A Differential–Developmental Model (DDM): Mental Speed, Attention Lapses, and General Intelligence (g)
Abstract
:1. Introduction
All models are wrong, but some are useful.([1], p. 202)
2. Assumptions of DDM
3. Chronometric Methods, Jensen’s Box, and DDM
4. Preliminary Findings
4.1. Individual Differences in Intelligence
4.2. Cognitive Development in Childhood and Aging
4.3. The Worst Performance Rule (WPR)
5. Future Directions
5.1. RT–MT Differences
5.2. Reaction Stimulus (RS) Duration
5.3. Fixed Interval Presentation of Reaction Stimulus (RS)
6. Implications, Qualifications, and Open Questions
6.1. DDM and Other Theories
6.2. DDM and Brain Morphology
6.3. DDM and Magnitude of Speed–Intelligence Relations
6.4. DDM, Speed-Accuracy Tradeoffs, and RT-IQ Correlations
6.5. Qualifications and Open Questions
7. Epilogue
Acknowledgements
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Box, G.E.P. Robustness in the strategy of scientific model building. In Robustness in Statistics; Launer, R.L., Wilkinson, G.N., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1979; pp. 201–236. [Google Scholar]
- Jensen, A.R. The g Factor: The Science of Mental Ability; Praeger: Westport, CT, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Jensen, A.R. Clocking the Mind: Mental Chronometry and Individual Differences; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Kail, R.; Salthouse, T.A. Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychol. 1994, 86, 199–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kail, R. Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 109, 490–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salthouse, T.A. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol. Rev. 1996, 103, 403–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coyle, T.R.; Pillow, D.R.; Snyder, A.C.; Kochunov, P. Processing speed mediates the development of general intelligence (g) in adolescence. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 1265–1269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coyle, T.R. A review of the worst performance rule: Evidence, theory, and alternative hypotheses. Intelligence 2003, 31, 567–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McVay, J.C.; Kane, M.J. Drifting from slow to “D’oh!”: Working memory capacity and mind wandering predict extreme reaction times and executive control errors. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 2012, 38, 525–549. [Google Scholar]
- Unsworth, N.; Redick, T.S.; Lakey, C.E.; Young, D.L. Lapses in sustained attention and their relation to executive control and fluid abilities: An individual differences investigation. Intelligence 2010, 38, 111–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, T.R. Effects of processing speed on intelligence may be underestimated: Comment on Demetriou et al. (2013). Intelligence 2013, 41, 732–734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dougherty, T.M.; Haith, M.M. Infant expectations and reaction time as predictors of childhood speed of processing and IQ. Dev. Psychol. 1997, 33, 146–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kochunov, P.; Coyle, T.; Lancaster, J.; Robin, D.A.; Hardies, J.; Kochunov, V.; Bartzokis, G.; Stanley, J.; Royall, D.; Schlosser, A.E.; et al. Processing speed is correlated with cerebral health markers in the frontal lobes as quantified by neuro-imaging. Neuroimage 2010, 15, 1190–1199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miller, E.M. Intelligence and brain myelination: A hypothesis. Pers. Individ. Differ. 1994, 17, 803–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fields, R.D. Changes in the brain’s white matter. Science 2010, 330, 768–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fry, A.; Hale, S. Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence: Evidence for developmental cascade. Psychol. Sci. 1996, 7, 237–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jensen, A.R. The theory of intelligence and its measurement. Intelligence 2011, 39, 171–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jensen, A.R. Understanding g in terms of information processing. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1992, 4, 271–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jensen, A.R. Mental chronometry and the unification of differential psychology. In Cognition and Intelligence: Identifying the Mechanisms of the Mind; Sternberg, R.J., Pretz, J.E., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2005; pp. 26–50. [Google Scholar]
- Hick, W.E. On the rate of gain of information. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1952, 4, 11–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deary, I.J.; Stough, C. Intelligence and inspection time: Achievements, prospects, and problems. Am. Psychol. 1996, 51, 599–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grudnik, J.L.; Kranzler, J.H. Meta-analysis of the relationship between intelligence and inspection time. Intelligence 2001, 29, 523–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jensen, A.R. Reaction time and psychometric g. In A Model for Intelligence; Eysenck, H.J., Ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1982; pp. 93–132. [Google Scholar]
- Aubert-Broche, B.; Fonov, V.; Leppert, I.; Pike, G.B.; Collins, D.L. Human brain myelination from birth to 4.5 years. In Medical Image Computing and Computer-Assisted Intervention—MICCAI 2008; Metaxas, D., Axel, L., Fichtinger, G., Székely, G., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2008; pp. 180–187. [Google Scholar]
- Larson, G.E.; Alderton, D.L. Reaction time variability and intelligence: A ‘‘worst performance’’ analysis of individual differences. Intelligence 1990, 14, 309–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, T.R. IQ is related to the worst performance rule in a memory task involving children. Intelligence 2001, 29, 117–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, T.R. IQ, the worst performance rule, and Spearman’s law: A reanalysis and extension. Intelligence 2003, 31, 473–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salthouse, T.A. Relations of successive percentiles of reaction time distributions to cognitive variables and adult age. Intelligence 1998, 26, 153–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kail, R.V. Longitudinal evidence that increases in processing speed and working memory enhance children’s reasoning. Child Dev. 2007, 18, 312–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kail, R.V.; Ferrer, E. Processing speed in childhood and adolescence: Longitudinal models for examining developmental change. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 1760–1770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Colom, R.; Abad, F.J.; Quiroga, M.A.; Shih, P.C.; Flores-Mendoza, C. Working memory and intelligence are highly related constructs, but why? Intelligence 2008, 36, 584–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colom, R.; Rebollo, I.; Palacios, A.; Juan-Espinosa, M.; Kyllonen, P.C. Working memory is (almost) perfectly predicted by g. Intelligence 2004, 32, 277–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gottfredson, L.S. Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 1997, 24, 79–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bjorklund, D.F. Children’s Thinking: Developmental Function and Individual Differences, 3rd ed.; Wadsworth: Belmont, CA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Demetriou, A.; Spanoudis, G.; Shayer, M.; Mouyi, A.; Kazi, S.; Platsidou, M. Cycles in speed-working memory-G relations: Towards a developmental-differential theory of mind. Intelligence 2013, 41, 34–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tourva, A.; Spanoudis, G.; Demetriou, A. Cognitive correlates of developing intelligence: The contribution of working memory, processing speed and attention. Intelligence 2016, 54, 136–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ratcliff, R. A theory of memory retrieval. Psychol. Rev. 1978, 85, 59–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ratcliff, R.; Schmiedek, F.; McKoon, G. A diffusion model explanation of the worst performance rule for reaction time and IQ. Intelligence 2008, 36, 10–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schubert, A.-L.; Frischkorn, G.T.; Hagemann, D.; Voss, A. Trait characteristics of diffusion model parameters. J. Intell. 2016, 4, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmiedek, F.; Oberauer, K.; Wilhelm, O.; Süß, H.-M.; Wittmann, W.W. Individual differences in components of reaction time distributions and their relations to working memory and intelligence. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2007, 136, 414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ratcliff, R.; Thapar, A.; McKoon, G. Effects of aging and IQ on item and associative memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2011, 140, 464–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cattell, R.B. Intelligence: Its Structure, Growth and Action; North-Holland: New York, NY, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Coyle, T.R. Relations among general intelligence (g), aptitude tests, and GPA: Linear effects dominate. Intelligence 2015, 53, 16–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spearman, C. The Abilities of Man: Their Nature and Measurement; AMS Press: New York, NY, USA, 1932. [Google Scholar]
- Coyle, T.R.; Purcell, J.M.; Snyder, A.C.; Kochunov, P. Non-g residuals of the SAT and ACT predict specific abilities. Intelligence 2013, 41, 114–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Phillips, L.H.; Rabbitt, P.M.A. Impulsivity and speed-accuracy strategies in intelligence test performance. Intelligence 1995, 21, 13–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, G.A.; Bewer, N. Slowness and age: Speed-accuracy mechanisms. Psychol. Aging 1995, 10, 238–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ceci, S.J. On the relation between microlevel processing efficiency and macrolevel measures of intelligence: Some arguments against current reductionism. Intelligence 1990, 14, 141–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neubauer, A.C.; Bucik, V. The mental speed-IQ relationship: Unitary or modular? Intelligence 1996, 22, 23–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGrew, K.S. CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence 2009, 37, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
1 | In the current article, the term “intelligence” refers to general intelligence (g), which reflects variance common to cognitive tests. g reflects the fact that people who perform well on one cognitive test generally perform well on all other tests, yielding positive correlations among the tests. Tests that correlate strongly with g (i.e., g-loaded tests) include IQ tests, fluid reasoning tests (e.g., Raven’s Matrices and Cattell’s Culture Fair Test), and academic aptitude tests (e.g., SAT, ACT, and PSAT). g contributes strongly to a test’s predictive validity at school and work (e.g., [2], pp. 274–294). |
2 | It should be emphasized that, in DDM, the explanandum is the age-related association between mental speed (the causal factor) and g (the outcome). DDM proposes that the association between speed and g can be explained by attention lapses after separating two key components of speed (movement time and reaction time). |
3 | Other indicators of attention lapses may include eye movement latencies in eye tracking studies, a suggestion made by an anonymous reviewer. Indeed, in infancy, eye movement latencies following the presentation of a target stimulus predict later IQ (at 5 years), with faster latencies predicting higher IQs [12]. These latencies may index attention lapses linked to IQ. In particular, if high-IQ individuals experience fewer (and shorter duration) attention lapses, their latencies following a target stimulus should be shorter and less variable (cf. [12]). Eye movement latencies represent one indicator of attention lapses. Ideally, attention lapses and other DDM constructs should be modeled using latent variable analysis and multiple indicators, which would reduce measurement error (e.g., method variance) and increase variance related to key constructs (e.g., lapses, speed, and g). |
4 | It is worth noting that g (the criterion) is strongly associated with fluid intelligence, defined as general reasoning and problem-solving ability, and crystallized intelligence, defined as acquired knowledge and skills. For example, Jensen ([2], p. 126) estimated the average g loading of the Verbal and Performance scales of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC). The Verbal and Performance subtests resemble crystallized and fluid types of tests, respectively. The average g loading of the two scales was 0.89 (a value close to the reliability of the WISC IQ), suggesting that fluid and crystallized abilities largely reflect g. |
5 | The model depicts paths from speed (RT) to lapses, which implies speed affects lapses, which in turn affect g. In particular, the model examines whether lapses mediate the link between speed and g, and whether the direct effect of speed on g becomes nonsignificant with lapses in the model. This pattern is consistent with developmental cascade models [16], which predict that lower order processes (e.g., mental speed) affect higher order processes (e.g., working memory), which in turn affect g. In addition, the pattern is consistent with theories suggesting that fast mental speed helps maintain goal representations and avoid attention lapses. An alternative model (not depicted) could reverse the direction of the path between speed and lapses. This model would examine whether speed mediates a link between lapses and g, a hypothesis consonant with theories that attention lapses represent lower order processes (perhaps reflecting neural noise) that affect speed, which in turn affects g (cf. [17]). Ultimately, the direction of the path depends on whether lapses are assumed to reflect basic processes (e.g., neural errors) that precede speed, or to reflect higher order cognition that follows speed. DDM depicts a pattern similar to that described by developmental cascade models (e.g., [16]), which assume that lower order processes (mental speed) predict higher order processes (attention lapses), which help maintain goal states during information processing. |
6 | A bit (binary digit) is a unit of information that can have only one of two values (commonly represented 0 or 1). In information theory, a bit is defined as the amount of information that reduces uncertainty by one-half. In Jensen’s paradigm, the set of response alternatives are normally 1, 2, 4, and 8 buttons, corresponding to 0, 1, 2, and 3 bits. |
7 | This article focuses on RT tasks, which require an overt response (a button release or press) following stimulus presentation. Another speeded task, which is not reviewed here, is inspection time (IT). IT measures speed of information intake, based on the shortest exposure duration required to detect a stimulus [21,22]. In a standard IT task, participants see two lines, one of which is longer than the other one, and are asked to identify which line is longer. IT does not require an overt response, which eliminates MT. However, IT does not parametrically vary complexity, making it unsuitable for studying complexity and intelligence. What is needed is an IT paradigm that systematically manipulates complexity. Such a paradigm would complement Jensen’s RT paradigm and allow the examination of relationships between intelligence and IT complexity. |
8 | Jensen’s paradigm has been used with 5- to 20-year-olds ([3], pp. 79–83). A reviewer wondered whether the paradigm could be used with children younger than 5 years of age. The answer is speculative. First, other RT paradigms with similar instructions (“identify the picture on the right”) have been used with children younger than 5 years [12], suggesting that younger children could perform Jensen’s paradigm. Second, increases in processing efficiency (via reductions in attention lapses) would be expected to yield faster RTs at any point in development, even in infancy. Indeed, stability in RTs has been observed from infancy to 5 years and has been linked to later IQ, presumably because faster RTs lead to more efficient information processing, which leads to better performance on IQ tests [12]. Finally, increases in processing efficiency are theoretically linked to increases in brain myelination, which undergoes rapid development in the first few years of life [24]. The development of myelination during this period would be expected to increase processing efficiency, which in turn would lead to faster and less variable RTs. |
9 | Some of these paper-and-pencil tests have been adapted for computer administration. For example, in a computer adaptation of the matching task, participants compare two stimuli (e.g., 8··8) and respond by pressing an “S” key for same, or a “D” key for different (e.g., [6], p. 407). Although the key presses in computer adaptations may reduce the influence of the motor component, they do not separate MT and RT and therefore cannot evaluate the relative contribution of each speed component. |
10 | An anonymous reviewer requested an example of a procedure to equate working memory and mental speed in complexity. Conceptually, such a procedure might involve performing a task analysis and (a) comparing tasks with similar levels of complexity (similar numbers of bits) or (b) comparing tasks with different levels of complexity after statistically controlling for complexity. A related approach might be to compare the influence of mental speed and working memory on the same task, which would effectively control for complexity. An example might be to use backward digit span (a working memory task) to measure rehearsal speed as an indicator of mental speed and items recalled as an indicator of working memory. In any case, the key issue is to perform a task analysis (a priori) to identify levels of complexity, which is needed to control for complexity. |
11 | Diffusion models also incorporate a non-decision parameter (Ter), which reflects the speed of non-decision processes such as stimulus encoding, motor programming, and motor speed, which may influence MT. Unfortunately, because this non-decision parameter measures both input (e.g., stimulus encoding) and output (e.g., motor speed) processes, it cannot isolate MT or provide an unbiased estimate of motor speed (cf. [39]). |
12 | It should be noted that age-related trends in RT are typically nonlinear, decreasing exponentially during childhood (5- to 20-years) and increasing nonlinearly during normal aging (20- to 80-years) ([3], pp. 77–78; [5], Figures 2 and 3; cf. [6], Figure 1). An anonymous reviewer noted that these nonlinear trends in RT may track nonlinear trends in white matter integrity (e.g., [13,14]), which is associated with RT speed and variability. In particular, nonlinear increases in white matter integrity during childhood, culminating with the frontal lobes, should correlate with nonlinear decreases in RT and RT variability. In contrast, nonlinear decreases in white matter during aging should be associated with the opposite pattern. |
© 2017 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Coyle, T. A Differential–Developmental Model (DDM): Mental Speed, Attention Lapses, and General Intelligence (g). J. Intell. 2017, 5, 25. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5020025
Coyle T. A Differential–Developmental Model (DDM): Mental Speed, Attention Lapses, and General Intelligence (g). Journal of Intelligence. 2017; 5(2):25. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5020025
Chicago/Turabian StyleCoyle, Thomas. 2017. "A Differential–Developmental Model (DDM): Mental Speed, Attention Lapses, and General Intelligence (g)" Journal of Intelligence 5, no. 2: 25. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5020025
APA StyleCoyle, T. (2017). A Differential–Developmental Model (DDM): Mental Speed, Attention Lapses, and General Intelligence (g). Journal of Intelligence, 5(2), 25. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5020025