Next Article in Journal
Imagery-Mediated Verbal Learning Depends on Vividness–Familiarity Interactions: The Possible Role of Dualistic Resting State Network Activity Interference
Next Article in Special Issue
Seeing a Face in a Crowd of Emotional Voices: Changes in Perception and Cortisol in Response to Emotional Information across the Senses
Previous Article in Journal
Multiple Levels of Control Processes for Wisconsin Card Sorts: An Observational Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Joint Modulation of Facial Expression Processing by Contextual Congruency and Task Demands
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Electrophysiological Responses to Emotional Facial Expressions Following a Mild Traumatic Brain Injury

by
Joanie Drapeau
1,2,3,
Nathalie Gosselin
2,3,
Isabelle Peretz
2,3 and
Michelle McKerral
1,2,*
1
Centre for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation (CRIR), IURDPM, CIUSSS du Centre-Sud-de-l’Île-de-Montréal, Montreal, QC H3S 2J4, Canada
2
Departement of Psychology, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC H3C 3J7, Canada
3
International Laboratory for Brain, Music and Sound Research (BRAMS), Montreal, QC H3C 3J7, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2019, 9(6), 142; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9060142
Submission received: 3 June 2019 / Revised: 11 June 2019 / Accepted: 17 June 2019 / Published: 18 June 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Perceptual and Affective Mechanisms in Facial Expression Recognition)

Abstract

:
The present study aimed to measure neural information processing underlying emotional recognition from facial expressions in adults having sustained a mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) as compared to healthy individuals. We thus measured early (N1, N170) and later (N2) event-related potential (ERP) components during presentation of fearful, neutral, and happy facial expressions in 10 adults with mTBI and 11 control participants. Findings indicated significant differences between groups, irrespective of emotional expression, in the early attentional stage (N1), which was altered in mTBI. The two groups showed similar perceptual integration of facial features (N170), with greater amplitude for fearful facial expressions in the right hemisphere. At a higher-level emotional discrimination stage (N2), both groups demonstrated preferential processing for fear as compared to happiness and neutrality. These findings suggest a reduced early selective attentional processing following mTBI, but no impact on the perceptual and higher-level cognitive processes stages. This study contributes to further improving our comprehension of attentional versus emotional recognition following a mild TBI.

1. Introduction

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) can lead to long-term physical, behavioral, and cognitive consequences affecting social participation [1,2]. A link between such difficulties and poor ability in recognizing emotions from facial expressions has also been suggested [3,4,5]. Indeed, emotional recognition is of crucial importance for successful social interactions. In particular, emotional facial expressions provide central information to understand others’ intentions and ultimately guide behavior.
Deficits in recognizing emotions from facial expressions, especially negative emotions (i.e., fear, anger, sadness, or disgust) as compared to positive ones (i.e., happiness), have been established in prior behavioral studies following moderate to severe TBI [3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11]. In fact, brain regions involved in emotional processing, such as the prefrontal cortex (i.e., ventromedial and orbitofrontal) and limbic structures (i.e., amygdala, temporal lobes, fusiform gyrus) [12,13,14], are frequently damaged or disrupted after a moderate or severe TBI, and even following mild TBI (mTBI) [15,16]. Specific nontraumatic lesions to ventromedial prefrontal lobe, anteromedial temporal lobe, or bilateral amygdala have been related to deficits in discriminating fearful and disgusted facial expressions [17,18,19]. The electrophysiological literature on moderate and severe TBI in adults has also demonstrated dysfunction in cortical processing of emotional facial expressions [20,21].
Despite compelling evidence of structural changes and neurochemical alterations following mTBI that occur in brain regions associated with emotional processing [16,22,23,24], little is known about emotional recognition from facial expressions after mTBI. In a previous behavioral study [10], adults with uncomplicated mTBI (i.e., without intracranial abnormality) showed comparable performance to healthy controls when recognizing happy, sad, and fearful faces, which contrasted with the performance of individuals with a complicated mTBI (i.e., with intracranial abnormality) or with moderate or severe TBI, who showed significant impairment in recognition of fearful faces. Prior studies of cognitive functioning suggested that standard neuropsychological tests or behavioral methods often fail to detect dysfunctions in the postacute stages of mTBI, whereas more precise and sensitive measures, such as event-related potentials (ERPs), can identify subtle neurofunctional changes related to, for example, working memory and visual attention [25,26].
ERPs represent an ideal method to reveal the rapid temporal sequence of neural processing underlying emotional recognition [27,28], making it possible to distinguish the relative contributions of early (e.g., attentional and perceptual factors) and later processing stages (e.g., executive components of attention, discrimination of emotions). Several early and late ERP components have been studied in relation to emotional processing from facial expressions in healthy individuals, such as the N1 (early selective attentional processing) [29], N170 (perceptual integration of facial features) [30], and anterior N2 (executive cognitive control [31,32] or high-level cognitive processes enabling discrimination between emotional categories [33,34]).
Some studies have reported that these ERP components are also modulated by emotional valence (negative, neutral, positive) of facial expressions; for instance, preferential processing (i.e., increased ERP amplitude) for negative emotional facial expressions has been demonstrated, as compared to neutral facial expressions [27,33,34,35,36,37]. Especially, fearful stimuli are thought to be prioritized for attention because of their biological, evolutionary, and behavioral relevance [38]. Furthermore, it has been proposed that performing an explicit cognitive task, such as categorization or oddball discrimination, may suppress or interfere with the automatic preferential processing of emotional stimuli [39]. Thus, an implicit or passive viewing task could be more appropriate to reflect the effect of emotional valence.
To our knowledge, only two studies have explored electrophysiological responses specifically related to the perceptual integration of emotional facial expressions following mTBI (i.e., N170) [40,41]. In the study of Zuj et al. [40], previously deployed military personnel having sustained an mTBI showed, compared to a noninjured military group, a reduction in the N170 component for all facial expressions studied (i.e., fearful, angry, happy, and neutral). In D’Hont et al. [41], the authors showed, in preschool children having sustained an mTBI, an absence of preferential processing of emotional facial expressions in the early stages (P1, N170), as compared to controls.
The present study aimed to measure neural information processing underlying emotional recognition from facial expressions in adults having sustained an mTBI in comparison to healthy uninjured individuals. To do so, we conducted an experiment in which we measured early (N1, N170) and later (N2) ERP components during presentation of fearful, neutral, and happy facial expressions. Rare stimuli (butterflies), which were irrelevant to the study, were also presented to ensure that all participants paid attention to stimuli. We postulated that ERPs reflecting the early attentional processing stage would be affected following mTBI as compared to healthy controls. Also, since the cortical processing of emotions from facial expressions has been shown to be altered following an mTBI, we expected that participants with mTBI would also demonstrate ERP changes associated with perceptual integration and discrimination of emotional facial expressions.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Thirty-nine participants were recruited for this study and the data of 21 participants were retained for primary analysis: 10 participants with uncomplicated (i.e., absence of intracranial lesions on brain imaging) mTBI and 11 healthy individuals. Nine participants with mTBI and five healthy individuals were removed from the analysis because of loss of trials (more than 50% of trials) due to eye blinks, movements, excessive sweating, or very large alpha oscillations which were mainly related to temporary temperature control issues in the testing environment. Number of trials included in EEG averaging were similar across conditions and groups, ranging from 35 to 50 trials per emotion category. Participants with TBI were recruited from the Centre de réadaptation Lucie-Bruneau in Montreal, Québec, Canada. Healthy controls were recruited through advertisements in the community. The study was approved by the Centre de recherche interdisciplinaire en réadaptation (CRIR) Research Ethics Committee. All participants provided a written informed consent prior to testing.
Participants included in the study had to have sustained an mTBI during a road traffic accident or a fall, and have presented at least one of the following parameters, as noted in medical records: Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) score (13–15/15), post-traumatic amnesia (PTA) duration (<24 h), and loss of consciousness (LOC) duration (<30 min) [42]. A negative CT scan or MRI result (no intracranial lesions) was also required (i.e., uncomplicated mTBI). Exclusion criteria for participants with TBI were: (1) younger than 18 or older than 55 years; (2) a psychiatric or neurological history, including more than one TBI; (3) a penetrating brain injury, such as assault with blunt or sharp object; (4) an uncorrected visual impairment; and (5) uses of psychostimulant, antidepressant, or antipsychotic treatment. This information was documented in the medical records or during the first appointment. An additional exclusion criterion for healthy controls was a history of TBI.
For group comparison purposes, intellectual functioning estimates (verbal IQ {vocabulary} and performance IQ {matrix reasoning} of the Wechsler adult intelligence scale-III {WAIS-III} [43], post-concussion symptoms (Post-Concussion Scale, PCS; 22 items rated 0-asymptomatic to 6-severely symptomatic, for a maximum score of 132 [44]) and symptoms related to depression (Beck Depression Inventory-II, BDI-II; 21 items rated 0-symptom absent to 3-severe symptoms, for a maximum score of 63 [45]) were measured.

2.2. Stimuli

Thirty grayscale static facial expressions (256 × 256 pixels) were selected from the STOIC database (http://www.mapageweb.umontreal.ca/gosselif/sroyetal_sub.pdf) [46]. Facial expressions were created by professional actors (five men, five women) and were intended to express fear, happiness, and neutrality (10 different stimuli per emotion category). These facial stimuli were cropped to exclude nonfacial cues and were equivalent in mean luminance and contrast. A grayscale picture of a butterfly (256 × 256 pixels) was also included. Examples of stimuli are presented in Figure 1.

2.3. Task and Procedure

Participants were presented with 330 trials in two blocks of 165 trials each (50 facial expressions per emotion category: fear, neutrality, happiness, and 15 butterflies), in a pseudo-random order, after a practice block of 33 trials (10 facial expressions per emotion category and three butterflies). All stimuli appeared at the center of the computer screen and were viewed from a distance of 1.14 meters. Each trial began with a fixation cross presented for 500 milliseconds (ms), followed by a visual stimulus (facial expression or butterfly) for 200 ms and a blank screen for 50 ms. An example of the task is presented in Figure 2. Participants were instructed to maintain fixation at the center of the computer screen, to avoid eye movements or blinks during visual stimuli presentation, and to press the space bar on the computer keyboard whenever a butterfly appeared. E-prime software was used for stimulus presentation and response recording.

2.4. ERP Data Recording and Analysis

EEG activity was recorded while participants performed the task, with 64 Ag/AgCl active electrodes (Active Two BioSemi) placed over the scalp, according to the extended 10–20 International EEG system [47]. Horizontal eye movements (HEOG) were recorded with electrodes placed on the external canthus of each eye. Vertical eye movements and eye blinks (VEOG) were recorded from an electrode placed on the inferior orbital region of the left eye and Fp1 electrode. Sampling rate was set to 512 Hz and the data were referenced to the average mastoids during analysis.
Brain Vision Analyzer (version 1.5) was used to analyze EEG data. Signals were filtered (0.01 Hz high-pass, 30 Hz low-pass) and were averaged offline. Eye movement artifacts were rejected semi-automatically with the ocular correction ICA method. Thus, trials with ocular artifacts at electrodes of interest (>70 µV), eye blinks (VEOG > 100 µV), and large horizontal eye movements (HEOG > 30 µV) were excluded from the analysis. EEGs were averaged separately for fear, neutrality, and happiness. For every trial, EEG epochs of 1000 ms (including a 200 ms prestimulus period) were averaged after artifact rejection. Epochs were then baseline corrected based on mean amplitude of activity recording during the 200 ms immediately prior to stimulus onset.
ERP components were measured (peak amplitude) by locating the highest peak within windows after stimulus onset [32]: N1 (50–150 ms), N170 (150–200 ms), and N2 (200–350 ms). According to prior emotion processing studies with healthy individuals, amplitude deflection of the N1 is largest over frontocentral electrodes [33,34], the N170 over lateral occipitotemporal sites [30,35,37], and the N2 at frontal sites [33,34].

2.5. Statistical Analysis

All ERP components were first investigated using visual inspection and amplitude measurement to detect which scalp regions (frontal, central, or posterior) and which electrode sites showed maximum amplitude. For each ERP component (N1, N170, N2) independently, data were analyzed using a general linear model repeated-measures ANOVA, with groups (two levels: mTBI, controls) as a between-subjects factor, and emotions (three levels: fear, neutrality, happiness) and electrode sites (when applicable) as within-subjects factors.

3. Results

3.1. Clinical and Demographic Data

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 1. The mTBI group did not differ from healthy controls according to age, gender, years of education, and intellectual functioning estimates. Healthy controls and participants with mTBI differed significantly according to PCS score (F(1,19) = 10.675, p = 0.004) and BDI-II score (F(1,19) = 11.505, p = 0.003). All participants presented a BDI-II score of 20 or below (i.e., cut-off for the presence of a possible moderate–severe depression), except for two participants with mTBI. However, these two participants scored higher on ‘somatic’ and ‘cognitive’ items (e.g., fatigue, sleep disturbances, concentration difficulties) compared to ‘affective’ items (e.g., sadness), which could be more related to the mTBI than to depression. Indeed, BDI-II scores were highly correlated with post-concussion symptoms scores (r = 0.868, p = 0.000).

3.2. Behavioral Responses

All participants performed accurately on butterfly detection (healthy controls: mean 100%; mTBI group: mean 97.3%, standard deviation 3.1), ensuring that they all fully paid attention to the stimuli.

3.3. Electrophysiological Responses to Emotional Facial Expressions

3.3.1. N1

N1 amplitude deflection was largest over FCz electrode (frontocentral site). Average waveforms within-subjects and average responses between-subjects for the mTBI group and the control group are shown in Figure 3, according to emotional expressions (fear, neutrality, happiness).
Analysis revealed a significant main effect of group (F(1,19) = 4.774, p = 0.042, η2 = 0.201), indicating that adults with mTBI showed reduced N1 amplitude as compared to healthy individuals in processing emotional facial expressions. No significant main effect of emotion (p = 0.470) and no significant interaction between groups and emotions (p = 0.855) were found.

3.3.2. N170

N170 amplitude deflections were largest over P9 (left hemisphere) and P10 (right hemisphere) electrodes (occipitotemporal sites). Average waveforms within-subjects and average responses between-subjects for the mTBI group and the control group are shown in Figure 4, according to emotional expressions (fear, neutrality, happiness).
Analysis comparing mTBI and control groups revealed no significant main effect of group (p = 0.331) and no significant interaction between emotion and group (p = 0.205), but a significant main effect of emotion (F(2,38) = 5.818, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.234), indicating that some emotions elicited larger amplitude than others, irrespective of the group. Bonferroni-adjusted pairwise comparisons revealed that fearful facial expressions elicited larger amplitude than neutral ones (p = 0.007), as did happy facial expressions (p = 0.007). There was also a significant main effect of electrode site (F(1,19) = 7.090, p = 0.015, η2 = 0.272), revealing a stronger amplitude in the right hemisphere, tempered by a significant interaction between electrode site and emotion (F(2,38) = 4.720, p = 0.022, η2 = 0.344). Bonferroni-adjusted pairwise comparisons revealed that, specifically, fearful facial expressions showed larger amplitude in the right hemisphere (p = 0.01) as compared to other emotions. No significant interaction between electrode site and group (p = 0.513) and no interaction between electrode site, group, and emotion (p = 0.240) were found.

3.3.3. N2

N2 amplitude deflections were largest over Fz electrode (frontal site). Average waveforms within-subjects and average responses between-subjects for the mTBI group and the control group are shown in Figure 5, according to emotional expressions (fear, neutrality, happiness).
Analysis revealed no significant main effect of group (p = 0.346) nor significant interaction between emotion and group (p = 0. 112), but a significant main effect of emotion, F(1,19) = 9.452, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.512, indicating that some emotions elicited larger amplitude than others, irrespective of the group. Bonferroni-adjusted pairwise comparisons revealed that both groups showed a preferential processing (i.e., larger amplitude) for fearful facial expressions, as compared to neutrality (p = 0.011) and happiness (p = 0.002).

4. Discussion

The aim of this original study was to measure neural information processing underlying emotion recognition from negative (fear), neutral, and positive (happy) facial expressions in adults having sustained an mTBI as compared to healthy individuals. According to our hypothesis, results indicated significant differences between groups, irrespective of emotional expressions, in the early attentional stage (N1), suggesting altered selective attentional processing for all emotions from facial expressions following mTBI, even in the context of attentional facilitation induced by butterfly detection. Findings also revealed that, contrary to what was hypothesized, the two groups showed similar perceptual integration of facial features (N170), with a preferential processing for fearful and happy facial expressions, as compared to neutral faces, which was stronger in the right hemisphere. Moreover, at a higher-level emotion discrimination stage (N2), both groups demonstrated preferential processing for fear as compared to happiness and neutrality.
Globally, these findings suggest modified early selective attentional processing following mTBI, in the context of preserved perceptual integration and higher-level emotion discrimination. Even though the sample size was small, the demonstrated effects were robust, with most effect sizes being strong, the others moderate. In a previous behavioral study, Drapeau et al. [10] also showed comparable performance between individuals with uncomplicated mTBI and healthy individuals for perceptual integration of facial features and facial emotion discrimination. These findings are also in line with previous research on mTBI or on repeated concussions which has previously documented such dysfunction in selective attention in general and specifically for emotional facial expressions [48,49,50].
In contrast, another study showed no attentional biases or enhanced processing of angry or fearful facial expressions and reduced N170 amplitudes to angry, fearful, and happy emotional facial expressions in a group of military personnel who had sustained an mTBI, compared to a non-TBI military group [41]. It is possible that in this latter study, the mTBIs sustained during deployment and combat reflected injuries toward the more severe spectrum of mTBI (similar to that of complicated mTBI, although intracranial injuries were not documented) due to the biomechanical forces involved in such contexts and since almost 40% of their sample had a previous mTBI, while this variable was an exclusion criterion in the present study. Hence, it is possible that individuals with mTBI who present more severe post-mTBI neuropathophysiological changes can show altered neural processing at higher-level cognitive stages enabling discrimination of fearful facial expressions, while adults having sustained a single uncomplicated mTBI appear to show changes only for selective attention, as demonstrated in the present study.
This is not surprising, as the level of cognitive functioning is associated with brain injury severity even in the mild severity spectrum [51,52]. In such cases, fearful or angry expressions in particular may overload attentional resources, thus negatively affecting subsequent facial expression processing as well as discrimination between emotion categories. Indeed, fear involves more facial features (e.g., raised forehead and eyebrows, open mouth, open eyes) to pay attention and to process, as compared to neutrality or happiness (i.e., smile). Some studies have also demonstrated that even in healthy individuals, attentional load can reduce facilitated processing of threatening faces during more elaborate cognitive stages involving executive attentional control [53] and also reduce activation of brain areas associated with fear processing, such as fusiform gyri [54].
Our findings of reduced early selective attentional processing following mTBI, but no impact, on the perceptual and higher-level cognitive processes stages related to facial emotion recognition also contrast with previous work showing impairments in negative emotion, especially fearful, recognition from facial expressions following moderate or severe TBI [3,5,6,8,9,10,11]. They also differ from prior studies indicating that altered emotion recognition from facial expressions is closely linked with attentional difficulties after moderate–severe TBI [55,56]. This study contributes to further improving our comprehension of attentional versus emotion recognition following an mTBI and points to the importance of carefully documenting the severity of TBI, even on the mild spectrum (e.g., according to common diagnostic criteria), when studying the different processing stages associated with recognition of facial emotional expressions, as different types and range of impairments may present according to TBI severity. For individuals with mTBI, interventions geared toward improving attentional functioning should be encouraged in individuals having sustained an mTBI and who show persistent post-mTBI symptoms after three months, as did the participants in the present study [57,58]. Training complex forms of attention could result in more effective allocation of attentional resources in general [59], although it is not known how this would specifically impact early attentional processing of emotional facial expressions.

5. Limitations

Sample size being a limitation of the present study, ERP studies in larger mTBI subgroups (e.g., uncomplicated, complicated, multiple mTBIs) are needed to better characterize the mechanisms underlying attentional processing and emotion recognition from facial expressions on the entire severity spectrum of mTBI. This would be of interest to identify those individuals who could benefit from cognitive intervention and remediation strategies. Future studies should also combine neuropsychological and behavioral measures with advanced high-definition functional brain imaging techniques (e.g., multiarray EEG and magnetoencephalography) in order to refine our understanding of the cortical underpinnings of normal versus altered attentional and facial expression processing after an mTBI.

6. Conclusions

This original study is the first systematic exploration of the temporal sequence of neural processing underlying emotion recognition from facial expressions in adults having sustained a single uncomplicated mTBI as compared to healthy individuals. The present findings indicate affected early attentional processing following mTBI but no impact on perceptual and higher-level cognitive processing stages. This study contributes to further improving our comprehension of emotion recognition following TBI.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.D., N.G., I.P., and M.M.; Data curation, J.D.; Formal analysis, J.D.; Funding acquisition, J.D., I.P., and M.M.; Investigation, J.D.; Methodology, J.D., N.G., I.P., and M.M.; Project administration, J.D. and M.M.; Resources, I.P. and M.M.; Software, N.G., I.P., and M.M.; Supervision, N.G., I.P., and M.M.; Visualization, J.D. and M.M.; Writing–original draft, J.D.; Writing–review & editing, J.D., N.G., I.P., and M.M.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada and the Centre de réadaptation Lucie-Bruneau (grants to M.M.), a Canada Research Chair (to I.P.), as well as the Fonds de recherche du Québec-Santé and the Faculté des études supérieures et postdoctorales de l’Université de Montréal (scholarships to J.D.).

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Centre de réadaptation Lucie-Bruneau for their help with recruitment of TBI participants. We thank the participants who took part in our study, as well as Nicolas Robitaille, Thomas Romeas, and Nathalie Bouloute for technical assistance.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. De Guise, E.; leBlanc, J.; Feyz, M.; Meyer, K.; Duplantie, J.; Thomas, H.; Abouassaly, M.; Champoux, M.C.; Couturier, C.; Lin, H.; et al. Long-term outcome after severe traumatic brain injury: The McGill interdisciplinary prospective study. J. Head Trauma. Rehabil. 2008, 23, 294–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Temkin, N.R.; Corrigan, J.D.; Dikmen, S.S.; Machamer, J. Social Functioning After Traumatic Brain Injury. J. Head Trauma. Rehabil. 2009, 24, 460–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Knox, L.; Douglas, J. Long-term ability to interpret facial expression after traumatic brain injury and its relation to social integration. Brain Cogn. 2009, 69, 442–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Milders, M.; Ietswaart, M.; Crawford, J.R.; Currie, D. Social behavior following traumatic brain injury and its association with emotion recognition, understanding of intentions, and cognitive flexibility. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2008, 14, 318–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Spikman, J.M.; Milders, M.V.; Visser-Keizer, A.C.; Westerhof-Evers, H.J.; Herben-Dekker, M.; van der Naalt, J. Deficits in Facial Emotion Recognition Indicate Behavioral Changes and Impaired Self-Awareness after Moderate to Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. PLOS ONE 2013, 8, e65581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Babbage, D.R.; Yim, J.; Zupan, B.; Neumann, D.; Tomita, M.R.; Willer, B. Meta-analysis of facial affect recognition difficulties after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology 2011, 25, 277–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bornhofen, C.; McDonald, S. Emotion perception deficits following traumatic brain injury: A review of the evidence and rationale for intervention. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2008, 14, 511–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Callahan, B.L.; Ueda, K.; Sakata, D.; Plamondon, A.; Murai, T. Liberal bias mediates emotion recognition deficits in frontal traumatic brain injury. Brain Cogn. 2011, 77, 412–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Ietswaart, M.; Milders, M.; Crawford, J.R.; Currie, D.; Scott, C.L. Longitudinal aspects of emotion recognition in patients with traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology 2008, 46, 148–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Drapeau, J.; Gosselin, N.; Peretz, I.; McKerral, M. Emotional recognition from dynamic facial, vocal and musical expressions following traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2017, 31, 221–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zupan, B.; Babbage, D.; Neumann, D.; Willer, B. Recognition of facial and vocal affect following traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2014, 28, 1087–1095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Adolphs, R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2002, 12, 169–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Fusar-Poli, P.; Placentino, A.; Carletti, F.; Landi, P.; Allen, P.; Surguladze, S.; Benedetti, F.; Abbamonte, M.; Gasparotti, R.; Barale, F.; et al. Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009, 34, 418–432. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  14. Phillips, M.L.; Drevets, W.C.; Rauch, S.L.; Lane, R. Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception. Boil. Psychiatry 2003, 54, 504–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Bigler, E.D. Structural imaging. In Textbook of Traumatic Brain Injury, 2nd ed; Silver, J.M., MacAllister, T.W., Yudofsky, S.C., Eds.; American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA, USA, 2011; pp. 73–91. [Google Scholar]
  16. Eierud, C.; Craddock, R.C.; Fletcher, S.; Aulakh, M.; King-Casas, B.; Kuehl, D.; LaConte, S.M. Neuroimaging after mild traumatic brain injury: Review and meta-analysis. NeuroImage Clin. 2014, 4, 283–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Adolphs, R.; Tranel, D.; Damasio, H.; Damasio, A. Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature 1994, 372, 669–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Gosselin, N.; Peretz, I.; Hasboun, D.; Baulac, M.; Samson, S. Impaired recognition of musical emotions and facial expressions following anteromedial temporal lobe excision. Cortex 2011, 47, 1116–1125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Vandekerckhove, M.; Plessers, M.; Van Mieghem, A.; Beeckmans, K.; Van Acker, F.; Maex, R.; Markowitsch, H.; Mariën, P.; Van Overwalle, F. Impaired facial emotion recognition in patients with ventromedial prefrontal hypoperfusion. Neuropsychology 2014, 28, 605–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dockree, P.M.; Robertson, I.H. Electrophysiological markers of cognitive deficits in traumatic brain injury: A review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2011, 82, 53–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Elting, J.W.; Maurits, N.; Van Weerden, T.; Spikman, J.; De Keyser, J.; Van Der Naalt, J. P300 analysis techniques in cognitive impairment after brain injury: Comparison with neuropsychological and imaging data. Brain Inj. 2008, 22, 870–881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Beauchamp, M.H.; Ditchfield, M.; Maller, J.J.; Catroppa, C.; Godfrey, C.; Rosenfeld, J.V.; Kean, M.J.; Anderson, V.A. Hippocampus, amygdala and global brain changes 10 Years after childhood traumatic brain injury. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2001, 29, 137–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Mayer, A.R.; Mannell, M.V.; Ling, J.; Gasparovic, C.; Yeo, R.A. Functional Connectivity in Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011, 32, 1825–1835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Yeo, R.A.; Gasparovic, C.; Merideth, F.; Ruhl, D.; Doezema, D.; Mayer, A.R. A Longitudinal Proton Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy Study of Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. J. Neurotrauma 2011, 28, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Gosselin, N.; Bottari, C.; Chen, J.-K.; Huntgeburth, S.C.; De Beaumont, L.; Petrides, M.; Cheung, B.; Ptito, A. Evaluating the cognitive consequences of mild traumatic brain injury and concussion by using electrophysiology. Neurosurg. Focus 2012, 33, E7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Lachapelle, J.; Bolduc-Teasdale, J.; Ptito, A.; McKerral, M. Deficits in complex visual information processing after mild TBI: Electrophysiological markers and vocational outcome prognosis. Brain Inj. 2008, 22, 265–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Hajcak, G.; Weinberg, A.; MacNamara, A.; Foti, D. ERPs and the study of emotion. In The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components; Kappenman, E.S., Luck, S.L., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 441–472. [Google Scholar]
  28. Olofsson, J.K.; Nordin, S.; Sequeira, H.; Polich, J. Affective picture processing: An integrative review of ERP findings. Boil. Psychol. 2008, 77, 247–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Vogel, E.K.; Luck, S.J. The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. Psychophysiol. 2000, 37, 190–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hinojosa, J.; Mercado, F.; Carretié, L. N170 sensitivity to facial expression: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 55, 498–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Folstein, J.R.; Van Petten, C. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology 2008, 45, 152–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Luck, S.J. A closer look at ERPs and ERP components. In An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique, 2nd Ed.; Luck, S.J., Ed.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, UK, 2014; pp. 35–70. [Google Scholar]
  33. Luo, W.; Feng, W.; He, W.; Wang, N.-Y.; Luo, Y.-J. Three stages of facial expression processing: ERP study with rapid serial visual presentation. NeuroImage 2010, 49, 1857–1867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Zhang, D.; Luo, W.; Luo, Y. Single-trial ERP analysis reveals facial expression category in a three-stage scheme. Brain Res. 2013, 1512, 78–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Blau, V.C.; Maurer, U.; Tottenham, N.; McCandliss, B.D. The face-specific N170 component is modulated by emotional facial expression. Behav. Brain Funct. 2007, 3, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Foti, D.; Hajcak, G.; Dien, J. Differentiating neural responses to emotional pictures: Evidence from temporal-spatial PCA. Psychophysiology 2009, 46, 521–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Leppänen, J.M.; Moulson, M.C.; Vogel-Farley, V.K.; Nelson, C.A. An ERP Study of Emotional Face Processing in the Adult and Infant Brain. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 232–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. LeDoux, J.E. The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life; Simon & Schuster Paperbacks: New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  39. Schupp, H.T.; Schmälzle, R.; Flaisch, T. Explicit semantic stimulus categorization interferes with implicit emotion processing. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 1738–1745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Zuj, D.V.; Felmingham, K.L.; Palmer, M.A.; Lawrence-Wood, E.; van Hooff, M.; Lawrence, A.J.; Bryant, R.A.; McFarlane, A.C. Neural activity and emotional processing following military deployment: Effects of mild traumatic brain injury and posttraumatic stress disorder. Brain Cogn. 2017, 118, 19–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. D’Hont, F.; Lassonde, M.; Thebault-Dagher, F.; Bernier, A.; Gravel, J.; Vannasing, P.; Beauchamp, M.H. Electrophysiological correlates of emotional face processing after mild traumatic brain injury in preschool children. Cogn. Affec. Behav. Neurosci. 2017, 17, 124–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Arlinghaus, K.A.; Pastorek, N.J.; Graham, D.P. Neuropsychiatric assessment. In Textbook of traumatic brain injury, 2nd Edition; Silver, J.M., MacAllister, T.W., Yudofsky, S.C., Eds.; American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA, USA, 2011; pp. 55–73. [Google Scholar]
  43. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (WAIS-III); Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  44. Lovell, M.R.; Iverson, G.L.; Collins, M.W.; Podell, K.; Johnston, K.M.; Pardini, D.; Pardini, J.; Norwig, J.; Maroon, J.C. Measurement of Symptoms Following Sports-Related Concussion: Reliability and Normative Data for the Post-Concussion Scale. Appl. Neuropsychol. 2006, 13, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Beck, A.T.; Steer, R.A.; Brown, G.K. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  46. Roy, S.; Roy, C.; Éthier-Majcher, C.; Fortin, I.; Belin, P.; Gosselin, F. STOIC: A database of dynamic and static faces expressing highly recognizable emotions. J. Vis. 2007, 7, 944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Jasper, H.H. The ten twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalography Clin. Neurophysiol. 1958, 10, 371–375. [Google Scholar]
  48. Kwok, F.Y.; Lee, T.M.C.; Leung, C.H.S.; Poon, W.S. Changes of cognitive functioning following mild traumatic brain injury over a 3-month period. Brain Inj. 2008, 22, 740–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. McInnes, K.; Friesen, C.L.; MacKenzie, D.E.; Westwood, D.A.; Boe, S.G. Mild Traumatic Brain Injury (mTBI) and chronic cognitive impairment: A scoping review. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, 0174847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Carrier-Toutant, F.; Guay, S.; Beaulieu, C.; Léveillé, É.; Turcotte-Giroux, A.; Papineau, S.; Brisson, B.; D’Hondt, F. Effects of Repeated Concussions and Sex on Early Processing of Emotional Facial Expressions as Revealed by Electrophysiology. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2018, 24, 673–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Carlozzi, N.E.; Kirsch, N.L.; Kisala, P.A.; Tulsky, D.S. An Examination of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales, Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) in Individuals with Complicated Mild, Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI). Clin. Neuropsychol. 2015, 29, 21–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Lange, R.T.; Iverson, G.L.; Franzen, M.D. Neuropsychological functioning following complicated vs. uncomplicated mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2009, 23, 83–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Van Dillen, L.F.; Derks, B. Working memory load reduces facilitated processing of threatening faces: An ERP study. Emotion 2012, 12, 1340–1349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Pessoa, L.; McKenna, M.; Gutiérrez, E.; Ungerleider, L.G. Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2002, 99, 11458–11463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Mancuso, M.; Magnani, N.; Cantagallo, A.; Rossi, G.; Capitani, D.; Galletti, V.; Robertson, I.H. Emotion recognition impairment in traumatic brain injury compared with schizophrenia spectrum: Similar deficits with different origins. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2015, 203, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Yim, J.; Babbage, D.R.; Zupan, B.; Neumann, D.; Willer, B. The relationship between facial affect recognition and cognitive functioning after traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2013, 27, 1155–1161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Cicerone, K.D.; Langenbahn, D.M.; Braden, C.; Malec, J.F.; Kalmar, K.; Fraas, M.; Felicetti, T.; Laatsch, L.; Harley, J.P.; Bergquist, T.; et al. Evidence-Based Cognitive Rehabilitation: Updated Review of the Literature From 2003 Through 2008. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabilit. 2011, 92, 519–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. McKerral, M.; Léveillé, G. Chapter 13: Return to work. When and how should I return to work after a concussion. In Sports Concussions Continuum: A Complete Guide to Recovery and Management; Gagnon, I., Ptito, A., Eds.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  59. Cicerone, K.D. Remediation of ’working attention’ in mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2002, 16, 185–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Examples of visual stimuli; (1) fearful facial expression, (2) neutral facial expression, (3) happy facial expression, (4) butterfly.
Figure 1. Examples of visual stimuli; (1) fearful facial expression, (2) neutral facial expression, (3) happy facial expression, (4) butterfly.
Brainsci 09 00142 g001
Figure 2. Example of the task.
Figure 2. Example of the task.
Brainsci 09 00142 g002
Figure 3. N1 responses at FCz electrode site, (a) within-subjects and (b) between-subjects, according to emotional expressions (fear, neutrality, happiness).
Figure 3. N1 responses at FCz electrode site, (a) within-subjects and (b) between-subjects, according to emotional expressions (fear, neutrality, happiness).
Brainsci 09 00142 g003
Figure 4. N170 responses at P9 electrode site (left hemisphere) and P10 electrode site (right hemisphere), (a) within-subjects and (b) between-subjects, according to emotional expressions (fear, neutrality, happiness).
Figure 4. N170 responses at P9 electrode site (left hemisphere) and P10 electrode site (right hemisphere), (a) within-subjects and (b) between-subjects, according to emotional expressions (fear, neutrality, happiness).
Brainsci 09 00142 g004
Figure 5. N2 responses at Fz electrodes site, (a) within-subjects and (b) between-subjects, according to emotional expressions (fear, neutrality, happiness).
Figure 5. N2 responses at Fz electrodes site, (a) within-subjects and (b) between-subjects, according to emotional expressions (fear, neutrality, happiness).
Brainsci 09 00142 g005
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the mTBI and control participants.
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the mTBI and control participants.
TBI
Participants
SexAge
(years)
Education (years)Cause of TBIGCSScoreLesion Site
on CT Scan
Months Post-InjuryBDI-IIPCSVerbal IQ
Estimate
Performance IQ
Estimate
mTBI16F2613Fall on ice15Normal311131105120
mTBI27M3614Fall >1-meter, work accident15Normal28649595
mTBI30F3318Face-to-face hit, hockey15Normal1807105120
mTBI34F2013Car accident15Normal42026100115
mTBI41F4617Car accident14Normal5916105100
mTBI42F3218Knee-to-head hit, basketball15Normal241528100100
mTBI44F2916Fall backward sitting on bench15Normal7718110125
mTBI46F4218Hit on head by basketball15Normal27420100110
mTBI47F4017Car accident15Normal6274095120
mTBI48F4216Fall on head, martial arts14Normal2626649595
CTRL101F4413 26105115
CTRL103F2918 614105115
CTRL105M3114 0095120
CTRL106M2616 34115115
CTRL107M2616 023100125
CTRL108F5114 815105115
CTRL109M3516 63100125
CTRL112M2912 34100115
CTRL114F3018 01115105
CTRL115F3020 01105120
CTRL116M2817 03120135

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Drapeau, J.; Gosselin, N.; Peretz, I.; McKerral, M. Electrophysiological Responses to Emotional Facial Expressions Following a Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 142. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9060142

AMA Style

Drapeau J, Gosselin N, Peretz I, McKerral M. Electrophysiological Responses to Emotional Facial Expressions Following a Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. Brain Sciences. 2019; 9(6):142. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9060142

Chicago/Turabian Style

Drapeau, Joanie, Nathalie Gosselin, Isabelle Peretz, and Michelle McKerral. 2019. "Electrophysiological Responses to Emotional Facial Expressions Following a Mild Traumatic Brain Injury" Brain Sciences 9, no. 6: 142. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9060142

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop