these actions A and B varies [10,11]. Because the inter-independency of motor features plays a major role in action file binding effects, the obtained association of the action file binding effect in the theta band with BA7 likely comprising interconnected functional systems representing motor features is plausible [69,71,73]. The data may therefore be interpreted such that in healthy controls, the representation of motor features is critical during action file binding.
In contrast to the healthy control group, action file binding effects in the theta band were associated with lateral regions of the superior frontal gyrus (BA9, BA10) in GTS patients. To interpret this finding, overarching theoretical conceptions on the functional architecture of the prefrontal cortex are helpful, particularly information on theoretical approaches to prefrontal cortex functioning [41,42]. According to this, there are three nested processing levels: (i). branching, (ii). episodic and (iii). contextual processes, which are implemented from anterior (polar) regions to more posterior regions of the lateral prefrontal cortex [41]. The anterior lateral prefrontal cortex is especially involved in episodic control, which is defined as control of ongoing behavior that is composed of multiple successive temporal frames or episodes [41]. Binding in the TEC framework strongly relies upon episodic retrieval [43]. This is related to the structure of experimental approaches assessing binding effects, in which each trial consists of a series of events, where stimuli are presented and/or responses have to be executed. Therefore, single trials in typical implementations of experiments examining binding processes build their own ‘episodic history’. This is also the case in the applied action file paradigm, where each trial has an ABBA design for different to-be executed responses [10,11]. Apparently, episodic processing of the ABBA response history is central in patients with GTS during action file coding. In light of the findings obtained for the healthy control group discussed above, the entire data pattern may be interpreted such that healthy controls and patients with GTS use different strategies to accomplish action file tasks. For healthy controls, the processing of different motor feature codes is central for action file processing; for patients with GTS, it is episodic processing.
Another important focus of the current study was on the analysis of theta activity in the pre-trial period and its relation to theta activity associated with action file binding effects. The pre-trial period likely reflects proactive control processes [36] similar to cognitive branching processes [41]. Because cognitive branching precedes processes of episodic control [41] playing a role in binding processes [43], the inter-relation of pre-trial and within-trial activity is of particular interest. In addition, the above discussion based on theoretical conceptions of the functional relevance of prefrontal structures, which suggests that episodic processing is most relevant during action file coding in GTS, underlines the relevance of this analysis. Considering the functional neuroanatomical level, correlations between pre-trial and within-trial activity were thus calculated using source-reconstructed TBA [36].
In patients with GTS, left-sided fronto-polar areas of the superior and middle frontal gyrus (BA10, BA11) were activated in the pre-trial interval. Fronto-polar region were implicated in cognitive branching processes [41,42] considered to enable/maintain a state/information that may be useful in the future [41]. This cognitive branching precedes episodic control processes associated with anterior regions of the lateral prefrontal cortex found to be associated with theta band activity during action file coding (i.e., within a trial) [41]. Importantly, the correlation analysis shows that theta-related pre-trial activity in the fronto-polar region was significantly negatively correlated with theta activity reflecting action file binding in GTS. As shown in Figure 4, theta activity in the entire analyzed period in the pre-trial interval was correlated with theta band activity in the first 200 ms after time point zero (i.e., within-trial activity after the presentation of stimulus B). Thus, stronger theta band activity during cognitive branching is related to lower episodic processing during action file coding in these patients. However, given the direction of the correlation with TBA during action file coding, it is possible that this activity reflects some form of preparatory processes or pre-activation of memory representations of motor response features. If these response features are pre-activated, they are more readily usable for the cascaded execution of responses (cf. ABBA trial design). This interpretation is in keeping with the notion that pre-trial TBA activity can increase the efficiency of motor response control processes [36]. Apparently, this interpretation has conceptual similarities with cognitive branching, which was proposed to be a function of the fronto-polar cortex [41]. The correlation therefore suggests that cognitive branching processes are directly relevant, i.e., may inform episodic processing of ABBA response history in patients with GTS. This contrasts with the healthy controls, where no significant correlations between pre-trial and within-trial TBA were obtained. This dissociation can only tentatively be interpreted. However, it seems that healthy controls, unlike patients with GTS, do not need to employ proactive control or cognitive branching process to still yield similarly high behavioral performance than patients with GTS. This underlines the above interpretation that patients with GTS accomplish action file coding and the integration of motor features to coherent action via qualitatively different neurophysiological processes.
In summary, in keeping with previous data, whereas behavioral performance during action file processing did not differ between GTS and controls, the underlying patterns of neural activity were profoundly different. Regarding within-trial processing, superior parietal regions (BA7) were predominantly engaged in healthy controls, but superior frontal regions (BA9, BA10) in GTS indicate that processing different motor feature codes is central for action file processing in healthy controls, whereas episodic processing is more relevant in GTS. Relations of pre-trial/within trial activity suggested a cascade of cognitive branching in fronto-polar areas followed by episodic processing in superior frontal regions in GTS. The findings show that future studies in patients with GTS should not only consider quantitative differences, but also should put emphasis on qualitatively different patterns of neural activity associated with GTS. Through the combined analysis of qualitative and quantitative differences, a more holistic picture of pathophysiological changes will be revealed, ultimately leading to a better conception of GTS and its treatment.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.B., P.W., A.M., C.F. and A.B.; methodology, P.W. and A.T.; software, P.W.; validation, C.B. and A.M.; formal analysis, P.W.; investigation, T.P., E.T.-F., A.W. and T.B.; resources, C.B., A.M. and V.R.; data curation, P.W.; writing—original draft preparation, P.W., C.B. and A.M.; writing—review and editing, all authors; visualization, P.W.; supervision, C.B.; project administration, C.B.; funding acquisition, C.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was partly funded by a Grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG), FOR 2698.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the TU Dresden (EK 361092017, date of approval: 15 June 2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5, 5th ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-0-89042-555-8. [Google Scholar]
  2. Pringsheim, T.; Holler-Managan, Y.; Okun, M.S.; Jankovic, J.; Piacentini, J.; Cavanna, A.E.; Martino, D.; Müller-Vahl, K.; Woods, D.W.; Robinson, M.; et al. Comprehensive Systematic Review Summary: Treatment of Tics in People with Tourette Syndrome and Chronic Tic Disorders. Neurology 2019, 92, 907–915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Beste, C.; Münchau, A. Tics and Tourette Syndrome—Surplus of Actions Rather than Disorder? Mov. Disord. 2018, 33, 238–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Buse, J.; Beste, C.; Roessner, V. Neural Correlates of Prediction Violations in Boys with Tourette Syndrome: Evidence from Harmonic Expectancy. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 2018, 19, 130–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kleimaker, M.; Takacs, A.; Conte, G.; Onken, R.; Verrel, J.; Bäumer, T.; Münchau, A.; Beste, C. Increased Perception-Action Binding in Tourette Syndrome. Brain 2020, 143, 1934–1945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Petruo, V.; Bodmer, B.; Brandt, V.C.; Baumung, L.; Roessner, V.; Münchau, A.; Beste, C. Altered Perception-Action Binding Modulates Inhibitory Control in Gilles de La Tourette Syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2019, 60, 953–962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Hommel, B.; Müsseler, J.; Aschersleben, G.; Prinz, W. The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A Framework for Perception and Action Planning. Behav. Brain Sci. 2001, 24, 849–878; discussion 878–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Treisman, A. The Binding Problem. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1996, 6, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Treisman, A.; Kahneman, D. Changing Views of Attention and Automaticity. In Varieties of Attention; Academic Press, Inc.: San Diego, CA, USA, 1984; pp. 29–61. [Google Scholar]
  10. Hommel, B. Event Files: Feature Binding in and across Perception and Action. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2004, 8, 494–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Stoet, G.; Hommel, B. Action Planning and the Temporal Binding of Response Codes. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1999, 25, 1625–1640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Mielke, E.; Takacs, A.; Kleimaker, M.; Schappert, R.; Conte, G.; Onken, R.; Künemund, T.; Verrel, J.; Bäumer, T.; Beste, C.; et al. Tourette Syndrome as a Motor Disorder Revisited—Evidence from Action Coding. NeuroImage Clin. 2021, 30, 102611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Cheng, B.; Braass, H.; Ganos, C.; Treszl, A.; Biermann-Ruben, K.; Hummel, F.C.; Müller-Vahl, K.; Schnitzler, A.; Gerloff, C.; Münchau, A.; et al. Altered Intrahemispheric Structural Connectivity in Gilles de La Tourette Syndrome. NeuroImage Clin. 2014, 4, 174–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Liu, Y.; Miao, W.; Wang, J.; Gao, P.; Yin, G.; Zhang, L.; Lv, C.; Ji, Z.; Yu, T.; Sabel, B.A.; et al. Structural Abnormalities in Early Tourette Syndrome Children: A Combined Voxel-Based Morphometry and Tract-Based Spatial Statistics Study. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e76105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Wen, H.; Liu, Y.; Wang, J.; Rekik, I.; Zhang, J.; Zhang, Y.; Tian, H.; Peng, Y.; He, H. Combining Tract- and Atlas-Based Analysis Reveals Microstructural Abnormalities in Early Tourette Syndrome Children. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016, 37, 1903–1919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Worbe, Y.; Marrakchi-Kacem, L.; Lecomte, S.; Valabregue, R.; Poupon, F.; Guevara, P.; Tucholka, A.; Mangin, J.-F.; Vidailhet, M.; Lehericy, S.; et al. Altered Structural Connectivity of Cortico-Striato-Pallido-Thalamic Networks in Gilles de La Tourette Syndrome. Brain 2015, 138, 472–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Buzsáki, G. Rhythms of the Brain; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2006; ISBN 978-0-19-530106-9. [Google Scholar]
  18. Herrmann, C.S.; Strüber, D.; Helfrich, R.F.; Engel, A.K. EEG Oscillations: From Correlation to Causality. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2016, 103, 12–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Siegel, M.; Donner, T.H.; Engel, A.K. Spectral Fingerprints of Large-Scale Neuronal Interactions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012, 13, 121–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Ward, L.M. Synchronous Neural Oscillations and Cognitive Processes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 553–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Cagle, J.N.; Okun, M.S.; Opri, E.; Cernera, S.; Molina, R.; Foote, K.D.; Gunduz, A. Differentiating Tic Electrophysiology from Voluntary Movement in the Human Thalamocortical Circuit. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2020, 91, 533–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Neumann, W.-J.; Huebl, J.; Brücke, C.; Lofredi, R.; Horn, A.; Saryyeva, A.; Müller-Vahl, K.; Krauss, J.K.; Kühn, A.A. Pallidal and Thalamic Neural Oscillatory Patterns in Tourette’s Syndrome. Ann. Neurol. 2018, 84, 505–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Alam, M.; Schwabe, K.; Lütjens, G.; Capelle, H.H.; Manu, M.; von Wrangel, C.; Müller-Vahl, K.; Schrader, C.; Scheinichen, D.; Blahak, C.; et al. Comparative Characterization of Single Cell Activity in the Globus Pallidus Internus of Patients with Dystonia or Tourette Syndrome. J. Neural Transm. 2015, 122, 687–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Giorni, A.; Windels, F.; Stratton, P.G.; Cook, R.; Silberstein, P.; Coyne, T.; Silburn, P.A.; Sah, P. Single-Unit Activity of the Anterior Globus Pallidus Internus in Tourette Patients and Posterior Globus Pallidus Internus in Dystonic Patients. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017, 128, 2510–2518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Jimenez-Shahed, J.; Telkes, I.; Viswanathan, A.; Ince, N.F. GPi Oscillatory Activity Differentiates Tics from the Resting State, Voluntary Movements, and the Unmedicated Parkinsonian State. Front. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Priori, A.; Giannicola, G.; Rosa, M.; Marceglia, S.; Servello, D.; Sassi, M.; Porta, M. Deep Brain Electrophysiological Recordings Provide Clues to the Pathophysiology of Tourette Syndrome. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 1063–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Marceglia, S.; Servello, D.; Foffani, G.; Porta, M.; Sassi, M.; Mrakic-Sposta, S.; Rosa, M.; Barbieri, S.; Priori, A. Thalamic Single-Unit and Local Field Potential Activity in Tourette Syndrome. Mov. Disord. 2010, 25, 300–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Molina, R.; Okun, M.S.; Shute, J.B.; Opri, E.; Rossi, P.J.; Martinez-Ramirez, D.; Foote, K.D.; Gunduz, A. Report of a Patient Undergoing Chronic Responsive Deep Brain Stimulation for Tourette Syndrome: Proof of Concept. J. Neurosurg. 2018, 129, 308–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Shute, J.B.; Okun, M.S.; Opri, E.; Molina, R.; Rossi, P.J.; Martinez-Ramirez, D.; Foote, K.D.; Gunduz, A. Thalamocortical Network Activity Enables Chronic Tic Detection in Humans with Tourette Syndrome. NeuroImage Clin. 2016, 12, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Maling, N.; Hashemiyoon, R.; Foote, K.D.; Okun, M.S.; Sanchez, J.C. Increased Thalamic Gamma Band Activity Correlates with Symptom Relief Following Deep Brain Stimulation in Humans with Tourette’s Syndrome. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e44215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Cavanagh, J.F.; Frank, M.J. Frontal Theta as a Mechanism for Cognitive Control. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2014, 18, 414–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Cohen, M.X. A Neural Microcircuit for Cognitive Conflict Detection and Signaling. Trends Neurosci. 2014, 37, 480–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Buzsáki, G.; Draguhn, A. Neuronal Oscillations in Cortical Networks. Science 2004, 304, 1926–1929. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Church, J.A.; Fair, D.A.; Dosenbach, N.U.F.; Cohen, A.L.; Miezin, F.M.; Petersen, S.E.; Schlaggar, B.L. Control Networks in Paediatric Tourette Syndrome Show Immature and Anomalous Patterns of Functional Connectivity. Brain 2009, 132, 225–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Worbe, Y.; Malherbe, C.; Hartmann, A.; Pélégrini-Issac, M.; Messé, A.; Vidailhet, M.; Lehéricy, S.; Benali, H. Functional Immaturity of Cortico-Basal Ganglia Networks in Gilles de La Tourette Syndrome. Brain 2012, 135, 1937–1946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Adelhöfer, N.; Beste, C. Pre-Trial Theta Band Activity in the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Correlates with Inhibition-Related Theta Band Activity in the Right Inferior Frontal Cortex. Neuroimage 2020, 219, 117052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Braver, T.S. The Variable Nature of Cognitive Control: A Dual Mechanisms Framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 2012, 16, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Cooper, P.S.; Wong, A.S.W.; Fulham, W.R.; Thienel, R.; Mansfield, E.; Michie, P.T.; Karayanidis, F. Theta Frontoparietal Connectivity Associated with Proactive and Reactive Cognitive Control Processes. Neuroimage 2015, 108, 354–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Cooper, P.S.; Wong, A.S.W.; McKewen, M.; Michie, P.T.; Karayanidis, F. Frontoparietal Theta Oscillations during Proactive Control Are Associated with Goal-Updating and Reduced Behavioral Variability. Biol. Psychol. 2017, 129, 253–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Koechlin, E. The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex. Science 2003, 302, 1181–1185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Koechlin, E.; Summerfield, C. An Information Theoretical Approach to Prefrontal Executive Function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 229–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Mansouri, F.A.; Koechlin, E.; Rosa, M.G.P.; Buckley, M.J. Managing Competing Goals—A Key Role for the Frontopolar Cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017, 18, 645–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Hommel, B. Action Control According to TEC (Theory of Event Coding). Psychol. Res. 2009, 73, 512–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Barbas, H.; Pandya, D.N. Architecture and Intrinsic Connections of the Prefrontal Cortex in the Rhesus Monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 1989, 286, 353–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Burman, K.J.; Reser, D.H.; Yu, H.-H.; Rosa, M.G.P. Cortical Input to the Frontal Pole of the Marmoset Monkey. Cereb. Cortex 2011, 21, 1712–1737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Sheehan, D.V.; Lecrubier, Y.; Sheehan, K.H.; Amorim, P.; Janavs, J.; Weiller, E.; Hergueta, T.; Baker, R.; Dunbar, G.C. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The Development and Validation of a Structured Diagnostic Psychiatric Interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 1998, 59 (Suppl. 20), 22–33. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  47. Sheehan, D.V.; Sheehan, K.H.; Shytle, R.D.; Janavs, J.; Bannon, Y.; Rogers, J.E.; Milo, K.M.; Stock, S.L.; Wilkinson, B. Reliability and Validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID). J. Clin. Psychiatry 2010, 71, 313–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Christiansen, H.; Hirsch, O.; Philipsen, A.; Oades, R.D.; Matthies, S.; Hebebrand, J.; Ueckermann, J.; Abdel-Hamid, M.; Kraemer, M.; Wiltfang, J.; et al. German Validation of the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scale-Self-Report: Confirmation of Factor Structure in a Large Sample of Participants with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 2013, 17, 690–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Robertson, M.M.; Banerjee, S.; Kurlan, R.; Cohen, D.J.; Leckman, J.F.; McMahon, W.; Pauls, D.L.; Sandor, P.; van de Wetering, B.J. The Tourette Syndrome Diagnostic Confidence Index: Development and Clinical Associations. Neurology 1999, 53, 2108–2112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Leckman, J.F.; Riddle, M.A.; Hardin, M.T.; Ort, S.I.; Swartz, K.L.; Stevenson, J.; Cohen, D.J. The Yale Global Tic Severity Scale: Initial Testing of a Clinician-Rated Scale of Tic Severity. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1989, 28, 566–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Woods, D.W.; Piacentini, J.; Himle, M.B.; Chang, S. Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (PUTS): Initial Psychometric Results and Examination of the Premonitory Urge Phenomenon in Youths with Tic Disorders. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2005, 26, 397–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Goetz, C.G.; Pappert, E.J.; Louis, E.D.; Raman, R.; Leurgans, S. Advantages of a Modified Scoring Method for the Rush Video-Based Tic Rating Scale. Mov. Disord. 1999, 14, 502–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Goodman, W.K.; Price, L.H.; Rasmussen, S.A.; Mazure, C.; Fleischmann, R.L.; Hill, C.L.; Heninger, G.R.; Charney, D.S. The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. I. Development, Use, and Reliability. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1989, 46, 1006–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Gallant, J.; Storch, E.A.; Merlo, L.J.; Ricketts, E.D.; Geffken, G.R.; Goodman, W.K.; Murphy, T.K. Convergent and Discriminant Validity of the Children’s Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale-Symptom Checklist. J. Anxiety Disord. 2008, 22, 1369–1376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Waldmann, H.-C. Kurzformen Des HAWIK-IV: Statistische Bewertung in Verschiedenen Anwendungsszenarien. Diagnostica 2008, 54, 202–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Hartman, D.E. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV (WAIS IV): Return of the Gold Standard. Appl. Neuropsychol. 2009, 16, 85–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Oldfield, R.C. The Assessment and Analysis of Handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9, 97–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Colzato, L.S.; Warrens, M.J.; Hommel, B. Priming and Binding in and across Perception and Action: A Correlational Analysis of the Internal Structure of Event Files. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2006, 59, 1785–1804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Takacs, A.; Bluschke, A.; Kleimaker, M.; Münchau, A.; Beste, C. Neurophysiological Mechanisms Underlying Motor Feature Binding Processes and Representations. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2020, 42, 1313–1327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Oostenveld, R.; Fries, P.; Maris, E.; Schoffelen, J.-M. FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data. Available online: https://www.hindawi.com/journals/cin/2011/156869/ (accessed on 15 January 2021).
  61. Gross, J.; Kujala, J.; Hamalainen, M.; Timmermann, L.; Schnitzler, A.; Salmelin, R. Dynamic Imaging of Coherent Sources: Studying Neural Interactions in the Human Brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 694–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Van Veen, B.D.; van Drongelen, W.; Yuchtman, M.; Suzuki, A. Localization of Brain Electrical Activity via Linearly Constrained Minimum Variance Spatial Filtering. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 1997, 44, 867–880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Ester, M.; Kriegel, H.-P.; Sander, J.; Xu, X. A Density-Based Algorithm for Discovering Clusters in Large Spatial Databases with Noise. In Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining, Portland, OR, USA, 2–4 August 1996; AAAI Press: Portland, Oregon, USA, 1996; pp. 226–231. [Google Scholar]
  64. Tzourio-Mazoyer, N.; Landeau, B.; Papathanassiou, D.; Crivello, F.; Etard, O.; Delcroix, N.; Mazoyer, B.; Joliot, M. Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain. Neuroimage 2002, 15, 273–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Benjamini, Y.; Hochberg, Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J. R. Stat. Society. Ser. B (Methodol.) 1995, 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  67. Morey, R.D.; Rouder, J.N. BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes Factors for Common Designs; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  68. Lee, M.D.; Wagenmakers, E.-J. Bayesian Cognitive Modeling: A Practical Course; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  69. Berlucchi, G.; Vallar, G. The History of the Neurophysiology and Neurology of the Parietal Lobe. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2018; Volume 151, pp. 3–30. ISBN 978-0-444-63622-5. [Google Scholar]
  70. Binkofski, F.; Buccino, G. The Role of the Parietal Cortex in Sensorimotor Transformations and Action Coding. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2018; Volume 151, pp. 467–479. ISBN 978-0-444-63622-5. [Google Scholar]
  71. Creem-Regehr, S.H. Sensory-Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Human Posterior Parietal Cortex Involved in Manual Actions. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2009, 91, 166–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Culham, J.C.; Valyear, K.F. Human Parietal Cortex in Action. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2006, 16, 205–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Di Russo, F.; Berchicci, M.; Bozzacchi, C.; Perri, R.L.; Pitzalis, S.; Spinelli, D. Beyond the “Bereitschaftspotential”: Action Preparation behind Cognitive Functions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 78, 57–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Favila, S.E.; Samide, R.; Sweigart, S.C.; Kuhl, B.A. Parietal Representations of Stimulus Features Are Amplified during Memory Retrieval and Flexibly Aligned with Top-Down Goals. J. Neurosci. 2018, 38, 7809–7821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Le, A.; Vesia, M.; Yan, X.; Crawford, J.D.; Niemeier, M. Parietal Area BA7 Integrates Motor Programs for Reaching, Grasping, and Bimanual Coordination. J. Neurophysiol. 2017, 117, 624–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Illustration of the task. Presented stimuli are visualized on a time scale relative to Stimulus B. A fixation cross was shown at all times. In the top left corner, the layout of the response keys (num pad on a keyboard) is shown. Participants had to respond to the Stimuli with the corresponding left or right index fingers. The figure depicts a feature overlap trial: both Stimulus A and B require a response with the left hand, as indicated by the arrowhead in Stimulus A and the ‘&’ symbol in Stimulus B (refer to Methods). Below the time axis, the time frames chosen for the pre- and within-trial interval are outlined.
Figure 1. Illustration of the task. Presented stimuli are visualized on a time scale relative to Stimulus B. A fixation cross was shown at all times. In the top left corner, the layout of the response keys (num pad on a keyboard) is shown. Participants had to respond to the Stimuli with the corresponding left or right index fingers. The figure depicts a feature overlap trial: both Stimulus A and B require a response with the left hand, as indicated by the arrowhead in Stimulus A and the ‘&’ symbol in Stimulus B (refer to Methods). Below the time axis, the time frames chosen for the pre- and within-trial interval are outlined.
Biomedicines 11 00393 g001
Figure 2. Within-trial results. For all subplots, the left side of the figure refers to the GTS group, whereas the right side refers to the HC group. (A) Significant channels and timeframes for channel-wise t-tests on theta power between the feature overlap and no-feature overlap conditions are shown. Warmer colors indicate smaller p-values (colormap threshold at p < 0.05). (B) Sensor-level time frequency representation of the difference between the feature overlap and no-feature overlap conditions over the channels showing significant differences in the theta band are shown (please refer to (A)). Topoplots show the mean theta power over the entire segment as well as the significant channels (indicated by a red cross). (C) Clusters as identified by the DBSCAN algorithm are shown. The identified clusters (refer to Section 3) are visualized on a template MRI. For voxels within the cluster, the source ratio (difference between the feature overlap levels divided by the sum of the overlap levels) is visualized. (D) Time-frequency representations of the theta band in in the respective clusters.
Figure 2. Within-trial results. For all subplots, the left side of the figure refers to the GTS group, whereas the right side refers to the HC group. (A) Significant channels and timeframes for channel-wise t-tests on theta power between the feature overlap and no-feature overlap conditions are shown. Warmer colors indicate smaller p-values (colormap threshold at p < 0.05). (B) Sensor-level time frequency representation of the difference between the feature overlap and no-feature overlap conditions over the channels showing significant differences in the theta band are shown (please refer to (A)). Topoplots show the mean theta power over the entire segment as well as the significant channels (indicated by a red cross). (C) Clusters as identified by the DBSCAN algorithm are shown. The identified clusters (refer to Section 3) are visualized on a template MRI. For voxels within the cluster, the source ratio (difference between the feature overlap levels divided by the sum of the overlap levels) is visualized. (D) Time-frequency representations of the theta band in in the respective clusters.
Biomedicines 11 00393 g002
Figure 3. Pre-trial results. (A) Clusters as identified by the DBSCAN algorithm in the pre-trial period are shown. The identified clusters (refer to 3. Results) are visualized on a template MRI. Please note that the clusters are adjacent to each other and therefore cannot be distinguished in this visualization. The Neural Activity Index (NAI) is given for voxels within the clusters with warmer colors representing higher values. (B) Time-frequency representations of the theta band in the identified clusters are shown.
Figure 3. Pre-trial results. (A) Clusters as identified by the DBSCAN algorithm in the pre-trial period are shown. The identified clusters (refer to 3. Results) are visualized on a template MRI. Please note that the clusters are adjacent to each other and therefore cannot be distinguished in this visualization. The Neural Activity Index (NAI) is given for voxels within the clusters with warmer colors representing higher values. (B) Time-frequency representations of the theta band in the identified clusters are shown.
Biomedicines 11 00393 g003
Figure 4. Main results of the correlations in the GTS group are shown. (A) Theta clusters: the top figure depicts theta band activity in the fronto-ventral cluster as identified by the DBSCAN algorithm in the pre-trial interval. The Neural Activity Index (NAI) is shown for voxels within the cluster. The bottom figure depicts the within-trial left fronto-dorsal theta cluster. For voxels within the cluster, the source ratio (difference between the feature overlap levels divided by the sum of the overlap levels) is color-coded. (B) The correlation map between the clusters from Figure 3A is shown. The correlation map shows the Pearson correlation for each time point between the pre- and within-trial tasks (color represents the r-value). (C) FDR-corrected q-values for the correlation map Figure 3B. Black areas indicate no significant correlation (q > 0.05), whereas bright areas indicate a significant correlation.
Figure 4. Main results of the correlations in the GTS group are shown. (A) Theta clusters: the top figure depicts theta band activity in the fronto-ventral cluster as identified by the DBSCAN algorithm in the pre-trial interval. The Neural Activity Index (NAI) is shown for voxels within the cluster. The bottom figure depicts the within-trial left fronto-dorsal theta cluster. For voxels within the cluster, the source ratio (difference between the feature overlap levels divided by the sum of the overlap levels) is color-coded. (B) The correlation map between the clusters from Figure 3A is shown. The correlation map shows the Pearson correlation for each time point between the pre- and within-trial tasks (color represents the r-value). (C) FDR-corrected q-values for the correlation map Figure 3B. Black areas indicate no significant correlation (q > 0.05), whereas bright areas indicate a significant correlation.
Biomedicines 11 00393 g004
Table 1. Clinical characteristics of the patients with GTS included in the study.
Table 1. Clinical characteristics of the patients with GTS included in the study.
PatientAgeSexIQDisease,
Duration,
Years
DCI
(0–100)
YGTSS
Total
(0–100)
YGTSS
Tics
(0–50)
Motor
Tics/Min
Rush
Score
(0–20)
PUTS
(9–36)
YBOCS
or
CY-BOCS
(0–40)
Medication
135Female11028441773.931510Aripiprazol
228Female982395513139133215Aripiprazol,
methylphenidat,
amitriptyline
318Male1178431774.510130
420Male1171556582817.313270Aripiprazol
528Male792072612124.892814Aripiprazol
624Male11296367278.711229
718Male851155503042.210170
829Male1061579n.a.n.a.29.4132413
922Female8018100462630.713180
1035Female96227647274014220Tiapride
1126Female971439551519.812190
1223Female84165037741.212180
1322Female961746201010.54240
1426Female1151956252556.49165
1523Male1111664312162.514270
1630Male1122354542485.214130
1719Female1171135301022.311130
1820Male10075958286816280Aripiprazol
1916Male86--452523167
2012Male101--32227.6716-
2112Male105--28182.4311-
2218Male98740131321.410.5-14Methylphenidate
2316Male12311-391977.213.5242
2417Male128-439949.29.5202
2516Male1124254410.44147
2613Female110834311138.210.500
2714Female1055365531.87.500
2814Male765318819.4800
2913Male1088544727101.617190
3013Male102-427713.43.5190
MEAN20.7-102.9 53.534.217.732.79.917.83.5
DCI = Diagnostic Confidence Index; PUTS = Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale; YBOCS = Yale Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale; CY-BOCS = Children’s Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale; YGTSS = Yale Global Tic Severity Scale.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wendiggensen, P.; Paulus, T.; Bluschke, A.; Takacs, A.; Toth-Faber, E.; Weissbach, A.; Bäumer, T.; Frings, C.; Roessner, V.; Münchau, A.; et al. Theta Activity Dynamics during Embedded Response Plan Processing in Tourette Syndrome. Biomedicines 2023, 11, 393. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines11020393

AMA Style

Wendiggensen P, Paulus T, Bluschke A, Takacs A, Toth-Faber E, Weissbach A, Bäumer T, Frings C, Roessner V, Münchau A, et al. Theta Activity Dynamics during Embedded Response Plan Processing in Tourette Syndrome. Biomedicines. 2023; 11(2):393. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines11020393

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wendiggensen, Paul, Theresa Paulus, Annet Bluschke, Adam Takacs, Eszter Toth-Faber, Anne Weissbach, Tobias Bäumer, Christian Frings, Veit Roessner, Alexander Münchau, and et al. 2023. "Theta Activity Dynamics during Embedded Response Plan Processing in Tourette Syndrome" Biomedicines 11, no. 2: 393. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines11020393

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop