Data in Context: How Digital Transformation Can Support Human Reasoning in Cyber-Physical Production Systems
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Challenges of Contextualization in Industrial Production Plants
1.2. Digital Transformation and Human–Machine Cooperation in CPPS
1.3. Aims of the Present Work
2. Cognitive Challenges of Reasoning in Context
2.1. Sampling the Available Information
2.1.1. Information Samples Are Biased
2.1.2. Selecting and Ignoring Particular Types of Information
2.1.3. Tasks and Information Sources Affect Information Search
2.1.4. Summary
2.2. Integrating Different Information Elements
2.2.1. Strategies of Information Integration
2.2.2. Task and Information Characteristics Affect Integration
2.2.3. Summary
2.3. Categorizing Objects and Events
2.3.1. Differentiation: Context-Dependent, Flexible Categorization
2.3.2. Generalization: The Role of Similarity
2.3.3. Summary
2.4. Reasoning about Causes
2.4.1. Covariation
2.4.2. Temporal Relationships
2.4.3. Prior Knowledge
2.4.4. Dealing with Complexity
2.4.5. Summary
3. Requirements: What Should Technologies Do to Support Humans?
- Provide models of the system and connect them with data:
- Make structural relations and rules explicit;
- Highlight constraints of the situation and equipment;
- Enable semantic zooming and switches between levels of abstraction;
- Process data in a context-dependent manner.
- Provide and integrate data from different sources:
- Make data from different sources available;
- Pre-process and debias data to obtain a valid picture;
- Make procedures of sampling and integration transparent.
- Process and integrate data across time:
- Provide timely information;
- Enable tracking of changes (past, current, and future data);
- Link current situation with historical data.
4. Technologies to Support Contextualization
4.1. Building and Interconnecting Formal Models
4.2. Sampling and Integrating Process Data
4.2.1. Sampling Dynamic Data
4.2.2. Integrating Data from Different Sources
4.3. Comparing the Present Situation with Historical Data
4.3.1. Finding Patterns in Dynamic Data
4.3.2. Storing and Retrieving Human Experience
5. Discussion
5.1. What Stands in the Way of Application?
5.2. The Question of Function Allocation
5.3. Limitations and Future Work
5.3.1. The Psychological Literature Does Not Always Adequately Address Real-World Demands
5.3.2. Only a Fraction of the Relevant Cognitive Challenges Was Addressed
5.3.3. Support Strategies from the Psychological Literature Were Not Considered
5.3.4. No Specific Implementations of Technologies Were Suggested
5.4. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Cognitive Factors | General Requirements for Operator Support Strategies | Examples |
---|---|---|
Information samples are biased | ||
Sampling biases | Reduce bias in the automated generation of samples | Do not perform product control at fixed intervals (e.g., every 100th casting mold) as they might coincide with temperature cycles |
Make the selection of automatically generated samples transparent | “Only every 100th casting mold is submitted to quality control” | |
Provide information as to whether samples are representative | “The frequency of this fault have only been determined for milk chocolate but not for caramel chocolate” | |
Provide hints when operators have ignored potentially relevant information | “You have not checked motor current trend charts, yet” | |
Support the generation of arguments against a given anchor or standard | “Please check whether the temperature value from the selected previous case applies to the current situation” | |
Availability heuristic | Make information available in an unbiased manner | Provide different data sources (e.g., temperature in end cooler, buffer, and packaging machine) and data types (e.g., temperature, soiling, and motor currents) |
Provide information about the base rates of process states and events | “Hollow bottoms are present in 20% of the bars overall” | |
Provide hints that other information sources are available | “The current hypothesis can also be checked by inspecting the motor current trend chart” | |
Conditional sampling | Make it explicit when events may be overrepresented in samples | “Chocolate bars have mostly been checked for hollow bottoms when faults have occurred, which may overestimate the impact of this deviation” |
Use presentation formats that aid Bayes reasoning | Frequency grids or frequency trees | |
Repeating choices that initially led to good outcomes | Provide hints about own previous choices and choices of others in similar situations | “In previous instances of this fault, you have only checked for hollow bottoms. Other operators have also checked motor current trend charts and vacuum suction” |
Make the relevance of different data sources and observations transparent | “Bar skewness after the stopper is predictive of this fault, while bar skewness before the stopper is not” | |
Make it explicit when the contribution of data sources is context-dependent | “Room temperature is predictive of this fault for milk chocolate but not dark chocolate” | |
Selecting and ignoring particular types of information | ||
Salient cues | Make cue validities explicit | “Hollow bottoms have low predictive value for this fault” |
Support evaluations of whether extreme values are relevant | “High room temperature is irrelevant for bad packaging quality” | |
Make non-salient but relevant cues accessible | Provide height measurement of chocolate bars, which can cause problems but is not visually perceivable for operators | |
Confirmation bias | Provide evidence/data in favor of opposing hypotheses | “You assume the cause for skewed bars to be insufficient ground contact due to hollow bottoms, but the motor current trend chart indicates too much grip due to a smeared conveyor belt” |
Make it transparent which hypotheses are supported by what information and which have not been tested sufficiently | “The hypothesis of insufficient ground contact as a cause for skewed bars is supported by hollow bottoms, but the motor current trend chart indicates a soiled conveyor belt” | |
Positive test strategy | Present data for situations in which the property of interest differs, thus mitigating the effect of illusory correlations | “Bars had hollow bottoms in 25% of the situations in which this fault occurred, but also in 18% of situations without a fault” |
Make unequal sample sizes transparent | “There are 311 measurements for milk chocolate but only 17 for caramel chocolate” | |
Weight evidence by sample size | “The 17 measurements for caramel chocolate may not be representative” | |
Re-interpreting information to fit hypotheses | Ask operators to make interpretations explicit, provide feedback about these interpretations | “What do you conclude from the observation that bars were skewed?” [Operator: “Insufficient ground contact due to hollow bottoms”] “This conclusion is problematic, because bar skewness can also be caused by too much grip on a smeared conveyor belt” |
Inattentional blindness | Direct operators’ attention to information they have not yet considered | “This problem can also be checked by inspecting the motor current trend chart” |
Ignoring contextual constraints | Make constraints and side-effects transparent | “Reducing machine speed below 500 will cause overflow in the buffer, which may ultimately force the molding unit to stop” |
Neglecting unknown or missing information | Make it explicit that data for some relevant aspects are not available | “Temperature of chocolate filling affects bar stability but cannot be measured” |
Highlight the consequences of missing information (what-if) | “If the (unavailable) temperature of chocolate filling strongly differs from temperature of the coating, this can lead to tensions and cause breakage” | |
Tasks and information sources affect information search | ||
Task complexity | Provide information in a task-/state-/context-dependent manner | “To determine whether the problem may have been caused by bar temperature, you also need to consider the temperature in the molding unit and whether this chocolate type is susceptible to temperature variations” (and provide specific values) |
Make additional information available on demand | “Click here to check information about the machine, from the molding unit, and from the production planning system…” | |
Provide domain information and problem solving information (instead of just basic data) for complex tasks | “Too low temperatures of the cold stamp lead to ice crystals, which cause instable walls” instead of just “The temperature in the end cooler is −10 °C” | |
Information overload | Filter data according to their relevance, use context-dependent filtering | Do not show foil characteristics in case of problems at the feed conveyor |
Highlight the relevance of information elements | “Temperature variations are particularly important for this fault” | |
Make consequences of misuse understandable (e.g., consequences of errors and filtering) | “If you ignore motor current trend charts, you may not find out whether this problem is caused by too much grip on the conveyor belt” | |
Make information sources explicit | “Too much grip on the conveyor belt can be estimated from motor current trend charts” | |
Provide information on different levels of abstraction and support switching | “Physical Form: hollow bottoms; Physical Function: insufficient ground contact on conveyor belt; Generalized Function: dysfunctional transport of chocolate bars; Abstract Function: frequent faults at carrier belt; Functional Purpose: low efficiency” | |
Accessibility | Increase accessibility of context information (e.g., right format, right level of detail, saving time, lots of information in one place) | Name and explain current problems in the molding unit (previous production step), instead of individual molding parameter values |
Make format and level of detail configurable | Let operators decide whether they want to see overall efficiency during the previous shift or individual faults | |
Integrate information from different sources | Present information about chocolate characteristics and state of the molding unit together with associated problems of the packaging machine | |
Familiarity | Make unfamiliar sources easy to access | Place a link to motor current trend charts on the main fault screen |
Highlight consequences of only considering particular (familiar) sources | “If you only look at bar skewness but not motor current trend charts, you may not see whether the problem is caused by too much grip on the conveyor belt” |
Cognitive Factors | General Requirements for Operator Support Strategies | Examples |
---|---|---|
Strategies of information integration | ||
Formal or informal | Integrate cues algorithmically, especially in low and high-validity situations | Automatically calculate whether speed of molding unit matches the speed of packaging machines |
Make algorithms transparent (i.e., use of data, algorithmic procedures), allowing operators to evaluate completeness and appropriateness | “The image processing algorithm has attended to the surface of the casting mold when determining that the mold was faulty” | |
Enable operators to include/exclude cues and change weights, show changes in outcomes | Operators can indicate that temperature is less important in a particular situation, and in consequence it receives less weight in the selection of similar cases | |
Make it explicit what important factors an algorithm cannot consider | “The algorithm ignores temperature of the filling as it cannot be measured, and air moisture as its specific effects are unknown” | |
Rule- or exemplar-based | Make appropriate rules available, depending on task, goals, and context | “High temperature may predict soiling of the conveyor belt, but more so for milk chocolate than dark chocolate” |
Support cue abstraction (i.e., make predictive power of cues explicit) | “Hollow bottoms are the strongest predictor of skewed chocolate bars” | |
Present each cue in terms of its presence/absence or value | “Hollow bottoms are present, temperature is 21 °C” | |
Support selection of suitable exemplars (cases) | “Case 12 matches the current situation in terms of temperature, motor currents, and chocolate type, Case 17 differs in chocolate type” | |
Highlight correspondence between cases and rules (i.e., case abstraction) | “Case 14 has a lower molding temperature but still is comparable as the bars remained in the buffer for longer, which also leads to warming” | |
Relying on heuristics | Support operators in assessing the context-dependent suitability of heuristics | “To determine whether there was a problem with a packing claw, it is sufficient to check whether every eighth bar was affected” |
Point out heuristics that lead to problematic outcomes | “Evaluating the height of the downholder in isolation is problematic, because a low downholder can be suitable if bars are smaller” | |
Reduce the need to rely on heuristics by providing algorithmic integration | Automatically calculate whether speed of molding unit matches the speed of packaging machines | |
Task and information characteristics affect integration | ||
Task complexity | Support operators in partitioning the task | “(1) Check where the problem starts occurring, (2) check whether all bars or only some of them are affected, (3) check bar characteristics like geometry and temperature, (4) check mechanical machine settings” |
Support data integration and provide overviews in complex tasks | Present information about chocolate characteristics and state of the molding unit together with associated problems of the packaging machine | |
Coherence | Highlight whether available information is coherent (i.e., points in the same direction) and point out mismatches | “Hollow bottoms are consistent with insufficient ground contact, but the motor current trend chart indicates a soiled conveyor belt” |
Organize information to provide overview and facilitate assessment of coherence | Present current values of all variables that affect chocolate smearing in one place (even when they stem from different production steps) | |
Validity of easily accessible information | Make valid/important information easy to access | Present the five parameters that best predict the current fault type on the main fault screen |
Make validity of information transparent | “Hollow bottoms have low predictive value for this fault” | |
Presentation format | Reduce search demands by integrating information from different sources | Present information about chocolate characteristics and state of the molding unit together with associated problems of the packaging machine |
Time pressure | Provide higher degree of automated integration in situations with high time pressure | Automatically integrate high temperature in molding unit, long time in buffer, high motor currents, and milk chocolate into “the conveyor belt may need to be cleaned” |
Cognitive Factors | General Requirements for Operator Support Strategies | Examples |
---|---|---|
Differentiation: context-dependent, flexible categorization | ||
Flexible category representations | Make interconnections of information transparent | “Both long times in the buffer and high temperatures may cause melting, but what is an appropriate temperature depends on chocolate type”; “bar weight often provides information about bar height” |
Allow operators to access information from different perspectives (e.g., levels of abstraction, task-dependent views) | “Physical Form: hollow bottoms; Physical Function: insufficient ground contact on conveyor belt; Generalized Function: dysfunctional transport of chocolate bars; Abstract Function: frequent faults at carrier belt; Functional Purpose: low efficiency” | |
Provide different instead of just similar cases | “Case 26 has a similar symptom (soiled conveyor belt) but different parameter values, and should be cross-checked as a differential diagnosis” | |
Facilitate information decomposition | “Conveyor belt soiling should be checked for all four conveyor belts, because if it only occurs at the vacuum belt, this may indicate wear of the vacuum hole edges” | |
Suggest different interpretations or prompt operators to generate them | “A soiled conveyor belt can indicate a temperature problem, but it can also indicate friction due to differential speed of adjacent conveyors” | |
Environmental contingencies | Provide information about relations and parameter interactions | “Temperature in the molding unit and time in the buffer both lead to warm chocolate, which can cause soiling. However, the two parameters can compensate each other” |
Highlight changes in regularities of events | “High temperature is less problematic today, because dark chocolate is being produced” | |
Highlight changes in relations between interacting parameters | “High temperature is less problematic now, because you have reduced machine speed” | |
Conceptual change | Provide factual information to help operators detect and correct false beliefs | “The turning wheel is not responsible for squished chocolate bars” |
Provide information about system structure and functional relations to support mental model generation and updating | “Hollow bottoms cause problems because they reduce ground contact on the conveyor belt, which may lead to misalignment of chocolate bars” | |
Provide semantic relations between concepts and events to make ontological structures understandable | “Temperature in the molding unit and time in the buffer both lead to warm chocolate, which can cause soiling. However, the two parameters can compensate each other” | |
Cue relevance | Highlight and explain context-specific changes in the relevance of information | “If machine speed is reduced, hollow bottoms are less problematic, as the bars are not pulled into a skewed position at conveyor boundaries as much” |
Generalization: the role of similarity | ||
Similarity depends on context | Highlight the features most important to determine similarity in the current context | “With milk chocolate, temperature the most important parameter to determine whether a previous case is similar” |
Similarity depends on focus of attention | Focus attention on relevant features depending on context | “If you want to find out whether soiling of conveyor belts may have caused the problem, you should focus on parameters that are associated with melting: temperature and time in the buffer” |
Surface/structural features | Show structural correspondence between situations | “Case 14 has a lower molding temperature but still is comparable as the bars remained in the buffer for longer, which also leads to warming” |
Provide information as relational categories rather than just entity categories | “Chocolate types that melt easily” and “chocolate types that break easily”, rather than just “milk chocolate” and “marzipan” | |
Highlight differences despite feature similarity | “In Case 13, machine speed and temperature were as high as in the current situation. However, as dark chocolate was produced, these parameters were suitable, while now they may be problematic” | |
Features beyond similarity | Make it explicit when comparisons should change depending on task goals | “If you need to solve the problem quickly, select cases that offer solutions relying on machine speed or cleaning instead of cases that require changes in the molding unit” |
Support comparison between situations according to different criteria, let operators manipulate these criteria | Offer case similarity calculation methods based on feature similarity, mechanism similarity, outcome quality, or required effort | |
Show information distribution within situation classes (e.g., variability of parameters) | “For this fault, temperature fluctuations are normal: temperature is very high in some cases of this fault class but not in others” |
Cognitive Factors | General Requirements for Operator Support Strategies | Examples |
---|---|---|
Covariation | ||
Magnitude of probabilities | Make base rates of causes and outcomes available | “Hollow bottoms occur in 20% of situations, but the problem of skewed bars after the stopper only occurs in 5%” |
Point out illusory correlations | “This fault type occurs almost as often when hollow bottoms are present and when they are absent” | |
Make absence of causal relations explicit | “Foil color differs between the current situation and the selected case but has no impact on the current problem” | |
Inattentional blindness for negative relations | Make negative causal relations explicit | “Machine cooling reduces the impact of long time in the buffer” |
Overshadowing and super-learning | Disentangle single causal effects | “Skewed bars can be caused by soiled conveyor belts, hollow bottoms, insufficient vacuum suction, and incorrect positioning of machine parts” |
Make interactions between causes explicit (e.g., additive, enhancing, suppressing) | “The effects of low temperatures in the molding unit are cancelled out when chocolate bars remain in the buffer for a long time” | |
Highlight simple correlations that do not contribute to causal effects | “Low weight of chocolate bars is associated with skewness, but this is not because weight causes skewness but because weight and skewness both are a consequence of hollow bottoms” | |
Type of task | State alternative outcomes in predictive reasoning tasks | “Low temperatures of chocolate filling may not only cause hollow bottoms but also tensions between coating and filling, which can lead to breakage” |
Highlight causal strengths in predictive reasoning tasks | “Low machine cooling only has a weak impact on conveyor belt soiling” | |
Temporal relations | ||
Temporal order | Provide information about temporal order and temporal dependencies of events | “First, high temperatures cause chocolate to melt and lead to soiling of the conveyor belts. This can result in skewed bars, which later may break upon contact with the carrier belt” |
Highlight temporal orders when they cannot easily be perceived | “A chocolate bar is first touched by the injector and then by the transfer finger, but it can quickly move back and forth between the two components, which may cause breakage” | |
Make it explicit when events follow each other but are not causally related | “Bars can break after they have moved into the turning wheel, but it is not the turning wheel that causes breakage” | |
Temporal contiguity | Minimize time delays in information presentation | Present past molding problems together with current packaging problems instead of at the time when they occur (about one hour delay) |
Increase time delays when no causal relation exists | Present the processes in the injection unit separately from processes in the turning wheel when explaining how the former cause breakage | |
Temporal variability | Make variable time lags between cause and effect transparent | “Problems in the molding unit can affect the packaging machine with a delay of 20 min to 1.5 h” |
Provide information about factors affecting the variability of delays | “The delay of molding problems affecting the packaging machine depends on the time that chocolate bars remain in the buffer” | |
Expecting longer delays | Provide information about delayed effects | Simulate how the current temperatures in the molding unit will affect the packaging process in one hour |
Prior knowledge | ||
Assumptions about causal roles of events | Make causal relations within the system and the causal roles of each factor transparent | “Temperature in the molding unit and time in the buffer both lead to warm chocolate, which can cause soiling. However, the two parameters can compensate each other” |
Point out common misconceptions for a given problem situation | “People often think that the turning wheel squishes the bars, but that is not true. The problem actually originates in the injection unit and only becomes visible at the turning wheel” | |
Selecting appropriate integration rules | Support integration of fragments of causal nets | Show causal diagram of factors from different process steps affecting the breakability of chocolate bars |
Make relations between causes explicit (e.g., additive, compensatory) | “Bar height and downholder height can compensate each other” | |
Show how relations are affected by context, highlight differences between situations | “Whether high temperatures cause skewed chocolate bars depends on machine speed, and the relation is stronger for milk chocolate than dark chocolate” | |
Experience | Specify the exact types of relations between events or elements of the plant instead of just their causal direction | Show mechanisms by which high temperatures cause skewed chocolate bars (soiling of conveyor belts, increasing grip on the belts, increasing the effects of speed differences at conveyor boundaries) |
Support novices by highlighting causal phenomena (e.g., negative feedback) | “Reducing machine speed can mitigate problems due to warm chocolate, but it also increases time in buffer, which can cause even more warming” | |
Dealing with complexity | ||
Linear reasoning | Make non-linear interactions and complex system features transparent (e.g., emergence) | Show how problems result from interactions of temperature, speed, machine and chocolate characteristics, and mechanical settings, with none of them being sufficient to cause problems |
Understanding causal behaviors and functions | Connect information about system structures to behaviors and functions | “Physical Form: hollow bottoms; Physical Function: insufficient ground contact on conveyor belt; Generalized Function: dysfunctional transport of chocolate bars; Abstract Function: frequent faults at carrier belt; Functional Purpose: low efficiency” |
Illusion of understanding | Provide information about actual system relations | “Temperature in the molding unit and time in the buffer both lead to warm chocolate, which can cause soiling. However, the two parameters can compensate each other” |
Prompt operators to think about the system in more specific terms | Show slow motion video to show how bars move back and forth between injector and transfer finger, instead of just informing operators that bars break in the injection unit | |
Describe problems on different abstraction levels and allow switching between them | “Physical Form: hollow bottoms; Physical Function: insufficient ground contact on conveyor belt; Generalized Function: dysfunctional transport of chocolate bars; Abstract Function: frequent faults at carrier belt; Functional Purpose: low efficiency” |
References
- Lau, N.; Jamieson, G.A.; Skraaning, G., Jr. Situation awareness in process control: A fresh look. In Proceedings of the 8th American Nuclear Society International Topical Meeting on Nuclear Plant Instrumentation & Control and Human-Machine Interface Technologies (NPIC & HMIT), San Diego, CA, USA, 22–26 July 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Müller, R.; Oehm, L. Process industries versus discrete processing: How system characteristics affect operator tasks. Cogn. Technol. Work 2019, 21, 337–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mumaw, R.J.; Roth, E.M.; Vicente, K.J.; Burns, C.M. There Is more to monitoring a nuclear power plant than meets the eye. Hum. Factors 2000, 42, 36–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- YA-711. Principles for Alarm System Design; Norwegian Petroleum Directorate: Stavanger, Norway, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Borst, C.; Flach, J.M.; Ellerbroek, J. Beyond ecological interface design: Lessons from concerns and misconceptions. IEEE Trans. Hum. Mach. Syst. 2014, 45, 164–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicente, K.J.; Rasmussen, J. Ecological interface design: Theoretical foundations. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. 1992, 22, 589–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rasmussen, J.; Goodstein, L. Decision support in supervisory control of high-risk industrial systems. Automatica 1987, 23, 663–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bennett, K.B. Ecological interface design and system safety: One facet of Rasmussen’s legacy. Appl. Ergon. 2017, 59, 625–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, P.J.; Bennett, K.B.; Stone, R.B. Representation aiding to support performance on problem-solving tasks. Rev. Hum. Factors Ergon. 2006, 2, 74–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Urbas, L.; Obst, M.; Stöss, M. Formal models for high performance HMI engineering. In IFAC Proceedings Volumes; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2012; Volume 45, pp. 854–859. [Google Scholar]
- Monostori, L. Cyber-physical production systems: Roots, expectations and R&D challenges. In Proceedings of the 47th CIRP Conference on Manufacturing Systems, Windsor, ON, Canada, 28–30 April 2014; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Wang, L.; Törngren, M.; Onori, M. Current status and advancement of cyber-physical systems in manufacturing. J. Manuf. Syst. 2015, 37, 517–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sabou, M.; Ekaputra, F.J.; Biffl, S. Semantic web technologies for data integration in multi-disciplinary engineering. In Multi-Disciplinary Engineering for Cyber-Physical Production Systems; Biffl, S., Lüder, A., Gerhard, D., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; pp. 301–329. [Google Scholar]
- Garcia, M.V.; Irisarri, E.; Perez, F.; Estévez, E.; Marcos, M. OPC-UA communications integration using a CPPS architecture. In Proceedings of the 2016 IEEE Ecuador Technical Chapters Meeting (ETCM), Guayaquil, Ecuador, 12–14 October 2016; IEEE: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ali, N.; Hong, J.-E. Failure detection and prevention for cyber-physical systems using ontology-based knowledge base. Computers 2018, 7, 68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sheridan, T.B. Supervisory control. In Handbook of Human Factors/Ergonomics; Salvendy, G., Ed.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1987; pp. 1295–1327. [Google Scholar]
- Parasuraman, R.; Wickens, C.D. Humans: Still vital after all these years of automation. Hum. Factors 2008, 50, 511–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parasuraman, R.; Manzey, D. Complacency and bias in human use of automation: An attentional integration. Hum. Factors 2010, 52, 381–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parasuraman, R.; Riley, V. Humans and automation: Use, misuse, disuse, abuse. Hum. Factors 1997, 39, 230–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woods, D.D.; Branlat, M. Hollnagel’s test: Being ‘in control’ of highly interdependent multi-layered networked systems. Cogn. Technol. Work 2010, 12, 95–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Endsley, M.R. From here to autonomy: Lessons learned from human-automation research. Hum. Factors 2017, 59, 5–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Müller, R. Cognitive challenges of changeability: Adjustment to system changes and transfer of knowledge in modular chemical plants. Cogn. Technol. Work 2019, 21, 113–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Müller, R.; Urbas, L. Cognitive challenges of changeability: Multi-level flexibility for operating a modular chemical plant. Chem. Ing. Tech. 2017, 89, 1409–1420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ruppert, T.; Jaskó, S.; Holczinger, T.; Abonyi, J. Enabling technologies for operator 4.0: A survey. Appl. Sci. 2018, 8, 1650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Romero, D.; Stahre, J.; Wuest, T.; Noran, O.; Bernus, P.; Fast-Berglund, Å.; Gorecky, D. Towards an operator 4.0 typology: A human-centric perspective on the fourth industrial revolution technologies. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Computers and Industrial Engineering (CIE46), Tianjin, China, 29–31 October 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Romero, D.; Stahre, J.; Taisch, M. The operator 4.0: Towards socially sustainable factories of the future. Comput. Ind. Eng. 2020, 139, 106128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fiedler, K. Beware of samples! A cognitive-ecological sampling approach to judgment biases. Psychol. Rev. 2000, 107, 659–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fiedler, K.; Kutzner, F. Information sampling and reasoning biases: Implications for research in judgment and decision making. In The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making; Keren, G., Wu, G., Eds.; Wiley: Chichester, UK, 2015; pp. 380–403. [Google Scholar]
- Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cogn. Psychol. 1973, 5, 207–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheyette, S.; Konstantinidis, E.; Harman, J.; Gonzalez, C. Choice adaptation to increasing and decreasing event probabilities. In Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Austin, TX, USA, 10–13 August 2016; Cognitive Science Society: Seattle, WA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Koehler, J.J.; Mercer, M. Selection neglect in mutual fund advertisements. Manag. Sci. 2009, 55, 1107–1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mussweiler, T.; Strack, F.; Pfeiffer, T. Overcoming the inevitable anchoring effect: Considering the opposite compensates for selective accessibility. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2000, 26, 1142–1150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Platzer, C.; Bröder, A. Most people do not ignore salient invalid cues in memory-based decisions. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2012, 19, 654–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Griffin, D.; Tversky, A. The weighing of evidence and the determinants of confidence. Cogn. Psychol. 1992, 24, 411–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nickerson, R.S. Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 1998, 2, 175–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fischer, P.; Schulz-Hardt, S.; Frey, D. Selective exposure and information quantity: How different information quantities moderate decision makers’ preference for consistent and inconsistent information. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 94, 231–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Klayman, J.; Ha, Y.-W. Confirmation, disconfirmation, and information in hypothesis testing. Psychol. Rev. 1987, 94, 211–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arocha, J.F.; Patel, V.L.; Patel, Y.C. Hypothesis generation and the coordination of theory and evidence in novice diagnostic reasoning. Med. Decis. Mak. 1993, 13, 198–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simons, D.J.; Chabris, C.F. Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception 1999, 28, 1059–1074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manzey, D.; Reichenbach, J.; Onnasch, L. Human performance consequences of automated decision aids. J. Cogn. Eng. Decis. Mak. 2012, 6, 57–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sharps, M.J.; Martin, S.S. “Mindless” decision making as a failure of contextual reasoning. J. Psychol. 2002, 136, 272–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sharps, M.J.; Hess, A.B.; Ranes, B. Mindless decision making and environmental issues: Gestalt/feature-intensive processing and contextual reasoning in environmental decisions. J. Psychol. 2007, 141, 525–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walters, D.J.; Fernbach, P.M.; Fox, C.R.; Sloman, S.A. Known unknowns: A critical determinant of confidence and calibration. Manag. Sci. 2017, 63, 4298–4307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Culnan, M.J. Environmental scanning: The effects of task complexity and source accessibility on information gathering behavior. Decis. Sci. 1983, 14, 194–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Byström, K.; Järvelin, K. Task complexity affects information seeking and use. Inf. Process. Manag. 1995, 31, 191–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eppler, M.J.; Mengis, J. The Concept of Information Overload: A review of literature from organization science, accounting, marketing, MIS, and related disciplines. Inf. Soc. 2004, 20, 325–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chewning, E.G.; Harrell, A.M. The effect of information load on decision makers’ cue utilization levels and decision quality in a financial distress decision task. Account. Organ. Soc. 1990, 15, 527–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sparrow, P. Strategy and cognition: Understanding the role of management knowledge structures, organizational memory and information overload. Creat. Innov. Manag. 1999, 8, 140–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schick, A.G.; Gordon, L.A.; Haka, S. Information overload: A temporal approach. Account. Organ. Soc. 1990, 15, 199–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hahn, M.; Lawson, R.; Lee, Y.G. The effects of time pressure and information load on decision quality. Psychol. Mark. 1992, 9, 365–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keller, K.L.; Staelin, R. Effects of quality and quantity of information on decision effectiveness. J. Consum. Res. 1987, 14, 200–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iyengar, S.S.; Lepper, M.R. When choice is demotivating: Can one desire too much of a good thing? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 79, 995–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Reilly, I.C.A. Variations in decision makers’ use of information sources: The impact of quality and accessibility of information. Acad. Manag. J. 1982, 25, 756–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fidel, R.; Green, M. The many faces of accessibility: Engineers’ perception of information sources. Inf. Process. Manag. 2004, 40, 563–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grove, W.M.; Meehl, P.E. Comparative efficiency of informal (subjective, impressionistic) and formal (mechanical, algorithmic) prediction procedures: The clinical-statistical controversy. Psychol. Public Policy Law 1996, 2, 293–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grove, W.M.; Zald, D.H.; Lebow, B.S.; Snitz, B.E.; Nelson, C. Clinical versus mechanical prediction: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Assess. 2000, 12, 19–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ægisdóttir, S.; White, M.J.; Spengler, P.M.; Maugherman, A.S.; Anderson, L.A.; Cook, R.S.; Nichols, C.N.; Lampropoulos, G.K.; Walker, B.S.; Cohen, G.R.; et al. The meta-analysis of clinical judgment project: Fifty-six years of accumulated research on clinical versus statistical prediction. Couns. Psychol. 2006, 34, 341–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D.; Klein, G.A. Conditions for intuitive expertise: A failure to disagree. Am. Psychol. 2009, 64, 515–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- von Helversen, B.; Rieskamp, J. Models of quantitative estimations: Rule-based and exemplar-based processes compared. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2009, 35, 867–889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Platzer, C.; Bröder, A. When the rule is ruled out: Exemplars and rules in decisions from memory. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2013, 26, 429–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bröder, A.; Newell, B.R.; Platzer, C. Cue integration vs. exemplar-based reasoning in multi-attribute decisions from memory: A matter of cue representation. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2010, 5, 326–338. [Google Scholar]
- Payne, J.W.; Bettman, J.R.; Johnson, E.J. Adaptive strategy selection in decision making. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1988, 14, 534–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gigerenzer, G.; Todd, P.M. ; The ABC Research Group. Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Todd, P.M.; Gigerenzer, G. Ecological Rationality: Intelligence in the World; Oxford University Press (OUP): New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Gigerenzer, G. Towards a rational theory of heuristics. In Minds, Models and Milieux: Commemorating the Centennial of the Birth of Herbert Simon; Frantz, R., Marsh, L., Eds.; Palgrave Macmillan: London, UK, 2016; pp. 34–59. [Google Scholar]
- Timmermans, D. The impact of task complexity on information use in multi-attribute decision making. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 1993, 6, 95–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glöckner, A.; Betsch, T. Decisions beyond boundaries: When more information is processed faster than less. Acta Psychol. 2012, 139, 532–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Platzer, C.; Bröder, A.; Heck, D.W. Deciding with the eye: How the visually manipulated accessibility of information in memory influences decision behavior. Mem. Cogn. 2014, 42, 595–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Glöckner, A.; Betsch, T. Multiple-reason decision making based on automatic processing. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2008, 34, 1055–1075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Söllner, A.; Bröder, A.; Hilbig, B.E. Deliberation versus automaticity in decision making: Which presentation format features facilitate automatic decision making? Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2013, 8, 278–298. [Google Scholar]
- Rieskamp, J.; Hoffrage, U. Inferences under time pressure: How opportunity costs affect strategy selection. Acta Psychol. 2008, 127, 258–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldstone, R.L.; Kersten, A.; Carvalho, P.F. Categorization and concepts. In Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience; Thompson-Schill, S.L., Wixted, J.T., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2018; pp. 1–43. [Google Scholar]
- Spiro, R.J.; Vispoel, W.P.; Schmitz, J.G.; Samarapungavan, A.; Boerger, A.E. Knowledge acquisition for application: Cognitive flexibility and transfer in complex content domains. In Executive Control Processes in Reading; Britton, B.K., Glynn, S.M., Eds.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1987; pp. 177–199. [Google Scholar]
- Krems, J.F. Cognitive flexibility and complex problem solving. In Complex Problem Solving: The European Perspective; Frensch, P.A., Funke, J., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1995; Chapter 8. [Google Scholar]
- Gonzalez, C.; Lerch, J.F.; Lebiere, C. Instance-based learning in dynamic decision making. Cogn. Sci. 2003, 27, 591–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valadao, D.F.; Anderson, B.; Danckert, J. Examining the influence of working memory on updating mental models. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2015, 68, 1442–1456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stöttinger, E.; Filipowicz, A.; Danckert, J.; Anderson, B. The effects of prior learned strategies on updating an opponent’s strategy in the rock, paper, scissors game. Cogn. Sci. 2014, 38, 1482–1492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Speekenbrink, M.; Shanks, D.R. Learning in a changing environment. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2010, 139, 266–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chi, M.T.H. Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and categorical shift. In International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change; Vosniadou, S., Ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 2008; pp. 61–82. [Google Scholar]
- George, D.N.; Kruschke, J.K. Contextual modulation of attention in human category learning. Learn. Behav. 2012, 40, 530–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kruschke, J.K. Category learning. In The Handbook of Cognition; Lamberts, K., Goldstone, R.L., Eds.; Sage: London, UK, 2005; pp. 183–201. [Google Scholar]
- Goldstone, R.L. The role of similarity in categorization: Providing a groundwork. Cognition 1994, 52, 125–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Medin, D.L.; Shoben, E.J. Context and structure in conceptual combination. Cogn. Psychol. 1988, 20, 158–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barsalou, L.W. Ad hoc categories. Mem. Cogn. 1983, 11, 211–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gentner, D.; Rattermann, M.; Forbus, K. The roles of similarity in transfer: Separating retrievability from inferential soundness. Cogn. Psychol. 1993, 25, 524–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Waltz, J.A.; Lau, A.; Grewal, S.K.; Holyoak, K.J. The role of working memory in analogical mapping. Mem. Cogn. 2000, 28, 1205–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chi, M.T.H.; Feltovich, P.J.; Glaser, R. Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cogn. Sci. 1981, 5, 121–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Murphy, G.L.; Medin, D.L. The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychol. Rev. 1985, 92, 289–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garcia-Retamero, R.; Hoffrage, U. How causal knowledge simplifies decision-making. Minds Mach. 2006, 16, 365–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shanks, D.R. Judging covariation and causation. In Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making; Koehler, D., Harvey, N., Eds.; Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2004; pp. 220–239. [Google Scholar]
- Vallée-Tourangeau, F.; A Murphy, R.; Baker, A.G. Contiguity and the outcome density bias in action-outcome contingency judgements. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 2005, 58 Pt B, 177–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maldonado, A.; Jiménez, G.; Herrera, A.; Perales, J.C.; Catena, A. Inattentional blindness for negative relationships in human causal learning. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2006, 59, 457–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dickinson, A. The 28th Bartlett memorial lecture causal learning: An associative analysis. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. B 2001, 54, 3–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Waldmann, M.R. Predictive versus diagnostic causal learning: Evidence from an overshadowing paradigm. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2001, 8, 600–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cobos, P.L.; Lopez, F.J.; Cano, A.; Almaraz, J.; Shanks, D.R. Mechanisms of predictive and diagnostic causal induction. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 2002, 28, 331–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aitken, M.R.; Larkin, M.J.; Dickinson, A. Super-learning of causal judgements. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. B 2000, 53, 59–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fernbach, P.M.; Darlow, A.; Sloman, S.A. Asymmetries in predictive and diagnostic reasoning. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2011, 140, 168–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pearl, J.; Mackenzie, D. The Book of Why: The New Science of Cause and Effect; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Lagnado, D.A.; Sloman, S.A. Time as a guide to cause. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2006, 32, 451–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shanks, D.R.; Pearson, S.M.; Dickinson, A. Temporal contiguity and the judgement of causality by human subjects. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1989, 41, 139–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lagnado, D.A.; Speekenbrink, M. The influence of delays in real-time causal learning. Open Psychol. J. 2010, 3, 184–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Moray, N. Process control. In International Encyclopedia of Ergonomics and Human Factors; Karwowski, W., Ed.; Taylor & Francis: London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2001; Volume 2, pp. 1148–1150. [Google Scholar]
- Buehner, M.J.; May, J. Knowledge mediates the timeframe of covariation assessment in human causal induction. Think. Reason. 2002, 8, 269–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hagmayer, Y.; Waldmann, M.R. How temporal assumptions influence causal judgments. Mem. Cogn. 2002, 30, 1128–1137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Waldmann, M.R. Knowledge-based causal induction. In Causal Learning; Shanks, D.R., Medin, D.L., Holyoak, K.J., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1996; pp. 47–88. [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths, T.L.; Tenenbaum, J.B. Theory-based causal induction. Psychol. Rev. 2009, 116, 661–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Waldmann, M.R.; Hagmayer, Y.; Blaisdell, A.P. Beyond the information given: Causal models in learning and reasoning. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 15, 307–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waldmann, M.R.; Hagmayer, Y. Estimating causal strength: The role of structural knowledge and processing effort. Cognition 2001, 82, 27–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perales, J.C.; Catena, A.; Maldonado, A. Inferring non-observed correlations from causal scenarios: The role of causal knowledge. Learn. Motiv. 2004, 35, 115–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waldmann, M.R. Combining versus analyzing multiple causes: How domain assumptions and task context affect integration rules. Cogn. Sci. 2007, 31, 233–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cartwright, N. Causation: One word, many things. Philos. Sci. 2004, 71, 805–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rottman, B.M.; Gentner, D.; Goldwater, M.B. Causal systems categories: Differences in novice and expert categorization of causal phenomena. Cogn. Sci. 2012, 36, 919–932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacobson, M.J. Problem solving about complex systems: Differences between experts and novices. In Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference of the Learning Sciences, Mahwah, NJ, USA, 14–17 June 2020; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Hmelo-Silver, C.E.; Marathe, S.; Liu, L. Fish swim, rocks sit, and lungs breathe: Expert-novice understanding of complex systems. J. Learn. Sci. 2007, 16, 307–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rozenblit, L.; Keil, F. The misunderstood limits of folk science: An illusion of explanatory depth. Cogn. Sci. 2002, 26, 521–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alter, A.L.; Oppenheimer, D.M.; Zemla, J.C. Missing the trees for the forest: A construal level account of the illusion of explanatory depth. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 99, 436–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lagnado, D.A.; Waldmann, M.R.; Hagmayer, Y.; Sloman, S.A. Beyond covariation: Cues to causal structure. In Causal Learning: Psychology, Philosophy, and Computation; Gopnik, A., Schulz, L., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 154–172. [Google Scholar]
- Wiedau, M.; von Wedel, L.; Temmen, H.; Welke, R.; Papakonstantinou, N. Enpro data integration: Extending DEXPI Towards the asset lifecycle. Chem. Ing. Tech. 2019, 91, 240–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernshausen, J.; Haller, A.; Holm, T.; Hoernicke, M.; Obst, M.; Ladiges, J. Namur modul type package—Definition. ATP Mag. 2016, 58, 72–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagner, C.; Grothoff, J.; Epple, U.; Drath, R.; Malakuti, S.; Gruner, S.; Hoffmeister, M.; Zimermann, P. The role of the Industry 4.0 asset administration shell and the digital twin during the life cycle of a plant. In Proceedings of the 22nd IEEE International Conference on Emerging Technologies and Factory Automation, Limassol, Cyprus, 12–15 September 2017; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Ekaputra, F.J.; Sabou, M.; Serral Asensio, E.; Kiesling, E.; Biffl, S. Ontology-based data integration in multi-disciplinary engineering environments: A review. Open J. Inf. Syst. 2017, 4, 1–26. [Google Scholar]
- Bamberg, A.; Urbas, L.; Bröcker, S.; Kockmann, N.; Bortz, M. What makes the digital twin an ingenious companion? Chem. Ing. Tech. 2020, 92, 192–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pfrommer, J.; Stogl, D.; Aleksandrov, K.; Navarro, S.E.; Hein, B.; Beyerer, J. Plug & produce by modelling skills and service-oriented orchestration of reconfigurable manufacturing systems. Automatisierungstechnik 2015, 63, 790–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wießner, A.; Saxena, A.; Marquardt, W. An ontology-based environment for effective collaborative and concurrent process engineering. In Proceedings of the 2010 IEEE International Conference on Industrial Engineering and Engineering Management, Macau, China, 7–10 December 2010; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Berners-Lee, T.; Hendler, J.; Lassila, O. The semantic web. Sci. Am. 2001, 284, 34–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Machado, L.M.O.; Souza, R.R.; Simões, M.G. Semantic web or web of data? A diachronic study (1999 to 2017) of the publications of Tim Berners-Lee and the World Wide Web Consortium. J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol. 2019, 70, 701–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bizer, C.; Heath, T.; Berners-Lee, T. Linked data: The story so far. In Semantic Services, Interoperability and Web Applications: Emerging Concepts; Sheth, A.P., Ed.; IGI Global: Hershey, PA, USA, 2011; pp. 205–227. [Google Scholar]
- Graube, M.; Pfeffer, J.; Ziegler, J.; Urbas, L. Linked data as integrating technology for industrial data. Int. J. Distrib. Syst. Technol. 2012, 3, 40–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prud’hommeaux, E.; Seaborne, A. SPARQL Query Language for RDF; World Wide Web Consortium: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Panda, S.K.; Blome, A.; Wisniewski, L.; Meyer, A. IoT retrofitting approach for the food industry. In Proceedings of the 24th IEEE International Conference on Emerging Technologies and Factory Automation (ETFA), Zaragoza, Spain, 10–13 September 2019; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Großmann, D.; Banerjee, S.; Schmied, S. Feldgerätedaten mit NOA in Cloud-Applikationen nutzen: Demonstrator auf basis bestehender feldgeräte und FDI. ATP Mag. 2018, 60, 53–58. [Google Scholar]
- Ngu, A.H.H.; Gutierrez, M.; Metsis, V.; Nepal, S.; Sheng, M. IoT middleware: A survey on issues and enabling technologies. IEEE Internet Things J. 2016, 4, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- VDI/VDE. VDI/VDE 2657—Blatt 1; Beuth Verlag GmbH: Berlin, Germany, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- OPC Foundation. Available online: https://opcfoundation.org/ (accessed on 26 March 2021).
- Iatrou, C.P.; Bauer, H.; Graube, M.; Hoppner, S.; Rahm, J.; Urbas, L. Hard real-time capable OPC UA server as hardware peripheral for single chip IoT systems. In Proceedings of the 24th IEEE International Conference on Emerging Technologies and Factory Automation (ETFA), Zaragoza, Spain, 10–13 September 2019; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Finn, N. Introduction to time-sensitive networking. IEEE Commun. Stand. Mag. 2018, 2, 22–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruckner, D.; Stănică, M.P.; Blair, R.; Schriegel, S.; Kehrer, S.; Seewald, M.; Sauter, T. An introduction to OPC UA TSN for industrial communication systems. Proc. IEEE 2019, 107, 1121–1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colombo, A.W.; Bangemann, T.; Karnouskos, S.; Delsing, J.; Stluka, P.; Harrison, R.; Jammes, F.; Lastra, J.L. Industrial Cloud-Based Cyber-Physical Systems; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Schulte, S.; Hoenisch, P.; Hochreiner, C.; Dustdar, S.; Klusch, M.; Schüller, D. Towards process support for cloud manufacturing. In Proceedings of the 18th International Enterprise Distributed Object Computing Conference, Ulm, Germany, 1–2 September 2014; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Cachada, A.; Barbosa, J.; Leitão, P.; Geraldes, C.A.S.; Deusdado, L.; Costa, J.; Teixeira, C.; Teixeira, J.; Moreira, A.H.J.; Miguel, P.; et al. Maintenance 4.0: Intelligent and predictive maintenance system architecture. In Proceedings of the 23rd International Conference on Emerging Technologies and Factory Automation (ETFA), Torino, Italy, 4–7 September 2018; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Shi, W.; Cao, J.; Zhang, Q.; Li, Y.; Xu, L. Edge computing: Vision and challenges. IEEE Internet Things J. 2016, 3, 637–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eifert, T.; Eisen, K.; Maiwald, M.; Herwig, C. Current and future requirements to industrial analytical infrastructure—Part 2: Smart sensors. Anal. Bioanal. Chem. 2020, 412, 2037–2045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Thomesse, J.-P. Fieldbus technology in industrial automation. Proc. IEEE 2005, 93, 1073–1101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cheng, J.; Chen, W.; Tao, F.; Lin, C.-L. Industrial IoT in 5G environment towards smart manufacturing. J. Ind. Inf. Integr. 2018, 10, 10–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klettner, C.; Tauchnitz, T.; Epple, U.; Nothdurft, L.; Diedrich, C.; Schröder, T.; Goßmann, D.; Banerjee, S.; Krauß, M.; Latrou, C.; et al. Namur open architecture. ATP Mag. 2017, 59, 20–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graube, M.; Urbas, L.; Hladik, J. Integrating industrial middleware in Linked Data collaboration networks. In Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on Emerging Technologies and Factory Automation (ETFA), Berlin, Germany, 6–9 September 2016; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Kang, Z.; Catal, C.; Tekinerdogan, B. Machine learning applications in production lines: A systematic literature review. Comput. Ind. Eng. 2020, 149, 106773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martí, L.; Sanchez-Pi, N.; Molina, J.M.; Garcia, A.C.B. Anomaly Detection Based on Sensor Data in Petroleum Industry Applications. Sensors 2015, 15, 2774–2797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Harrou, F.; Sun, Y.; Khadraoui, S. Amalgamation of anomaly-detection indices for enhanced process monitoring. J. Loss Prev. Process. Ind. 2016, 40, 365–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lv, F.; Wen, C.; Bao, Z.; Liu, M. Fault diagnosis based on deep learning. In Proceedings of the 2016 American Control Conference (ACC), Boston, MA, USA, 6–8 July 2016; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- van den Kerkhof, P.; Gins, G.; Vanlaer, J.; van Impe, J.F. Dynamic model-based fault diagnosis for (bio)chemical batch processes. Comput. Chem. Eng. 2012, 40, 12–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kanawaday, A.; Sane, A. Machine learning for predictive maintenance of industrial machines using IoT sensor data. In Proceedings of the 8th IEEE International Conference on Software Engineering and Service Science (ICSESS), Beijing, China, 24–26 November 2017; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Adadi, A.; Berrada, M. Peeking inside the black-box: A Survey on explainable artificial intelligence (XAI). IEEE Access 2018, 6, 52138–52160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guidotti, R.; Monreale, A.; Ruggieri, S.; Turini, F.; Giannotti, F.; Pedreschi, D. A survey of methods for explaining black box models. ACM Comput. Surv. 2019, 51, 1–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Xu, K.; Ba, J.; Kiros, R.; Cho, K.; Courville, A.; Salakhudinov, R.; Zemel, R.; Bengio, Y. Show, attend and tell: Neural image caption generation with visual attention. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Machine Learning, Lille, France, 6–11 July 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Bussone, A.; Stumpf, S.; O’Sullivan, D. The role of explanations on trust and reliance in clinical decision support systems. In Proceedings of International Conference on Healthcare Informatics, Dallas, TX, USA, 21–23 October 2015; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Cramer, H.; Evers, V.; Ramlal, S.; van Someren, M.; Rutledge, L.; Stash, N.; Aroyo, L.; Wielinga, B.J. The effects of transparency on trust in and acceptance of a content-based art recommender. User Model. User Adapt. Interact. 2008, 18, 455–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kulesza, T.; Stumpf, S.; Burnett, M.; Yang, S.; Kwan, I.; Wong, W.-K. Too much, too little, or just right? Ways explanations impact end users’ mental models. In Proceedings of the 2013 IEEE Symposium on Visual Languages and Human Centric Computing, San Jose, CA, USA, 15–19 September 2013; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Ray, A.; Yao, Y.; Kumar, R.; Divakaran, A.; Burachas, G. Can you explain that? Lucid explanations help human-AI collaborative image retrieval. In Proceedings of the AAAI Conference on Human Computation and Crowdsourcing, Stevenson, WA, USA, 28–30 October 2019; AAAI: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- de Mantaras, R.L.; McSherry, D.; Bridge, D.; Leake, D.; Smyth, B.; Craw, S.; Faltings, B.; Maher, M.L.; Cox, M.T.; Forbus, K.D.; et al. Retrieval, reuse, revision and retention in case-based reasoning. Knowl. Eng. Rev. 2005, 20, 215–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McSherry, D. Explanation in recommender systems. Artif. Intell. Rev. 2005, 24, 179–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sørmo, F.; Cassens, J.; Aamodt, A. Explanation in case-based reasoning—Perspectives and goals. Artif. Intell. Rev. 2005, 24, 109–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, H.; Wu, Z. On case-based knowledge sharing in semantic web. In Proceedings of the 15th IEEE International Conference on Tools with Artificial Intelligence, Sacramento, CA, USA, 3–5 November 2003; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Heitmann, B.; Hayes, C. Enabling case-based reasoning on the web of data. In Proceedings of the 18th International Conference on Case-Based Reasoning (ICCBR), Alessandria, Italy, 19–22 July 2010. [Google Scholar]
- McSherry, D.; Hassan, S.; Bustard, D. Conversational case-based reasoning in self-healing and recovery. In Proceedings of the European Conference on Case-Based Reasoning, Trier, Germany, 1–4 September 2008; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Carrick, C.; Yang, Q.; Abi-Zeid, I.; Lamontagne, L. Activating CBR systems through autonomous information gathering. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Case-Based Reasoning, Seeon Monastery, Germany, 27–30 July; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Shadbolt, N.; Smart, P.R. Knowledge elicitation: Methods, tools and techniques. In Evaluation of Human Work; Wilson, J.R., Sharples, S., Eds.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2015; pp. 163–200. [Google Scholar]
- Perrow, C. Normal Accidents: Living With High-Risk Technologies; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Hollnagel, E. Coping with complexity: Past, present and future. Cogn. Technol. Work 2012, 14, 199–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glynn, A.N.; Kashin, K. Front-door versus back-door adjustment with unmeasured confounding: Bias formulas for front-door and hybrid adjustments with application to a job training program. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 2018, 113, 1040–1049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Majschak, J.-P. Grundlagen für Maschinensysteme der Stoffverarbeitung. In Dubbel—Taschenbuch für den Maschinenbau, 24th ed.; Grote, K.-H., Feldhusen, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany; New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Lukoianova, T.; Rubin, V.L.; Vashchilko, T. Veracity roadmap: Is big data objective, truthful and credible? Adv. Classif. Res. Online 2013, 24, 4–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Graube, M.; Hensel, S.; Iatrou, C.; Urbas, L. Information models in OPC UA and their advantages and disadvantages. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Emerging Technologies and Factory Automation (ETFA), Limassol, Cyprus, 12–15 September 2017; Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers (IEEE): New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Onnasch, L.; Wickens, C.D.; Li, H.; Manzey, D. Human performance consequences of stages and levels of automation. Hum. Factors 2014, 56, 476–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bainbridge, L. Ironies of automation. Automatica 1983, 19, 775–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christoffersen, K.; Woods, D.D. How to make automated systems team players. In Advances in Human Performance and Cognitive Engineering Research; Salas, E., Ed.; Emerald Group Publishing Limited: Bingley, UK, 2002; pp. 1–12. [Google Scholar]
- Klein, G.A.; Woods, D.D.; Bradshaw, J.M.; Hoffman, R.R.; Feltovich, P.J. Ten challenges for making automation a “team player” in joint human-agent activity. IEEE Intell. Syst. 2004, 19, 91–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Norman, D.A. The “problem” of automation: Inappropriate feedback and interaction, not “over-automation”. In Human Factors in Hazardous Situations; Broadbent, D.E., Baddeley, A., Reason, J.T., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1990; pp. 585–593. [Google Scholar]
- Hirsch-Kreinsen, H. Wandel von Produktionsarbeit—Industrie 4.0. WSI Mitteilungen 2014, 6, 421–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jackson, P.R.; Wall, T.D. How does operator control enhance performance of advanced manufacturing technology? Ergonomics 1991, 34, 1301–1311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wall, T.D.; Cordery, J.; Clegg, C.W. Empowerment, performance, and operational uncertainty: A theoretical integration. Appl. Psychol. 2002, 51, 146–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dostert, J.; Müller, R. Motivational assistance system design for industrial production: From motivation theories to design strategies. Cogn. Technol. Work 2020, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dekker, S.W.A.; Woods, D.D. MABA-MABA or abracadabra? Progress on human-automation co-ordination. Cogn. Technol. Work 2002, 4, 240–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dougherty, S.E. Partnering People With Deep Learning Systems: Human Cognitive Effects of Explanations. Ph.D. Thesis, Robinson College of Business, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Papenmeier, A.; Englebienne, G.; Seifert, C. How model accuracy and explanation fidelity influence user trust. In Proceedings of the IJCAI 2019 Workshop on Explainable Artificial Intelligence, Macau, China, 11 August 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Mercier, H.; Sperber, D. Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 2011, 34, 57–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Smith, C.L. Control of Batch Processes; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Funke, J. Complex problem solving: A case for complex cognition? Cogn. Process. 2010, 11, 133–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dörner, D.; Funke, J. Complex problem solving: What it is and what it is not. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cialdini, R.B.; Goldstein, N.J. Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2004, 55, 591–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colman, A.M.; Gold, N. Team reasoning: Solving the puzzle of coordination. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 1770–1783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Salmon, P.M.; Stanton, N.; Walker, G.H.; Baber, C.; Jenkins, D.P.; McMaster, R.; Young, M.S. What really is going on? Review of situation awareness models for individuals and teams. Theor. Issues Ergon. Sci. 2008, 9, 297–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vakkari, P. Task complexity, problem structure and information actions. Inf. Process. Manag. 1999, 35, 819–837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shanteau, J. How much information does an expert use? Is it relevant? Acta Psychol. 1992, 81, 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verplanken, B.; Aarts, H.; Van Knippenberg, A. Habit, information acquisition, and the process of making travel mode choices. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 27, 539–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rasmussen, J. Ecological interface design for reliable human-machine systems. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 1999, 9, 203–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCrudden, M.T.; Schraw, G.; Lehman, S.; Poliquin, A. The effect of causal diagrams on text learning. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2007, 32, 367–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corter, J.; Mason, D.; Tversky, B.; Nickerson, J. Identifying causal pathways with and without diagrams. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Boston, MA, USA, 20–23 July 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett, K.B.; Flach, J.M. Visual momentum redux. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2012, 70, 399–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldwater, M.B.; Gentner, D. On the acquisition of abstract knowledge: Structural alignment and explication in learning causal system categories. Cognition 2015, 137, 137–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goldstone, R.L.; Wilensky, U. Promoting transfer by grounding complex systems principles. J. Learn. Sci. 2008, 17, 465–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Cognitive Factors | General Requirements for Operator Support Strategies | Examples |
---|---|---|
Sampling the available information | ||
Conditional sampling | Make it explicit when events may be overrepresented in samples | “Chocolate bars have mostly been checked for hollow bottoms when faults have occurred, which may overestimate the impact of this distortion” |
Availability heuristic | Make information available in an unbiased manner | Provide different data sources (e.g., temperature in end cooler, buffer, and packaging machine) and data types (e.g., temperature, soiling, and motor currents) |
Integrating different information elements | ||
Rule- versus exemplar-based | Support cue abstraction (i.e., make predictive power of cues explicit) | “Hollow bottoms are the strongest predictor of skewed chocolate bars” |
Relying on heuristics | Support operators in assessing the context-dependent suitability of heuristics | “To determine whether there was a problem with a packing claw, it is sufficient to check whether every eighth bar was affected” |
Categorizing objects and events | ||
Environmental contingencies | Highlight changes in relations between interacting parameters | “High temperature is less problematic now, because you have reduced machine speed” |
Conceptual change | Provide factual information to help operators detect and correct false beliefs | “The turning wheel is not responsible for squished chocolate bars” |
Reasoning about causes | ||
Overshadowing and super-learning | Make interactions between causes explicit (e.g., additive, enhancing, suppressing) | “The effects of low temperatures in the molding unit are cancelled out when chocolate bars remain in the buffer for a long time” |
Temporal variability | Provide information about factors affecting the variability of delays | “The delay of molding problems affecting the packaging machine depends on the time that chocolate bars remain in the buffer” |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Müller, R.; Kessler, F.; Humphrey, D.W.; Rahm, J. Data in Context: How Digital Transformation Can Support Human Reasoning in Cyber-Physical Production Systems. Future Internet 2021, 13, 156. https://doi.org/10.3390/fi13060156
Müller R, Kessler F, Humphrey DW, Rahm J. Data in Context: How Digital Transformation Can Support Human Reasoning in Cyber-Physical Production Systems. Future Internet. 2021; 13(6):156. https://doi.org/10.3390/fi13060156
Chicago/Turabian StyleMüller, Romy, Franziska Kessler, David W. Humphrey, and Julian Rahm. 2021. "Data in Context: How Digital Transformation Can Support Human Reasoning in Cyber-Physical Production Systems" Future Internet 13, no. 6: 156. https://doi.org/10.3390/fi13060156
APA StyleMüller, R., Kessler, F., Humphrey, D. W., & Rahm, J. (2021). Data in Context: How Digital Transformation Can Support Human Reasoning in Cyber-Physical Production Systems. Future Internet, 13(6), 156. https://doi.org/10.3390/fi13060156