Self-Organized Criticality and Cognitive Control Reasoned by Effort Minimization
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Model
2.1. External-Observer Perspective
- ○
- Without the subject’s actions, the system exhibits overdamped instability governed by the equation , where is the time scale of the instability development.
- ○
- If the subject’s perception were perfect, than the subject’s response to the system deviation x could be described in terms of the following:
- (i)
- Some additional “force” acting on the system and directed towards the equilibrium ;
- (ii)
- The subject’s choice of the coefficient such that the system dynamics
The value of can be interpreted as the ratio of the time scale characterizing the subject’s reaction intensity that keeps the system at the instability boundary and the time scale of the current reaction intensity, . As it must, for , the coefficient , and the shorter the reaction time , the more stable the system motion. - ○
- The bounded capacity of the subject’s perception in monitoring the system dynamics and the resulting response is taken into account via introducing the “force” , where we have the following:
- (iii)
- The functional form of coincides with that of the perfect response (item i).
- (iv)
- However, the value of the coefficient , the control variable, cannot be recognized by the subject precisely nor controlled perfectly.
- ○
- Under normal conditions, i.e., when the subject’s control is not close to failure and no urgent actions are necessary, the time scale characterizing the current subject’s reaction should be about the reaction time at the instability onset, . So the temporal scale is used below as the time unit. In these terms, the equation governing the system dynamics under the subject’s control (specified in item ii) is reduced to the dimensionless form (1), and we obtain the relations
2.2. First-Person Perspective
- –
- The scale-free properties of human perception uncertainty underpinning Weber’s law (Section 2.2.1);
- –
- The dependence of effort in monitoring and controlling the system dynamics on action strategies (Section 2.2.1);
- –
- The multi-channel functioning of sensory modalities (Section 2.2.2);
- –
- The bounded capacity of human cognition making the precise control over the dynamics of controlled systems impossible (Section 2.2.3 and Section 2.2.4).
2.2.1. Uncertainty in Subject’s Perception of System States
- –
- The sensory perception of the quantities x and , as well as the rates , of their variations in time;
- –
- The mental estimation of the values of x, , , and with respect to the acceptability of the current system state.
- The subject follows the alternative strategy in controlling the system dynamics;
- The mental estimation of system states plays the leading role in the subject’s control, and the sensory perception is well balanced with the mental estimation.
2.2.2. Fine-Control Mode and Two Types of Strategies: Subject’s Perfect Perception Limit
2.2.3. Critical-Control Mode: Subject’s Perfect Perception
2.2.4. Concept of Dynamical Traps and the Governing Equation of Subject’s Actions
2.2.5. Summary: The Governing Equation of Subject’s Actions (-Dynamics)
- –
- The left-hand term , the stimuli for the subject’s active behavior quantified by , , and actually meet Weber’s law reflecting the scale-free properties of human perception;
- –
- The last two terms on the right-hand side just represent the two channels of the visual modality, processing separately sustained and transient information;
- –
- The dynamical trap function describes the subject’s intermittent behavior, in particular, the control stagnation when the subject is not able to recognize how the current state should be changed;
- –
- The first term on the right-hand side is due to the effort minimization when the subject’s active behavior is not necessary;
- –
- The introduced noise, the second term, allows for uncontrollable factors that are outside of the subject’s cognition.
3. Results of Numerical Simulation and Discussion
3.1. The Used Model Parameters
3.2. The Case of Fine-Control Dominance
3.3. The Case of Critical-Control Dominance
4. Conclusions
- –
- The scale-free properties of human perception of external stimuli underpinning, in particular, Weber’s law;
- –
- The multi-channel structure of sensory modalities processing the sustained and transient components of sensory information separately;
- –
- The bounded capacity of human cognition that gives rise to intermittent control and is responsible for the emergence of a certain region in the extended phase space, where the subject’s active behavior is stagnated;
- –
- The minimization of effort when the subject’s active behavior is not necessary;
- –
- The delay in human reaction taken into account via the introduction of the extended phase space consisting of (i) phase variables describing the states of a physical system under human control and (ii) phase variables describing the internal states of the subject.
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | It should be noted that there are various numerical criteria different in their principles as well as particular details that enable one to categorize observed phenomena as the self-organized criticality. For their review and sophisticated discussion, a reader may be referred to Ref. [40]. |
References
- Bak, P.; Tang, C.; Wiesenfeld, K. Self-Organized Criticality: An Explanation of 1/f Noise. Phys. Rev. Lett. 1987, 59, 381–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marković, D.; Gros, C. Power laws and self-organized criticality in theory and nature. Phys. Rep. 2014, 536, 41–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watkins, N.W.; Pruessner, G.; Chapman, S.C.; Crosby, N.B.; Jensen, H.J. 25 Years of Self-organized Criticality: Concepts and Controversies. Space Sci. Rev. 2016, 198, 3–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muñoz, M.A. Colloquium: Criticality and dynamical scaling in living systems. Rev. Mod. Phys. 2018, 90, 031001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tadić, B.; Melnik, R. Self-Organised Critical Dynamics as a Key to Fundamental Features of Complexity in Physical, Biological, and Social Networks. Dynamics 2021, 1, 181–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bonachela, J.A.; Muñoz, M.A. Self-organization without conservation: True or just apparent scale-invariance? J. Stat. Mech. Theory Exp. 2009, 2009, P09009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sornette, D. Sweeping of an instability: An alternative to self-organized criticality to get powerlaws without parameter tuning. J. Phys. I 1994, 4, 209–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sornette, D. Critical Phenomena in Natural Sciences: Chaos, Fractals, Selforganization and Disorder: Concepts and Tools; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kinouchi, O.; Brochini, L.; Costa, A.A.; Campos, J.G.F.; Copelli, M. Stochastic oscillations and dragon king avalanches in self-organized quasi-critical systems. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 3874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramos, R.T.; Sassi, R.B.; Piqueira, J.R.C. Self-organized criticality and the predictability of human behavior. New Ideas Psychol. 2011, 29, 38–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karsai, M.; Jo, H.H.; Kaski, K. Bursty Human Dynamics; Springer International Publishing AG: Cham, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cabrera, J.L.; Milton, J.G. On-Off Intermittency in a Human Balancing Task. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2002, 89, 158702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cabrera, J.L.; Bormann, R.; Eurich, C.; Ohira, T.; Milton, J. State-Dependent Noise and Human Balance Control. Fluct. Noise Lett. 2004, 4, L107–L117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cabrera, J.L.; Milton, J.G. Human stick balancing: Tuning Lèvy flights to improve balance control. Chaos Interdiscip. J. Nonlinear Sci. 2004, 14, 691–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nagatani, T. Power-Law Distribution and 1/f-Noise of Waiting Time near Traffic-Jam Threshold. J. Phys. Soc. Jpn. 1993, 62, 2533–2536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laval, J.A. Self-organized criticality of traffic flow: Implications for congestion management technologies. Transp. Res. Part C Emerg. Technol. 2023, 149, 104056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kerner, B.S. Introduction to Modern Traffic Flow Theory and Control: The Long Road to Three-Phase Traffic Theory; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lubashevsky, I.; Morimura, K. Physics of Mind and Car-Following Problem. In Complex Dynamics of Traffic Management, Encyclopedia of Complexity and Systems Science Series; Kerner, B.S., Ed.; Springer Science+Business Media, LLC: New York, NY, USA, 2019; pp. 559–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Insperger, T.; Milton, J. Delay and Uncertainty in Human Balancing Tasks; Springer Nature Switzerland AG: Cham, Switzerland, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suzuki, Y.; Morimoto, H.; Kiyono, K.; Morasso, P.G.; Nomura, T. Dynamic Determinants of the Uncontrolled Manifold during Human Quiet Stance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lubashevsky, I.; Plavinska, N. Physics of the Human Temporality: Complex Present; Understanding Complex Systems; Springer International Publishing AG: Cham, Switzerland, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Helbing, D. Traffic and related self-driven many-particle systems. Rev. Mod. Phys. 2001, 73, 1067–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landau, L.D.; Lifshitz, E.M. Mechanics, 3rd ed.; Course of Theoretical Physics; Elsevier Butterworth-Heinemann: Burlington, MA, USA, 1976; Volume 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patzelt, F.; Pawelzik, K. Criticality of Adaptive Control Dynamics. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2011, 107, 238103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gescheider, G.A. Psychophysics: The Fundamentals, 3rd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Milton, J.; Meyer, R.; Zhvanetsky, M.; Ridge, S.; Insperger, T. Control at stability’s edge minimizes energetic costs: Expert stick balancing. J. R. Soc. Interface 2016, 13, 20160212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fu, C.; Suzuki, Y.; Morasso, P.; Nomura, T. Phase resetting and intermittent control at the edge of stability in a simple biped model generates 1/f-like gait cycle variability. Biol. Cybern. 2020, 114, 95–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lubashevsky, I. Psychophysical laws as reflection of mental space properties. Phys. Life Rev. 2019, 31, 276–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cowan, N. Sensational Memorability: Working Memory for Things We See, Hear, Feel, or Somehow Sense. In Mechanisms of Sensory Working Memory: Attention and Performance XXV; Jolicoeur, P., Lefebvre, C., Martinez-Trujillo, J., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2015; pp. 5–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Teghtsoonian, R. Range Effects in Psychophysical Scaling and a Revision of Stevens’ Law. Am. J. Psychol. 1973, 86, 3–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stigliani, A.; Jeska, B.; Grill-Spector, K. Differential sustained and transient temporal processing across visual streams. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2019, 15, e1007011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wickelgren, W.A. Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics. Acta Psychol. 1977, 41, 67–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cross, D.V. Sequential dependencies and regression in psychophysical judgments. Percept. Psychophys. 1973, 14, 547–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loram, I.D.; Gollee, H.; Lakie, M.; Gawthrop, P.J. Human control of an inverted pendulum: Is continuous control necessary? Is intermittent control effective? Is intermittent control physiological? J. Physiol. 2011, 589, 307–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lubashevsky, I. Human Fuzzy Rationality as a Novel Mechanism of Emergent Phenomena. In Handbook of Applications of Chaos Theory; Skiadas, C.H., Skiadas, C., Eds.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA; Taylor & Francis Group: London, UK, 2016; pp. 827–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zgonnikov, A.; Lubashevsky, I.; Kanemoto, S.; Miyazawa, T.; Suzuki, T. To react or not to react? Intrinsic stochasticity of human control in virtual stick balancing. J. R. Soc. Interface 2014, 11, 20140636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lubashevsky, I. Physics of the Human Mind; Springer International Publishing AG: Cham, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gardiner, C.W. Handbook of Stochastic Methods: For Physics, Chemistry and the Natural Sciences, 3rd ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Rößler, A. Runge-Kutta Methods for the Strong Approximation of Solutions of Stochastic Differential Equations. SIAM J. Numer. Anal. 2010, 48, 922–952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McAteer, R.T.J.; Aschwanden, M.J.; Dimitropoulou, M.; Georgoulis, M.K.; Pruessner, G.; Morales, L.; Ireland, J.; Abramenko, V. 25 Years of Self-organized Criticality: Numerical Detection Methods. Space Sci. Rev. 2015, 198, 217–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lubashevskiy, V.; Lubashevsky, I. Self-Organized Criticality and Cognitive Control Reasoned by Effort Minimization. Systems 2023, 11, 271. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems11060271
Lubashevskiy V, Lubashevsky I. Self-Organized Criticality and Cognitive Control Reasoned by Effort Minimization. Systems. 2023; 11(6):271. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems11060271
Chicago/Turabian StyleLubashevskiy, Vasily, and Ihor Lubashevsky. 2023. "Self-Organized Criticality and Cognitive Control Reasoned by Effort Minimization" Systems 11, no. 6: 271. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems11060271
APA StyleLubashevskiy, V., & Lubashevsky, I. (2023). Self-Organized Criticality and Cognitive Control Reasoned by Effort Minimization. Systems, 11(6), 271. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems11060271