Next Article in Journal
When Love Comes at a Cost: Mental Health Outcomes in Older Adults Providing Grandparental Care
Previous Article in Journal
Formative Development and Acceptability of a Lifestyle Weight Management Intervention for Breast Cancer Survivors in Greece: The NutriLife Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Motor and Sensory Benefits of Mirror Therapy in Children and Adolescents with Unilateral Cerebral Palsy: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Comparison of Developmental Profiles of Preschool Children with Down Syndrome, Global Developmental Delay, and Developmental Language Disorder

by
Mónica-Alba Ahulló-Fuster
1,
M. Luz Sánchez-Sánchez
2,*,
Alejandro Monterrubio-Gordón
3 and
Maria-Arantzazu Ruescas-Nicolau
2
1
Department of Radiology, Rehabilitation and Physiotherapy, Faculty of Nursing, Physiotherapy and Podiatry, Complutense University of Madrid, 28040 Madrid, Spain
2
Physiotherapy in Motion, Multispeciality Research Group (PTinMOTION), Department of Physiotherapy, University of Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain
3
Department of Physiotherapy, Faculty of Physiotherapy, Universitat de València, 46010 Valencia, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Healthcare 2025, 13(14), 1684; https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare13141684
Submission received: 11 May 2025 / Revised: 7 July 2025 / Accepted: 11 July 2025 / Published: 13 July 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Health Services in Children's Physiotherapy)

Abstract

Background/Objectives: Developmental disabilities substantially affect the daily lives of children and their families. Although interest in examining the developmental profiles of children with various disabilities has grown, few studies have systematically compared them. This study aimed to characterize the developmental profiles of preschool-aged children with different disabilities. It was hypothesized that developmental profiles would differ depending on the type of developmental disability. Methods: A cross-sectional study was conducted. Scores on the Battelle® Developmental Inventory, 2nd Edition (BDI−2) were retrieved for a non-probabilistic convenience sample of 46 children diagnosed with Down syndrome (DS) (n = 22), global developmental delay (GDD) (n = 17), and developmental language disorder (DLD) (n = 7) upon completion of an early intervention program. Developmental quotients (DQs) for the overall BDI−2 and for each domain were determined. Results: The children’s mean age was 42.39 ± 5.23 months (range: 30–57). Significant differences were observed among groups with regard to global DQ and all domain-specific DQs (p ≤ 0.01). The GDD group demonstrated the highest DQs across all domains and globally, in comparison to the other groups. Conversely, children with DS had substantially lower DQs across all domains and globally compared to those with GDD, and in the motor and communication domains compared to children with DLD. Conclusions: These findings underscore the importance of early intervention strategies to improve communication in children with DS and highlight the need for regular assessments to monitor progress and identify potential limitations, particularly during the preschool-to-school transition. Additionally, specialists should advise parents of children with DLD to adopt specific behaviors that support the development of their children’s social, adaptive, and language skills.

1. Introduction

The initial 5 years of life, often designated as early childhood, represent a critical period during which the developing brain is highly receptive to external stimuli and novel learning experiences [1,2]. This heightened sensitivity promotes rapid maturation and development, thereby shaping the child’s overall growth [1]. However, during this developmental stage, substantial delays in typical developmental processes can occur, potentially resulting in developmental disabilities [2]. Globally, it is estimated that approximately 5 million children aged 0–5 years live with moderate to severe disabilities [3]. The prevalence of these conditions, however, varies substantially across different geographical regions. Developmental disabilities are more commonly reported in low- and middle-income countries than in high-income ones [3,4].
Developmental disabilities have a substantial impact on the lives of affected children, resulting in functional limitations across multiple areas due to delays in achieving key developmental milestones [4]. These delays may be caused by various childhood conditions, most frequently including Down syndrome (DS), neuropsychomotor or global developmental delay (GDD), and developmental language disorder (DLD) [5]. DS arises from congenital abnormalities and is the most common aneuploid condition present at birth [5,6,7]. GDD is a relatively common developmental disorder for which no underlying cause is readily apparent [8,9], and it typically involves delays in at least two developmental domains [10,11]. DLD, by contrast, is characterized by impaired expressive and/or receptive language skills, despite normal hearing and appropriate non-verbal intelligence [5,6,7,12,13]. It occurs in the absence of neurological damage, structural oral anomalies, or other medical conditions [12,13,14,15], and is most frequently diagnosed in children under 5 years of age [16].
In addition, it should be noted that despite their distinct etiologies and presenting characteristics, the neurodevelopmental profiles of children with these conditions exhibit substantial variability [17]. The manifestation of delays in these developmental domains often impedes the child’s ability to engage in daily activities, exerting a detrimental effect on both the child and their family and community. This, in turn, increases the child’s dependence on caregivers, thereby compromising their overall quality of life [18,19]. Consequently, the early detection and timely rehabilitation of children with developmental disabilities is of paramount importance [4].
Despite the availability of early interventions, the global burden of developmental disabilities has shown limited improvement since the 1990s [19]. This underscores the need for a comprehensive analysis and comparison of the developmental profiles of children with such disabilities. The scientific literature includes studies examining the developmental profiles of children with various medical conditions, such as severe acute malnutrition [20], middle cerebral artery infarction, periventricular infarction [21], and autism spectrum disorder [22]. Nevertheless, analyzing developmental profiles across different medical conditions is important, as children with varying conditions may respond differently to rehabilitation interventions [21]. Such an approach may offer a broader perspective on common challenges and highlight inadequacies in current intervention strategies.
However, research examining and comparing the developmental profiles of children with different developmental disabilities remains limited. Many studies focus on only one or two groups of children with disabilities, or divide children with the same diagnosis into subgroups [14,15,16,17], and only a few compare multiple groups with diverse developmental conditions [23]. Moreover, existing studies tend to focus on a limited number of developmental areas [24,25,26], overlooking the importance of thoroughly evaluating multiple domains. Furthermore, most research in this field centers on newborns, school-aged children, or preadolescents [25,26,27,28], with relatively few studies involving preschool-aged children who have received therapeutic intervention in an early intervention center (EIC) [23].
To address this gap, the primary objective of this study was to analyze the developmental profiles of preschool-aged children with various disabilities upon completion of an EIC program. Specifically, a comparison was made between the developmental profiles of children diagnosed with DS, GDD, and DLD. These profiles were assessed using the Battelle® Developmental Inventory, 2nd Edition (BDI−2). For the purpose of this study, developmental quotients (DQs) for the complete inventory and for each domain were determined. It was hypothesized that children with different developmental disorders would demonstrate distinct developmental profiles, depending on their specific diagnoses.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design and Participants

The present study adopted a retrospective cross-sectional study design. Participants were obtained from Asindown, an association of people with developmental delays in Valencia, Spain. The clinical records of children who attended the EIC of Asindown were retrieved over a period of 3 months. Consequently, non-probabilistic convenience sampling was utilized. The statistical power of the study was subsequently assessed post hoc using G*Power v3.1 software. The power was determined to be 99.9% at a 0.05 Type I error (two-tailed), with an effect size of 1.07 (ηp2 = 0.536 derived from the comparison of global DQ among groups using a one-way analysis of variance [ANOVA]; please, refer to the results section), and a sample size of 46 participants.
Eligibility for the records under scrutiny was contingent upon the following criteria: (1) children with isolated diagnoses of DS, GDD, or DLD at the time of enrollment in the EIC; (2) attendance of an early intervention program during preschool years [29]; and (3) a developmental assessment using the second edition of the BDI−2 prior to completion of the EIC. Records were excluded if they (1) included multiple concurrent diagnoses (e.g., DS and GDD); (2) presented any comorbidities; (3) were incomplete; or (4) did not include all BDI−2 domains.
Written informed consent was obtained by the center from all participants involved in the study for data extraction, and a confidentiality agreement was signed with Asindown. The clinical records never left the EIC. To ensure anonymity and protect participants’ identities, a numerical code was assigned to each participant. The study was conducted in full compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki [30] and was approved by the authors’ institutional ethics committee (approval number 1794786). Additionally, the report adheres to the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) guidelines [31].

2.2. Procedures

A researcher regularly visited the EIC to extract data over a period of 3 months. Information on children who attended the early intervention program was collected in an Excel spreadsheet. Clinical and demographic data (sex, age in months, ethnicity, and clinical diagnosis), as well as developmental functioning (BDI−2), were retrieved from medical records.
The BDI−2 [32] is a widely used, standardized instrument designed to assess key developmental skills through observation, supplemented by parental report, in children from birth to 7 years and 11 months. It is a discriminative norm-referenced measure of developmental activities. It identifies strengths and weaknesses in children’s developmental areas to help create personalized intervention programs. In the collaborating EIC, prior to admission to primary school, the BDI−2 was administered by members of the early intervention team, who were experienced in assessing children with disabilities and trained in BDI−2 administration. Specifically, a neuropsychologist assessed the personal/social and cognitive domains, a physiotherapist the motor domain, a speech and language therapist the communication domain, and an occupational therapist the adaptive domain. Depending on each child’s individual characteristics, each professional required between 15 and 60 min to assess the corresponding domain. For this study, data from this assessment were of interest.
The BDI−2 consists of 341 items grouped into five domains: (1) personal/social; (2) motor; (3) communication; (4) cognitive; and (5) adaptive [33]. Each item is scored on a 3-point scale (0: no ability in the skill, 1: emerging ability, 2: acquired skill) [33,34]. Item scores are summed to produce a domain score, and the sum of domain scores produces a global score. To allow for comparison among individuals of different ages, raw scores are converted to age equivalents using the normative scales provided by the inventory. Subsequently, domain-specific and global DQs were calculated using the following formula: age equivalent/chronological age × 100 [35]. The global DQ has a mean value of 100 and a standard deviation of 15 [32]. For the purpose of statistical analysis, DQs were used. The BDI−2 has demonstrated acceptable to excellent psychometric properties in terms of reliability and consistency, and its validity has been established for use across a range of developmental disorders [36]. It has also been used in studies similar to ours [23,37,38].

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics software (v. 24, IBM Corp; Armonk, NY, USA). Data are presented as frequency (percentage) or mean ± standard deviation, as appropriate. The normality of continuous variables was assessed with the Shapiro–Wilk test. Sample characteristics were compared between groups using a one-way ANOVA and the chi-squared (χ2) test, as appropriate.
To evaluate differences between groups in the DQs of each domain and the global DQ of the BDI−2, several ANOVAs were conducted. To assess ANOVAs effect size, partial eta-squared (ηp2) was calculated. Post hoc comparisons with Bonferroni correction were performed to identify differences between pairs of groups. Effect sizes according to Hedges’ g were also calculated to ensure comparability in the pairwise analyses.
Finally, to determine whether the global DQ of each group differed from the standard reference value (mean = 100), three one-sample t-tests were conducted (one per group). Cohen’s method was used to calculate the corresponding effect sizes. A p-value of <0.05 was considered the threshold for statistical significance.

3. Results

A total of 170 children were enrolled at the EIC at the time of this study. Of these children, 71 were not expected to be discharged to a primary school, 26 did not have a definitive diagnosis, 9 presented comorbidities, 8 were transferred to another center, and 3 were hospitalized. Therefore, 51 children were assessed using the BDI−2, but only 46 children (22 with DS, 17 with GDD, and 7 with DLD) met the inclusion criteria. Data from these 46 children were included in the study. Figure 1 illustrates the flow of participants.
The sample consisted of 32 male and 14 female children, with a mean age of 42.4 ± 6.1 months. All participants identified as Caucasian. Upon division of the sample into groups (Table 1), it was observed that the three groups (DS, GDD, and DLD) did not differ significantly in terms of age (F(2, 43) = 1.765, p = 0.183). However, a statistically significant difference was identified among the groups with respect to sex (χ2(2) = 6.170, p = 0.046). Notably, the DLD group consisted exclusively of male participants (Table 1). Nevertheless, the association between sex and clinical diagnosis was weak (C = 0.29, p = 0.122).
Data are expressed as mean ± standard deviation or frequency (percentage). Between-group differences were calculated by using a one-way ANOVA for continuous data and the chi-squared test for categorical data. ES: effect size. Partial eta-squared (η2p) was used for continuous variables and the contingency coefficient (C) for categorical variables. DS: Down syndrome; GDD: global developmental delay; DLD: Developmental Language Disorder.
The findings of the between-group comparisons of domain-specific DQs and the global DQ of the BDI−2 are shown in Table 2 and Figure 2. The ANOVAs revealed statistically significant differences for all studied variables, indicating the presence of differences among the three groups.
In the post hoc comparisons (Figure 2a), the mean global DQ of the GDD group was significantly higher than that of the DS group (t(37) = 7.01, p < 0.001, g = 2.326) and the DLD group (t(22) = 3.20, p = 0.008, g = 1.121). However, the DS and DLD groups did not differ significantly (t(27) = 1.89, p = 0.193, g = −0.964).
The pairwise comparisons of domain-specific DQs showed that children with GDD had significantly higher scores than those with DS and DLD in the personal−social domain (t(37) = 5.42, p < 0.001, g = 1.790; t(22) = 3.93, p < 0.001, g = 1.424, respectively) (Figure 2b) and the adaptive domain (t(37) = 3.93, p < 0.001, g = 1.287; t(22) = 2.67, p = 0.03, g = 1.078, respectively) (Figure 2c). In the motor domain (Figure 2d), the DS group exhibited significantly lower DQs compared to both the GDD group (t(37) = 6.78, p < 0.001, g = −2.130) and the DLD group (t(27) = 3.07, p = 0.010, g = −1.442). In the communication domain (Figure 2e), children with DS demonstrated lower DQs compared to those with GDD (t(37) = 8.32, p < 0.001, g = −2.915) and DLD (t(27) = 3.58, p = 0.02, g = −2.309). In the cognitive domain (Figure 2f), the GDD group showed significantly higher mean DQs than the DS group (t(37) = 6.65, p < 0.001, g = 2.383) and the DLD group (t(22) = 2.57, p = 0.04, g = 0.956).
Comparison of the global mean DQ of the three groups with the BDI−2 normative value (mean = 100) revealed a significant difference in all three groups, as indicated by the one-sample t-test: DS group: t(21) = 24.53, p < 0.001, r = 0.982; GDD group: t(16) = 4.03, p = 0.001, r = 0.710; and DLD group: t(6) = 5.59, p = 0.001, r = 0.915. All groups exhibited lower global DQs (Table 2) in comparison to the BDI−2 reference mean.

4. Discussion

The present study has shown that, as hypothesized, children with developmental disorders exhibit different developmental profiles depending on their specific diagnostic condition. This is consistent with findings from other studies comparing groups of children with different health diagnoses [23,37,39,40].
Our results showed that, compared to individuals with DS and DLD, children with GDD obtained the highest global and domain-specific DQs, scoring, on average, above 70 in some domains (personal/social and adaptive), above 80 in others (motor and communication), and 100 in the cognitive domain. Moreover, the GDD group achieved an average global DQ score of 84.25 ± 16.09, which was significantly higher than the average scores obtained by the DS (54.74 ± 8.65) and DLD (65.49 ± 16.33) groups. However, previous research has reported a lower developmental profile in children with GDD compared to, for example, those born prematurely [23] or children with DLD [37]. As Horowitz and Matson [39] and Sharma et al. [11] noted, differences in children’s developmental levels may be influenced by multiple factors, such as the etiology of GDD, the nature and extent of early intervention, or the age at the time of assessment. These factors could explain the discrepancies between previous findings and ours. Moreover, toddlers with GDD often experience delays across multiple domains [9], making the clinical presentation of GDD heterogeneous. Notably, these delays tend to be more severe and pronounced in specific developmental areas when children with GDD present additional comorbid conditions, such as autism spectrum disorder [41,42] or intellectual disability [43,44]. For this reason, some researchers highlight the need for genetic testing to determine the underlying nature of GDD [45,46]. It is widely accepted that GDD is characterized by diverse phenotypes and a complex etiology [46]. Given that additional pathologies were an exclusion criterion in this study, it is highly probable that the members of our GDD group were relatively more homogeneous in this regard.
Regarding differences between groups in specific developmental domains, participants diagnosed with DS in the present study exhibited the most deficient motor domain scores when compared with those diagnosed with DLD and GDD. This is consistent with findings from other studies [23,39,47,48]. Children with DS frequently experience motor delays, primarily due to hypotonia and ligament laxity, which contribute to joint instability and hinder motor development [49,50]. Consequently, they struggle to achieve key gross motor milestones such as sitting, standing, and walking [25,49], and often exhibit difficulties with fine motor skills, including manual dexterity and bimanual coordination [47,51]. Notably, motor difficulties may persist beyond childhood, even when early intervention has been provided [52,53].
Our results also showed that the DS group had greater impairments in the communication domain compared to the other two groups. While some studies suggest that children with DS exhibit communication skills similar to those of peers with other developmental disabilities [23,39,48], it is important to consider that previous research often assessed them at a younger age. However, language impairments in children with DS tend to become more evident during the transition from preschool to school age [23,54], which aligns with the developmental period in which our sample was evaluated. Therefore, our findings highlight the need to develop early intervention strategies for communication in children with DS, as well as the importance of regular assessments to monitor progress and detect potential limitations [55], especially during this critical period.
In children with DS, receptive language is typically more developed than expressive language, with expressive difficulties primarily affecting phonology and syntax [54,56]. Additionally, hearing impairments and the distinctive orofacial anatomy associated with DS further hinder speech perception and production [57]. Structural differences, such as an enlarged tongue and an underdeveloped maxilla, contribute to a higher incidence of articulatory errors in individuals with DS [58,59].
Another notable finding of this study was the difference in cognitive abilities among groups. The GDD group obtained substantially higher scores in the cognitive domain of the BDI−2 compared to the DS and DLD groups. While a previous study similar to ours [23] found no cognitive differences between individuals with DS and those with other developmental disabilities, it is well established that DS is associated with cognitive limitations. However, the scientific literature acknowledges that these limitations exhibit a degree of inter-individual variability, suggesting the presence of distinct cognitive profiles within this population [60,61]. Specific areas, such as memory [62] and executive functioning [63], tend to be particularly affected [64]. Furthermore, research on general intelligence suggests that children with DS develop at a slower pace than their typically developing peers [65]. In this regard, our results underscore the importance of addressing the cognitive aspects of DS through therapeutic interventions. Exercise interventions, in particular, have been shown to produce substantial enhancements in cognitive functioning in individuals with DS [66].
Additionally, the lower cognitive scores observed in the DLD group of our study are consistent with previous research showing that children with DLD experience alterations in cognitive skills [67,68,69]. Studies have identified difficulties across multiple cognitive do-mains, including visuospatial working memory [68,70], phonological short-term memory [71], executive functioning [67], and cognitive flexibility [72]. Consequently, the literature has extensively explored whether the core deficit in DLD is primarily cognitive or linguistic [73,74,75], highlighting the complexity of this condition.
In our study, significant differences were also observed in the personal/social and adaptive domains, with the DLD and DS groups obtaining lower scores compared to the GDD group. A recent systematic review found that children with DLD experience social difficulties, particularly in peer interactions such as turn-taking and game behavior [76], as well as challenges in social–emotional functioning [77]. Based on this, professionals may advise parents of children with DLD to adopt specific parenting practices—such as fostering and modeling positive relationships and supporting opportunities to practice social and adaptive skills—to help their children improve their abilities. This, in turn, could contribute to language development [78,79].
Likewise, toddlers with DS have been shown to exhibit limitations in social skills when compared to typically developing children [57,80]. In addition to social interactions, a child’s relationship with their environment is shaped by adaptive behavior, which supports increasing independence, purposeful engagement, and responsibility [81]. Consistent with previous research [82,83], our findings indicate that children with DS and DLD demonstrate reduced capabilities in adaptive behavior. However, some studies suggest that adaptive behavior in these groups may be either superior [37] or comparable to that of other developmental disabilities [23], underscoring the need for further research in this area.
Lastly, in this study, children—regardless of their specific pathology—exhibited lower global DQs compared to the normative reference values established in the BDI−2. This finding aligns with outcomes from previous studies [23,37,39,84]. Typically, the DQs of children with developmental disabilities are evaluated using composite scores derived from multiple developmental scales to determine the presence and extent of developmental delays. The persistence of these disabilities varies considerably depending on the condition. While some children may overcome their diagnosis through maturation and early intervention programs, others face limitations that are intrinsic to their pathology. This is particularly evident in genetic conditions [85,86,87], such as DS, where full recovery of certain functions is not feasible [88]. In contrast, children with DLD and GDD often show a more favorable prognosis [8,89]. Nevertheless, accurate and timely intervention and assessment are essential in all cases to achieve the best possible developmental outcomes for children with developmental disabilities.

Limitations

There are several limitations to this study that should be acknowledged. The participants were drawn from a single center, which may have introduced selection bias. However, the requirements for access to an EIC remain consistent across all Spanish centers. Furthermore, the small sample size in the DLD group (n = 7) substantially limits the generalizability of the results and increases the risk of a Type II error. While this is a notable limitation, the current scientific literature on the developmental profiles of preschool-aged children with DLD remains sparse. In this context, the study contributes to an important area, as ensuring that children are ready to learn when they start school has become a key priority. However, the general education curriculum often does not accommodate preschool children with developmental delays [90]. Therefore, it is essential to identify developmental profiles at this stage, especially in frequently occurring medical conditions.
Additionally, several factors may have influenced our results. This was a retrospective analysis based solely on BDI−2 scores obtained either at the conclusion of early therapeutic intervention or just before EIC discharge. Consequently, future research should evaluate and compare children’s progress both upon entering and just before leaving the EIC to gain a more comprehensive understanding of the developmental trajectories of GDD, DLD, and DS. Moreover, our study did not explore the underlying causes of GDD in the participants. Future studies are encouraged to investigate developmental profiles in this population while considering the etiology of the disorder and other potential confounding factors that may influence children’s outcomes (e.g., sex, age, duration, and intensity of the intervention, socioeconomic status, level of family involvement, family environment, and concurrent therapies).
Due to the design of the study, the assessors were not blinded to the children’s diagnoses. However, the researcher who performed the statistical analysis was blinded. Lastly, as in previous studies [91], assessments were conducted by a multidisciplinary team experienced in evaluating children with disabilities and trained in administering the BDI−2. It should be noted that, due to the operational structure of the center, each domain of the BDI−2 was evaluated by a professional from the area most closely related to that domain (e.g., a physiotherapist evaluated the motor domain). As such, the same professional evaluated all children within the same domain.

5. Conclusions

The developmental profiles of children with developmental delays vary depending on their medical diagnosis. Children with GDD exhibit a more favorable developmental profile—both globally and across BDI−2 domains—compared to those with DLD or DS. Additionally, children diagnosed with DS show lower scores in the motor and communicative domains than those with GDD or DLD. However, all three groups present global DQ scores lower than the BDI−2 reference value, confirming the presence of developmental delay.
From a clinical perspective, the findings underscore the need to implement early intervention strategies targeting communication in children with DS, along with the importance of regular assessments to monitor progress and identify potential limitations, particularly during the transition from preschool to school. Specialists are also encouraged to counsel parents of children with DLD to adopt specific behaviors aimed at enhancing their children’s social and adaptive skills, with the potential to improve communication abilities.
These findings highlight the importance of assessing developmental profiles in children with different types of developmental disabilities, as this can guide the creation of personalized intervention strategies and support parental guidance, ultimately improving outcomes for these children. Future multicenter and longitudinal studies could corroborate and expand upon these findings by tracking progress from admission to discharge in a larger sample of EIC users.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.L.S.-S. and A.M.-G.; methodology, M.L.S.-S. and A.M.-G.; formal analysis, M.-A.R.-N.; investigation, M.L.S.-S. and A.M.-G.; resources, M.L.S.-S. and A.M.-G.; data curation, M.-A.R.-N.; writing—original draft preparation, M.-A.A.-F. and M.-A.R.-N.; writing—review and editing, M.-A.A.-F., M.L.S.-S., and M.-A.R.-N.; supervision, M.L.S.-S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding. The first author (MAAF) was appointed under a predoctoral fellowship for researchers in training (CT63/19-CT64/19) at the Complutense University of Madrid during manuscript preparation.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Human Research of the Universitat de València (approval number 1794786, approval date: 12 September 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained by the center from all subjects involved in the study for extracting their data, and a confidentiality agreement was signed with Asindown.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, [MLSS], upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We want to praise the collaboration of the EIC of Asindown, and, in particular, Cristina Alemany Peñarrubia, for allowing access to the necessary data and facilitating the execution of this research. We also appreciate the collaboration of Sandra Pla Sanchis for her assistance with the questionnaire used in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
BDI−2Battelle® Developmental Inventory, 2nd Edition
DQDevelopmental Quotient
DSDown Syndrome
EICEarly Intervention Centre
GDDGlobal developmental delay
DLDDevelopmental Language Disorder

References

  1. National Research Council (US) and Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Integrating the Science of Early Childhood De-velopment; Shonkoff, J.P.; Phillips, D.A. (Eds.) From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2000; p. 9824. ISBN 978-0-309-06988-5. [Google Scholar]
  2. Choo, Y.Y.; Agarwal, P.; How, C.H.; Yeleswarapu, S.P. Developmental Delay: Identification and Management at Primary Care Level. Singap. Med. J. 2019, 60, 119–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Olusanya, B.O.; Kancherla, V.; Shaheen, A.; Ogbo, F.A.; Davis, A.C. Global and Regional Prevalence of Disabilities among Children and Adolescents: Analysis of Findings from Global Health Databases. Front. Public Health 2022, 10, 977453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Smythe, T.; Zuurmond, M.; Tann, C.J.; Gladstone, M.; Kuper, H. Early Intervention for Children with Developmental Disabilities in Low and Middle-Income Countries—The Case for Action. Int. Health 2021, 13, 222–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Chen, L.; Wang, L.; Wang, Y.; Hu, H.; Zhan, Y.; Zeng, Z.; Liu, L. Global, Regional, and National Burden and Trends of Down Syndrome From 1990 to 2019. Front. Genet. 2022, 13, 908482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Arbuzova, S.; Hutchin, T.; Cuckle, H. Mitochondrial Dysfunction and Down’s Syndrome. BioEssays 2002, 24, 681–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Gardiner, K.; Herault, Y.; Lott, I.T.; Antonarakis, S.E.; Reeves, R.H.; Dierssen, M. Down Syndrome: From Understanding the Neurobiology to Therapy. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 14943–14945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Dornelas, L.D.F.; Duarte, N.M.D.C.; Magalhães, L.D.C. [Neuropsychomotor developmental delay: Conceptual map, term definitions, uses and limitations]. Rev. Paul. Pediatr. 2015, 33, 88–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Shevell, M.; Ashwal, S.; Donley, D.; Flint, J.; Gingold, M.; Hirtz, D.; Majnemer, A.; Noetzel, M.; Sheth, R.D.; Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology; et al. Practice Parameter: Evaluation of the Child with Global Developmental Delay: Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology and The Practice Committee of the Child Neurology Society. Neurology 2003, 60, 367–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Aldosari, A.N.; Aldosari, T.S. Comprehensive Evaluation of the Child with Global Developmental Delays or Intellectual Disability. Clin. Exp. Pediatr. 2024, 67, 435–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Sharma, A.R.; Siddiqui, M.S.; Magar, S.; Kale, A.; Nelanuthala, M.; Singh, S.P. The Etiological Profile of Global Developmental Delay at a Tertiary Care Hospital in India: An Observational Study. Cureus 2023, 15, e41066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Lancaster, H.S.; Camarata, S. Reconceptualizing Developmental Language Disorder as a Spectrum Disorder: Issues and Evidence. Intl. J. Lang. Comm. Disor. 2019, 54, 79–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Bishop, D.V.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Thompson, P.A.; Greenhalgh, T.; The CATALISE-2 Consortium. Phase 2 of CATALISE: A Multinational and Multidisciplinary Delphi Consensus Study of Problems with Language Development: Terminology. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 1068–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Kumar, A.; Zubair, M.; Gulraiz, A.; Kalla, S.; Khan, S.; Patel, S.; Fleming, M.F.; Oghomitse-Omene, P.T.; Patel, P.; Qavi, M.S.S. An Assessment of Risk Factors of Delayed Speech and Language in Children: A Cross-Sectional Study. Cureus 2022, 14, e29623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Leonard, L.B. Specific Language Impairment Across Languages. Child Dev. Perspect. 2014, 8, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Calder, S.D.; Brennan-Jones, C.G.; Robinson, M.; Whitehouse, A.; Hill, E. The Prevalence of and Potential Risk Factors for Developmental Language Disorder at 10 Years in the Raine Study. J. Paediatr. Child Health 2022, 58, 2044–2050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Koul, A.M.; Ahmad, F.; Bhat, A.; Aein, Q.; Ahmad, A.; Reshi, A.A.; Kaul, R.R. Unraveling Down Syndrome: From Genetic Anomaly to Artificial Intelligence-Enhanced Diagnosis. Biomedicines 2023, 11, 3284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Rao, P.T.; Guddattu, V.; Solomon, J.M. Response Abilities of Children with Down Syndrome and Other Intellectual Developmental Disorders. Exp. Brain Res. 2017, 235, 1411–1427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Global Research on Developmental Disabilities Collaborators Accelerating Progress on Early Childhood Development for Children under 5 Years with Disabilities by 2030. Lancet Glob. Health 2022, 10, e438–e444. [CrossRef]
  20. Saleem, J.; Zakar, R.; Mushtaq, F.; Bukhari, G.M.J.; Fischer, F. Comparative Analysis of Developmental Profile between Normal and Severe Acute Malnourished Under-Five Children in Pakistan: A Multicentre Cross-Sectional Study. BMJ Open 2021, 11, e048644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fehlings, D.; Krishnan, P.; Ragguett, R.M.; Campbell, C.; Gorter, J.W.; Hunt, C.; Kawamura, A.; Kim, M.; McCormick, A.; Mesterman, R.; et al. 95 A Comparison of the Developmental Profiles of Individuals with Hemiplegic Cerebral Palsy Associated with Middle Cerebral Artery and Periventricular Venous Infarctions. Paediatr. Child Health 2019, 24, e36–e37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Roizen, N.; Friedman, S.; Vanderbilt, D.; Cacia, J.; Fussell, J.; Hansen, R.; Hofer, J.; Sideridis, G.; Stein, R.E.K.; Barbaresi, W. Developmental Profiles of Young Children Referred for Concern for Autism Spectrum Disorder: DBPNet Study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Matson, J.L.; Hess, J.A.; Sipes, M.; Horovitz, M. Developmental Profiles from the Battelle Developmental Inventory: A Comparison of Toddlers Diagnosed with Down Syndrome, Global Developmental Delay and Premature Birth. Dev. Neurorehabil. 2010, 13, 234–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Bedford, R.; Pickles, A.; Lord, C. Early Gross Motor Skills Predict the Subsequent Development of Language in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism Res. 2016, 9, 993–1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Malak, R.; Kostiukow, A.; Krawczyk-Wasielewska, A.; Mojs, E.; Samborski, W. Delays in Motor Development in Children with Down Syndrome. Med. Sci. Monit. 2015, 21, 1904–1910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Channell, M.M. Cross-Sectional Trajectories of Mental State Language Development in Children with Down Syndrome. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2020, 29, 760–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Wu, J.; Beerse, M.; Ajisafe, T.; Liang, H. Walking Dynamics in Preadolescents with and without Down Syndrome. Phys. Ther. 2015, 95, 740–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Cha, J.Y.; Min, S.K.; Yoon, T.H.; Jee, Y.S. Gross Motor Function and Health Fitness in Adults with Autistic Spectrum Disorder and Intellectual Disability: Single-Blind Retrospective Trial. J. Exerc. Rehabil. 2020, 16, 258–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Olusanya, B.O.; Wright, S.M.; Smythe, T.; Khetani, M.A.; Moreno-Angarita, M.; Gulati, S.; Brinkman, S.A.; Almasri, N.A.; Figueiredo, M.; Giudici, L.B.; et al. Early Childhood Development Strategy for the World’s Children with Disabilities. Front. Public Health 2024, 12, 1390107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. World Medical Association. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. JAMA 2013, 310, 2191–2194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Vandenbroucke, J.P.; von Elm, E.; Altman, D.G.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Pocock, S.J.; Poole, C.; Schlesselman, J.J.; Egger, M.; STROBE Initiative. Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE): Explanation and Elaboration. PLoS Med. 2007, 4, e297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Newborg, J. Battelle Developmental Inventory: Second Edition; Riverside Publishing: Itasca, IL, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  33. Sipes, M.; Matson, J.L.; Turygin, N. The Use of the Battelle Developmental Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-2) as an Early Screener for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Dev. Neurorehabil. 2011, 14, 310–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Vanderbilt, D.L.; Schrager, S.M.; Llanes, M.A.; Hamilton, A.; Seri, I.; Chmait, R.H. Predictors of Two-Year Cognitive Performance after Laser Surgery for Twin-Twin Transfusion Syndrome. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 2014, 211, 388.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Moraleda-Barreno, E.; Romero-López, M.; Cayetano-Menéndez, M.J. La prueba de cribado del inventario de desarrollo de Battelle para la detección precoz de alteraciones del desarrollo en parálisis cerebral. An. Pediatr. 2011, 75, 372–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Alfonso, V.C.; Rentz, E.A.; Chung, S. Review of the Battelle Developmental Inventory, Second Edition. J. Early Child Infant. Psychol. 2010, 6, 21–40. [Google Scholar]
  37. Shevell, M.; Majnemer, A.; Platt, R.W.; Webster, R.; Birnbaum, R. Developmental and Functional Outcomes in Children with Global Developmental Delay or Developmental Language Impairment. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2005, 47, 678–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Goldin, R.L.; Matson, J.L.; Beighley, J.S.; Jang, J. Autism Spectrum Disorder Severity as a Predictor of Battelle Developmental Inventory—Second Edition (BDI-2) Scores in Toddlers. Dev. Neurorehabil. 2014, 17, 39–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Horovitz, M.; Matson, J.L. Developmental Milestones in Toddlers with Atypical Development. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 2278–2282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Hattier, M.A.; Matson, J.L.; Sipes, M.; Turygin, N. Communication Deficits in Infants and Toddlers with Developmental Disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 2108–2113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Shan, L.; Feng, J.Y.; Wang, T.T.; Xu, Z.D.; Jia, F.Y. Prevalence and Developmental Profiles of Autism Spectrum Disorders in Children with Global Developmental Delay. Front. Psychiatry 2022, 12, 794238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Taddei, M.; Bulgheroni, S.; Toffalini, E.; Pantaleoni, C.; Lanfranchi, S. Developmental Profiles of Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Global Developmental Delay: A Study with the Griffiths III Scales. Autism Res. 2023, 16, 1344–1359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Liao, L.-H.; Chen, C.; Peng, J.; Wu, L.-W.; He, F.; Yang, L.-F.; Zhang, C.-L.; Wang, G.-L.; Peng, P.; Ma, Y.-P.; et al. Diagnosis of Intellectual Disability/Global Developmental Delay via Genetic Analysis in a Central Region of China. Chin. Med. J. 2019, 132, 1533–1540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Masri, A.T.; Oweis, L.; Ali, M.; Hamamy, H. Global Developmental Delay and Intellectual Disability in the Era of Genomics: Diagnosis and Challenges in Resource Limited Areas. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 2023, 230, 107799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Wu, D.; Li, R. Genetic Analysis of Neurodevelopmental Disorders in Children. Front. Child. Adolesc. Psychiatry 2022, 1, 987339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Zhang, J.; Xu, Y.; Liu, Y.; Yue, L.; Jin, H.; Chen, Y.; Wang, D.; Wang, M.; Chen, G.; Yang, L.; et al. Genetic Testing for Global Developmental Delay in Early Childhood. JAMA Netw. Open 2024, 7, e2415084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Ferreira-Vasques, A.T.; Lamônica, D.A.C. Motor, Linguistic, Personal and Social Aspects of Children with Down Syndrome. J. Appl. Oral. Sci. 2015, 23, 424–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Will, E.A.; Roberts, J.E. Motor Influences on Communication: Comparisons between Down Syndrome and Fragile X Syndrome. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2021, 126, 460–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Jain, P.D.; Nayak, A.; Karnad, S.D.; Doctor, K.N. Gross Motor Dysfunction and Balance Impairments in Children and Adolescents with Down Syndrome: A Systematic Review. Clin. Exp. Pediatr. 2022, 65, 142–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Galli, M.; Rigoldi, C.; Brunner, R.; Virji-Babul, N.; Giorgio, A. Joint Stiffness and Gait Pattern Evaluation in Children with Down Syndrome. Gait Posture 2008, 28, 502–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Padia, N.; Bose, M.; Parab, S. Determinants of Hand Function in Children and Adolescent with Down Syndrome-A Scoping Review. J. Hand Ther. 2023, 36, 622–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Mahoney, G.; Robinson, C.; Fewell, R.R. The Effects of Early Motor Intervention on Children with Down Syndrome or Cerebral Palsy: A Field-Based Study: J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2001, 22, 153–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Spanò, M.; Mercuri, E.; Randò, T.; Pantò, T.; Gagliano, A.; Henderson, S.; Guzetta, F. Motor and Perceptual–Motor Competence in Children with Down Syndrome: Variation in Performance with Age. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 1999, 3, 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Martin, G.E.; Klusek, J.; Estigarribia, B.; Roberts, J.E. Language Characteristics of Individuals with Down Syndrome. Top. Lang. Disord. 2009, 29, 112–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Lipkin, P.H.; Macias, M.M.; Council on Children with Disabilities, Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics; Norwood, K.W.; Brei, T.J.; Davidson, L.F.; Davis, B.E.; Ellerbeck, K.A.; Houtrow, A.J.; Hyman, S.L.; et al. Promoting Optimal Development: Identifying Infants and Young Children with Developmental Disorders Through Developmental Surveillance and Screening. Pediatrics 2020, 145, e20193449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Del Hoyo Soriano, L.; Villarreal, J.C.; Sterling, A.; Edgin, J.; Berry-Kravis, E.; Hamilton, D.R.; Thurman, A.J.; Abbeduto, L. The Association between Expressive Language Skills and Adaptive Behavior in Individuals with Down Syndrome. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 20014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Hendrix, J.A.; Amon, A.; Abbeduto, L.; Agiovlasitis, S.; Alsaied, T.; Anderson, H.A.; Bain, L.J.; Baumer, N.; Bhattacharyya, A.; Bogunovic, D.; et al. Opportunities, Barriers, and Recommendations in down Syndrome Research. Transl. Sci. Rare Dis. 2021, 5, 99–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Kent, R.D.; Vorperian, H.K. Speech Impairment in Down Syndrome: A Review. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2013, 56, 178–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Hashimoto, M.; Igari, K.; Hanawa, S.; Ito, A.; Takahashi, A.; Ishida, N.; Koyama, S.; Ono, T.; Sasaki, K. Tongue Pressure during Swallowing in Adults with down Syndrome and Its Relationship with Palatal Morphology. Dysphagia 2014, 29, 509–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Onnivello, S.; Pulina, F.; Locatelli, C.; Marcolin, C.; Ramacieri, G.; Antonaros, F.; Vione, B.; Caracausi, M.; Lanfranchi, S. Cognitive Profiles in Children and Adolescents with Down Syndrome. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 1936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Tsao, R.; Kindelberger, C. Variability of Cognitive Development in Children with Down Syndrome: Relevance of Good Reasons for Using the Cluster Procedure. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2009, 30, 426–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Godfrey, M.; Lee, N.R. A Comprehensive Examination of the Memory Profile of Youth with Down Syndrome in Comparison to Typically Developing Peers. Child. Neuropsychol. 2020, 26, 721–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Tungate, A.S.; Conners, F.A. Executive Function in Down Syndrome: A Meta-Analysis. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2021, 108, 103802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Daunhauer, L.A.; Fidler, D.J. The Down Syndrome Behavioral Phenotype: Implications for Practice and Research in Occupational Therapy. Occup. Ther. Health Care 2011, 25, 7–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Patterson, T.; Rapsey, C.M.; Glue, P. Systematic Review of Cognitive Development across Childhood in Down Syndrome: Implications for Treatment Interventions. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2013, 57, 306–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Duan, X.; Li, M. The Effect of Exercise on Improving Cognitive Function in People with Down’s Syndrome: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Eur. J. Pediatr. 2025, 184, 349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Flores Camas, R.A.; Leon-Rojas, J.E. Specific Language Impairment and Executive Functions in School-Age Children: A Systematic Review. Cureus 2023, 15, e43163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Hick, R.; Botting, N.; Conti-Ramsden, G. Cognitive Abilities in Children with Specific Language Impairment: Consideration of Visuo-spatial Skills. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2005, 40, 137–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Reichenbach, K.; Bastian, L.; Rohrbach, S.; Gross, M.; Sarrar, L. Cognitive Functions in Preschool Children with Specific Language Impairment. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2016, 86, 22–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Vugs, B.; Hendriks, M.; Cuperus, J.; Knoors, H.; Verhoeven, L. Developmental Associations Between Working Memory and Language in Children with Specific Language Impairment: A Longitudinal Study. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2017, 60, 3284–3294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Graf Estes, K.; Evans, J.L.; Else-Quest, N.M. Differences in the Nonword Repetition Performance of Children with and Without Specific Language Impairment: A Meta-Analysis. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2007, 50, 177–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Pauls, L.J.; Archibald, L.M.D. Executive Functions in Children with Specific Language Impairment: A Meta-Analysis. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2016, 59, 1074–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Bishop, D.V.M. What Causes Specific Language Impairment in Children? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 15, 217–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Tomas, E.; Vissers, C. Behind the Scenes of Developmental Language Disorder: Time to Call Neuropsychology Back on Stage. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019, 12, 517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Rice, M.L. Causal Pathways for Specific Language Impairment: Lessons from Studies of Twins. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2020, 63, 3224–3235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Lloyd-Esenkaya, V.; Russell, A.J.; Clair, M.C.S. What Are the Peer Interaction Strengths and Difficulties in Children with Developmental Language Disorder? A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Vissers, C.; Koolen, S. Theory of Mind Deficits and Social Emotional Functioning in Preschoolers with Specific Language Impairment. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Rivero, M.; Vilaseca, R.; Cantero, M.-J.; Valls-Vidal, C.; Leiva, D. Relations between Positive Parenting Behavior during Play and Child Language Development at Early Ages. Children 2023, 10, 505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Landry, S.H.; Smith, K.E.; Swank, P.R.; Guttentag, C. A Responsive Parenting Intervention: The Optimal Timing across Early Childhood for Impacting Maternal Behaviors and Child Outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 44, 1335–1353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Næss, K.-A.B.; Nygaard, E.; Ostad, J.; Dolva, A.-S.; Lyster, S.-A.H. The Profile of Social Functioning in Children with Down Syndrome. Disabil. Rehabil. 2017, 39, 1320–1331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Will, E.A.; Caravella, K.E.; Hahn, L.J.; Fidler, D.J.; Roberts, J.E. Adaptive Behavior in Infants and Toddlers with Down Syndrome and Fragile X Syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2018, 177, 358–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Valera-Pozo, M.; Adrover-Roig, D.; Pérez-Castelló, J.A.; Sanchez-Azanza, V.A.; Aguilar-Mediavilla, E. Behavioral, Emotional and School Adjustment in Adolescents with and without Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) Is Related to Family Involvement. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Washington, K.N.; Thomas-Stonell, N.; McLeod, S.; Warr-Leeper, G. Outcomes and Predictors in Preschoolers with Speech-Language and/or Developmental Mobility Impairments. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 2015, 31, 141–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Pérez-Pereira, M.; Fernández, M.P.; Gómez-Taibo, M.L.; Martínez-López, Z.; Arce, C. A Follow-Up Study of Cognitive Development in Low Risk Preterm Children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Global Research on Developmental Disabilities Collaborators Developmental Disabilities among Children Younger than 5 Years in 195 Countries and Territories, 1990-2016: A Systematic Analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Glob. Health 2018, 6, e1100–e1121. [CrossRef]
  86. Rustom, H.; Hassan Eltorki, Y.; Adil Shah Khoodoruth, M.; Abdallah, O.; Al-Khuzaei, N.; Iqbal, N.; Alabdulla, N. Genetic Etiology of Adult Intellectual Disability (ID) of Unknown Cause in Qatar: A Retrospective Study. Qatar Med. J. 2022, 2022, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Zablotsky, B.; Black, L.I.; Maenner, M.J.; Schieve, L.A.; Danielson, M.L.; Bitsko, R.H.; Blumberg, S.J.; Kogan, M.D.; Boyle, C.A. Prevalence and Trends of Developmental Disabilities among Children in the US: 2009–2017. Pediatrics 2019, 144, e20190811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Antonarakis, S.E.; Skotko, B.G.; Rafii, M.S.; Strydom, A.; Pape, S.E.; Bianchi, D.W.; Sherman, S.L.; Reeves, R.H. Down Syndrome. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 2020, 6, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Reilly, S.; Tomblin, B.; Law, J.; McKean, C.; Mensah, F.K.; Morgan, A.; Goldfeld, S.; Nicholson, J.M.; Wake, M. Specific Language Impairment: A Convenient Label for Whom? Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2014, 49, 416–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. McGoey, K.E.; McCobin, A.; Venesky, L.G. Early Childhood Assessment for Diverse Learners. In Psychoeducational Assessment and Intervention for Ethnic Minority Children: Evidence-Based Approaches; Graves, S.L., Blake, J.J., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2016; pp. 115–131. ISBN 978-1-4338-2174-5. [Google Scholar]
  91. Troxel, M.; Sheldrick, R.C.; Eisenhower, A.; Carter, A.S. Patterns and Correlates of Developmental Profiles Using the Battelle Developmental Inventory Among Children in Early Intervention. J. Early Interv. 2024, 46, 544–561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow diagram of participants. EIC: Early Intervention Center; BDI: Battelle Developmental Inventory, 2nd Edition; DS: Down syndrome; GDD: global developmental delay; DLD: Developmental Language Disorder.
Figure 1. Flow diagram of participants. EIC: Early Intervention Center; BDI: Battelle Developmental Inventory, 2nd Edition; DS: Down syndrome; GDD: global developmental delay; DLD: Developmental Language Disorder.
Healthcare 13 01684 g001
Figure 2. Results of the post hoc pairwise comparisons of the BDI−2 global DQ and specific-domain DQs: (a) global DQ; (b) personal–social domain DQ; (c) adaptative domain DQ; (d) motor domain DQ; (e) communication domain DQ; (f) cognitive domain DQ.
Figure 2. Results of the post hoc pairwise comparisons of the BDI−2 global DQ and specific-domain DQs: (a) global DQ; (b) personal–social domain DQ; (c) adaptative domain DQ; (d) motor domain DQ; (e) communication domain DQ; (f) cognitive domain DQ.
Healthcare 13 01684 g002
Table 1. Sample characteristics by group.
Table 1. Sample characteristics by group.
DS Group
N = 22
GDD Group
n = 17
DLD Group
n = 7
p ValueES
Age (months)44.1 ± 5.540.6 ± 6.741.4 ± 5.10.1830.076
Sex:
Men
Women

13 (59.1)
9 (40.9)

12 (70.6)
5 (29.4)

7 (100)
0 (0)
0.0460.290
Ethnicity:
Caucasian

22 (100)

17 (100)

7 (100)

1

-
Table 2. Comparisons of the BDI−2 global and domain-specific developmental quotients (DQ) among groups.
Table 2. Comparisons of the BDI−2 global and domain-specific developmental quotients (DQ) among groups.
DS Group
n = 22
GDD Group
n = 17
DLD Group
n = 7
Between-Group Differences
F(2, 43)p ValueES
Global DQ54.74 ± 8.6584.25 ± 16.0965.49 ± 16.3324.616<0.0010.534
Domain-specific DQs:
Personal-social DQ52.32 ± 9.4474.66 ± 15.1252.12 ± 15.6916.472<0.0010.434
Adaptive DQ55.51 ± 15.3776.92 ± 17.4256.67 ± 19.928.4410.0010.282
Motor DQ56.82 ± 11.1784.10 ± 14.1573.40 ± 11.7923.432<0.0010.521
Communication DQ43.21 ± 7.2285.87 ± 20.1767.57 ± 17.0540.179<0.0010.651
Cognitive DQ62.48 ± 13.90100.36 ± 17.5279.98 ± 27.1222.116<0.0010.507
DQ: Developmental quotient; DS: Down Syndrome; GDD: global developmental delay; DLD: Developmental Language Disorder; ES: partial η2 effect size statistics (ηp2 = 0.01, small effect; ηp2 = 0.06, medium effect; ηp2 = ≥ 0.14, large effect).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ahulló-Fuster, M.-A.; Sánchez-Sánchez, M.L.; Monterrubio-Gordón, A.; Ruescas-Nicolau, M.-A. A Comparison of Developmental Profiles of Preschool Children with Down Syndrome, Global Developmental Delay, and Developmental Language Disorder. Healthcare 2025, 13, 1684. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare13141684

AMA Style

Ahulló-Fuster M-A, Sánchez-Sánchez ML, Monterrubio-Gordón A, Ruescas-Nicolau M-A. A Comparison of Developmental Profiles of Preschool Children with Down Syndrome, Global Developmental Delay, and Developmental Language Disorder. Healthcare. 2025; 13(14):1684. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare13141684

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ahulló-Fuster, Mónica-Alba, M. Luz Sánchez-Sánchez, Alejandro Monterrubio-Gordón, and Maria-Arantzazu Ruescas-Nicolau. 2025. "A Comparison of Developmental Profiles of Preschool Children with Down Syndrome, Global Developmental Delay, and Developmental Language Disorder" Healthcare 13, no. 14: 1684. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare13141684

APA Style

Ahulló-Fuster, M.-A., Sánchez-Sánchez, M. L., Monterrubio-Gordón, A., & Ruescas-Nicolau, M.-A. (2025). A Comparison of Developmental Profiles of Preschool Children with Down Syndrome, Global Developmental Delay, and Developmental Language Disorder. Healthcare, 13(14), 1684. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare13141684

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop