Intra-Individual Variability from a Lifespan Perspective: A Comparison of Latency and Accuracy Measures
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials and Procedure
2.3. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Reaction Times Tasks
3.1.1. Intra-Individual Mean (iM)
- Simple RT task. The main effect of age was significant, F(2, 554) = 74.78, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.21. Children were significantly slower than older adults (p < 0.001), themselves being significantly slower than younger adults (p < 0.001). Furthermore, children were significantly slower than younger adults (p < 0.001).
- Choice RT tasks. For the LI and CC tasks, the main effect of age was significant, F(2, 554) = 257.23, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.48 and F(2, 554) = 175.42, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.39 respectively. For both tasks, children were significantly slower than older adults (p < 0.001), themselves being significantly slower than younger adults (p < 0.001). Furthermore, children were significantly slower than younger adults (p < 0.001).
- Processing speed tasks. For the DI task, the main effect of age was significant, F(2, 554) = 170.97, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.38. Children were significantly slower than younger adults (p < 0.001), themselves significantly faster than older adults (p < 0.001). Children did not differ significantly from older adults. For the LC task, the main effect of age was significant, F(2, 552) = 161.99, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.37. Children were significantly slower than older adults (p < 0.001), the latter being significantly slower than younger adults (p < 0.001). Furthermore, children were significantly slower than younger adults (p < 0.001). A main effect of condition was also observed, F(1, 552) = 2067.01, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.79. Participants were significantly faster in the 6 letters condition than in the 9 letters condition. Finally, the age group x condition interaction was significant, F(2, 552) = 26.54, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.09. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that the age group effect was significant for all conditions (all ps < 0.001). Moreover, the main effect of condition was significant for all age groups (all ps < 0.001); the interaction reflects a more pronounced effect for children and older adults than for younger adults.
- Stroop task. The main effect of age was significant, F(2, 548) = 164.56, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.38. Children were significantly slower than older adults (p < 0.001), themselves being significantly slower than younger adults (p < 0.001). Furthermore, children were significantly slower than younger adults (p < 0.001). A main effect of condition was also observed F(1, 548) = 2721.60, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.83. Participants were significantly faster in the neutral condition compared to incongruent condition. Finally, the age group x condition interaction was significant, F(2, 548) = 44.96, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.14. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that the age group effect was significant for all conditions (all ps < 0.001). The main effect of condition was significant for all age groups (all ps < 0.001); this effect seemed more pronounced for children and older adults than for the younger adults, as shown by a significant interaction effect.
3.1.2. Intra-Individual Standard Deviation of Residual Scores (iSDr)
- Simple RT task. The main effect of age was significant F(2, 554) = 99.18, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.26. Children were significantly more variable than older adults (p < 0.001), who were significantly more variable than younger adults (p < 0.001). Furthermore, children were significantly more variable than younger adults (p < 0.001).
- Choice RT tasks. For the LI and CC task, the main effect of age was significant, F(2, 554) = 180.83, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.40 and F(2, 554) = 190.26, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.41 respectively. Children were significantly more variable than older adults (p < 0.001), themselves being significantly more variable than younger adults (p < 0.001). Furthermore, children were significantly more variable than younger adults (p < 0.001).
- Processing speed tasks. For the DI task, the main effect of age was significant, F(2, 554) = 215.70, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.44. Children were significantly more variable than older adults (p < 0.001), themselves being significantly more variable than younger adults (p < 0.001). Furthermore, children were significantly more variable than younger adults (p < 0.001). For the LC task, the main effect of age was significant, F(2, 552) = 211.35, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.43. Children were significantly more variable than older adults (p < 0.001), themselves being significantly more variable than younger adults (p < 0.001). Furthermore, children were significantly more variable than younger adults (p < 0.001). A main effect of condition was also obtained, F(1, 552) = 29.06, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.05. Participants were significantly less variable in the 6 letters condition than in the 9 letters condition. Finally, the age group x condition interaction was significant, F(2, 552) = 5.42, p < 0.005, η2 = 0.02. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that the age group effect was significant for all conditions (all ps < 0.001). Moreover the main effect of condition was significant for younger adults and older adults (all ps < 0.001), but not for children.
- Stroop task. The main effect of age was significant, F(2, 548) = 251.85, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.48. Children were significantly more variable than older adults (p < 0.001), themselves significantly more variable than younger adults (p < 0.001). Furthermore, children were significantly more variable than younger adults (p < 0.001). A main effect of condition was also significant, F(1, 548) = 32.23, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.06. Participants were significantly less variable in the neutral condition than in the incongruent condition. Finally, the age group x condition interaction was significant, F(2, 548) = 32.23, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.06. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that the age group effect was significant for all conditions (all ps < 0.001). Moreover, the main effect of condition was significant for all age groups (all ps < 0.001), this effect seemed more pronounced for older adults, leading to a significant age group x condition interaction.
3.2. Working Memory Tasks
3.2.1. Intra-Individual Mean (iM)
- Reading span task. The analysis of the mean number of correctly recalled words indicated a main effect of age group, F(2, 511) = 57.31, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.18. Children recalled significantly fewer words than older adults (p < 0.001), themselves recalling significantly fewer words than younger adults (p < 0.001). Furthermore, children recalled significantly less words than younger adults (p < 0.001). A main effect of list length was also found, F(1, 511) = 504.61, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.50. Participants recalled significantly fewer words for the n list than for the n + 1 list. Finally, the age group x list length interaction was significant, F(2, 511) = 9.23, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.04. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that the age effect was significant for both n and n + 1 lists (p < 0.001). Moreover, the list length effect was significant for all age groups (p < 0.001); this effect seemed more pronounced for younger adults than for children and older adults, leading to a significant age group x list length interaction.
- Matrices task—Simple positions. The main effect of age group was significant, F(2, 550) = 138.53, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.34. Children recalled significantly fewer positions than younger adults (p < 0.001). Older adults recalled significantly fewer positions than younger adults (p < 0.001). Children did not differ significantly from older adults. A main effect of list length was also obtained, F(1, 550) = 992.25, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.64. Participants recalled significantly fewer positions for the n list than for the n + 1 list. Finally, the age group x list length interaction was significant, F(2, 550) = 15.31, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.05. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that the main age effect was significant for both n and n + 1 lists. Children recalled significantly fewer positions than younger adults and older adults recalled significantly fewer positions than younger adults; p < 0.001. Moreover, the list length effect was significant for all age groups (p < 0.001); this effect seemed more pronounced for younger adults (cf. significant age x condition interaction).
- Matrices task—word-position associations. Results indicated only a main effect of age, F(2, 548) = 121.68, p < 0.000, ηp2 = 0.31. Children recalled significantly fewer associations than older adults (p < 0.006), themselves recalling significantly fewer associations than younger adults (p < 0.000). Furthermore, children recalled significantly fewer associations than younger adults (p < 0.000). The effects of list length and its interaction with age were not significant.
3.2.2. Intra-Individual Standard Deviation of Residual Scores (iSDr)
- Reading span task. Results indicated only a main effect of list length, F(1, 511) = 23.57, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.04. Participants were significantly less variable for the n list than for the n + 1 list. Neither the effects of age group nor its interaction with list length were significant.
- Matrices task—Simple positions. Neither effects of age and list length nor their interactions were significant.
- Matrices Task—word-position associations. Results indicated only a main effect of age, F(2, 548) = 29.47, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.10. Children were significantly less variable than younger adults (p < 0.001). Older adults were significantly less variable than younger adults (p < 0.001). Children did not differ significantly from older adults.
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Age Groups | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Children | Adults | |||||||||
Young | Old | Young | Young-Old | Old-Old | ||||||
N | 100 | 101 | 137 | 117 | 102 | |||||
Female | 38 | 54 | 117 | 90 | 75 | |||||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
Age | 9.5 | 0.50 | 11.50 | 0.50 | 21.71 | 2.53 | 64.82 | 2.68 | 76.15 | 4.65 |
Vocabulary Score | - | - | - | - | 34.67 | 3.25 | 38.04 | 4.38 | 37.37 | 4.91 |
Raven | 34.14 | 8.39 | 39.51 | 7.11 | 52.15 | 4.91 | 39.53 | 7.54 | 32.90 | 9.54 |
Children | Young Adults | Older Adults | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Rspan task Span level | Min | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Max | 5 | 6 | 6 | |
M | 2.39 | 3.15 | 2.85 | |
SD | 0.63 | 0.90 | 0.90 | |
Matrices task Span level | Min | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Max | 7 | 7 | 7 | |
M | 3.4 | 5.71 | 3.91 | |
SD | 1.45 | 1.45 | 1.23 |
Age Groups | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Children | Adults | |||||||||||
Young | Old | Young | Young-Old | Old-Old | ||||||||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |||
iM | SRT | 381.92 | 74.88 | 338.35 | 54.38 | 272.89 | 39.16 | 324.42 | 76.48 | 343.91 | 72.95 | |
LI | 605.34 | 97.71 | 525.81 | 88.85 | 372.38 | 43.53 | 446.26 | 64.32 | 470.75 | 75.10 | ||
CC | 536.80 | 106.33 | 454.75 | 87.46 | 325.69 | 39.26 | 417.52 | 68.44 | 446.66 | 82.86 | ||
DI | 1924.67 | 396.48 | 1551.83 | 419.32 | 1040.56 | 221.47 | 1569.50 | 302.43 | 1783.05 | 372.95 | ||
LC | 6 letters | 4038.80 | 1323.61 | 3213.18 | 385.40 | 1838.17 | 423.31 | 2771.18 | 624.07 | 3200.73 | 912.54 | |
9 letters | 5493.01 | 1646.70 | 4687.25 | 1336.96 | 2798.36 | 654.47 | 4029.01 | 950.60 | 4646.51 | 1198.88 | ||
ST | Neutral | 885.56 | 147.50 | 799.29 | 133.41 | 594.78 | 75.85 | 694.66 | 93.16 | 758.45 | 118.29 | |
Incongruent | 1074.46 | 188.02 | 965.99 | 165.44 | 720.89 | 102.31 | 875.75 | 137.56 | 987.62 | 177.51 | ||
iSDr | SRT | 12.18 | 2.94 | 10.60 | 2.94 | 7.16 | 2.15 | 8.64 | 2.31 | 9.90 | 3.06 | |
LI | 13.23 | 3.59 | 11.27 | 3.10 | 6.42 | 1.98 | 7.97 | 2.11 | 9.29 | 3.15 | ||
CC | 13.58 | 3.91 | 10398 | 3.54 | 5.93 | 1.78 | 8.25 | 2.27 | 9.56 | 2.57 | ||
DI | 13.42 | 3.08 | 10.68 | 3.47 | 5.69 | 1.91 | 8.74 | 2.39 | 9.82 | 2.18 | ||
LC | 6 letters | 14.38 | 5.29 | 10.71 | 4.47 | 5.03 | 1.67 | 7.70 | 2.64 | 8.72 | 3.01 | |
9 letters | 14.01 | 3.93 | 11.25 | 3.84 | 5.83 | 1.81 | 8.38 | 2.30 | 9.44 | 2.72 | ||
ST | Neutral | 14.03 | 3.61 | 11.91 | 3.80 | 6.16 | 1.91 | 7.03 | 2.26 | 8.49 | 3.17 | |
Incongruent | 13.22 | 2.95 | 11.59 | 2.73 | 7.16 | 1.92 | 8.24 | 2.33 | 9.52 | 2.62 |
Age Groups | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Children | Adults | |||||||||||
Young | Old | Young | Young-Old | Old-Old | ||||||||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |||
iM | Reading span | n | 1.92 | 0.41 | 2.22 | 0.59 | 2.83 | 0.76 | 2.59 | 0.82 | 2.31 | 0.67 |
n + 1 | 2.12 | 0.47 | 2.52 | 0.61 | 3.27 | 0.69 | 2.95 | 0.81 | 2.68 | 0.69 | ||
Matrices tasks—Positions | n | 3.04 | 1.19 | 3.66 | 1.40 | 5.32 | 1.41 | 3.41 | 1.17 | 3.16 | 0.97 | |
n + 1 | 3.66 | 1.28 | 4.40 | 1.57 | 6.14 | 1.53 | 3.98 | 1.17 | 3.64 | 0.97 | ||
Matrices tasks—Associations | n + 1 | 1.98 | 0.70 | 2.52 | 0.54 | 3.24 | 0.63 | 2.65 | 0.66 | 2.33 | 0.59 | |
n + 2 | 1.98 | 0.65 | 2.53 | 0.64 | 3.30 | 0.75 | 2.50 | 0.77 | 2.21 | 0.63 | ||
iSDr | Reading span | n | 8.09 | 2.66 | 8.70 | 3.75 | 9.20 | 4.84 | 9.71 | 4.71 | 9.98 | 4.40 |
n + 1 | 9.26 | 2.00 | 9.85 | 2.38 | 10.14 | 2.90 | 10.18 | 2.81 | 9.96 | 2.69 | ||
Matrices tasks—Positions | n | 9.32 | 4.53 | 10.0 | 4.97 | 8.49 | 6.03 | 9.28 | 4.98 | 8.94 | 4.99 | |
n + 1 | 9.67 | 3.62 | 9.08 | 3.80 | 8.92 | 5.02 | 10.34 | 4.57 | 10.1 | 4.0 | ||
Matrices tasks—Associations | n + 1 | 9.01 | 2.58 | 9.97 | 2.64 | 11.28 | 3.40 | 9.73 | 3.59 | 9.82 | 3.03 | |
n + 2 | 8.93 | 2.49 | 9.77 | 2.47 | 11.49 | 3.42 | 10.08 | 2.80 | 9.87 | 2.49 |
References
- Reuchlin, M. Apports de la méthode différentielle à la psychologie générale. J. Psychol. 1981, 4, 377–395. [Google Scholar]
- Cronbach, L.J. The two disciplines of scientific psychology. Am. Psychol. 1957, 12, 671–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nesselroade, J.R. The Warp and Woof of the Developmental Fabric. In Visions of Development, the Environment, and Aesthetics: The Legacy of Joachim F. Wohlwill; Downs, R., Liben, L., Palermo, D., Eds.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1991; pp. 213–240. [Google Scholar]
- Hultsch, D.F.; MacDonald, S.W. Intra-individual variability in performance as a theoretical window onto cognitive aging. In New Frontiers in Cognitive Aging; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2004; pp. 65–88. [Google Scholar]
- Walhovd, K.B.; Fjell, A.M. White matter volume predicts reaction time instability. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 2277–2284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Williams, B.R.; Hultsch, D.F.; Strauss, E.H.; Hunter, M.A.; Tannock, R. Inconsistency in Reaction Time Across the Life Span. Neuropsychology 2005, 19, 88–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jensen, A.R. Clocking the Mind: Mental Chronometry and Individual Differences; Elsevier: Oxford, UK, 2006; ISBN 978-0-08-046372-8. [Google Scholar]
- De Ribaupierre, A. Why Should Cognitive Developmental Psychology Remember that Individuals Are Different? Res. Hum. Dev. 2015, 12, 237–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Der Maas, H.L.J.; Dolan, C.V.; Grasman, R.P.P.P.; Wicherts, J.M.; Huizenga, H.M.; Raijmakers, M.E.J. A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 113, 842–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hultsch, D.F.; Strauss, E.; Hunter, M.A.; MacDonald, S.W.S. Intra-individual variability, cognition, and aging. In The Handbook of Aging and Cognition; Craik, F.I.M., Salthouse, T.A., Eds.; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 491–556. [Google Scholar]
- Borella, E.; de Ribaupierre, A.; Cornoldi, C.; Chicherio, C. Beyond interference control impairment in ADHD: Evidence from increased intra-individual variability in the color-stroop test. Child Neuropsychol. J. Norm. Abnorm. Dev. Child. Adolesc. 2013, 19, 495–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leth-Steensen, C.; Elbaz, Z.K.; Douglas, V.I. Mean response times, variability, and skew in the responding of ADHD children: A response time distributional approach. Acta Psychol. 2000, 104, 167–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegler, R.S. Cognitive Variability: A Key to Understanding Cognitive Development. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 1994, 3, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Geert, P. Variability and fluctuation: A dynamic view. In The Jean Piaget symposium series. In Change and Development: Issues of Theory, Method, and Application; Amsel, E., Renninger, K.A., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1997; pp. 193–212. [Google Scholar]
- Hultsch, D.F.; MacDonald, S.W.; Hunter, M.A.; Levy-Bencheton, J.; Strauss, E. Intra-individual variability in cognitive performance in older adults: Comparison of adults with mild dementia, adults with arthritis, and healthy adults. Neuropsychology 2000, 14, 588–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anstey, K.J.; Christensen, H.; Butterworth, P.; Easteal, S.; Mackinnon, A.; Jacomb, T.; Maxwell, K.; Rodgers, B.; Windsor, T.; Cherbuin, N.; et al. Cohort profile: The PATH through life project. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2012, 41, 951–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bielak, A.A.M.; Cherbuin, N.; Bunce, D.; Anstey, K.J. Intra-individual variability is a fundamental phenomenon of aging: Evidence from an 8-year longitudinal study across young, middle, and older adulthood. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 50, 143–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Der, G.; Deary, I.J. IQ, reaction time and the differentiation hypothesis. Intelligence 2003, 31, 491–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghisletta, P.; Fagot, D.; Lecerf, T.; de Ribaupierre, A. Amplitude of Fluctuations and Temporal Dependency in Intra-individual Variability. GeroPsych J. Gerontopsychol. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2013, 26, 141–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghisletta, P.; Renaud, O.; Fagot, D.; Lecerf, T.; de Ribaupierre, A. Age and sex differences in intra-individual variability in a simple reaction time task. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2017, 0165025417739179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, S.-C.; Huxhold, O.; Schmiedek, F. Aging and attenuated processing robustness. Evidence from cognitive and sensorimotor functioning. Gerontology 2004, 50, 28–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Li, S.-C.; Lindenberger, U.; Hommel, B.; Aschersleben, G.; Prinz, W.; Baltes, P.B. Transformations in the couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span. Psychol. Sci. 2004, 15, 155–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lindenberger, U.; von Oertzen, T. Variability in cognitive aging: From taxonomy to theory. Lifesp. Cognit. Mech. Chang. 2006, 297–314. [Google Scholar]
- Lövdén, M.; Li, S.-C.; Shing, Y.L.; Lindenberger, U. Within-person trial-to-trial variability precedes and predicts cognitive decline in old and very old age: Longitudinal data from the Berlin Aging Study. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 2827–2838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- MacDonald, S.W.S.; Hultsch, D.F.; Dixon, R.A. Performance variability is related to change in cognition: Evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Psychol. Aging 2003, 18, 510–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nelson, E.A.; Dannefer, D. Aged heterogeneity: Fact or fiction? The fate of diversity in gerontological research. Gerontologist 1992, 32, 17–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bielak, A.A.; Anstey, K.J. Intra-individual Variability in Attention across the Adult Life Span. In Handbook of Intra-individual Variability across the Life Span; Diehl, M., Hooker, K., Sliwinski, M., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 160–175. [Google Scholar]
- Fagot, D.; Mella, N. Evolution de la variabilité cognitive au cours de la vie. In Différences et Variabilités en Psychologie; Juhel, J., Rouxel, G., Eds.; Presses Universitaires de Rennes: Rennes, France, 2015; pp. 179–195. ISBN 978-2-7535-3622-7. [Google Scholar]
- De Ribaupierre, A. Pourquoi faut-il étudier la variabilité intra-individuelle lorsqu’on s’intéresse au développement cognitif? In Différences et Variabilités en Psychologie; Juhel, J., Rouxel, G., Eds.; Presses Universitaires de Rennes: Rennes, France, 2015; pp. 159–178. [Google Scholar]
- Williams, B.R.; Strauss, E.H.; Hultsch, D.F.; Hunter, M.A. Reaction Time Inconsistency in a Spatial Stroop Task: Age-Related Differences through Childhood and Adulthood. Aging Neuropsychol. Cognit. 2007, 14, 417–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Robertson, S.; Myerson, J.; Hale, S. Are there age differences in intra-individual variability in working memory performance? J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2006, 61, P18–P24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salthouse, T.A. Implications of within-person variability in cognitive and neuropsychological functioning for the interpretation of change. Neuropsychology 2007, 21, 401–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kochan, N.A.; Bunce, D.; Pont, S.; Crawford, J.D.; Brodaty, H.; Sachdev, P.S. Is intra-individual reaction time variability an independent cognitive predictor of mortality in old age? Findings from the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0181719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- MacDonald, S.W.S.; Stawski, R.S. Intra-individual Variability—An Indicator of Vulnerability or Resilience in Adult Development and Aging? In Handbook of Intra-Individual Variability across the Life Span; Diehl, M., Hooker, K., Sliwinski, M., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 231–257. ISBN 978-0-415-53486-4. [Google Scholar]
- West, R.; Murphy, K.J.; Armilio, M.L.; Craik, F.I.M.; Stuss, D.T. Lapses of intention and performance variability reveal age-related increases in fluctuations of executive control. Brain Cognit. 2002, 49, 402–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmiedek, F.; Lövdén, M.; Lindenberger, U. On the relation of mean reaction time and intra-individual reaction time variability. Psychol. Aging 2009, 24, 841–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Geert, P.; van Dijk, M. Focus on variability: New tools to study intra-individual variability in developmental data. Infant Behav. Dev. 2002, 25, 340–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Dijk, M.; van Geert, P. The nature and meaning of intra-individual variability in development in the early life span. In Handbook of Intra-Individual Variability across the Life Span; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2015; pp. 37–58. [Google Scholar]
- Allaire, J.C.; Marsiske, M. Intra-individual Variability May Not Always Indicate Vulnerability in Elders’ Cognitive Performance. Psychol. Aging 2005, 20, 390–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stawski, R.S.; Smith, J.; MacDonald, S.W.S. Intra-individual Variability and Covariation across Domains in Adulthood and Aging. In Handbook of Intra-Individual Variability across the Lifesapn; Diehl, M., Hooker, K., Sliwinski, M.J., Eds.; Routledge Handbooks Online: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 258–279. ISBN 978-0-415-53486-4. [Google Scholar]
- De Ribaupierre, A. Working memory and attentional processes across the lifespan. In Lifespan Development of Human Memory; Graf, P., Otha, N., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001; pp. 59–80. [Google Scholar]
- De Ribaupierre, A.; Poget, L.; Pons, F. The age variable in cognitive developmental psychology. In Human Clocks. The Bio-Cultural Meanings of Age; Population, Family, and Society; Sauvain-Dugerdil, C., Leridon, H., Mascie-Taylor, N., Eds.; Peter Lang: Bern, Switzerland, 2005; pp. 101–123. ISBN 978-3-03910-785-8. [Google Scholar]
- Tamnes, C.K.; Fjell, A.M.; Westlye, L.T.; Ostby, Y.; Walhovd, K.B. Becoming Consistent: Developmental Reductions in Intra-individual Variability in Reaction Time Are Related to White Matter Integrity. J. Neurosci. 2012, 32, 972–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Papenberg, G.; Bäckman, L.; Chicherio, C.; Nagel, I.E.; Heekeren, H.R.; Lindenberger, U.; Li, S.-C. Higher intra-individual variability is associated with more forgetting and dedifferentiated memory functions in old age. Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 1879–1888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fjell, A.M.; Westlye, L.T.; Amlien, I.K.; Walhovd, K.B. Reduced White Matter Integrity Is Related to Cognitive Instability. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 18060–18072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mella, N.; de Ribaupierre, S.; Eagleson, R.; de Ribaupierre, A. Cognitive Intra-individual Variability and White Matter Integrity in Aging. Sci. World J. 2013, 2013, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moy, G.; Millet, P.; Haller, S.; Baudois, S.; de Bilbao, F.; Weber, K.; Lövblad, K.; Lazeyras, F.; Giannakopoulos, P.; Delaloye, C. Magnetic resonance imaging determinants of intra-individual variability in the elderly: Combined analysis of grey and white matter. Neuroscience 2011, 186, 88–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Westlye, L.T.; Walhovd, K.B.; Dale, A.M.; Bjørnerud, A.; Due-Tønnessen, P.; Engvig, A.; Grydeland, H.; Tamnes, C.K.; Ostby, Y.; Fjell, A.M. Life-span changes of the human brain white matter: Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and volumetry. Cereb. Cortex 2010, 20, 2055–2068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grydeland, H.; Walhovd, K.B.; Tamnes, C.K.; Westlye, L.T.; Fjell, A.M. Intracortical myelin links with performance variability across the human lifespan: Results from T1- and T2-weighted MRI myelin mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 18618–18630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- MacDonald, S.W.S.; Hultsch, D.F.; Dixon, R.A. Predicting impending death: Inconsistency in speed is a selective and early marker. Psychol. Aging 2008, 23, 595–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bunce, D.; MacDonald, S.W.S.; Hultsch, D.F. Inconsistency in serial choice decision and motor reaction times dissociate in younger and older adults. Brain Cognit. 2004, 56, 320–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hultsch, D.F.; MacDonald, S.W.S.; Dixon, R.A. Variability in reaction time performance of younger and older adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2002, 57, P101–P115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Neuringer, A. Reinforced variability in animals and people: Implications for adaptive action. Am. Psychol. 2004, 59, 891–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Golay, P.; Lecerf, T.; Fagot, D. Against coefficient of variation for estimation of intra-individual variability with accuracy measures. Tutor. Quant. Methods Psychol. 2013, 9, 6–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Ribaupierre, A.; Ghisletta, P.; Lecerf, T. Inter- and Intra-Individual Variability across the Lifespan; University of Geneva: Geneva, Switzerland, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, J.C.; Court, J.H.; Raven, J. Progressive Matrices Standard (PM38); Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée: Paris, France, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Park, D.C.; Lautenschlager, G.; Hedden, T.; Davidson, N.S.; Smith, A.D.; Smith, P.K. Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol. Aging 2002, 17, 299–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schneider, W.; Eschman, A.; Zuccolotto, A. E-Prime User’s Guide; Psychology Software Tools, Inc.: Pittsburgh, PA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Delaloye, C.; Ludwig, C.; Borella, E.; Chicherio, C.; de Ribaupierre, A. L’Empan de lecture comme épreuve mesurant la capacité de mémoire de travail: Normes basées sur une population francophone de 775 adultes jeunes et âgés. Rev. Eur. Psychol. Appl./Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 2008, 58, 89–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robert, C.; Borella, E.; Fagot, D.; Lecerf, T.; de Ribaupierre, A. Working memory and inhibitory control across the life span: Intrusion errors in the Reading Span Test. Mem. Cognit. 2009, 37, 336–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hofer, S.; Alwin, D. Handbook of Cognitive Aging: Interdisciplinary Perspectives; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Mella, N.; Fagot, D.; Lecerf, T.; de Ribaupierre, A. Working memory and intra-individual variability in processing speed: A lifespan developmental and individual-differences study. Mem. Cognit. 2015, 43, 340–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Craik, F.I.M.; Salthouse, T.A. The Handbook of Aging and Cognition; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 1992; ISBN 978-0-8058-0713-4. [Google Scholar]
- Salthouse, T.A. Theoretical Perspectives on Cognitive Aging; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins, L.; Myerson, J.; Hale, S.; Fry, A.F. Individual and developmental differences in working memory across the life span. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1999, 6, 28–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mella, N.; Fagot, D.; de Ribaupierre, A. Dispersion in cognitive functioning: Age differences over the lifespan. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2016, 38, 111–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
1 | For the Reading span task, a memory score was computed only when the participants responded correctly for at least 85% of the sentences. This precaution was adopted to insure that participants did process the sentence while retaining the last word, that is, that the task was a dual one. The participants who made more errors when judging the sentence were attributed a missing score. Note that the sentences were simple. Overall, forty-one participants (essentially children) were discarded from the analyses because they did not reach the 85% accuracy criterion on the judgment task. |
2 | The Stroop task was administered twice, at one week intervals, to split the task because it had a large number of items (432 in total). The correlations were high so that analyses were conducted on the total number of trials. |
3 | The Reading span task was administered twice, at one week intervals, to test for possible retest effects. Performance was very similar; consequently, scores were computed on 20 items by condition. |
4 | In these analyses, we analyzed the number of correctly recalled words/position by trial and not the percentage of success on the task (often used as an accuracy score). This allows the computing of a standard deviation across trials (20 trials by condition for reading span and 10 trials by condition for matrices task), but is of course computed on a range that is smaller in low span individuals. Note also that the errors in recall were, for the most part, omissions. |
5 | Analyses on the coefficient of variation (CV) have also been conducted, the results on the reaction times (RTs) score are substantially the same as those obtained with the standard deviations. However, the use of CV has often been criticized, on the basis of our ignorance of its distribution. More importantly, we did not use CV because we wanted to use the same indices on both types of measures, namely, RTs and accuracy. Indeed, Golay, Fagot & Lecerf (2013) showed that the CV cannot be used, or at least generates problems, on accuracy data. Contrary to RTs, accuracy data have a lower and an upper bound, whereas RTs have only a lower bound. Moreover, these authors also showed that the CV is influenced by the number of items which is an issue when dealing with missing data. Therefore the intra-individual standard deviations seemed more appropriate than CVs. |
6 | Remember that the WM tasks were not strictly identical for all participants, as they were adapted to their individual span (see Table A2). This is an optimal solution given the range of age and individual differences in the sample: If the task had been strictly identical for all, it would have been much too difficult for children (or too easy for younger adults), and would have tapped a very different capability while also yielding discouragement in the participants. This was not a problem for the RT tasks, as most of them are very simple. Consequently, age differences in the mean level of WM tasks might reflect both the level of the task and the participant’s WM capacity. Using a percentage score (e.g., ratio of the correctly remembered words on the mean difficulty presented) would not provide much relevant information because it would simply reflect how well the task is adapted to the individuals’ capacity. |
7 | Additional analyses were conducted on RTs, by refining the age group comparisons (see Table A1). First, these analyses showed that whichever the task, young children (9–10 years) were slower and more variable than older children (11–12 years). Second, for SRT, LI, CC and DI tasks, no difference between young-old adults (<70 years) and old-old adults (≥70 years) was obtained on iM. For LC and ST tasks, young-old adults were faster than old-old adults. As concerns the iSDr analyses, except for the LC task, young-old adults were less variable than old-old adults (see Table A3 for more details). |
8 | Additional analyses, refining the age group comparisons, were conducted on accuracy scores. First, the analyses of iM showed that whichever the task, young children (9–10 years) recalled fewer items than older children (11–12 years). No significant difference between these two groups was obtained for iSDr in all tasks. Second, young-old adults recalled more items than old-old adults only for Matrices task word-position associations. No significant difference between young-old and old-old adults was obtained for iSDr in any task (see Table A4 for more details). |
Age Groups | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Children | Young Adults | Older Adults | ||||
N | 201 | 137 | 219 | |||
female | 92 | 117 | 165 | |||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
Age | 10.50 | 1.12 | 21.71 | 2.53 | 70.10 | 6.78 |
Vocabulary Score | _ | _ | 34.67 | 3.25 | 37.73 | 4.64 |
Raven | 36.84 | 8.21 | 52.15 | 4.91 | 36.47 | 9.12 |
Category | Task | Instructions | Condition | Trial | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Latency score | Simple reaction Time | SRT | To detect as quickly as possible when a stimulus appeared on the screen | - | 120 |
Choice reaction time | LI | To detect on which of two sides the longest of two lines was located | - | 120 | |
CS | To detect on which of two sides one of the six crosses changed into a square | - | 120 | ||
Processing speed | LC | to decide whether two series of letters were identical or not | 6 or 9 letters | 60 | |
DI | to determine whether a number–symbol pair was similar to a reference matrix | - | 144 | ||
Interference | ST | To name the color in which words or signs were written | Neutral Congruent Incongruent | 144 144 144 | |
Accuracy score | Working memory task | Rspan | To memorize and recall words | Span level Span + 1 level | 20 20 |
Matrices | To memorize and recall positions | Span level Span + 1 level | 10 10 | ||
To memorize and recall word- positions associations | Span + 1 level Span + 2 level | 10 10 |
Age Groups | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Children | Young Adults | Older Adults | ||||||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |||
iM | SRT | 360.03 | 68.78 | 272.89 | 39.16 | 333.50 | 73.41 | |
LI | 565.38 | 101.31 | 372.38 | 43.52 | 457.66 | 70.46 | ||
CS | 495.57 | 105.41 | 325.69 | 39.26 | 431.16 | 76.75 | ||
DI | 1737.32 | 447.94 | 1040.56 | 221.47 | 1668.96 | 352.86 | ||
LC | 6 letters | 3626.06 | 1236.06 | 1838.17 | 423.31 | 2971.24 | 799.48 | |
9 letters | 5092.16 | 1550.37 | 2798.36 | 654.47 | 4316.61 | 1114.48 | ||
ST | Neutral | 842.21 | 146.76 | 594.77 | 75.85 | 724.31 | 110.05 | |
Incongruent | 1019.96 | 184.77 | 720.89 | 102.30 | 927.75 | 166.67 | ||
iSDr | SRT | 11.38 | 3.04 | 7.17 | 2.15 | 9.23 | 2.75 | |
LI | 12.24 | 3.48 | 6.42 | 1.98 | 8.58 | 2.72 | ||
CS | 12.27 | 3.94 | 5.93 | 1.78 | 8.86 | 2.50 | ||
DI | 12.04 | 3.55 | 5.69 | 1.91 | 9.23 | 2.35 | ||
LC | 6 letters | 12.56 | 5.22 | 5.03 | 1.67 | 8.17 | 2.86 | |
9 letters | 12.64 | 4.11 | 5.83 | 1.81 | 8.88 | 2.56 | ||
ST | Neutral | 12.97 | 3.85 | 6.16 | 1.91 | 7.71 | 2.81 | |
Incongruent | 12.40 | 2.95 | 7.16 | 1.92 | 8.83 | 2.55 |
Age Groups | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Children | Young Adults | Older Adults | ||||||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |||
iM | Reading span | List length n | 2.08 | 0.54 | 2.83 | 0.76 | 2.46 | 0.76 |
List length n + 1 | 2.35 | 0.58 | 3.27 | 0.69 | 2.82 | 0.77 | ||
Matrices tasks—Positions | List length n | 3.35 | 1.33 | 5.32 | 1.41 | 3.30 | 1.09 | |
List length n + 1 | 4.03 | 1.48 | 6.14 | 1.53 | 3.82 | 1.10 | ||
Matrices tasks—associations | List length n + 1 | 2.25 | 0.68 | 3.24 | 0.63 | 2.51 | 0.65 | |
List length n + 2 | 2.25 | 0.70 | 3.29 | 0.75 | 2.37 | 0.73 | ||
iSDr | Reading span | List length n | 8.43 | 3.31 | 9.20 | 4.84 | 9.84 | 4.56 |
List length n + 1 | 9.59 | 2.23 | 10.14 | 2.90 | 10.08 | 2.75 | ||
Matrices tasks—Positions | List length n | 9.70 | 4.76 | 8.49 | 6.03 | 9.12 | 4.98 | |
List length n + 1 | 9.38 | 3.72 | 8.92 | 5.03 | 10.23 | 4.31 | ||
Matrices tasks—associations | List length n + 1 | 9.50 | 2.65 | 11.28 | 4.40 | 9.77 | 3.34 | |
List length n + 2 | 9.35 | 2.51 | 11.49 | 3.42 | 9.98 | 2.66 |
© 2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Fagot, D.; Mella, N.; Borella, E.; Ghisletta, P.; Lecerf, T.; De Ribaupierre, A. Intra-Individual Variability from a Lifespan Perspective: A Comparison of Latency and Accuracy Measures. J. Intell. 2018, 6, 16. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6010016
Fagot D, Mella N, Borella E, Ghisletta P, Lecerf T, De Ribaupierre A. Intra-Individual Variability from a Lifespan Perspective: A Comparison of Latency and Accuracy Measures. Journal of Intelligence. 2018; 6(1):16. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6010016
Chicago/Turabian StyleFagot, Delphine, Nathalie Mella, Erika Borella, Paolo Ghisletta, Thierry Lecerf, and Anik De Ribaupierre. 2018. "Intra-Individual Variability from a Lifespan Perspective: A Comparison of Latency and Accuracy Measures" Journal of Intelligence 6, no. 1: 16. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6010016
APA StyleFagot, D., Mella, N., Borella, E., Ghisletta, P., Lecerf, T., & De Ribaupierre, A. (2018). Intra-Individual Variability from a Lifespan Perspective: A Comparison of Latency and Accuracy Measures. Journal of Intelligence, 6(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6010016