Predicting Intellectual Ability and Scholastic Outcomes with a Single Item: From Early Childhood to Adulthood
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Method
2.1. Participants
2.2. Q-sort Methodology and the California Child Q-Set (CCQ) Item “High Intellectual Capacity”
At ages 3 and 4, each child was described by three nursery school teachers who had worked with the children a minimum of 5 months before completing the descriptions. Teachers also received training and met with the project director who explained the rationale, provided written instructions to the CCQ, and answered questions about item meanings. Teachers then independently did a Q-sort for a child who was not in the study (usually from a previous year) but was known to all of the teachers. The item descriptions were discussed, and usually a second child was described to check understandings. At age 4, each child was again described via the CCQ procedure but by an entirely different set of three nursery school teachers equivalently trained.[17]
2.3. Intellectual Ability and Scholastic Outcomes
2.4. Controls and Covariates
2.5. Sex and Ethnicity
2.6. Physical Attractiveness
2.7. The General Factor of Personality (GFP)
2.8. Parental Education
2.9. Socioeconomic Status (SES)
3. Results
3.1. Bivariate Correlations between “High Intellectual Capacity” Rating, Intellectual Ability, and Scholastic Outcomes
Intellectual and Scholastic Indices | Full Sample | Males Only | Females Only | White Only | Black Only |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
WPPSI age 4 | 0.56 ***(111) | 0.55 ***(53) | 0.60 ***(58) | 0.52 ***(74) | 0.29(29) |
WISC age 11 | 0.69 ***(104) | 0.76 ***(53) | 0.59 ***(51) | 0.53 ***(69) | 0.72 ***(28) |
WAIS age 18 | 0.67 ***(102) | 0.71 ***(50) | 0.62 ***(52) | 0.56 ***(70) | 0.60 **(25) |
SATMath | 0.36 **(60) | 0.41 *(26) | 0.21(34) | 0.36 *(45) | n < 10 |
SATVerbal | 0.54 ***(60) | 0.64 ***(26) | 40 *(34) | 0.51 ***(45) | n < 10 |
High-School GPA | 0.48 ***(97) | 0.62 ***(49) | 0.24(48) | 0.35 **(65) | 0.36(26) |
Years of Education age 32 | 0.46 ***(79) | 0.64 ***(35) | 0.34 *(44) | 0.44 ***(62) | 0.53(13) |
3.2. Regression Analyses Predicting Intellectual Ability and Scholastic Outcomes from “High Intellectual Capacity” Rating, while Controlling for Potential Confounds
Dependent Variable | N | Step 1—Control Variables | Total R2 | Step 2—“High Intellectual Capacity” | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Attractiveness | GFP | SES | EducationMother | EducationFather | ΔR2 | |||
WPPSI age 4 | 96 | −0.01 | 0.28 ** | 0.10 | 0.36 * | −0.01 | 0.31 *** | 0.04 * |
WISC age 11 | 87 | −0.11 | 0.36 *** | 0.19 | 0.28 * | −0.12 | 0.44 *** | 0.06 ** |
WAIS age 18 | 85 | −0.01 | 0.22 * | 0.15 | 0.38 ** | 0.03 | 0.41 *** | 0.07 ** |
SATMath | 55 | 0.01 | 0.08 | 0.15 | 0.41 * | 0.00 | 0.18 * | 0.07 * |
SATVerbal | 55 | 0.11 | 0.10 | 0.25 | 0.34 * | 0.05 | 0.28 ** | 0.13 ** |
High-School GPA | 82 | −0.06 | 0.12 | 0.01 | 0.24 | 0.13 | 0.14 * | 0.11 ** |
Years of Education age 32 | 69 | −0.03 | 0.33 *** | 0.08 | 0.25 | 0.09 | 0.28 ** | 0.04 |
3.3. Relationship between “High Intellectual Capacity” Rating and Measured Intelligence in Predicting Future Intellectual Ability and Scholastic Outcomes
Dependent Variable | N | WPPSI Age 4 | “High Intellectual Capacity” | R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|
WISC age 11 | 83 | β = 0.52 *** | β = 0.33 *** | 0.59 *** |
WAIS age 18 | 81 | β = 0.43 *** | β = 0.38 *** | 0.52 *** |
SATMath | 49 | β = 0.41 ** | β = 0.26 | 0.32 *** |
SATVerbal | 49 | β = 0.18 | β = 0.50 *** | 0.35 *** |
High-School GPA | 77 | β = 0.29 * | β = 0.33 ** | 0.30 *** |
Years of Education age 32 | 66 | β = 0.09 | β = 0.50 *** | 0.31 *** |
Dependent Variable | N | WISC Age 11 | “High Intellectual Capacity” | R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|
WAIS age 18 | 99 | β = 0.72 *** | β = 0.17 * | 0.71 *** |
SATMath | 59 | β = 0.68 *** | β = 0.10 | 0.53 *** |
SATVerbal | 59 | β = 0.37 ** | β = 0.39 ** | 0.40 *** |
High-School GPA | 94 | β = 0.31 * | β = 0.28 * | 0.30 *** |
Years of Education age 32 | 77 | β = 0.26 | β = 0.31 * | 0.26 *** |
Dependent Variable | N | WAIS Age 18 | “High Intellectual Capacity” | R2 |
---|---|---|---|---|
SATMath | 59 | β = 0.43 *** | β = 0.21 | 0.33 *** |
SATVerbal | 59 | β = 0.44 *** | β = 0.34 ** | 0.44 *** |
High-School GPA | 94 | β = 0.53 *** | β = 0.11 | 0.37 *** |
Years of Education age 32 | 78 | β = 0.43 *** | β = 0.21 | 0.33 *** |
4. Discussion
Possible Mechanisms
5. Limitations and Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Fuhrman, R.W.; Bodenhausen, G.V.; Lichtenstein, M. On the trait implications of social behaviors: Kindness, intelligence, goodness, and normality ratings for 400 behavior statements. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 1989, 21, 587–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, N.A.; Hall, J.A.; Smith LeBeau, L. Who’s smart? Beliefs about the expression of intelligence in social behavior. Represent. Res. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 25, 34–42. [Google Scholar]
- Raty, H.; Snellman, L. Children's images of an intelligent person. J. Soc. Behav. Personal. 1997, 12, 773–784. [Google Scholar]
- Borkenau, P.; Liebler, A. Observable attributes as manifestations and cues of personality and intelligence. J. Personal. 1995, 63, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Borkenau, P.; Liebler, A. Convergence of stranger ratings of personality and intelligence with self-ratings, partner ratings, and measured intelligence. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 65, 546–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, N.A. Appearing smart: The impression management of intelligence, person perception accuracy, and behavior in social interaction. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2007, 33, 325–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reynolds, D.J.; Gifford, R. The sounds and sights of intelligence: A lens model channel analysis. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 27, 187–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, N.A.; Hall, J.A.; Colvin, C.R. Accurate intelligence assessments in social interactions: Mediators and gender effects. J. Personal. 2003, 71, 465–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carney, D.R.; Colvin, C.R.; Hall, J.A. A thin slice perspective on the accuracy of first impressions. J. Res. Personal. 2007, 41, 1054–1072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zebrowitz, L.A.; Collins, M.A. Accurate social perception at zero acquaintance: The affordances of a Gibsonian approach. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 1997, 1, 204–223. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, N.A. Nonverbal perception. In Handbook of Social Cognition; Fiske, S.T., Macrae, C.N., Eds.; Sage: London, UK, 2012; pp. 196–215. [Google Scholar]
- Zebrowitz, L.A.; Hall, J.A.; Murphy, N.A.; Rhodes, G. Looking smart and looking good: Facial cues to intelligence and their origins. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2002, 28, 238–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kenny, D.A.; West, T.V. Zero acquaintance: Definitions, statistical model, findings, and process. In First Impressions; Skowronski, J., Ambady, N., Eds.; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 129–146. [Google Scholar]
- Blackman, M.C.; Funder, D.C. The effect of information on consensus and accuracy in personality judgment. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 34, 164–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Funder, D.C.; Colvin, C.R. Friends and strangers: Acquaintanceship, agreement, and the accuracy of personality judgment. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 55, 149–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vazire, S. Who knows what about a person? The Self-Other Knowledge Asymmetry (SOKA) model. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 98, 281–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Block, J.; Block, J.H. Block and Block Longitudinal Study, 1969–1999. Murray Research Archive [Distributor]. 2006. V1 [Version]. Available online: http://dvn.iq.harvard.edu/dvn/dv/mra/faces/study/StudyPage.xhtml?globalId=hdl:1902.1/NGQCIPIDUK&studyListingIndex=2_e3e414a92b8ffc99ae5ad3c701fb (accessed on 20 August 2013).
- Block, J.; Block, J.H. Venturing a 30-year longitudinal study. Am. Psychol. 2006, 61, 315–327. [Google Scholar]
- Block, J. The Q-Sort Method in Personality Assessment and Psychiatric Research; Charles C. Thomas: Springfield, IL, USA, 1961. [Google Scholar]
- Asendorpf, J.B.; van Aken, M.A. Correlates of the temporal consistency of personality patterns in childhood. J. Personal. 1991, 59, 689–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oshri, A.; Rogosch, F.A.; Cicchetti, D. Child maltreatment and mediating influences of childhood personality types on the development of adolescent psychopathology. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2013, 42, 287–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rammstedt, B.; Riemann, R.; Angleitner, A.; Borkenau, P. Resilients, overcontrollers, and undercontrollers: The replicability of the three personality prototypes across informants. Eur. J. Personal. 2004, 18, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, S.; Schalet, B.D.; Hicks, B.M.; Zucker, R.A. Identifying early childhood personality dimensions using the California Child Q-Set and prospective associations with behavioral and psychosocial development. J. Res. Personal. 2013, 47, 339–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bimler, D.; Kirland, J.; Fitzgerald, H.E.; Zucker, R.A. Convergence of internal and external structure for the California Child Q-set. Span. J. Psychol. 2010, 13, 454–468. [Google Scholar]
- Mackintosh, N.J. IQ and Human Intelligence; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Kanazawa, S. Intelligence and physical attractiveness. Intelligence 2011, 39, 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dusek, J.B.; Joseph, G. The bases of teacher expectancies: A meta-analysis. J. Educ. Psychol. 1983, 75, 327–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eagly, A.H.; Ashmore, R.D.; Makhijani, M.G.; Longo, L.C. What is beautiful is good, but…: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 110, 109–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaplan, R.M. Is beauty talent? Sex interaction in the attractiveness halo effect. Sex Roles 1978, 4, 195–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moore, F.R.; Filippou, D.; Perrett, D.I. Intelligence and attractiveness in the face: Beyond the attractiveness halo effect. J. Evol. Psychol. 2011, 9, 205–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rushton, P.; Irwing, P. The general factor of personality: Normal and abnormal. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences; Chamorro-Premuzic, T., von Stumm, S., Furnham, A., Eds.; Wiley-Blackwell: Malden, MA, USA, 2011; pp. 132–161. [Google Scholar]
- Dunkel, C.S. The general factor of personality and general intelligence: Evidence of substantial association. Intelligence 2013, 41, 423–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loehlin, J.C.; Martin, N.G. General and supplementary factors of personality in genetic and environmental correlation matrices. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2013, 54, 761–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brooks, B.L. A study of low scores in Canadian children and adolescents on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). Child Neuropsychol. 2011, 17, 281–289. [Google Scholar]
- Sirin, S.R. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Rev. Educ. Res. 2005, 75, 417–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neisser, U.; Boodoo, G.; Bouchard, T.J.; Boykin, A.W.; Brody, N.; Urbina, S. Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. Am. Psychol. 1996, 51, 77–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Warner, W.L. Social Class in America: A Manual of Procedure for the Measurement of Social Status; Science Research Associates: Chicago, IL, USA, 1949. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal, R.; Jacobson, L. Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher Expectations and Student Intellectual Development. Holt: New York, NY, USA, 1968. [Google Scholar]
- Jussim, L.; Harber, K.D. Teacher expectations and self-fulfilling prophecies: Knowns and unknowns, resolved and unresolved controversies. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2005, 9, 131–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, J.A.; Gunnery, S.D. Gender differences in nonverbal communication. In Nonverbal Communication; Hall, J.A., Knapp, M.L., Eds.; deGruyter Mouton: Berlin, Germany, 2013; Volume 2; pp. 639–669. [Google Scholar]
- Hicks, L.E. Some properties of ipsative, normative, and forced-choice normative measures. Psychol. Bull. 1970, 74, 167–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Block, J. A comparison between ipsative and normative ratings of personality. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1957, 54, 50–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shedler, J,; Weston, D. The Shedler-Westen Assessment Procedure (SWAP): Making personality diagnosis clinically meaningful. J. Personal. Assess. 2007, 81, 41–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sherman, R.A.; Figuerado, A.J.; Funder, D.C. The behavioral correlates of overall and distinctive life history strategy. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 105, 873–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jussim, L. Social Perception and Social Reality: Why Accuracy Dominates Bias and Self Fulfilling Prophecy. Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Gottfreson, L. Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 1997, 24, 79–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
© 2014 by the authors; licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
Share and Cite
Dunkel, C.S.; Murphy, N.A. Predicting Intellectual Ability and Scholastic Outcomes with a Single Item: From Early Childhood to Adulthood. J. Intell. 2014, 2, 68-81. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence2030068
Dunkel CS, Murphy NA. Predicting Intellectual Ability and Scholastic Outcomes with a Single Item: From Early Childhood to Adulthood. Journal of Intelligence. 2014; 2(3):68-81. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence2030068
Chicago/Turabian StyleDunkel, Curtis S., and Nora A. Murphy. 2014. "Predicting Intellectual Ability and Scholastic Outcomes with a Single Item: From Early Childhood to Adulthood" Journal of Intelligence 2, no. 3: 68-81. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence2030068
APA StyleDunkel, C. S., & Murphy, N. A. (2014). Predicting Intellectual Ability and Scholastic Outcomes with a Single Item: From Early Childhood to Adulthood. Journal of Intelligence, 2(3), 68-81. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence2030068