Next Article in Journal
Single-Nucleotide Polymorphisms in Oxidative Stress-Related Genes and the Risk of a Stroke in a Polish Population—A Preliminary Study
Previous Article in Journal
Neurocognitive Impairment in Severe Mental Illness. Comparative study with Spanish Speaking Patients
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Brief Report

Test Anxiety in Autistic University Students—Preliminary Results from a German-Speaking Sample

Educational Psychology Lab, Institute of Educational Science, University of Bern, 3012 Bern, Switzerland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2021, 11(3), 390; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030390
Submission received: 23 February 2021 / Revised: 13 March 2021 / Accepted: 17 March 2021 / Published: 19 March 2021

Abstract

:
Autism is a neurodevelopmental condition associated with increased levels of anxiety. However, whether autism is related to heightened test anxiety as one situation-specific type of anxiety has not yet been examined. This question may be relevant for the achievement and well-being of autistic people in educational settings (e.g., at universities). In this study, we took a first step to investigate whether autistic university students have increased test anxiety. A sample of 16 German-speaking university students completed an established diagnostic instrument to measure test anxiety and its components of worry, emotionality, cognitive interference, and lack of confidence. The scores of the autistic students were compared with the test anxiety means and percentile ranks of the standardization sample for the applied test anxiety measure (n = 1350). For an additional comparison, the test anxiety means and percentile ranks of non-autistic university students (n = 101) were assessed during the last third of the semester; that is, close to the examinations. Overall, the results suggest that autistic university students have remarkably increased test anxiety. Although the present findings must be considered preliminary, they suggest that text anxiety in educational settings may be a neglected significant problem for autistic people that requires further attention in research and practice.

1. Introduction

Autism is a neurodevelopmental condition that is defined by (a) difficulties in the culturally common forms of social interaction and communication and (b) restrictive and repetitive behaviors [1]. Studies have revealed that individuals on the autism spectrum show increased levels of anxiety (e.g., social anxiety [2]), corresponding to the fact that people on the autism spectrum have a higher susceptibility to anxiety at the neuronal level [3]. A well-known specific type of anxiety is test anxiety. Test anxiety research has a long tradition (over 100 years) and has provided various and important insights into educational contexts [4]. Research in the field of autism has been conducted intensively in recent years and could result in diverse findings regarding test anxiety. The link between these two research areas is novel and relevant for the educational–psychological field. At the same time, the educational–psychological field is important and useful for autism research [5]. To date, the relationship between test anxiety and autism has not been explored and remains a research gap.
Generally, anxiety is defined as an aversive emotional experience that is caused by a threat and is characterized by increased arousal, tension, nervousness, and worry [6]. Test anxiety is considered a situation-specific disposition (trait). Persons high in test anxiety tend to react in evaluative exams or other performance situations with increased anxiety [7]. Test anxiety can lead to the avoidance of learning opportunities and learning and performance impairments [4,6,8]. It also reduces students’ well-being [9]. People on the autism spectrum often have diminished well-being [10,11]. For autistic individuals who are in educational contexts, such as university students, test anxiety could exacerbate their reduced well-being and interfere with their educational progress.
Test anxiety is composed of emotional and cognitive components [8,12]. The emotional component includes automatic arousal and the affect caused by the exam situation [12]. The cognitive component includes worries about being evaluated and failing as well as the expectation of aversive consequences [12]. Similar to emotionality, worry is activated in stressful situations (e.g., exams, evaluations [8]). Cognitive interference and lack of confidence are two additional components of test anxiety [7,13] that have been included in the concept by some authors (e.g., [14]). Cognitive interference refers to impaired cognition in test situations, whereas lack of confidence refers to the absence of optimism about one’s performance and self-control during test situations [15]. These four components of test anxiety are included in the present study.
We explored whether adults on the autism spectrum show increased test anxiety compared to non-autistic adults. We assumed that autistic university students have an increased likelihood of being test anxious, as people on the autism spectrum are generally prone to anxiety [2,3], and they are in a phase of life in which test situations are salient and important [4]. Autistic university students are also considered an appropriate examination group because of the limited support services provided to adults on the autism spectrum (especially persons with high-functional autism), and their dropout rate from university might be high [16]. Heightened test anxiety scores in autistic students would point to the need for more specific support for this group.
As a first step in combining autism and test anxiety research, we compared German-speaking autistic university students’ averaged test anxiety scores with the scores of a standardization (norming) sample for an established test anxiety measure. As another comparison sample, we asked non-autistic university students to complete the same test anxiety measure during the last third of the semester; that is, close to the examinations. With the examinations approaching, test anxiety may become more prevalent among test-anxious students and potentially lead to higher self-reports of test anxiety. If autistic students’ test anxiety scores were still higher, this would suggest that they have considerably increased test anxiety.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The autistic university students were recruited in German-speaking autism internet forums. From a larger sample, the participants who indicated being university students and were formally diagnosed with an autism spectrum condition were selected for the purpose of this study. The sample consisted of 16 university students (56% female, 38% male, and 6% diverse; Mage = 24.81, SDage = 4.52). All of the students indicated having sufficient German language skills.
The autistic university student group was compared with the nonclinical standardization sample of university students for the applied test anxiety measure (n = 1350; 59% female and 41% male; Mage = 22.85, SDage = 4.51) [14]. As another comparison group (close-to-examination non-autistic university student sample), 101 university students were recruited at a German university in January 2020 during the last third of the semester (79% female and 21% male; Mage = 22.47, SDage = 3.76). None of these students were diagnosed with an autism spectrum condition, and they completed the same test anxiety measure as the autistic university students in this study.

2.2. Procedures

To recruit autistic university students, access to an online survey was published in different German-speaking autism internet forums. For complete access to the autism internet forum, only registered members were allowed. The online survey was programmed on Unipark [17].
The participants were first welcomed and asked for their informed consent. Next, the autistic university students completed the brief version of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient for adults (AQ-10) in German [18,19]. The AQ-10 is a widely applied self-report used as a screening tool for suspected autistic disorder and for measuring the extent of autistic traits in autism research [20,21]. It consists of 10 items capturing autistic core traits (e.g., “I usually concentrate more on the whole picture rather than the small details” and “I find it easy to do more than one thing at once”). Each item was rated on a four-point Likert-type scale. Definitely/slightly agree and definitely/slightly disagree are awarded one and zero points, respectively (or zero/one point when the items are reversed), which are summed to form the total score. Higher scores reflect more pronounced autistic traits.
Following the AQ-10, the autistic university students completed the standardized Prüfungsangstfragebogen (PAF, test anxiety questionnaire) [14], which is well validated, designed for university students, and frequently used in German-speaking areas. The PAF contains 20 items in total. The four subdimensions of test anxiety, namely, worry (e.g., “I think about how important the exam is to me”), emotionality (e.g., “I feel anxious”), cognitive interference (e.g., “Suddenly, thoughts shoot through my head that block me”), and lack of confidence (e.g., “I trust in my performance,” reversed), are measured with five items each. Each item was rated on a four-point Likert-type scale (1 = almost never to 4 = almost always). Higher scores represent higher test anxiety. Afterwards, sociodemographic information was requested, and the autistic participants were thanked and carefully debriefed.
The close-to-examination non-autistic university student sample was recruited by approaching students who happened to be on the campus of a German university at the time and asking them to participate in a short questionnaire study. After giving their informed consent, the students provided sociodemographic information and completed the paper-and-pencil version of the PAF. Afterwards, they were thanked and debriefed.
In this study, Cronbach’s α for the AQ-10 was satisfactory (0.74) in the autistic university student sample. The Cronbach’s αs for the total test anxiety score across the 20 PAF items were high for the autistic university student sample (0.93), the PAF standardization sample (0.88), and the close-to-examination non-autistic university student sample (0.89). The Cronbach’s αs for the PAF subscales were also high for the autistic university student sample, the PAF standardization sample, and the close-to-examination non-autistic university student sample (worry: 0.93/0.81/0.85; emotionality: 0.92/0.86/0.87; cognitive interference: 0.91/0.79/0.78; lack of confidence: 0.88/0.85/0.86).

2.3. Analysis Strategy

The following t-tests refer to the specific means in the sample of autistic university students and the corresponding means in the two non-clinical comparison samples (Table 1). Bootstrapping was applied to test the robustness of the mean differences. Bootstrapping is a technique for taking repeated samples (with replacements) from the present dataset, which allows calculation of confidence intervals for the statistic of interest (in the present case, the difference between two means) [22]. Whenever bootstrapping was used in this study, it was bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa), and it was based on 1000 bootstrap samples [22]. The percentile ranks according to the test anxiety questionnaire manual [14] for the different samples were also reported for interpretation purposes. According to the manual, percentile ranks between 25 and 75 are considered unremarkable, whereas percentile ranks less than 25 and more than 75 are considered remarkably low and high, respectively [14].

3. Results

3.1. Total Test Anxiety Scores

Test anxiety was significantly higher in the autistic university student sample than in the standardization sample (mean difference = 0.67, BCa 95% CI [0.31, 0.98], t[15] = 3.93, p = 0.001, d = 0.98) and the 2020 close-to-examination sample (mean difference = 0.46, BCa 95% CI [0.10, 0.77], t[15] = 2.69, p = 0.02, d = 0.67). The percentile ranks were 91 for the autistic student sample, 56 for the standardization sample, and 71 for the 2020 close-to-examination sample.

3.2. Worry Subscale Scores

The worry scores of the autistic university student sample did not differ from the worry scores of the standardization sample (mean difference = 0.32, BCa 95% CI [−0.17, 0.74], t[15] = 1.24, p = 0.23, d = 0.31) and the 2020 close-to-examination sample (mean difference = 0.05, BCa 95% CI [−0.46, 0.51], t[15] = 0.18, p = 0.86, d = 0.04). The percentile ranks were 72 for the autistic student sample, 51 for the standardization sample, and 61 for the 2020 close-to-examination sample.

3.3. Emotionality Subscale Scores

Autistic university students had higher emotionality scores than the university students in the standardization sample (mean difference = 0.83, BCa 95% CI [0.36, 1.23], t[15] = 3.73, p = 0.002, d = 0.93) and the 2020 close-to-examination sample (mean difference = 0.65, BCa 95% CI [0.24, 1.05], t[15] = 2.92, p = 0.01, d = 0.73). The percentile ranks were 86 for the autistic student sample, 56 for the standardization sample, and 66 for the 2020 close-to-examination sample.

3.4. Cognitive Interference Subscale Scores

Autistic university students were more likely to experience cognitive interference than the university students in the standardization sample (mean difference = 0.85, BCa 95% CI [0.28, 1.38], t[15] = 3.60, p = 0.003, d = 0.90) and the 2020 close-to-examination sample (mean difference = 0.58, BCa 95% CI [0.15, 0.95], t[15] = 2.45, p = 0.03, d = 0.61). The percentile ranks were 89 for the autistic student sample, 57 for the standardization sample, and 68 for the 2020 close-to-examination sample.

3.5. Lack of Confidence Subscale Scores

Autistic university students had higher lack of confidence scores than the university students in the standardization sample (mean difference = 0.69, BCa 95% CI [0.30, 1.04], t[15] = 3.56, p = 0.003, d = 0.89) and the 2020 close-to-examination sample (mean difference = 0.58, BCa 95% CI [0.21, 0.90], t[15] = 2.99, p = 0.009, d = 0.75). The percentile ranks were 86 for the autistic student sample, 56 for the standardization sample, and 56 for the 2020 close-to-examination sample.

3.6. Autistic Traits

The extent of autistic university students’ autistic traits correlated strongly with the total test anxiety scores (r[14] = 0.80, p < 0.001), the worry subscale scores (r[14] = 0.63, p = 0.008), the emotionality subscale scores (r[14] = 0.77, p < 0.001), and the cognitive interference subscale scores (r[14] = 0.61, p = 0.01) but not with the lack of confidence subscale scores (r[14] = 0.32, p = 0.22).

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to examine whether test anxiety is increased in German-speaking autistic university students. Overall, autistic participants had a higher total test anxiety score, a higher emotionality subscale score, a higher cognitive interference subscale score, and a higher lack of confidence subscale score compared with the standardization sample in an established test anxiety diagnostic questionnaire for German-speaking university students [14]. These scores were also higher for the autistic participants compared with a sample of university students who reported their test anxiety during the last third of the semester (i.e., close to the examinations). Total test anxiety, emotionality, cognitive interference, and lack of confidence were remarkably high in the autistic participants, as the sample’s percentile ranks exceeded the 75% threshold [14], indicating a problematically strong text anxiety, on average. In the comparison samples, the percentile ranks were in the unremarkable range, even for those individuals who were close to their examinations.
Interestingly, autistic participants’ worry subscale scores were unremarkable. Thus, the tendency to worry in test situations was not the cause of their relatively high total test anxiety. This null result suggests that the reasons for increased anxiety in autistic people, even beyond the test anxiety domain [23], are not primarily found at the cognitive level. For the other facets of test anxiety, explanations for the higher scores in autistic people, apart from worrying, are available. High test-anxious emotionality may be explained by neuronally caused hyper-excitability in autistic people [3]. An increased tendency for cognitive interference may stem from autistic people’s difficulties in executive functioning, including a decreased ability for selective attention [24]. Regarding lack of confidence, autistic people have recently been shown to be less self-deceptive than non-autistic people [25], which may result in their lower self-confidence [26].
We also found that autistic university students’ extent of autistic traits was strongly associated with higher total test anxiety and three of the test anxiety subscale scores. Thus, even within the autistic population, the severity of autistic symptoms may be relevant for the degree of test anxiety. However, we do not want to overstate these relationships, as very recent research has revealed some psychometric deficits of the 10-item AQ that we used in this study [27,28].
With respect to the implications, the present findings suggest that test anxiety can be added to the specific anxieties that have been shown to be increased in autistic people, such as social anxiety [2]. The extensive test anxiety literature [4] may be relevant and useful for autism researchers to understand autistic individuals’ experiences in the educational domain. The test anxiety literature also provides well-examined interventions for test-anxious people, such as anxiety management training or cognitive-attentional training [4]; potentially, these interventions may be useful for autistic students. Universities that offer counselling for autistic students could consider their increased test anxiety and offer possible solutions.
The present results must be considered preliminary because of the limitations of the study, which threaten internal, external, and conclusion validity. The sample of autistic university students was small and limited to German-speaking university students. Test anxiety was assessed by self-reports, which usually may be biased by socially desirable response tendencies. Autistic people seem to have a lowered tendency to self-deceive [25], which could influence their self-reports of test anxiety. In addition, although we used an established diagnostic instrument, we measured test anxiety at a general level only. It has been shown that test anxiety for oral and written exams can be differentiated [29]. Possibly, autistic university students are more anxious during oral examinations, as their difficulties in communication and social interaction may be a significant additional burden during the test situation.
Future research may seek to overcome these limitations. The self-regulation of test anxiety [30,31] may also be a relevant topic for further research, considering that autistic people often have difficulties in executive functioning [32,33]. In this regard, research should also examine which of the previously developed and scientifically evaluated interventions for test anxious people [4] are most useful for autistic university students. Given their increased levels of the test anxiety components emotionality and cognitive interference, autistic university students might benefit from emotion-focused regulation techniques (e.g., anxiety management training) as well as cognitive-focused interventions improving attentional control (e.g., cognitive-attentional training). Moreover, the differences between male and female autistic students require further investigation. Finally, whether the present findings can be replicated, for instance, in samples of school students, is of interest.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.Z., M.Z. and A.B.; methodology, A.Z., M.Z. and A.B.; validation, A.Z. and A.B.; formal analysis, A.Z. and A.B.; investigation, A.Z., M.Z. and A.B.; resources, A.B.; data curation, A.B.; writing—original draft preparation, A.Z.; writing—review and editing, M.Z. and A.B.; visualization, A.Z., M.Z. and A.B.; supervision, A.B.; project administration, A.B.; funding acquisition, A.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant from the Uranus Foundation, Switzerland.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Human Sciences at the University of Bern (reference number: 2019-05-00004).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bellini, S. Social skill deficits and anxiety in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Focus Autism Dev. Disabil. 2004, 19, 78–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Markram, K.; Markram, H. The Intense World Theory—A unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2010, 4, 224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Zeidner, M. Test Anxiety: The State of the Art; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  5. Hillier, A.; Goldstein, J.; Murphy, D.; Trietsch, R.; Keeves, J.; Mendes, E.; Queenan, A. Supporting university students with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 2017, 22, 20–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Spielberger, C.D.; Vagg, P.R. Test anxiety: A transactional process model. In Test Anxiety: Theory, Assessment, and Treatment; Spielberger, C.D., Vagg, P.R., Eds.; Taylor & Francis: Washington, DC, USA, 1995; pp. 3–14. [Google Scholar]
  7. Hodapp, V.; Glanzmann, P.G.; Laux, L. Theory and Measurement of Test Anxiety as a Situation-Specific Trait. In Test Anxiety: Theory, Assessment, and Treatment; Spielberger, C.D., Vagg, P.R., Eds.; Taylor & Francis: Washington, DC, USA, 1995; pp. 47–58. [Google Scholar]
  8. Eysenck, M.W. Anxiety: The Cognitive Perspective; Erlbaum: Hove, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  9. Steinmayr, R.; Crede, J.; McElvany, N.; Wirthwein, L. Subjective well-being, test anxiety, academic achievement: Testing for reciprocal effects. Front. Psychol. 2016, 6, 1994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Lord, C.; McCauley, J.B.; Pepa, L.A.; Huerta, M.; Pickles, A. Work, living, and the pursuit of happiness: Vocational and psychosocial outcomes for young adults with autism. Autism 2020, 24, 1691–1703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Stimpson, N.J.; Hull, L.; Mandy, W. The association between autistic traits and mental well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 2021, 22, 287–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Liebert, R.M.; Morris, L.W. Cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychol. Rep. 1967, 20, 975–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Carver, C.S.; Scheier, M.F. Self-Focused Attention in Test-Anxiety: A General Theory Applied to a Specific Phenomenon. In Advances in Test Anxiety Research; van der Ploeg, H.M., Schwarzer, R., Spielberger, C.D., Eds.; Swets & Zeitlinger: Lisse, The Netherlands, 1984; Volume 3, pp. 3–20. [Google Scholar]
  14. Hodapp, V.; Rohrmann, S.; Ringeisen, T. PAF–Prüfungsangstfragebogen [PAF–Test Anxiety Questionnaire]; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  15. Keith, N.; Hodapp, V.; Schermelleh-Engel, K.; Moosbrugger, H. Cross-sectional and longitudinal confirmatory factor models for the German Test Anxiety Inventory: A construction validation. Anxiety Stress Coping 2003, 16, 251–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Freeth, M.; Bullock, T.; Milne, E. The distribution of and relationship between autistic traits and social anxiety in a UK student population. Autism 2012, 17, 571–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Questback. Unipark. EFS Survey [Computer Software], Questback, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  18. Freitag, C.M.; Retz-Junginger, P.; Retz, W.; Seitz, C.; Palmason, H.; Meyer, J.; Rösler, M.; von Gontard, A. Evaluation der deutschen Version des Autismus-Spektrum-Quotienten (AQ)–die Kurzversion AQ-k [German adaptation of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evaluation and short version AQ-k]. Z. Klin. Psychol. Psychother. 2007, 36, 280–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Allison, C.; Auyeung, B.; Baron-Cohen, S. Toward brief “Red Flags” for autism screening: The Short Autism Spectrum Quotient and the Short Quantitative Checklist for Autism in toddlers in 1000 cases and 3000 controls [corrected]. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. 2012, 51, 202–212.e7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Bertrams, A.; Schlegel, K. Speeded reasoning moderates the inverse relationship between autistic traits and emotion recognition. Autism 2020, 24, 2304–2309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Lundin, A.; Kosidou, K.; Dalman, C. Measuring autism traits in the adult general population with the brief Autism-Spectrum Quotient, AQ-10: Findings from the Stockholm Public Health Cohort. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019, 49, 773–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Field, A. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics; Sage: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  23. Hollocks, M.J.; Lerh, J.W.; Magiati, I.; Meiser-Stedman, R.; Brugha, T.S. Anxiety and depression in adults with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 2019, 49, 559–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Belmonte, M.K.; Yurgelun-Todd, D.A. Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism. Cognit. Brain Res. 2003, 17, 651–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Bertrams, A. Less illusion of a just world in people with formally diagnosed autism and higher autistic traits. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. von Hippel, W.; Trivers, R. The evolution and psychology of self-deception. Behav. Brain Sci. 2011, 34, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Bertrams, A. Internal reliability, homogeneity, and factor structure of the ten-item Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ-10) with two additional response categories. Exp. Results 2021, 2, e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Taylor, E.; Livingston, L.; Clutterbuck, R.; Shah, P. Psychometric concerns with the 10-item Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ10) as a measure of trait autism in the general population. Exp. Results 2020, 1, e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Sparfeldt, J.R.; Rost, D.H.; Baumeister, U.M.; Christ, O. Test anxiety in written and oral examinations. Learn Individ. Differ. 2013, 24, 198–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bertrams, A.; Englert, C.; Dickhäuser, O. Self-control strength in the relation between trait test anxiety and state anxiety. J. Res. Personal. 2010, 44, 738–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Englert, C.; Bertrams, A. The role of self-control strength in the development of state anxiety in test situations. Psychol. Rep. 2013, 112, 976–991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Hughes, C.; Russell, J.; Robbins, T.W. Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia 1994, 32, 477–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ozonoff, S.; Pennington, B.F.; Rogers, S.J. Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1991, 32, 1081–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations (in brackets) of Test Anxiety Scores in the Autistic University Student Sample, the PAF Standardization Sample [14], and the Close-to-Examination Non-autistic University Student Sample (January 2020).
Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations (in brackets) of Test Anxiety Scores in the Autistic University Student Sample, the PAF Standardization Sample [14], and the Close-to-Examination Non-autistic University Student Sample (January 2020).
Autistic University StudentsPAF Standardization Sample2020 University Student Sample
(n = 16)(n = 1350)(n = 101)
Total test anxiety score2.86 (0.68)2.19 (0.46)2.40 (0.50)
Worry subscale2.93 (1.02)2.61 (0.67)2.88 (0.73)
Emotionality subscale2.85 (0.89)2.02 (0.72)2.20 (0.76)
Cognitive interference subscale2.63 (0.94)1.78 (0.60)2.05 (0.66)
Lack of confidence subscale3.03 (0.77)2.34 (0.62)2.45 (0.61)
Note. PAF = Prüfungsangstfragebogen (test anxiety questionnaire). Scores were obtained by averaging the item responses in a (sub)scale. The responses were given on the following scale: 1 (almost never), 2 (sometimes), 3 (often), and 4 (almost always).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zavery, A.; Zäch, M.; Bertrams, A. Test Anxiety in Autistic University Students—Preliminary Results from a German-Speaking Sample. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 390. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030390

AMA Style

Zavery A, Zäch M, Bertrams A. Test Anxiety in Autistic University Students—Preliminary Results from a German-Speaking Sample. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(3):390. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030390

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zavery, Alafia, Myriam Zäch, and Alex Bertrams. 2021. "Test Anxiety in Autistic University Students—Preliminary Results from a German-Speaking Sample" Brain Sciences 11, no. 3: 390. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030390

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop