Next Article in Journal
The Struggle to Define Pilgrimage
Next Article in Special Issue
Is There ‘Spiritual Intelligence’? An Evaluation of Strong and Weak Proposals
Previous Article in Journal
The Saint Dionysius and Saint Margaret Altarpiece from the Cathedral of València
Previous Article in Special Issue
Transgressive Behaviours and Readiness to Engage in Interreligious Dialogue in Young Adulthood
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Personality Traits and the Spiritual and Moral Intelligence of Early Adulthood in Poland: Research Reports

Faculty of Social Sciences, Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, 20-950 Lublin, Poland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Religions 2023, 14(1), 78; https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14010078
Submission received: 9 November 2022 / Revised: 14 December 2022 / Accepted: 28 December 2022 / Published: 5 January 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Spiritual Intelligence: Problems, Challenges and Solutions)

Abstract

:
Spiritual and moral intelligence have recently become popular research questions in studies. The aim of the present study was to investigate the relationship between spiritual and moral intelligence. It was also intended to link spiritual and moral intelligence with personality traits among people in early adulthood (aged 18 to 35) in Poland. In addition, in order to better understand the group, it was also planned to investigate the association of spiritual and moral intelligence with age. The theoretical framework for our study is the concept of spiritual intelligence as consisting of four dimensions: critical existential thinking, personal meaning-making, transcendental awareness and conscious state expansion. Measurements were based on the concept of moral intelligence as consisting of the four main dimensions of integrity, responsibility, compassion and forgiveness, and subcategories (competencies) including integrity, honesty, courage, confidentiality, commitment, personal responsibility, accountability for decisions, self-control, helping others, caring for others (compassion), understanding others’ feelings (altruism) and understanding their emotional needs (faith, belief). A total of 160 individuals aged 18 to 35 years from Poland were surveyed using the Spiritual Intelligence Self-Report Inventory, the Moral Intelligence Scale, the Ten-Item Personality Inventory. The article reports on the results of this study.

1. Introduction

In the literature, intelligence is sometimes perceived as the ability to: adapt to given circumstances (Binet and Simon 1904), benefit from experience (Sternberg 2000), solve new problems (Bingham 1937), think purposefully, deal effectively with the environment (Wechsler 1958). Current research sometimes treats intelligence as a multidimensional and multifaceted construct that includes different types of intelligence: social, practical, analytical, cognitive and emotional (Ayoub and Aljughaiman 2016; González-Treviño et al. 2020; Yousefi and Heshmati 2015).
Currently, there is a growing interest in spiritual and moral intelligence. Spirituality, like morality, is sometimes considered an intrinsic part of human nature. However, it is sometimes perceived as a separate construct that does not fit into the categories of moral or social development. Spirituality can be a source of meaning, openness and human development (Socha 2000). It can help to explain various events and provide hope, value and comfort. Morality is sometimes defined as a system of rules and values for human behaviour that is generally accepted by society or by a specific group of people (Asif et al. 2020). Spirituality supports morality in the realisation of important values such as freedom of justice (Bagherpur et al. 2022). Ongoing research into the link between spirituality and morality has shown that spiritual people perform better in teamwork (Mitroff and Denton 1999). They are also characterised by being more honest, kind and supportive of others in their problems, needs (Cash and Gray 2000; Krishnakumar and Neck 2002; Beazley and Gemmill 2006). Spiritual principles and values help to better understand moral issues, to see their depth, to make the right decisions (McGhee and Grant 2008). Spirituality shapes personality and is an important centre for the development of morality, where the meaning of human existence is concentrated. Today, researchers in many countries are devoting their research to the problem of moral, spiritual and personal human functioning (Rozuel 2011; Strohminger and Nichols 2014).
Moral and spiritual intelligence are helpful in solving problems concerning values, searching for the meaning of life events. They make it possible to look at a problem in a wider meaningful context, to make an adequate judgement of a given situation. Spiritual intelligence is not the result of just a combination of spirituality and intelligence, just as moral intelligence is not a simple combination of morality and intelligence. Both spiritual and moral intelligence derives from a substrate of cognitive, emotional, socio-moral processes and personal development. For a better understanding of the human being, it therefore seems important to look at spiritual intelligence, moral intelligence and to know the relationship between spiritual intelligence, moral intelligence and personality traits, which are dependent on genetic predisposition and environment (Bleidorn et al. 2014).
Spiritual intelligence is sometimes described as a form of intelligence that enables people to solve problems, to achieve certain goals in life (Emmons 1999, 2000a). It is also sometimes perceived as a set of adaptive capacities based on non-material and transcendental aspects of reality (King and DeCicco 2009).
Moral intelligence as commonly understood means the ability to distinguish right from wrong, to have rulesand norms of behaviour. It is also sometimes considered as an ability acquired and developed during life (Bahrami et al. 2012). It is related to an individual’s ability to process and manage ethical issues (Bezanjani et al. 2019). Nowadays, for young adults it is not a problem to cope with the processing of incoming information and technological developments. On the other hand, it is difficult to maintain respect for oneself and other people, to comply with current norms, standards and to search for meaning in everyday activities and life problems. The individualism, materialism and consumerism currently being promoted are leading to a growing fragility of the value system and an increase in narcissistic tendencies (Hyde 2008). The negative effects of the changes taking place can be seen in the current economic, political and environmental crises (Clarken 2010). The social and economic functioning of modern nations will depend on the level of moral and spiritual culture of young adults (Shevchenko et al. 2019).
During early adulthood, which lasts until about 18 to 35 years of age, young adults shape their own self, find their place in the world, enter into various life roles and developmental tasks (Havighurst 1981), show greater awareness, emotional stability and, to a lesser extent, agreeableness (Bleidorn 2015). These three traits characterise individuals demonstrating greater social maturity (Roberts and Mroczek 2008). Changes in personality structure contribute to more positive relationships, professional success and better health functioning (Roberts et al. 2007). During early adulthood, moral development is dominated by the conventional level (referring to social conventions). Individuals at this stage of life develop towards the post-conventional level. When attempting to solve moral problems, young adults appeal to the wisdom knowledge they possess. The purpose of this article is to examine the correlation between spiritual intelligence, moral intelligence and to test the assumption regarding the links between spiritual and moral intelligence and the age of the respondents. It also seems important to test the predictive role of personality traits in connection with spiritual and moral intelligence.

1.1. Spiritual Intelligence

Spiritual intelligence is sometimes perceived as a type of intelligence that enables a person to understand the mystery of life and its meaning. Gardner argues that‚ “if we humans can relate to the world of nature, we can also relate to the supernatural world - to the cosmos that extends beyond what we can perceive directly, to the mystery of our own existence, and to life-and-death experiences that transcend what we routinely encounter” (Gardner 1999, p. 54). The term of spiritual intelligence arose from the combination of intelligence and spirituality (Emmons 2000a; Mahasneh et al. 2015). In the field of psychology, this construct is controversial, due to the existing theoretical limitations, the lack of coherent definitions of the issue (Oman 2013; Skrzypińska 2014, 2020; Streib and Hood 2016). Spiritual intelligence is sometimes referred to as a third type of intelligence (Zohar and Marshall 2000), a mental capacity (King 2008), an ability to discern, search for meaning, solve problems (Nasel 2004), a set of adaptive mental capacities (King and DeCicco 2009), an ability to ask existential questions (Nasel 2004), and involves a high degree of individual and social awareness (Sadiku and Musa 2021).
Zohar and Marshall (2004) describe spiritual intelligence as an ability that allows people to discover and experience deeper levels of meaning and purpose in human existence and cherished values, while achieving the highest possible level of motivation. Furnham (2008) points out that‚ “it is the ability to master a set of diffuse and abstract concepts about being, but also mastering the craft of altering one’s consciousness in attaining a certain state of being” (Furnham 2008, p. 212). King (2008) described spiritual intelligence as “a set of mental capacities which contribute to the awareness, integration, and adaptive application of the nonmaterial and transcendent aspects of one’s existence, leading to such outcomes as deep existential reflection, enhancement of meaning, recognition of a transcendent self, and mastery of spiritual states” (p. 56). As Gardner (2000, p. 27) claims, “whether spirituality should be considered an intelligence depends upon definitions and criteria”. Gardner (2006, p. 20) believed that spiritual intelligence is “the intelligence of big questions”, as it relates to the human predisposition to ask basic questions about existence.
Spiritual intelligence consists of two components: transcendence and mysticism. The first component of intelligence is sometimes associated with going beyond physicality. Mysticism is sometimes described as having an awareness of ultimate reality (Emmons 2000a). King (2008) identifies four components of spiritual intelligence: critical existential thinking, personal meaning production, transcendental awareness, and conscious state expansion. Critical existential thinking (CET) involves thinking about one’s existence. It is sometimes described as the ability to critically contemplate meaning, purpose, skills that lead to original existential relationships, contemplating non-existential issues related to one’s own life.
Personal meaning production (PMP) is characterised by the ability to derive personal meaning and goals from all experiences of a mental and physical nature and the ability to live according to a specific life purpose. It represents the core of spiritual intelligence. The personal meaning production increases the motivation to take action, provides a sense of stability, order and direction. A person learns to see meaning in everyday activities, work, events both positive and negative.
Transcendental awareness (TA) involves the ability to intensify transcendental dimensions/patterns of the self, the physical world, other persons, when experiencing states of consciousness accompanied by the ability to identify a correlation with one’s Self and the physical world. It allows one to perceive reality more deeply, to penetrate the material world, to connect interdependent structures, to build relationships with the Self.
Conscious state expansion (CSE) is characterised by the ability to enter and exit higher states of consciousness (King 2008). A higher state of consciousness involves a narrowing or widening of the scope of attention. States of consciousness are achieved through methods such as self-contemplation, prayer and meditation.
Emmons (2000a) identified the traits of spiritually intelligentpeople, which included the ability to: transcend, enter higher states of consciousness, make meaning of everyday activities, relationships, use resources to solve problems, show forgiveness, be humble, be compassionate (being virtuous). The last of the indicated traits, due to its controversial nature, has been removed (Emmons 2000b).
Research conducted on spiritual intelligence and personality traits in the Big Five model has shown positive associations of conscientiousness, agreeableness and extraversion with spiritual intelligence. A negative correlation was shown between spiritual intelligence and neuroticism (Amrai et al. 2011). Idris et al. (2021), in the research conducted among trainee teachers, showed that there was a positive correlation of spiritual intelligence with extraversion and openness to experience. In contrast, no link was found between spiritual intelligence and acceptance, emotional stability and mindfulness. A similar research was conducted by Mahasneh and collaborators (2015), who found a positive and statistically significant correlation between dimensions of spiritual intelligence (critical existential thinking, personal meaning-production, transcendental awareness and conscious state expansion) and personality traits (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness).
Beshlideh et al. (2011) conducted a research on the correlation between spiritual intelligence and personality traits in male students. The results showed a significant correlation between extraversion, agreeableness and conscientiousness. A regression analysis by Madalaimuthu and Kadhiravan (2016) saw that spiritual intelligence accounted for 18% of the variance in extraversion, 5% of the variance in agreeableness, 11% of the variance in conscientiousness and 6% of the variance in emotional stability.
Research by Altalha (2020) showed that spiritual intelligence, age, gender, and specialisation are independent predictors of mental health. Desai (2016) showed that age has a significant impact on adult spirituality.
According to Zohar and Marshall (2000), spiritual intelligence helps us to experience life at a deeper level of meaning. It can be a helpful skill in dealing with issues of right and wrong. Seybold and Hill (2001) suggested that spiritual practices, commitments and beliefs are associated with positive mental and physical health outcomes, positive interactions with others and higher levels of quality of life. According to Emmons (2000a, 2000b), spiritual intelligence can protect a person from undesirable and maladaptive behaviours. The findings of the research conducted so far motivate the search for links between spiritual intelligence and moral behaviour, the formation of the ability to distinguish between right and wrong.

1.2. Moral Intelligence

Moral intelligence means doing things at the right time and for the right reasons (Yaghoobi et al. 2018). It is sometimes described as a guide for people to help them integrate their optimal actions (Lee and Hwang 2016). Moral intelligence can also be defined “as the mental capacity to determine how universal human principles should be applied to our personal values, goals, and actions” (Lennick and Kiel 2005, p. 7). Borba (2001), on the other hand, defines moral intelligence as the ability to distinguish right from wrong, to have ethical beliefs and to act in accordance with them. Moral intelligence includes social ethics, which are the general principles that build society, and personal ethics, which include individual behaviour and the virtues possessed by a person (Olusola and Samson 2015).
Lennick and Kiel (2005) identified four components of moral intelligence: integrity, responsibility, compassion and forgiveness. Integrity characterises a person’s behaviour in accordance with one’s principles, beliefs and values. It shows a strong connection with keeping promises, telling the truth and defending important values. In various situations in life, people with integrity act in accordance with their conscience. People characterised by a high degree of honesty are rational, independent and fair (Becker 1998). Responsibility is admitting one’s mistakes, failures, accepting responsibility for oneself, others and one’s choices (Stone-Johnson 2014). Accepting responsibility for one’s actions moves a person towards success and forces one in some way to avoid undesirable behaviour.
Forgiveness involves showing tolerance for the mistakes of others and one’s own. Compassion is characterised by concern for others, caring about their goals. It occurs when one observes other people in their difficulties and suffering and is then motivated to help them (Hojjat and Cramer 2013).
Moral intelligence is characterised by certain knowledge, certain behaviours, ways of thinking, acting and feeling. It allows a person to recognise what is right or wrong without contributing to changes in a person’s actions, abilities, or feelings (Beigi and Tabaeian 2014). Borba (2001) identifies seven virtues that underpin moral intelligence; these include: empathy (the ability to understand and feel other people’s problems); conscience (the ability to act according to principles of right action); self-control (involves control of the mind, action, thus enabling one to act appropriately), respect (the ability to appreciate others and acting in a friendly manner); kindness (showing concern, affection towards other people); tolerance (respecting the rights, dignity of others, taking into account the difference of their views and religion); honesty (openness in thinking, acting).
Mohagheghi et al. (2021) conducted a research on moral intelligence and personality traits. The results showed a positive correlation between moral intelligence and personality traits (honesty, modesty, conscientiousness and openness to experience).
Some researchers recognise that certain personality traits, for example, conscientiousness, agreeableness, neuroticism, have an impact on the moral orientation adopted and spiritual abilities manifested (Beshlideh et al. 2011). Golipour et al. (2014) conducted research on the correlation of spiritual and moral intelligence as important variables in the lives of Iranian volunteers. The results of the research showed a positive correlation between moral and spiritual intelligence and sense of life. Spiritual intelligence explained 5.6% of the observed difference in meaning of life among men and 18.6% among women. Furthermore, moral intelligence explained the variation in meaning of life among 13.2% of men and 6.8% of women. Ramezanzadeh and Moslehamirdehi (2020) showed in research conducted among students that spiritual and moral intelligence could significantly predict 25% of students’ resilience (p < 0.01).
Research by Moghadas and Khaleghi (2013) found a correlation between the components of moral intelligence (telling the truth, keeping promises, taking responsibility for personal choices, admitting mistakes and failures, accepting responsibility for serving others, actively caring for others and being able to accept others’ mistakes) and age.

1.3. Personality Traits in the Big Five Model

Personality is defined as “the set of psychological traits and mechanisms within the individual that are organised and relatively enduring and that influence his or her interactions with, and adaptations to, the intra-psychic, physical and social environments” (Larsen and Buss 2014, p. 4). Personality is sometimes defined as a set of traits that are revealed in a person’s behaviour, thinking and everyday situations (Allemand and Mehl 2017). Costa and McCrae (1992) describe personality as physical and intellectual traits, habits that include specific emotional, thinking and behavioural patterns in a variety of situations.
Personality traits are defined as certain dispositions to perform a certain type of response in different situations (Rauch and Frese 2007). Personality is sometimes referred to relatively-fixed behavioural inclinations related to well-being, health and relationships (Tetzner et al. 2020). A well-known and widely accepted concept of personality is the Five Factor Model of Personality (Big Five), which consists of five personality factors: conscientiousness, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and neuroticism (McCrae and Costa 1997). The Big Five model, according to Goldberg (1993), makes it possible to predict behavioural patterns and social outcomes. Personality traits as indicated by various analyses are relatively stable over the course of a life (Johnson 1997). Over the course of a human life, conscientiousness and agreeableness remain relatively stable traits (De Vries et al. 2021). As they enter adulthood, young people’s experiences increase in agreeableness, emotional stability and conscientiousness (Arnett 2000; Bleidorn et al. 2014).
Conscientiousness is associated with a sense of self-control, responsibility to others, following rules, being hardworking, striving to maintain high standards, and being dutiful (Jackson and Roberts 2017; Shiner and DeYoung 2013). The trait is also associated with perseverance and planning (Jackson and Hill 2019).
Extraversion is sometimes equated with sociability, positive emotionality, entering new situations, and assertiveness (Shiner and DeYoung 2013; Smillie et al. 2019). It positively correlates with well-being, seeking support in difficult situations (Sun et al. 2019). Individuals with low levels of this trait are closed-minded and tend to withdraw from social exposure because it represents a large psychological cost for them (Tetzner et al. 2020).
Openness to experience is associated with a willingness to engage in new activities, learning about new ideas, curiosity, and cognitive exploration (Schwaba 2019). This trait is also associated with creative problem solving and autonomy (Kandler et al. 2016).
Agreeableness is sometimes linked to traits such as empathy, compassion, modesty, and kindness (Soto and John 2017; Tackett et al. 2019). Agreeable individuals strive to have cooperative, harmonious interactions with other people.
Neuroticism is sometimes associated with experiencing emotions such as uncertainty, sadness, irritability and anxiety (Shiner and DeYoung 2013). It is also associated with social maladjustment, specific difficulties with cognitive processing, e.g., negative perceptions of self, including one’s competence (Shackman et al. 2016).

1.4. The Present Study

It should be noted that very little research has been undertaken to date on the relationship between spiritual and moral intelligence. Recent studies have indicated associations of spiritual intelligence with personality traits (positive with conscientiousness, agreeableness, extraversion and negative with neuroticism; Amrai et al. 2011; Beshlideh et al. 2011; Farsani et al. 2013; Sood et al. 2012) and moral intelligence with personality traits (conscientiousness, openness to experience, modesty and honesty; Mohagheghi et al. 2021). There have also been studies on the relationship of moral intelligence, spiritual intelligence, social intelligence and emotional intelligence and the relationship of spiritual intelligence and moral intelligence (Golipour et al. 2014). The present study aimed to investigate the relationship between spiritual and moral intelligence. In addition, in order to better analyse the group, it was also planned to investigate the relationship of spiritual and moral intelligence with age. It was also intended to examine the relationship between spiritual and moral intelligence and personality traits among young people in early adulthood from Poland. So far, no research has been conducted in Poland which would allow the relationship between spiritual and moral intelligence and age and personality traits to be discerned in young people in early adulthood.

2. Materials and Methods

On the basis of the literature review, the following hypotheses were put forward:
H1. 
There is a correlation between selected aspects of spiritual intelligence and moral intelligence in individuals in early adulthood.
H2. 
Personality traits play a predictive role in the association with spiritual intelligence and moral intelligence in young adults.

2.1. Procedure and Respondents

The online survey was conducted from 25 April to 30 September 2022 using the LimeSurvey. The instructions included information about the persons conducting the survey, the topic of the survey, anonymity, voluntary participation and the purpose of the survey. The method of reaching respondents was the snowball sampling and the method of sending out e-mail invitations. Respondents had the option to split the survey into parts. A total of 405 surveys were collected, of which 160 were fully completed. Only surveys that were fully completed were used for further analysis. A total of 160 people (101 women and 59 men) with an age range of 18 to 35 years (M = 24.40; SD = 4.55) were surveyed. The majority of respondents were unmarried. The largest number of respondents lived in cities with more than 300,000 inhabitants (33%), the smallest in towns with up to 10,000 inhabitants (6%). Most respondents had siblings (84%). The majority of respondents had secondary education (53% of respondents) and higher education (43% of respondents). The fewest people had primary education (1%).

2.2. Methods

The Spiritual Intelligence Self-Report Inventory (SISRI-24) is a scale based on King’s definition and the four fundamental components of spiritual intelligence: critical existential thinking, personal meaning production, transcendental awareness and conscious state expansion (King 2008). In the Polish adaptation: Skrzypińska and Atroszko (2017). The survey questionnaire consists of 24 questions, which are assigned to 4 subscales: critical existential thinking (CET), personal meaning production (PMP), transcendental awareness (TA), and conscious state expansion (CSE). The respondents give their answers on a five-point Likert scale: 1“this is completely untrue about me”; 2 “this is not completely true about me”; 3 “there is some truth in this about me”; 4 “there is a lot of truth in this about me”; 5 “this is completely true about me”. Sample questions from the subscales: critical existential thinking (“I happen to spend time contemplating the purpose or cause of my existence”), personal meaning-making (“I am able to define the purpose and cause of my existence”), transcendental awareness (“I am aware of a deeper purpose between myself and other people”), expanding state of consciousness (“I am able to reach higher states of consciousness”).
King reported Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the subscales of 0.78, 0.78, 0.87 and 0.91 for critical existential thinking, personal meaning production, transcendental awareness and conscious state expansion, respectively. The calculated Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the subscales: critical existential thinking, personal meaning making, transcendental consciousness state expansion were 0.886, 0.747, 0.849, 0.904, respectively.
The Polish adaptation of the SISIR-24 questionnaire showed a four-factor structure similar to the original method. There was no overall SI factor in the Polish sample (Atroszko et al. 2021).
The Yakut-Moral Intelligence Scale (Yakut and Yakut 2021), consists of 20 questions. The scale contains four subscales defined as empathy, conscience, self-control and courtesy. Factor analysis determined that the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin coefficient value was 0.776 statistically (Yakut and Yakut 2021). The reliability of the scale as determined by the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient is 0.845. The items of the survey questionnaire are rated on a five-point Likert (1 “Strongly disagree”, 2 “Disagree”, 3 “I am not sure”, 4 “Agree” and 5 “Completely agree” (Yakut and Yakut 2021)). The scale does not have a Polish adaptation. For the purposes of the article, the survey questionnaire was translated into Polish.
Example subscale items: empathy (‘When I see someone sad, it makes me sad too’), conscience (‘Conscience is important in communication’), self-control (‘I can control my emotions such as anger, joy and sadness’), politeness (‘In conversation, I respect the rights of the people I am talking to’).
The Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient calculated on the scores obtained for the scale was 0.842. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the individual subscales was: for the empathy scale 0.679, for the conscientiousness scale 0.828, for self-control 0.851, and for the courtesy scale 0.994.
The Ten Item Personality Inventory TIPI (Gosling et al. 2003), TIPI-PL in the Polish adaptation (Sorokowska et al. 2014), contains 10 items that form five scales: emotional stability, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness.
The respondent answers on a seven-point scale from 1- strongly disagree to 7- strongly agree. Sample questions for the subscales: Emotional stability (“Not worried, emotionally stable”), Extraversion (“Enjoys the company of others, active, optimistic”), Conscientiousness (“Conscientious, disciplined”), Openness to experience (“Open to new experiences, in a complex way of perceiving the world”), agreeableness (“According, kind”).
The reliability of the scale, as determined by Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, is between 0.45 and 0.83. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient calculated for each subscale of emotional stability, extraversion, conscientiousness, openness to experience, agreeableness was 0.634, 0.683, 0.647, 0.443, 0.592. The scale was chosen because of its satisfactory psychometric qualities and its compatibility with the adopted five-factor theory of personality.

3. Results

The interpretation of the results obtained was based on the recalculated results obtained using the three research tools. Statistical calculations were carried out using the IBM SPSS Statistics 28.0 programme. In the first step of the analyses carried out, the distributions of the quantitative variables were examined. For this purpose, basic descriptive statistics were calculated together with the Kolomogorov-Smirnov test to check the normality of the distribution, as shown in Table 1.
The results obtained from the Kolomogrov-Smirnov test indicate that the distribution is not normal for most scales. However, given the central limit theorem, which states that a trait assumes a normal distribution when there are at least 30 respondents and the fact that no measure of skewness exceeded an absolute value of 1 (George and Mallery 2016), it can be assumed that the examined variables take on a distribution close to normal. The first aim of the research was to determine the correlations occurring between moral and spiritual intelligence. In the first stage of the analyses, Pearson’s R correlation was carried out (see Table 2).
There are positive correlations between spiritual and moral intelligence scales in individuals in early adulthood. Respondents showed positive correlations of the moral intelligence subscale of empathy with critical existential thinking (r = 0.340), transcendental awareness (r = 0.280), and conscious state expansion (r = 0.206). The moral intelligence conscience subscale showed positive correlations with critical thinking (r = 0.267), personal meaning production (r = 0.238), transcendental awareness (r = 0.342) and conscious state expansion (r = 0.232). The self-control subscale from the moral intelligence scale showed positive correlations with personal meaning production (r = 0.328) and transcendental awareness (r = 0.174). Courtesy, however, from the moral intelligence subscales showed a positive correlation with critical existential thinking (r = 0.168), personal meaning production (r = 0.285) and transcendental awareness (r = 0.241).
The second aim of the study was to make correlations between spiritual, moral intelligence and age (see Table 2). In the analyses conducted, a positive correlation of the self-control moral intelligence subscale with age was found (r = 0.223). The other subscales of moral and spiritual intelligence showed no significant correlation with age.
The third aim of the analyses conducted was to test whether personality traits play a predictive role in the relationship between spiritual intelligence and moral intelligence in young adults, a multiple regression analysis was conducted with personality traits as predictors of spiritual intelligence (cf. Table 3) and a multiple regression analysis with personality traits as predictors of moral intelligence (cf. Table 4). One assumption of the regression model is the presence of independence of observation errors. The Durbin-Watson test was used to assess autocorrelation among the residuals. All the models obtained had a Durbin-Watson index of 1.5–1.9, which justifies the use of regression, as the predictors are correlated with each other to a small degree. An input regression model was used in the analyses. This type of variable selection procedure allows all variables to be entered.
A regression model of the dependence of spiritual intelligence on four predictors was found to be statistically significant F(5.154) = 12.349, p < 0.001; the model explains 26.3% of the variance in the dependent variable. Emotional stability proved to be the most important predictor in explaining personal meaning production (beta = 0.258). Next, in terms of importance, was extraversion (beta = 0.190), followed by openness to experience (beta = 0.181) and conscientiousness (beta = 0.156).
A regression model of the dependence of moral intelligence dimension of self-control on a single predictor was found to be statistically significant F(5.154) = 11.083, p<0.001. The model explains 24.1% of the variance. Emotional stability proved to be a significant predictor (beta = 0.445).

4. Discussion

In the research conducted, the first aim was to examine the relationship between spiritual and moral intelligence in people in early adulthood from Poland. The second aim was, when analysing the study group, to additionally take into account age and its relationship with spiritual and moral intelligence. The third aim was to examine the relationship between spiritual and moral intelligence and personality traits.
The analyses conducted in the first hypothesis revealed the presence of positive relationships between the dimensions of spiritual intelligence (critical existential thinking, personal meaning production, transcendental awareness and conscious state expansion) and the dimensions of moral intelligence (empathy, conscience, self-control and courtesy) in people in early adulthood. In a study by Ramezanzadeh and Moslehamirdehi (2020), there was a similar association between spiritual and moral intelligence. Spiritual and moral intelligence have been shown to be helpful in solving one’s own and other people’s problems. They contribute to a better understanding of one’s own and others’ difficulties in life (Emmons 1999; Nasel 2004). On the basis of analyses, it appears that a better knowledge of oneself in the world and the ability to ask existential questions may be helpful in understanding and experiencing difficulties experienced by other people (Borba 2001; Nasel 2004). The ability to experience normal as well as deeper states’ and experience deeper states of consciousness has been shown to be helpful in motivating oneself to act for others (Borba 2001; Zohar and Marshall 2004). A person, according to analyses, who has a critical view of their life, skills and life goals, is more likely to be guided by principles of right action in that life (Borba 2001; King 2008). Living in harmony with one’s life goals allows one to better adhere to certain norms and rules in daily functioning (King and DeCicco 2009; King 2008). The ability to build harmony with the physical world, one’s own “I”, to enter and go beyond higher states of consciousness promotes actions in line with one’s own rules of conduct (Borba 2001; Emmons 2000a; King 2008; Zohar and Marshall 2004). According to the analyses, people who experience connectivity with the world and their actions are more in control of their actions and act in accordance with the norms (Borba 2001; King 2008). The results of the conducted analyses allowed us to perceive a relationship between critical reference to the importance of one’s goals, oneself in the world and the manifestation of caring, positive feelings towards other people (King 2008; Lee and Hwang 2016; Yakut and Yakut 2021).
In addition, when analysing the study group, age was taken into account as a factor that could contribute to a better understanding of the functioning of the analysed group. In the analyses conducted, there was a relationship between the subscale of moral intelligence self-control and age. Self-control is an important element in the formation of morality, the regulation of behaviour (Hirtenlehner and Kunz 2016; Morán-Soto and Benson 2018; Olivares and Ceglie 2020; Vitell et al. 2009). Similar correlations in ongoing research were seen by Tangney et al. (2004), who showed that a person with high levels of self-control has greater attachment to professed values and, thus, finds it more difficult to violate professed moral principles. Previous research indicates that self-control is associated with age (Greenberg 2008; Pratt 2016). By adolescence, there is a propensity for sensation-seeking in young people, a greater need to take risks. In later stages of life, self-control is somewhat stabilised due to the maturation processes taking place (Caspi and Roberts 2001; Hay and Meldrum 2016; Pratt 2016).
The relationship between spiritual intelligence, moral intelligence and personality traits was also analysed in the third hypothesis. Personality traits, as indicated by the results of the analyses, play a predictive role in the relationship between spiritual intelligence and moral intelligence in early adolescents. In the case of spiritual intelligence, the dimension of personal meaning making had the best fit, while in moral intelligence the dimension of self-control in association with personality traits had the best fit.
For the spiritual intelligence dimension of creating personal meaning, emotional stability, extraversion, openness to experience and conscientiousness were found to be significant predictors. Emotionally stable individuals are characterised by calmness, the ability to cope with stress, and the absence of feelings of inner tension and irritability (Kraczla 2017). They more easily perceive the significance of life events experienced and have a sense of control over their lives (King 2008).
Extraversion was also found to be a positive predictor of spiritual intelligence. Individuals characterised as sociable, friendly in social interactions, and entering new situations are more likely to establish the importance of life goals, define their interests and also set life plans (King 2008; Shiner and DeYoung 2013; Smillie et al. 2019; Sun et al. 2019). Sternberg (1997) saw similar relationships.
Additionally, openness to experience is a predictor of spiritual intelligence, which has been confirmed in ongoing research. Openness to experience characterises people who are eager to engage in new tasks, demonstrate cognitive curiosity (Schwaba 2019), and seek creative solutions to problems when experiencing difficulties (Gardner 2000; Kandler et al. 2016; Nasel 2004). People who are open to experiences perceive meaning in the daily actions they take, both positive and negative (King 2008).
Conscientiousness has also been shown to be an important predictor of the personal meaning-making dimension of spiritual intelligence. Conscientious individuals who are characterised by being responsible to others, following rules, and striving to maintain high standards are more likely to seek meaning in experienced difficult situations (King 2008; Nasel 2004). They show greater self-motivation, which provides them with greater stability, dutifulness and a sense of meaning in the activities they pursue (Jackson and Roberts 2017; Shiner and DeYoung 2013). Research by Saroglou (2010) also found a positive relationship between conscientiousness and spiritual intelligence. A conscientious person relates his or her activities to professed values, seldom succumbs to negative emotions, and experiences a dreamlessness of pursued activities.
Spiritual intelligence has been shown to be positively related to personality traits: extraversion, conscientiousness and agreeableness, (Amrai et al. 2011) and emotional stability (Madalaimuthu and Kadhiravan 2016).
Emotional stability has been shown to play an important predictive role in moral intelligence in the self-control dimension. Emotionally stable individuals cope better with tension and stress (Kraczla 2017), in situations where it is necessary to optimise their own actions (Lee and Hwang 2016). Even when experiencing various stressors, they are able to defend the values that are most important to them act in accordance with their conscience and take responsibility for their actions (Borba 2001; Lennick and Kiel 2005). Being able to control one’s own actions contributes to being more in line with certain norms of behaviour (Borba 2001).
The research carried out has identified some links between spiritual and moral intelligence in people in early adulthood. Some connections between spiritual intelligence, moral intelligence and personality traits were also recognised. The changes taking place in the personality of young people are conducive to the formation of a mature personality characterised by emotional stability, openness to experience, extraversion, conscientiousness and self-control (Bleidorn et al. 2014; Roberts and Mroczek 2008).

5. Limitations

The research was carried out on a group of 160 people with a clear female predominance. In conducting similar surveys in the future, it would be advisable to aim for equal groups of respondents. In conducting the research, the researchers relied on the availability of the respondent. Given that the survey sample does not accurately represent the population, the analyses conducted have a lower level of generalisability compared to probability sampling. In the future, when conducting this type of research, it would be worthwhile to use stratified sampling. The age of the people analysed is also a limitation of the research conducted. The sample is clearly dominated by young adults in the upper age range of early adulthood. When planning similar research in the future, it would be worth extending the age range of the persons analysed to include the developmental period of middle adulthood, in which the cognitive processes taking place, personality changes (larger distance, autonomy, level of reflexivity) may be relevant to the moral and spiritual functioning of persons. The research presented here is a preliminary report allowing us to see some relationships between spiritual and moral intelligence and personality traits. Further in-depth analyses are necessary to gain a deeper understanding of the relationships between the variables analysed.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.D. and E.R.; methodology, M.D. and E.R.; software, M.D.; validation, M.D.; formal analysis, M.D. and E.R.; investigation, M.D.; resources, M.D.; data curation, M.D.; writing—original draft preparation, M.D.; writing—review and editing, M.D. and E.R.; visualization, M.D.; supervision, M.D. and E.R.; project administration, M.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by Ethics Committee for Scientific Research, Institute Psychology of The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, protocol code: KEBN_10/2022, date of approval: 6 April 2022.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data supporting reported results are available in the Institutional Repository of the John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin at the link http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12153/3946 (accessed on 27 December 2022).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Allemand, Mathias, and Matthias R. Mehl. 2017. Personality assessment in daily life: A roadmap for future personality development research. In Personality Development Across the Lifespan. Edited by Jule Specht. Cambridge: Elsevier Academic Press, pp. 437–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Altalha, Ghada Saad Suliman. 2020. The Role of Spiritual Intelligence, Age, Gender, and Specialization in Predicting University Students’ Mental Health. Available online: https://www.eimj.org/uplode/images/photo/The_Role_of_Spiritual_Intelligence..pdf (accessed on 8 November 2022).
  3. Amrai, Kourosh, Ali Farahani, Mojtaba Ebrahimi, and Valiollah Bagherian. 2011. Relationship between Personality Traits and Spiritual Intelligence among University Students. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 15: 609–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Arnett, Jeffrey Jensen. 2000. Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist 55: 469–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Asif, Tahseen, Ouyang Guangming, Muhammad Asif Haider, Jordi Colomer, Sumaira Kayani, and Noor ul Amin. 2020. Moral education for sustainable development: Comparison of university teachers’ perceptions in China and Pakistan. Sustainability 12: 3014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Atroszko, Paweł A., Katarzyna Skrzypińska, and Julia M. Balcerowska. 2021. Is There a General Factor of Spiritual Intelligence? Factorial Validity of the Polish Adaptation of Spiritual Intelligence Self-Report Inventory. Journal of Religion and Health 60: 3591–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Ayoub, Alaa Eldin A., and Abdullah M. Aljughaiman. 2016. A Predictive Structural Model for Gifted Students’ Performance: A Study Based on Intelligence and its Implicite Theories. Learning and Individual Differences 51: 11–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bagherpur, Masoumeh, Hasan Abdollahzadeh, Mahbobeh Eskandary Rad, and Marjan Kolagar. 2022. Effectiveness of spirituality-based problem solving on moral responsibility, and spiritual education of students. International Journal of Children’s Spirituality 27: 10–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bahrami, Mohammad Amin, Maryam Asami, Azadeh Fatehpanah, Arefeh Dehghani Tafti, and Gholamreza Ahmadi Tehrani. 2012. Moral intelligence status of the faculty members and staff of the Shahid Sadoughi University of Medical Sciences of Yazd. Iranian Journal of Medical Ethics and History of Medicine 5: 81–95. [Google Scholar]
  10. Beazley, Debra, and Gary Gemmill. 2006. Spirituality and Servant Leader Behavior. Journal of Management, Spirituality & Religion 3: 258–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Becker, Thomas. 1998. Integrity in organisations: Beyond honesty and conscientiousness. Academy of Management Review 23: 154–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Beigi, Khatereh Mohammad, and A. Tabaeian. 2014. Relationship between moral intelligence and coping strategies with conflicts among Taekwondo coaches of Iran with demographic characteristics. International Journal of Sport Studies 4: 1075–79. [Google Scholar]
  13. Beshlideh, Kiumars, Morteza Charkhabi, Mojtaba AmanollahNejad Kalkhoran, and Seyed-Ali Marashi. 2011. Relationship between Personality Traits and Spiritual Intelligence in Male Students of Shahid Chamran University at Ahvaz. International Journal of Psychology 5: 21–34. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bezanjani, Kaveh Nouhi, Hamdollah Manzari Tavakoli, Sanjar Salajeghe, and Ayyub Sheikhi. 2019. Authentic Leadership and Moral Intelligence of Nurses in Kerman University of Medical Sciences Hospitals. Journal of Payavard Salamat 13: 13–23. [Google Scholar]
  15. Binet, Alfred, and Th. Simon. 1904. Méthodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic du niveau intellectuel des anormaux. L’Année Psychologique 11: 191–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bingham, Walter Van Dyke. 1937. Aptitudes and Aptitude Testing. New York: Harper & Brothers. [Google Scholar]
  17. Bleidorn, Wiebke. 2015. What Accounts for Personality Maturation in Early Adulthood? Current Directions in Psychological Science 24: 245–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Bleidorn, Wiebke, Christian Kandler, and Avshalom Caspi. 2014. The Behavioural Genetics of Personality Development in Adulthood—Classic, Contemporary, and Future Trends. European Journal of Personality 28: 244–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Borba, Michele. 2001. Building Moral Intelligence: The Seven Essential Virtues that Teach Kids to Do the Right Thing. Binghamton: Taylor & Francis LLC. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cash, Karen C., and George R. Gray. 2000. A framework for accommodating religion and spirituality in the workplace. Academy of Management Perspectives 14: 124–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Caspi, Avshalom, and Brent W. Roberts. 2001. Personality development across the life-course: The argument for change and continuity. Psychological Inquiry 12: 49–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Clarken, Rodney H. 2010. Considering moral intelligence as a part of holistic education. Moral Intelligence. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Denver, CO, USA, April 30–May 4; pp. 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  23. Costa, Paul T., and Robert R. McCrae. 1992. The five-factor model of personality and its relevance to personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders 6: 343–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. De Vries, Jantje Hinrika, Maik Spengler, Andreas Frintrup, and Patrick Mussel. 2021. Personality Development in Emerging Adulthood-How the Perception of Life Events and Mindset Affect Personality Trait Change. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 671421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Desai, Aditi. 2016. Efficacy of gender and age on spirituality and psychological well-being of adults. The International Journal of Indian Psychology 3: 83–92. [Google Scholar]
  26. Emmons, Robert. 1999. The Psychology of Ultimate Concerns: Motivation and Spirituality in Personality. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  27. Emmons, Robert. 2000a. Is spirituality an intelligence? Motivation, cognition, and the psychology of ultimate concern. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 10: 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Emmons, Robert. 2000b. Spirituality and intelligence: Problems and prospects. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 10: 57–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Farsani, Maryam Eslami, Shahram Arofzad, and Taghi Agha Hosaini. 2013. The study of the relationship spirituals intelligence with personality traits among physical education managers in Isfahan province. Advances in Applied Science Research 4: 140–44. [Google Scholar]
  30. Furnham, Adrian. 2008. Personality and Intelligence at Work: Exploring and Explaining Individual Differences at Work. London: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis (UK); Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Gardner, Howard. 1999. Intelligence Reframed: Multiple Intelligences for the 21st Century. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gardner, Howard. 2000. A case against spiritual intelligence. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 10: 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Gardner, Howard. 2006. Multiple Intelligences: New Horizons in Theory and Practice. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  34. George, Darren, and Paul Mallery. 2016. IBM SPSS Statistics 23 Step by Step: A Simple Guide and Reference, 14th ed. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  35. Goldberg, Lewis R. 1993. The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist 48: 26–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Golipour, Sadigheh, Sara Haghighat, and Davood Manavipour. 2014. The Relationship between Spiritual and Moral Intelligence with the Meaning of Life among the Volunteers of the Emergent Reaction. The Neuroscience Journal of Shefaye Khatam 2: 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. González-Treviño, Irma Marcela, Georgina Mayela Núñez-Rocha, Jesús Marco Valencia-Hernández, and Arturo Arrona-Palacios. 2020. Assessment of multiple intelligences in elementary school students in Mexico: An exploratory study. Heliyon 6: e03777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Gosling, Samuel D., Peter J. Rentfrow, and William B. Swann Jr. 2003. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality 37: 504–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Greenberg, David. 2008. Age, sex, and racial distributions of crime. In Out of Control. Assessing the General Theory of crime. Edited by Erich Goode. Stanford: Stanford University Press, pp. 38–48. [Google Scholar]
  40. Havighurst, Robert J. 1981. Developmental Tasks and Education. New York: Longman. [Google Scholar]
  41. Hay, Carter, and Ryan Meldrum. 2016. Self-Control and Crime Over the Life Course. London: Sage Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  42. Hirtenlehner, Helmut, and Franziska Kunz. 2016. The interaction between self-control and morality in crime causation among older adults. European Journal of Criminology 13: 393–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Hojjat, Mahzad, and Duncan Cramer, eds. 2013. Positive Psychology of Love, 2013rd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hyde, Brendan. 2008. I Wonder What You Think Really, Really Matters? Spiritual Questing and Religious Education. Religious Education 103: 32–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Idris, Zuraini, Ku Suhaila Ku Johari, and Abu Yazid Abu Bakar. 2021. Relationship between Personality Trait and Spiritual Intelligence among Trainee Teachers at the Institute of Teacher Education Malaysia. International Journal of Academic Research in Business and Social Sciences 11: 2333–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Jackson, Joshua J., and Brent W. Roberts. 2017. Conscientiousness. In The Oxford Handbook of the Five Factor Model. Edited by Thomas A. Widiger. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 133–47. [Google Scholar]
  47. Jackson, Joshua J., and Patrick L. Hill. 2019. Lifespan development of conscientiousness. In Handbook of Personality Development. Edited by Dan P. McAdams, Rebecca L. Shiner and Jennifer L. Tackett. New York: Guilford, pp. 153–70. [Google Scholar]
  48. Johnson, John. 1997. Units of analysis for the description and explanation of personality. In Handbook of Personality Psychology. Edited by Robert Hogan and Stephen Briggs. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 73–93. [Google Scholar]
  49. Kandler, Christian, Rainer Riemann, Alois Angleitner, Frank M. Spinath, Peter Borkenau, and Lars Penke. 2016. The nature of creativity: The roles of genetic factors, personality traits, cognitive abilities, and environmental sources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 111: 230–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. King, David B. 2008. Rethinking Claims of Spiritual Intelligence: A Definition, Model, and Measure. Master’s thesis, Trent University, Peterborough, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
  51. King, David B., and Teresa L. DeCicco. 2009. A viable model and self-report measure of spiritual intelligence. International Journal of Transpersonal Studies 28: 68–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Kraczla, Magdalena. 2017. Osobowość jako czynnik zachowań menedżerskich w świetle teorii Wielkiej Piątki [Personality as a factor in managerial behaviour in the light of the Big Five theory]. Zeszyty Naukowe. Organizacja i Zarządzanie/Politechnika Śląska 105: 195–208. [Google Scholar]
  53. Krishnakumar, Sukumarakurup, and Christopher P. Neck. 2002. The “what”, “why” and “how” of spirituality in the workplace. Journal of Managerial Psychology 17: 153–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Larsen, Randy, and David Buss. 2014. Personality Psychology: Domains of Knowledge About Human Nature, 5th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill Education. [Google Scholar]
  55. Lee, Chang Seek, and Yeoun Kyung Hwang. 2016. The structural relationships between social support, emotional intelligence, self-esteem, and hope in rural elementary school students. Indian Journal of Science and Technology 9: 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  56. Lennick, Doug, and Fred Kiel. 2005. Moral Intelligence: Enhancing Business Performance and Leadership Success. New Jersey: Wharton School Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  57. Madalaimuthu, A., and Subramanian Kadhiravan. 2016. Influence of Spiritual Intelligence on Personality Factors Among College Students. International Journal of Education and Psychological Research 5: 75–77. [Google Scholar]
  58. Mahasneh, Ahmad M., Nizar A. Shammout, Ziad M. Alkhazaleh, Ahmed F. Al-Alwan, and Jawhara D. Abu-Eita. 2015. The relationship between spiritual intelligence and personality traits among Jordanian university students. Psychology Research and Behavior Management 8: 89–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. McCrae, Robert R., and Paul T. Costa. 1997. Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist 52: 509–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. McGhee, Peter, and Patricia Grant. 2008. Spirituality and Ethical Behaviour in the Workplace: Wishful Thinking or Authentic Reality. Electronic Journal of Business Ethics and Organization Studies 13: 61–69. [Google Scholar]
  61. Mitroff, Ian I., and Elizabeth A. Denton. 1999. A study of spirituality in the workplace. Sloan Management Review, Summer 40: 83–92. [Google Scholar]
  62. Moghadas, Maedeh, and Maryam Khaleghi. 2013. Investigate of relationship between moral intelligence and distress tolerance in Isfahan staff. International Journal of Research In Social Sciences 2: 26–30. [Google Scholar]
  63. Mohagheghi, Hossein, Mehran Farhadi, Khosro Rashid, and Arezou Delfan Beiranvand. 2021. Predicting moral behavior based on moral intelligence and personality traits: The mediating role of the structure self-transcendence. Journal of Fundamentals of Mental Health 23: 273–84. [Google Scholar]
  64. Morán-Soto, Gustavo, and Lisa Benson. 2018. Relationship of Mathematics Self-efficacy and Competence with Behaviors and Attitudes of Engineering Students with Poor Mathematics preparation. International Journal of Education in Mathematics, Science and Technology (IJEMST) 6: 200–20. [Google Scholar]
  65. Nasel, Dagmar. 2004. Spiritual Orientation in Relation to Spiritual Intelligence: A Consideration of Traditional Christianity and New Ageindividualistic Spirituality. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University of South Australia, Adelaide, Australia. [Google Scholar]
  66. Olivares, Vidal, and Robert J. Ceglie. 2020. The Intergenerational Transmission of Mathematics Attitudes. International Journal of Education in Mathematics, Science and Technology (IJEMST) 8: 76–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Olusola, Olayiwola Idowu, and Ajayi Oluwagbemiga Samson. 2015. Moral Intelligence: An Antidote to Examination Malpractices in Nigerian Schools. Universal Journal of Educational Research 3: 32–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Oman, Doug. 2013. Defining religion and spirituality. In Handbook of the Psychology of Religion and Spirituality. Edited by Raymond F. Paloutzian and Crystal L. Park. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 23–47. [Google Scholar]
  69. Pratt, Travis. 2016. A self-control/life-course theory of criminal behavior. European Journal of Criminology 13: 129–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Ramezanzadeh, Soraya, and Hadi Moslehamirdehi. 2020. Explain the relationship between moral intelligence, spiritual intelligence and psychological resilience in students: The role of mediating spiritual intelligence. Survay In Teaching Humanities 6: 29–38. [Google Scholar]
  71. Rauch, Andreas, and Michael Frese. 2007. Let’s put the person back into entrepreneurship research: A meta-analysis on the relationship between business owners’ personality traits, business creation, and success. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 16: 353–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Roberts, Brent W., and Daniel Mroczek. 2008. Personality trait change in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science 17: 31–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Roberts, Brent W., Nathan Kuncel, Rebecca Shiner, Avsholom Caspi, and Lewis Goldberg. 2007. The Power of Personality: The Comparative Validity of Personality Traits, Socioeconomic Status, and Cognitive Ability for Predicting Important Life Outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science 2: 313–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Rozuel, Cécile. 2011. The Moral Threat of Comparmentalization: Self, Roles and Responsibility. Journal of Business Ethics 102: 685–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Sadiku, Matthew N. O., and Sarhan M. Musa. 2021. Spiritual Intelligence. In A Primer on Multiple Intelligence. Edited by Matthew N. O. Sadiku and Sarhan M. Musa. Cham: Springer, pp. 55–68. [Google Scholar]
  76. Saroglou, Vassilis. 2010. Religiousness as a cultural adaptation of basic traits: A five-factor model perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Review 14: 108–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  77. Schwaba, Ted. 2019. The structure, measurement, and development of openness to experience cross adulthood. In Handbook of Personality Development. Edited by Dan P. McAdams, Rebecca L. Shiner and Jennifer L. Tackett. New York: Guilford, pp. 185–200. [Google Scholar]
  78. Seybold, Kevin S., and Peter C. Hill. 2001. The role of religion and spirituality in mental and physical health. Current Directions in Psychological Science 10: 21–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Shackman, Alexander J., Mellisa D. Stockbridge, Rachael M. Tillman, Claire M. Kaplan, Do P. Tromp, Andrew S. Fox, and Matthias Gamer. 2016. The Neurobiology of Dispositional Negativity and Attentional Biases to Threat: Implications for Understanding Ankiety Disorders in Adults and Youth. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology 7: 311–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Shevchenko, Ylia, Luydmyla Moskalyova, Olga Kanarova, and Olga Poznanska. 2019. Development of a System for Improving Future Teachers’ Readiness for the Child’s Spiritual and Moral Development in a Cross-Cultural Space. Journal of History Culture and Art Research 8: 251–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Shiner, Rebecca L., and Colin G. DeYoung. 2013. The structure of temperament and personality traits: A developmental perspective. In The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology. Edited by Philip David Zelazo. New York: Oxford University Press, vol. 2, pp. 113–41. [Google Scholar]
  82. Skrzypińska, Katarzyna. 2014. The Threefold Nature of Spirituality (TNS) in a Psychological Cognitive Framework. Archive for Psychology of Religion 36: 277–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Skrzypińska, Katrzyna. 2020. Does Spiritual Intelligence (SI) exist? A theoretical investigation of a tool useful for finding the meaning of life. Journal of Religion and Health 60: 500–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Skrzypińska, Katarzyna, and Paweł A. Atroszko. 2017. SISRI-24 tłumaczenie i adaptacja. Polish adaptation of Spiritual Intelligence Self-Report Inventory (SISRI-24). Project. [Google Scholar]
  85. Smillie, Luke D., Margaret L. Kern, and Mirko Uljarevic. 2019. Extraversion: Description, development, and mechanisms. In Handbook of Personality Development. Edited by Dan P. McAdams, Rebecca L. Shiner and Jennifer L. Tackett. New York: Guilford, pp. 118–36. [Google Scholar]
  86. Socha, Paweł red. 2000. Duchowy rozwój człowieka. Fazy życia, osobowość, wiara, religijność. [Human spiritual development. Life phases, personality, faith, religiosity]. Kraków: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego. [Google Scholar]
  87. Sood, Sarita, Arti Bakhshi, and Richa Gupta. 2012. Relationship personality traits, spiritual intelligence and well-being in university students. Journal of Education and Practice 3: 55–59. [Google Scholar]
  88. Sorokowska, Agnieszka, Aleksandra Słowińska, Anita Zbieg, and Piotr Sorokowski. 2014. Polska adaptacja testu Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI)–TIPI-PL–wersja standardowa i internetowa. [Polish adaptation of the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI) test-TIPI-PL-standard and web version]. Wrocław: WrocLab, Instytut Psychologii, Uniwersytet Wrocławski. [Google Scholar]
  89. Soto, Christopher J., and Oliver P. John. 2017. The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 113: 117–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Sternberg, Robert J. 1997. The concept of intelligence and its role in lifelong learning and success. American Psychologist 52: 1030–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Sternberg, Robert J., ed. 2000. Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  92. Stone-Johnson, Corrie, ed. 2014. Responsible leadership. Educational Administration Quarterly 50: 645–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Streib, Heinz, and Ralph W. Hood. 2016. Semantics and Psychology of “Spirituality”. A Cross-Cultural Analysis. Cham, Heidelberg, New York, Dordrecht and London: Springer International Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  94. Strohminger, Nina, and Shaun Nichols. 2014. The essential moral self. Cognition 131: 159–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Sun, Fan, Meixia Dai, Lizi Lin, Xiang Sun, Aja Louise Murray, Bonnieb Auyeung, and Jin Jing. 2019. Psychometric properties of the chinese version of autism spectrum quotient-children’s version: A sex-specific analysis. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research 12: 303–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  96. Tackett, Jennifer L., Maciel M. Hernández, and Nancy Eisenberg. 2019. Agreeableness. In Handbook of Personality Development. Edited by Dan P. McAdams, Rebecca L. Shiner and Jennifer L. Tackett. New York: Guilford, pp. 171–84. [Google Scholar]
  97. Tangney, June P., Roy F. Baumeister, and Angie L. Boone. 2004. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality 72: 271–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Tetzner, Julia, Michael Becker, and Naemi D. Brandt. 2020. Personality-achievement associations in adolescence—Examining associations across grade levels and learning environments. Journal of Personality 88: 356–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Vitell, Scott John, Mark N. Bing, Kristl H. Davison, Anthony P. Ammeter, Bart L. Garner, and Milorad M. Novicevic. 2009. Religiosity and Moral Identity: The mediating role of self-control. Journal of Business Ethics 88: 601–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Wechsler, David. 1958. The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence, 4th ed. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins Co. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Yaghoobi, Abolghasem, Fatane Fathi, and Srwa Mohmadzadeh. 2018. Meta-analysis of moral intelligence studies. Journal of Educational Psychology Studies 15: 247–70. [Google Scholar]
  102. Yakut, Selahattin, and İdris Yakut. 2021. Yakut—Moral Intellıgence Scale. The Journal of International Social Research 14: 836–42. [Google Scholar]
  103. Yousefi, Pooneh, and Hosein Heshmati. 2015. Moral intelligence and its position in nursing profession. DSME 2: 65–73. [Google Scholar]
  104. Zohar, Danah, and Ian Marshall. 2000. SQ: Spiritual Intelligence: The Ultimate Intelligence. New York: Bloomsbury Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  105. Zohar, Danah, and Ian Marshall. 2004. Spiritual Capital: Wealth We Can Live By. San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler Publishers. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Basic descriptive statistics of the measured variables with the Kolomogorov-Smirnov test.
Table 1. Basic descriptive statistics of the measured variables with the Kolomogorov-Smirnov test.
VariableMSDSk.BSk.Kurt.B.Kurt.Min.Max.K-S.p
Age 24.404.550.850.19−0.420.3818.0035.000.202<0.001 *
TIPI_Stability3.661.690.110.19−1.120.381.007.000.124<0.001 *
TIPI_Extraversion5.031.48−0.720.19−0.170.381.007.000.152<0.001 *
TIPI_Conscientiousness4.881.46−0.640.19−0.280.381.007.000.152<0.001 *
TIPI_Openness4.521.13−0.100.19−0.120.381.507.000.117<0.001 *
TIPI_Agreeableness5.401.13−0.700.190.490.381.007.000.153<0.001 *
Ya_Em.3.820.59−0.220.19−0.100.382.405.000.0800.014
Ya_Con.4.010.71−0.740.191.130.381.005.000.101<0.001 *
Ya_Se.3.370.85−0.430.19−0.250.381.005.000.0900.003
Ya_C.3.550.70−0.350.19−0.410.381.805.000.102<0.001 *
SI_CET22.497.19−0.180.19−0.750.387.0035.000.0720.040
SI_PMP16.804.71−0.330.19−0.390.385.0025.000.0890.004
SI_TA23.766.27−0.360.19−0.430.387.0035.000.0780.019
SI_CSE11.745.010.310.19−0.720.385.0025.000.105<0.001 *
Note: Ya_Em.—Yakut_Empathy; Ya_Con.—Yakut_Conscience; Yakut_Se.—Yakut_Self-control; Yakut_C.—Yakut_Courtesy; SI_CET—SI_Critical existential thinking; SI_PMP—SI_Personal meaning production; SI_TA—SI_Transcendental awareness; SI_CSE—SI_Conscious state expansion.* Lower limit of actual significance.
Table 2. Pearson’s R correlations between moral and spiritual intelligence scales and age.
Table 2. Pearson’s R correlations between moral and spiritual intelligence scales and age.
Ya_Em.Ya_Con.Ya_Se.Ya_C.SI_CETSI_PMPSI_TASI_CSE
Ya_Em.1-------
Ya_Con.0.442 **1------
Ya_Se.0.0900.156 *1-----
Ya_C.0.339 **0.429 **0.346 **1----
SI_CET0.340 **0.267 **0.0300.168 *1---
SI_PMP0.1100.238 **0.328 **0.285 **0.386 **1--
SI_TA0.280 **0.342 **0.174 *0.241 **0.625 **0.642 **1-
SI_CSE0.206 **0.232 **0.1500.1200.536 **0.429 **0.651 **1
Age−0.0260.1160.223 **0.137−0.0560.100−0.097−0.035
Note: Ya_Em.—Yakut_Empathy; Ya_Con.—Yakut_Conscience; Yakut_Se.—Yakut_Self-control; Yakut_C.—Yakut_Courtesy; SI_CET—SI_Critical existential thinking; SI_PMP—SI_Personal meaning production; SI_TA—SI_Transcendental awareness; SI_CSE—SI_Conscious state expansion.** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05.
Table 3. Results of multiple regression analyses with personality traits as predictors of spiritual intelligence in specific domains.
Table 3. Results of multiple regression analyses with personality traits as predictors of spiritual intelligence in specific domains.
Variable βTFRR2AdjustedR2
SI_CET 0.8900.1680.028−0.003
SI_PMP 12.349 **0.5350.2860.263
TIPI_Sta0.2583.217 **
TIPI_Open0.1812.526 *
TIPI_Ex0.1902.327 *
TIPI_Con0.1562.082 *
SI_TA 2.470 *0.2720.0740.044
TIPI_Open0.2032.489 *
SI_CSE 0.8800.1670.028−0.004
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 4. Results of multiple regression analyses with personality traits as predictors of moral intelligence in specific domains.
Table 4. Results of multiple regression analyses with personality traits as predictors of moral intelligence in specific domains.
Variable βTFRR2AdjustedR2
Ya_Em. 2.533 *0.2760.0760.046
TIPI_Sta−0.217−2.381 *
TIPI_Ex0.2202.370 *
Ya_Con. 2.968 *0.2960.0880.058
TIPI_Agr0.1892.352 *
TIPI_Open−0.183−2.259 *
Ya_Se. 11.083 **0.5140.2650.241
TIPI_Sta0.4455.470* *
Ya_C. 4.199 **0.3460.1200.091
TIPI_Agr0.2282.885 **
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Dacka, M.; Rydz, E. Personality Traits and the Spiritual and Moral Intelligence of Early Adulthood in Poland: Research Reports. Religions 2023, 14, 78. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14010078

AMA Style

Dacka M, Rydz E. Personality Traits and the Spiritual and Moral Intelligence of Early Adulthood in Poland: Research Reports. Religions. 2023; 14(1):78. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14010078

Chicago/Turabian Style

Dacka, Monika, and Elżbieta Rydz. 2023. "Personality Traits and the Spiritual and Moral Intelligence of Early Adulthood in Poland: Research Reports" Religions 14, no. 1: 78. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14010078

APA Style

Dacka, M., & Rydz, E. (2023). Personality Traits and the Spiritual and Moral Intelligence of Early Adulthood in Poland: Research Reports. Religions, 14(1), 78. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14010078

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop