Next Article in Journal
Workplace Health and Wellbeing in Small and Medium Sized Enterprises (SMEs): A Mixed Methods Evaluation of Provision and Support Uptake
Previous Article in Journal
Exploring the Efficacy of Aboriginal Men’s Socioemotional Healing Programs in Australia: A Scoping Review of Evaluated Programs
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Borderline Personality Symptoms, Body Modification, and Emotional Regulation

1
Department of Psychology, Union College, Schenectady, NY 12308, USA
2
Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2025, 22(1), 89; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph22010089
Submission received: 18 December 2024 / Accepted: 26 December 2024 / Published: 10 January 2025

Abstract

:
Many people with BPD (borderline personality disorder) experience emotional dysregulation and thus engage in NSSI (non-suicidal self-injury), potentially in the pursuit of emotional regulation. However, research is lacking on whether body modifications (piercings, tattoos, etc.) are linked to BPD in a similar way to NSSI. In the current study, we hypothesized (1) that body modifications are associated with BPD symptoms, (2) that emotional regulation and self-expression motivations for body modifications specifically account for variance in BPD symptoms, and (3) that NSSI craving correlates with body modification craving. Participants (N = 199, ages 18–67, located in the USA) were surveyed on BPD symptomatology, NSSI craving, emotional regulation abilities, and the presence of body modifications. The extent of tattooing (number of tattoos and percentage of body surface covered) was not significantly associated with BPD symptomatology, but the number of piercings was. Individuals with higher BPD symptomatology were not more likely to report emotional regulation and self-expression as motivations for obtaining body modifications. NSSI craving scores were significantly positively correlated with body modification craving scores. Body modification may be an alternative method of emotional regulation to NSSI in individuals with BPD, which clinicians may want to consider when treating those with BPD and NSSI.

1. Introduction

About 1.6% of the global population is estimated to meet the diagnostic criteria for borderline personality disorder (BPD) [1]. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5-TR (DSM-5-TR) defines BPD as “a pervasive pattern of instability of interpersonal relationships, self-image, and affects, and marked impulsivity, beginning by early adulthood and present in a variety of contexts” [1]. Diagnostic criteria for BPD include desperate efforts to avoid abandonment, volatile interpersonal relationships, identity disturbance, impulsivity, suicidal ideation and behavior, mood instability, feelings of chronic emptiness, intense anger, paranoid ideation, and non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) [1]. In particular, BPD is a disorder characterized by emotional dysregulation, which may lead individuals to seek any release from overwhelming feelings, even if that release is maladaptive in the long term. Clinicians are advised to look for cutting scars and other evidence of NSSI when evaluating a patient with BPD [2]. Treatments for BPD typically focus on distress tolerance, mindfulness, emotional regulation, and interpersonal skills [2].
The phenomenon of NSSI involves intentional harm to oneself without suicidal motivations. While NSSI occurs in the general population and with other psychiatric disorders (estimated lifetime prevalence = 22.0%) [3], its prevalence is particularly elevated in BPD [4], with prevalence estimates of 65–80% in BPD samples [5,6]. Furthermore, many patients report “cravings” to engage in NSSI, similar to cravings seen in addictions [7]. The notable difference between cravings for substances and NSSI is that NSSI is craved primarily as a response to negative emotions rather than also as a means of eliciting positive emotions [8]. Nonetheless, other research has supported addictive-like features of NSSI [9]. In addition to emotional regulation, motives to engage in NSSI include self-derogation and interpersonal functions [6]. To assess whether NSSI provided temporary relief from negative emotions for those with personality disorders such as BPD, Snir et al. [10] prompted participants with BPD and avoidant personality disorder (APD) at five random times throughout the day to complete an experience-sampling digital diary, in which they described their affective, interpersonal, and behavioral (including NSSI) experiences at the current moment. With both BPD and APD patients, feelings of perceived rejection/isolation increased prior to engaging in NSSI and decreased afterward. Although participants with BPD and APD endorsed using NSSI for relief from unwanted emotions, participants with BPD and APD reported no significant change in general negative affect after engaging in NSSI [10]. This may speak to the function of NSSI across various personality disorders. Furthermore, in individuals who had previously engaged in NSSI and people with BPD, physiological arousal decreased during and after listening to an audio clip on the topic of NSSI [11,12]. Thus, people may engage in NSSI to cope with emotional dysregulation, perceiving that it aids in emotion regulation, whether it succeeds in doing so.
It may be possible that some with BPD might elect to get body modifications (i.e., tattoos or piercings) for a similar emotional regulation purpose. Studies have investigated the link between symptoms of BPD and body modifications in clinical and non-clinical populations. When surveying adolescents in a socio-rehab center, individuals with a greater number of body modifications were more likely to engage in alcohol/drug use, aggressive behavior, and self-destructive behavior [13], similar to BPD symptoms of impulsivity and difficulty controlling anger [1]. Wohlrab et al. [14] listed broad categories that may explain motivations for obtaining body modifications in a non-clinical sample. These categories include beauty/art/fashion, individuality, personal narrative, physical endurance, group affiliations/commitment, resistance, spirituality/cultural tradition, addiction, sexual motivation, and no specific reason. Although not clearly stated, some of these categories may be broadly extrapolated to underlying motives for NSSI and may relate to BPD symptoms. For example, individuality may relate to the concept of self-expression. Since many people with BPD experience an unstable and unclear self-concept, permanently altering the body in a way that reflects one’s uniqueness may combat discomfort from an unclear self-concept. The personal narrative motive may also be considered a form of emotional regulation; ‘reclaiming’ a part of one’s body that was once violated may aid in sustaining a more positive emotional outlook. Those with BPD, however, may experience different motives for body modification than in non-clinical samples, such as emotional regulation. Emotional regulation [15] and body modification [16] were investigated in victims of childhood abuse, many of whom have BPD [15]. Participants who had reported childhood abuse/neglect on a survey were more likely to report body modification, and participants who had endorsed more severe childhood abuse/neglect reported more piercings and tattoos than those with less severe abuse/neglect [16]. Although Ernst et al. [16] did not assess individual motivations for body modification, researchers suggested that coping with prior adversity and expressing autonomy may be a motivation for body modification. Similarly, physical endurance motivation refers to being able to withstand the pain of the body modification process [14], which may be used in a manner similar to NSSI. Lastly, addiction refers to the potential addictive component of getting new body modifications [14], which may relate to proneness to addictive behaviors seen among those with BPD [17] or cravings for engaging in NSSI [8].
Body modifications, NSSI, and emotional regulation may be uniquely interconnected in people with BPD. Blay et al. [4] evaluated BPD patients’ symptoms, symptom severity, NSSI, and body modifications. Approximately 69.8% of BPD patients had at least one tattoo [4], compared to a general population rate of around 27.3% [18]. Furthermore, the number of piercings an individual had was significantly positively correlated with their NSSI scores [4]. Similarly, among participants recruited through Craigslist advertisements, there was a positive correlation between the total number of body modifications a participant had (piercing, tattooing, scarification, etc.) and their degree of BPD symptoms [19]. Finally, some who engage in NSSI obtain tattoos to cover up scars or to prevent further NSSI [20], as they may not want to scar their tattoos.
Body modifications may even be an explicit alternative to NSSI or a protective factor against NSSI. In a study by Claes et al. [21], patients with eating disorders were asked to complete a self-report questionnaire, including assessments of self-injury habits and tattoos and body piercings. A negative correlation was found between body modifications and self-injury, and patients with piercings exhibited less severe eating disorder symptoms [16]. Ernst et al. [16] and Claes et al. [21] suggest that body modifications may serve as a socially acceptable form of altering the body for the sake of regulating aversive emotional states. Among participants subscribing to a tattooing and body modification magazine, those who reported cutting themselves had a greater number of piercings than those who did not cut themselves, were more likely to report being addicted to body modifications, and more frequently reported the expected physical pain as motivation for getting body modifications [22]. However, this study did not evaluate whether there was a linear relationship between the number and surface area of body modifications and the amount or frequency of NSSI. Consistent with the proposal that body modifications may serve as a protective factor against NSSI [21], participants recruited from internet piercing communities, in which 50% of participants reported a history of NSSI, stated that NSSI decreased after obtaining piercings, and 25% reported that their self-harm ceased altogether [23]. Thus, body modifications may replace or correlate with decreased self-injury over time.
The concept of body modifications as a method for emotional regulation and an alternative to NSSI is exemplified in a case study by Anderson and Sansone [24]. The case details a 19-year-old man with depression who used tattooing to regulate undesirable emotional states and to ward off thoughts of self-harm. He reported that the physical pain of tattooing provided a temporary distraction from emotional pain that would ‘take [his] mind off it.’ The young man asserted that scars from cutting would have been considered embarrassing, whereas his tattoos were considered “cool”. He also explained that more intense emotional pain would lead him to choose a more sensitive area for tattoo placement [24]. Thus, it is clear that emotional regulation, NSSI, and body modifications relate; however, it remains unclear whether body modifications are being used as a more adaptive or socially acceptable emotional regulation strategy for those who would otherwise engage in NSSI.
The current study aimed to extend the work of Blay et al. [4]. Rather than recruiting individuals diagnosed with BPD, the current study investigated BPD symptom severity in the general population. Specifically, we hypothesized that (H1) NSSI craving would be significantly associated with body modifications craving scores among participants who had body modifications; (H2) body modifications (e.g., number of tattoos/piercings, percentage of tattoo body coverage, locations of tattoos/piercings) would account for significant variance in BPD symptom scores; and (H3) self-expression and emotional regulation motivations for body modifications would account for significant variance in BPD symptom scores.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants (N = 199) were recruited through the researchers’ departmental subject pool and flyers distributed across college campuses, posted on social media, and posted in local tattoo parlors and hair salons in a mid-sized metropolitan area in the Northeast U.S. to survey a wide variety of individuals. This recruitment strategy was selected to maximize variability within the sample in terms of age and presence/absence of body modifications, which should improve the generalizability of the results. We were specifically targeting individuals who would have higher numbers of body modifications by recruiting from tattoo and piercing parlors in person and via their social media platforms and recruiting in hair salons to balance the sample with individuals who may be less likely to have body modifications. Individuals 18 and older were eligible to participate in the study and were able to enter a raffle for one of three cash prizes (USD 200, USD 100, and USD 50) following participation. The sample size was determined by the time frame available for completion of this study by the internal funding mechanism, attempting to recruit as many participants as possible within that time frame for adequate statistical power. Responses were collected from October 2023 to January 2024. Participants’ M(SD)AGE was 26.87 (10.74) years old, ranging from 18–66. Participants’ demographic information is presented in Table 1 (due to researcher error, demographics were not collected for the first 95 participants. Demographic information is presented for the remaining 104 participants).

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Borderline Symptom List

The Borderline Symptom List (BSL) [25] is a 23-item, Likert-type scale with response options ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very strong). Responses are averaged together, with higher scores reflecting greater BPD symptomatology. Examples of scale items include “My mood rapidly cycled in terms of anxiety, anger, and depression”, “Criticism had a devastating effect on me”, and “I felt disgusted by myself”. The authors reported a Cronbach’s α of 0.97 and strong evidence of convergent and discriminant validity. The Cronbach’s α of the BSL for the current study was 0.95.

2.2.2. Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale

The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) [26] is a 36-item, Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). Responses are averaged after reverse scoring select items, and higher scores indicate increased difficulty with emotional regulation. The scale is comprised of 6 factors, labeled Nonacceptance of Emotional Responses, Difficulties Engaging in Goal-Directed Behavior, Impulse Control Difficulties, Lack of Emotional Awareness, Limited Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies, and Lack of Emotional Clarity. Questions include: “I experience my emotions as overwhelming and out of control”, “When I’m upset, I become angry with myself for feeling that way”, and “When I’m upset, I have difficulty controlling my behaviors”. The authors reported a Cronbach’s α of 0.93, and construct validity was supported in the validation sample. For the current study, the DERS Cronbach’s α was 0.95 for the total scale. Subscale Cronbach’s α’s were as follows: Nonacceptance of Emotional Responses α = 94, Difficulties Engaging in Goal-Directed Behavior α = 0.89, Impulse Control Difficulties α = 0.90, Lack of Emotional Awareness α = 0.63, Limited Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies α = 0.90, and Lack of Emotional Clarity α = 0.84.

2.2.3. Suicidal Behavior and Body Modification

The Suicidal Behavior and Body Damage and Modifications Scale (SBBDM-S) [4] evaluates the lifetime presence of suicidal behavior, NSSI, and body modifications. The scale consists of 10 items, with 1–4 investigating suicidal behavior, 5–8 investigating body modifications, and 9–10 investigating NSSI. Within the body modifications portion, three sub-scores indicated the total number of piercings, the percentage of tattoo body coverage, and the total body modifications score (sum of the previous two subscales). The SBBDM-S was adapted from its original form for the purpose of the current study. This adapted version consists only of the body modifications portion. Some additional questions were added to investigate the number of body modifications an individual has, their locations, and how long it has been since obtaining their most recent body modifications.

2.2.4. Craving Experience Questionnaire

The Craving Experience Questionnaire (CEQ) [27] is a 10-item scale originally designed to assess the urgency of cravings in smokers and nicotine users, with response options on a spectrum from 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely). Example items include “At that time, how strong was the urge to have …?” and “At that time, how vividly did you imagine how your body would feel?” and “At that time, how intrusive were the thoughts?” Crobach’s α was 0.91 for the strength form of the scale and 0.94 for the frequency form of the scale in the validation sample. The CEQ’s validity was supported among smokers and in a modified version assessing alcohol cravings. The current study adapted this scale to investigate cravings for NSSI and body modifications, removing two items asking about sensory stimuli associated with nicotine use. Cronbach’s α for the CEQ in the current study was 0.95 for the NSSI adaptation and 0.90 for the body modifications adaptation.

2.2.5. Motivations for Body Modification Scale (MBMS)

Currently, no measures exist that assess a wide range of motivations for obtaining body modifications, especially pertaining to emotional regulation and self-expression. In a literature review, Wohlrab et al. [14] reported important motivations for body modifications such as beauty/art/fashion, individuality, personal narrative, physical endurance against pain, group affiliations/commitment, social resistance/protest, spirituality/cultural tradition, addiction to body modifications, sexual motivation, and no specific reason. Thus, we developed the MBMS to include items assessing these categories, along with emotional regulation and self-expression motivations for obtaining body modifications. The original MBMS consisted of 17 questions on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Information regarding the development and initial validity of the scale and its factor structure are provided in Appendix A. The final MBMS included 15 total items, comprised of three factors assessing Emotion Regulation (α = 0.89), Expression/Autonomy (α = 0.81), and Social Identity (α = 0.63).

2.3. Procedure

This study was preregistered at https://osf.io/x5wkb/?view_only=f1dae1f78c3a4bf4b2d158fb60d12d1a (accessed on 9 December 2024), and responses were collected via Qualtrics. After consenting to the survey, participants completed questionnaires consisting of the BSL and the modified CEQ-NSSI. Participants were then asked if they had any body modifications (tattoos and piercings excluding first earlobe piercings). Participants who endorsed body modifications were asked to complete the body modification portion of the adapted SBBDM-S, the body modifications adaptation of the CEQ, and the MBMS (designed for this study). After completing the study, all participants were provided with a debriefing statement that further detailed the purpose of the study and contained a link to enter the raffle.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

We planned two linear regression analyses to evaluate (1) if the number of tattoos and piercings participants reported and tattoo coverage (predictor variables, entered simultaneously) accounted for variance in BSL scores (outcome variable); (2) if emotional regulation and self-expression (MBMS Emotion Regulation and Expression/Autonomy subscale scores, predictor variables entered simultaneously) accounted for variance in BSL scores (outcome variable); and (3) if NSSI craving scores significantly positively correlated with body modification craving scores. Correlations and descriptive statistics are also reported. Data and analyses were conducted using SPSS software version 28. Using G*Power version 3.1.9.5, sensitivity analysis showed that for a sample of 199, with a power of 0.90, with three tested predictors of seven total possible predictors, the analysis would have the power to detect an effect size of f2 = 0.073, which represents a small-to-medium effect size, suggesting sufficient sample size for the planned regression analyses in the current study.

3. Results

Under half of our sample (40.5%) reported having tattoos. Of those who reported tattoos, a majority (64.2%) reported four or fewer tattoos. Over half of our sample (55%) did not have piercings. Of those who have piercings, half (50%) of the sample reported having less than seven piercings. Thus, our sample had more tattoos than a large Pew research sample of American participants (15–24%) and more piercings than a national probability sample of 500 adults aged 18–50 (14%) [28,29]. Descriptive information and inter-correlations are provided in Table 2.

3.1. Body Modification and NSSI Craving: H1

Supporting H1, NSSI craving was strongly positively correlated with body modification craving among participants who had body modifications (r = 0.56, p < 0.001). See Table 2.

3.2. Extent of Body Modifications: H2

The model was statistically significant: the number and extent of tattoos and piercings explained a significant proportion of variance in BSL scores, F (3, 59) = 4.04, p = 0.011, R2 = 0.17. In contrast to hypotheses, the number of tattoos an individual had, along with the approximate percentage of tattoo body coverage, did not significantly account for the variance in BSL scores (p = 0.32 and p = 0.80, respectively). Supporting hypotheses, BSL score variance was significantly accounted for by the number of piercings an individual had (p = 0.001), such that each additional piercing was associated with a 0.10 increase in BSL score. See Table 3 for results.

3.3. Motivations for Body Modifications: H3

The model was not statistically significant, F (3, 129) = 1.36, p = 0.26, R2 = 0.03. Emotion Regulation, Expression/Autonomy, and Social Identity scores were not significantly associated with BSL scores (p = 0.16–0.72), contrary to hypotheses. There was some evidence of heteroscedasticity; however, transformations of the variables yielded similar non-significant results. Thus, untransformed data are reported for ease of interpretation. See Table 4 for results.

4. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between BPD symptomatology, body modification, and motivations to acquire additional body modifications. This included assessing whether BPD symptoms were associated with body modifications, whether body modifications are motivated by emotional regulation and self-expression, and whether cravings to engage in NSSI were correlated with cravings to obtain additional body modifications. Blay et al. [4] investigated emotional regulation as a motivator for body modifications in a clinical population with BPD, finding that NSSI correlated with the total number of body modifications obtained by this population. The current study extends these results by looking at a non-clinical population, investigating self-expression motivations for body modifications, and examining the relationship between cravings for NSSI and body modification.
We hypothesized that the extent of body modifications an individual had would be associated with their level of BPD symptomatology, such that an increased amount of body modification would indicate greater BPD psychopathology. This had been found previously [4,18]. Contrary to our initial hypotheses, neither the number of tattoos nor the percentage of body coverage of tattoos was associated with BPD symptoms. However, the number of piercings an individual had did significantly explain variance in BPD symptomatology, as hypothesized. These results are consistent with findings from previous studies, in which individuals who reported engaging in NSSI, often related to BPD, had a greater number of piercings [4,22]. However, the lack of correlation between BPD features and tattoos is partly inconsistent with Blay et al. [4]. This may be due to our non-clinical sample, as Blay et al. collected data directly from an outpatient sample with BPD [4]. Blay and colleagues found significant associations between body modifications and total BPD scores, as measured by the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-5, but a non-significant association was found between body modification and BSL scores [4], which was the measure used in the current study. Thus, it is possible that the BSL is less sensitive to BPD symptoms that are associated with body modification than the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-5. Importantly, Blay et al. found that the number of piercings was significantly associated with emotional dysregulation, whereas the percentage of the body covered by tattoos was significantly associated with sensation-seeking [4]. Similarly, recent research among 762 non-clinical adults found a stronger relationship between piercings and symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder than between tattoos and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms, though both were statistically significant [30].
It is possible that having tattoos may be regarded as more socially acceptable than having piercings, especially when concerning more ‘extreme’ piercings [28]. This is supported by the rise in tattoo popularity, with an online Ipsos poll [31] reporting a rise in tattoos from 21% of the population in 2012 to 30% in 2019. Minimal research has been conducted on societal acceptance of individuals with multiple tattoos versus multiple piercings. However, based on the rise in tattoo acceptance broadly, individuals with and without BPD symptoms may be equally likely to get tattoos due to increased social acceptance, such that they may be less predictive of BPD psychopathology.
We hypothesized that participants with body modifications would be more likely to report emotional regulation and self-expression as motivations for obtaining their body modifications if they were higher in BPD pathology. In contrast to the hypotheses, neither emotional regulation nor self-expression motivations were significantly associated with BPD pathology. Emotional dysregulation is heightened in individuals who engage in NSSI, and NSSI appears to serve as an emotional regulator [32]. If NSSI and body modifications are interconnected among individuals high in BPD symptomatology, then emotional regulation may serve as a motivational factor for obtaining body modifications, similar to its motivating NSSI. Although emotional regulation motivations for body modifications were not significantly associated with BPD psychopathology, as hypothesized, they were significantly positively correlated with NSSI cravings (r = 0.24, p < 0.001), body modification craving (r = 0.49, p < 0.001), and the percent of body coverage by tattoos (r = 0.29, p < 0.001), the number of tattoos (r = 0.30, p < 0.001), and the number of piercings (r = 0.24, p < 0.001), strongly suggesting the interrelationships of these motivations for body modification and both craving for NSSI and further body modifications (see Table 2). Additionally, although self-expression/autonomy motivations for body modifications were not significantly associated with BPD symptoms, as hypothesized, they were significantly positively correlated with body modification cravings (r = 0.5, p < 0.001), and the number of tattoos (r = 0.23, p < 0.05) and piercings (r = 0.21, p < 0.05).
As hypothesized, NSSI craving scores were significantly associated with body modification craving scores. Importantly, both NSSI and body modification cravings were significantly correlated with almost all DERS subscales as well, even though the DERS was not associated with the number and percentage coverage of body modifications. Given that emotional regulation motivations for body modifications were significantly associated with NSSI craving scores, people may use body modifications as an alternative form of emotional regulation that is more socially acceptable than engaging in self-harming behaviors. This is supported by case studies, including the young man who reported using the pain of tattooing as a distraction from NSSI cravings and emotional fluctuations [24]. Similarly, a veteran who met the criteria for multiple psychiatric disorders, including BPD, disclosed that he had engaged in typically observed NSSI behaviors but preferred the pain of tattooing due to its social acceptance. He reported that the tattooing process combatted negative emotional states, tension, and feelings of numbness [33,34]. The connection between NSSI and body modification may, therefore, be driven by emotional regulation for individuals with BPD symptoms, with body modifications being endorsed by some individuals due to their greater social acceptance over other NSSI methods. However, some people with BPD may not view body modification as a means of regulating their emotions, but emotional regulation may be a side effect of body modification. This is likely due to the endorphin release from self-injury: Directly after engaging in NSSI, individuals demonstrated a significant increase in salivary β-endorphin levels [35]. These endorphin releases have not been explicitly studied with regard to body modifications, although Wohlrab et al. [14] and Weiler et al. [36] interpolate that this may be a motivation for body modifications among some individuals.
It is likely that many individuals without significant psychopathology are motivated to obtain additional body modifications as a means of self-expression and autonomy and for social identity reasons, as our scale suggested. Additional research is needed to better understand under what circumstances and for whom differing motivations may guide decisions to obtain body modifications.

4.1. Limitations

Despite the important findings in the current study, there are some notable limitations. First, the study was cross-sectional in nature; our data is therefore only correlational and cannot be used to infer causality. Second, there is also a potential for self-report and selection bias despite efforts to recruit a diverse sample. Third, demographic information was missing for half of the participants due to researcher error. This diminishes the ability to draw conclusions regarding differences between demographic groups, rendering the generalizability of the current findings unknown. Fourth, although the study was advertised in multiple locations to attract a range of respondents, many participants were younger adults, based on the available demographic information, who may have only recently become old enough to legally obtain body modifications without parental consent. Additionally, we did not question participants regarding cultural–historical motivations for body modifications. Two recent reviews of motivations for body modifications do speak in greater detail about cultural, historical, and social motivations for body modifications [36,37]. Furthermore, we did not account for recruitment sources in our survey and are therefore unable to provide a breakdown of how many participants found the study from various recruitment sources and are therefore unable to with confidence confirm heterogeneity in our sample beyond the existing demographic information. Additionally, the emotional regulation and self-expression motivations for body modification scales were developed solely for this study and were not previously validated measures. The Social Identity subscale was brief, consisting of only three items, and internal consistency was inadequate. We only measured NSSI craving using a scale initially designed to measure substance use craving, though some evidence suggests these cravings differ from one another [8]. While the BSL scale had many questions on thoughts of NSSI, there were no subscales examining the incidence of NSSI itself. These omissions, while limitations, were in pursuit of survey brevity, as shorter surveys have been shown to increase survey completion [38]. Finally, we did not collect longitudinal data on the progression of body modifications along with BPD symptoms and NSSI and body modification craving over time.

4.2. Future Directions

Future researchers should survey a larger group of participants with more body modifications so that participants have a wide range of body modification coverage for analyses. Additionally, researchers can determine if age is a factor in the relationship between BPD symptoms and body modification. Body modifications may be a more modern form of coping or self-expression endorsed by younger individuals, whereas older adults may have developed more generationally sanctioned coping skills or other adaptive coping skills over time. This is supported by the finding that the severity of BPD symptoms wanes with age [39]. Future research should, as previously mentioned, examine endorphin release and body modifications. Additionally, future research should survey people with BPD and investigate if their level of concern with social acceptability moderates their preference for either NSSI or body modification. Individuals with BPD who are seeking a more socially acceptable method of inflicting pain on themselves to regulate their emotions may elect to obtain body modifications, which are more likely to be perceived as “cool” rather than living with self-harm scars that may result in stigma [40]. Lastly, longitudinal and experience-sampling methods may better identify the temporal relationships between NSSI craving, body modification craving, and engaging in NSSI and body modification.

4.3. Conclusions and Implications

The implications of this study are important for furthering understanding of BPD and related psychopathology. Clinicians may benefit from assessing whether a patient is using body modification as an NSSI alternative and if it is adaptive or maladaptive. Overall, there are many research gaps involving body modifications and their possible emotion regulation function, considering their recent rise in popularity.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.A. and D.C.W.; methodology, V.A. and D.C.W.; validation, D.C.W.; formal analysis, V.A. and D.C.W.; investigation, V.A.; resources, D.C.W.; data curation, V.A.; writing—original draft preparation, V.A.; writing—review and editing, D.C.W. and N.G.; supervision, D.C.W.; project administration, D.C.W.; funding acquisition, V.A. and D.C.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding, but received internal funding from Union College.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of Union College (protocol code 23019 and 26 September 2023).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

This study was preregistered here: https://osf.io/x5wkb/?view_only=f1dae1f78c3a4bf4b2d158fb60d12d1a (accessed on 9 December 2024), and data will be made publicly available following the publication of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

BPD—Borderline Personality Disorder, APD—Avoidant Personality Disorder, NSSI—Non-Suicidal Self-Injury; DSM-5-TR—Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5-TR; BSL—Borderline Symptom List; DERS—The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; SBBDM-S—Suicidal Behavior and Body Damage and Modifications Scale; CEQ—Craving Experience Questionnaire; MBMS—Motivations for Body Modification Scale (MBMS).

Appendix A

Initial validation of the Motivations for Body Modifications Scale (MBMS)
Data analysis
Given the lack of a validated measure of motivations for body modifications (BMs) that included emotional regulation and self-expression motivations along with a range of other potential motivations for seeking BMs, we developed a scale assessing broad-ranging motivations for BMs, the Motivations for Body Modification Scale (MBMS). To provide initial information regarding the validity of the scale, we conducted exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to determine the factor structure of the scale and select a final item pool. Prior to conducting EFA, multivariate outliers were identified and removed from the analysis, and items were analyzed for skewness and kurtosis, with transformations applied to any non-normal items [41]. Items were considered acceptable with skewness statistics < |2| and kurtosis < |7| [42]. An EFA with principal axis factoring and oblique Promax rotation was used to examine the factor structure of the initial MBMS item pool. An oblique Promax rotation was selected because it is expected that underlying factors would be correlated [33]. Bartlett’s test of sphericity and the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy were used to assess the factorability of the items in the initial MBMS item pool. Items are considered to be appropriate for factor analysis when Bartlett’s test of sphericity is statistically significant, and the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy is ≥0.60 [41].
The number of factors selected to be retained in the MBMS was determined using both theoretical and statistical guidance. Statistical guidance included parallel analysis, visual examination of the scree plot, and the Kaiser–Guttman criterion of retaining factors with Eigenvalues > 1.0 [41,43,44,45], which provided a maximum number of acceptable factors. Theoretical guidance was used to reduce factors, if necessary, such that factor structures aligned with hypothesized constructs and resulted in meaningful factors in which items were conceptually related.
Item analysis was conducted to select a final item pool that best fits the factors. Tabachnick and Fidell [41] recommend that items should correlate with one another with an r ≥ 0.30, so initial items were removed in which >50% of the inter-item correlation was <0.30. Items with low primary factor loadings, defined as <0.40 or with a secondary factor loading of 0.30 or above, were also removed [41,46]. Items with correlations of >0.80 were considered to measure an overlapping facet of MBMS, so these items were also removed to yield a final, more parsimonious item set.
Additionally, we examined Pearson’s bivariate correlations between the MBMS and other measures used in the current study, including the DERS, BSL, NSSI, and BM craving scales, and provided norms for the measure.
Results
Exploratory Factor Analysis
No cases were identified as multivariate outliers, and all items were considered acceptable with regard to normality as assessed by skewness and kurtosis statistics [42]. Inter-item correlation matrix examination showed low inter-item correlations between Items 15 “I obtained body modifications for the sake of identifying with a group”, 16 “My body modifications are a form of protest against others (i.e., family or society)”, 17 “My body modifications are part of a cultural tradition or an expression of spirituality”, and 19 “I made an impulsive decision to obtain body modification with no specific reasoning”. However, these items were retained prior to EFA for theoretical reasons, as their lack of correlation with the other motivations was purposeful in assessing differing motivations that were identified in the literature [14]. No items had inter-item correlations > 0.80. Examination of the initial scree plot and parallel analysis both suggested that a maximum of four factors was optimal for the MBMS, and four factors had Eigenvalues > 1.0.
An EFA with principal axis factoring and oblique Promax rotation was constrained to four factors based on the scree plot and parallel analysis and accounted for 63.39% of the variance in the items. Bartlett’s test of sphericity and the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy supported the factorability of the items in the initial MBMS item pool. The initial item solution suggested that the fourth factor did not add to model fit, as only two items (Items 7 and 8) loaded negatively onto this factor (−0.417, −0.518, respectively), which loaded positively on factor 1. There was also significant cross-loading for Item 12 (“Body modification has allowed me to express my individuality.”) in the four-factor solution. Additionally, the fourth factor accounted for a relatively small proportion of variance in the total factor solution (6.98%), so the fourth factor was removed from a subsequent EFA. Lastly, Item 19, “I made an impulsive decision to obtain body modification with no specific reasoning”, loaded poorly across all factors, so it was removed prior to re-running the EFA.
The second EFA was constrained to three factors, with Items 1–18 included. This solution accounted for a total of 58.51% of the variance in the items. The three-factor solution, including Items 1–18, included one item with significant cross-loadings (Item 9: “Seeking relief from unwanted emotions has played a role in my decision to obtain body modifications”). Item 8, “My body modifications are a reflection of my thoughts and feelings”, did not reach the 0.40 threshold on any factor, so it was removed. Lastly, Item 10, “Giving myself an outlet for emotional expression has played a role in my decision for body modifications”, did not load on the Expression/Autonomy factor, as expected, so this item was also removed, and the three-factor solution was re-run without Items 8, 9, 10, and 19.
The third solution appropriately accounted for all items across three factors, accounting for 62.46% of the variance in the items. The final solution is presented in Table A1, including the rotated factor pattern matrix, with Eigenvalues and percent variance explained for each factor. Interpretation of the factors suggests that Factor 1 assesses Emotion Regulation with seven items, such as “While/after obtaining a body modification, I experienced a decrease in negative thoughts”. Factor 2 assesses motivations related to Expression/Autonomy, with seven items, such as “While/after obtaining a body modification, I experienced a decrease in negative thoughts”. Factor 3 assesses Social Identity motivations, consisting of three items, such as “I obtained body modifications for the sake of identifying with a group”.
Norms and Correlations
Internal consistency was adequate for the first two factors but was somewhat low for the third factor, likely because it contains only three items, so it may lack sufficient stability. Future research should determine whether the addition of other Social Identity motivation items may improve the stability of this factor. Correlations between the three subscales of the MBMS are presented in Table 2. The Emotion Regulation subscale was significantly correlated with both the Expression/Autonomy subscale (r = 0.58) and the Social Identity subscale (r = 0.23) of the MBMS, along with the NSSI and Body Modification Craving Scales (rs = 0.24, 0.49, respectively). The Expression/Autonomy subscale was also significantly associated with the Social Identity subscale (r = 0.22) as well as the Goals subscale of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS, r = 0.21) and the body modification craving scale (r = 0.55). Norms for the MBMS subscales are presented in Table 2. Overall, the MBMS appears to be a reliable and valid assessment of motivations to engage in body modification. We recommend that authors do not combine the subscales into a total scale, given that the subscale assesses varied motivations for obtaining body modifications, such that it lacks theoretical utility.
Table A1. Rotated Factor Pattern Matrix, with Eigenvalues and Percent Variance Explained for the MBMS Subscales for EFA.
Table A1. Rotated Factor Pattern Matrix, with Eigenvalues and Percent Variance Explained for the MBMS Subscales for EFA.
Original Item NumberItemFactor 1:
Emotion Regulation
Factor 2:
Expression/Autonomy
Factor 3:
Social Identity
1While/After obtaining a body modification, my mood improved.0.500.23−0.14
2While/After obtaining a body modification, I experienced a decrease in negative thoughts.0.670.15−0.08
3While/After obtaining a body modification, I felt less emotionally tense.0.76−0.12−0.001
4The experience of body modification has helped me to feel more in control of my own emotions.0.81−0.030.06
5The experience of body modification has allowed me to take my mind off of unwanted thoughts and feelings.0.93−0.190.03
6The experience of body modification has provided me with relief from built-up emotional pressure.0.92−0.120.008
7Body modification has allowed me to express my internal feelings in an external form.0.610.180.043
11I obtained body modifications for their artistic appeal.−0.220.660.03
12Body modification has allowed me to express my individuality.−0.1030.870.026
13Obtaining body modifications has provided me with a sense of ’reclaiming’ my body.0.390.490.092
14Body modification enables me to affirm my autonomy.0.130.690.009
18The more body modifications I obtain, the stronger my desire to obtain more.0.160.54−0.12
15I obtained body modifications for the sake of identifying with a group. −0.14−0.020.64
16My body modifications are a form of protest against others (i.e., family or society).0.050.110.63
17My body modifications are a part of a cultural tradition or an expression of spirituality.0.12−0.0650.52
Eigenvalues6.061.711.60
Percent variance explained40.4011.3710.70
Note: Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring; Rotation Method: Promax with Kaiser Normalization. The rotation converged in eight iterations. Items that load on each factor are bolded for clarity.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Leichsenring, F.; Fonagy, P.; Heim, N.; Kernberg, O.F.; Leweke, F.; Luyten, P.; Salzer, S.; Spitzer, C.; Steinert, C. Borderline personality disorder: A comprehensive review of diagnosis and clinical presentation, etiology, treatment, and current controversies. World Psychiatry 2024, 23, 4–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Xiao, Q.; Song, X.; Huang, L.; Hou, D.; Huang, X. Global prevalence and characteristics of non-suicidal self-injury between 2010 and 2021 among a non-clinical sample of adolescents: A meta-analysis. Front. Psychiatry 2022, 13, 912441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Blay, M.; Hasler, R.; Nicastro, R.; Pham, E.; Weibel, S.; Debbané, M.; Perroud, N. Body modifications in borderline personality disorder patients: Prevalence rates, link with non-suicidal self-injury, and related psychopathology. Borderline Personal. Disord. Emot. Dysregulation 2023, 10, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Clarkin, J.F.; Widiger, T.A.; Frances, A.; Hurt, S.W.; Gilmore, M. Prototypic typology and the borderline personality disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1983, 92, 263–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Soloff, P.; Lis, J.; Kelly, T.; Cornelius, J.; Ulrich, R. Self-mutilation and suicidal behavior in borderline personality disorder. J. Personal. Disord. 1994, 8, 257–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Pritchard, T.R.; Fedchenko, C.A.; Lewis, S.P. Self-injury is my drug: The functions of describing nonsuicidal self-injury as an addiction. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2021, 209, 628–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Victor, S.E.; Glenn, C.R.; Klonsky, E.D. Is non-suicidal self-injury an “addiction”? A comparison of craving in substance use and non-suicidal self-injury. Psychiatry Res. 2012, 197, 73–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Himelein-Wachowiak, M.; Giorgi, S.; Kwarteng, A.; Schriefer, D.; Smitterberg, C.; Yadeta, K.; Bragard, E.; Devoto, A.; Ungar, L.; Curtis, B. Getting “clean” from nonsuicidal self-injury: Experiences of addiction on the subreddit r/selfharm. J. Behav. Addict. 2022, 11, 128–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Snir, A.; Rafaeli, E.; Gadassi, R.; Berenson, K.R.; Downey, G. Explicit and inferred motives for nonsuicidal self-injurious acts and urges in borderline and avoidant personality disorders. Personal. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 2015, 6, 267–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Brain, K.L.; Haines, J.; Williams, C.L. The psychophysiology of self-mutilation: Evidence of tension reduction. Arch. Suicide Res. 1998, 4, 227–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Haines, J.; Williams, C.L.; Brain, K.L.; Wilson, G.V. The psychophysiology of self-mutilation. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1995, 104, 471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Maksimova, N.; Nikolaev, E.; Petunova, S.; Zakharova, A.; Lazareva, E.; Dulina, G.; Grigorieva, N.; Baranova, E.; Velieva, S. Body modification and self-destructive behavior in adolescents: Psychological manifestations; Conference presentation. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Pedagogy, Communication and Sociology ICPCS 2019, Ningbo, China, 25–26 May 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Wohlrab, S.; Stahl, J.; Kappeler, P.M. Modifying the body: Motivations for getting tattooed and pierced. Body Image 2007, 4, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Bozzatello, P.; Rocca, P.; Baldassarri, L.; Bosia, M.; Bellino, S. The role of trauma in Early onset Borderline Personality Disorder: A Biopsychosocial perspective. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 721361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Ernst, M.; Borkenhagen, A.; Fegert, J.M.; Brähler, E.; Plener, P.L. The association of childhood abuse and neglect with tattoos and piercings in the population: Evidence from a representative community survey. BMC Psychol. 2022, 10, 105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Kienast, T.; Stoffers, J.; Bermpohl, F.; Lieb, K. Borderline personality disorder and comorbid addiction: Epidemiology and treatment. Dtsch. Ärzteblatt Int. 2014, 111, 280–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kluger, N. An update on cutaneous complications of permanent tattooing. Expert Rev. Clin. Immunol. 2019, 15, 1135–1143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Vizgaitis, A.L.; Lenzenweger, M.F. Pierced identities: Body modification, borderline personality features, identity, and self-concept disturbances. Personal. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 2018, 10, 154–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Oscherwitz, M.E.; Godinich, B.M.; Patel, R.H.; Avila, C.; Neman, S.; Saberi, S.A.; Mariencheck, M.C.; Jorizzo, J.L.; Pichardo, R.; Taylor, S.; et al. Self-inflicted lesions in dermatology: The scars of self-harm. JAAD Rev. 2024, 1, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Claes, L.; Vandereycken, W.; Vertommen, H. Self-care versus self-harm: Piercing, tattooing, and self-injuring in eating disorders. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2004, 13, 11–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Stirn, A.; Hinz, A. Tattoos, body piercings, and self-injury: Is there a connection? investigations on a core group of participants practicing body modification. Psychother. Res. 2008, 18, 326–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Wessel, A.; Kasten, E. Body Piercing and Self-Mutilation: A multifaceted relationship. Am. J. Appl. Psychol. 2014, 3, 104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Anderson, M.; Sansone, R.A. Tattooing as a means of acute affect regulation. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2003, 10, 316–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Bohus, M.; Kleindienst, N.; Limberger, M.F.; Stieglitz, R.; Domsalla, M.; Chapman, A.L.; Steil, R.; Philipsen, A.; Wolf, M. The short version of the Borderline Symptom List (BSL-23): Development and initial data on psychometric properties. Psychopathology 2008, 42, 32–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Gratz, K.L.; Roemer, L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, fac-tor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2004, 26, 41–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. May, J.; Andrade, J.; Kavanagh, D.J.; Feeney, G.F.; Gullo, M.J.; Statham, D.J.; Skorka-Brown, J.; Connolly, J.M.; Cassimatis, M.; Young, R.M.; et al. The Craving Experience Questionnaire: A brief, theory-based measure of consummatory desire and craving. Addiction 2014, 109, 728–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Pew Research Center. How Many Americans Have Tattoos, Why, and Do They Regret It? Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/short-reads/2023/08/15/32-of-americans-have-a-tattoo-including-22-who-have-more-than-one/ (accessed on 18 August 2023).
  29. Laumann, A.E.; Derick, A.J. Tattoos and body piercings in the United States: A national data set. J. Am. Acad. Dermatol. 2006, 55, 413–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Glans, M.R.; Nilsson, J.; Bejerot, S. Tattoos, piercings, and symptoms of ADHD in non-clinical adults: A cross-sectional study. Front. Psychiatry 2024, 14, 1224811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Jackson, C. More Americans Have Tattoos Today than Seven Years Ago. Ipsos. Available online: https://www.ipsos.com/en-us/news-polls/more-americans-have-tattoos-today (accessed on 29 August 2019).
  32. Andover, M.S.; Morris, B.W. Expanding and clarifying the role of emotion regulation in Nonsuicidal Self-Injury. Can. J. Psychiatry 2014, 59, 569–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Mann, A.J.; Van Voorhees, E.E.; Patel, T.A.; Wilson, S.M.; Gratz, K.L.; Calhoun, P.S.; Beckham, J.C.; Kimbrel, N.A. Nail-biting, scab-picking, and tattooing as nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI): A deviant case series analysis of the proposed NSSI disorder diagnostic criteria. J. Clin. Psychol. 2020, 76, 2296–2313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Mann, A.J.; Tull, M.T.; Gratz, K.L. Hiding in the open: Consideration of nonsuicidal self-injury by proxy as a clin-ically meaningful construct. Suicide Life-Threat. Behav. 2022, 52, 1024–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Störkel, L.M.; Karabatsiakis, A.; Hepp, J.; Kolassa, I.; Schmahl, C.; Niedtfeld, I. Salivary beta-endorphin in nonsuicidal self-injury: An ambulatory assessment study. Neuropsychopharmacology 2021, 46, 1357–1363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Weiler, S.M.; Mühlenbeck, C.; Jacobsen, T. Body alteration: On the mental function of body modification and body decoration. Cult. Psychol. 2024, 1354067X241242414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Owens, R.; Filoromo, S.J.; Landgraf, L.A.; Lynn, C.D.; Smetana, M.R.A. Deviance as an historical artefact: A scoping review of psychological studies of body modification. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2023, 10, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Galesic, M.; Bosnjak, M. Effects of questionnaire length on participation and indicators of response quality in a web survey. Public Opin. Q. 2009, 73, 349–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Biskin, R. The lifetime course of borderline personality disorder. Can. J. Psychiatry 2015, 60, 303–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Kleindienst, N.; Priebe, K.; Borgmann, E.; Cornelisse, S.; Krüger, A.; Ebner-Priemer, U.W.; Dyer, A. Body self-evaluation and physical scars in patients with borderline personality disorder: An observational study. Borderline Personal. Disord. Emot. Dysregulation 2014, 1, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S. Using Multivariate Statistics; Pearson: London, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  42. Hair, J.; Black, W.C.; Babin, B.J.; Anderson, R.E. Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th ed.; Pearson Educational International: London, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  43. Guttman, L. Some necessary conditions for common factor analysis. Psychometrika 1954, 19, 149–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Kaiser, H.F. The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1960, 20, 141–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Watkins, M.W. Determining parallel analysis criteria. J. Mod. Appl. Stat. Methods 2006, 5, 344–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Schaefer, L.M.; Harriger, J.A.; Heinberg, L.J.; Soderboerg, T.; Thompson, J.K. Development and Validation of the Sociocultural Attitudes To-wards Appearance Questionnaire-4-Revised (SATAQ-4R). Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2017, 50, 104–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Demographic Information on Gender, Race, Ethnicity, and Household Income of Participants.
Table 1. Demographic Information on Gender, Race, Ethnicity, and Household Income of Participants.
CategorySubcategoryPercent
GenderCisgender women35.0%
Cisgender men12.5%
Transgender woman0.0%
Transgender man0.0%
Nonbinary3.5%
Prefer not to say1.0%
Data not collected48.0%
RaceWhite or Caucasian40.0%
Black or African American2.0%
Native American0.5%
Asian2.5%
Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander0.0%
Mixed Race4.5%
Other1.5%
Prefer not to say3.0%
Data not collected46.0%
EthnicitySpanish/Latino/Hispanic origin4.5%
Not of Spanish/Latino/Hispanic origin46.5%
Prefer not to say1.0%
Data not collected48%
Household IncomeLess than USD 25,0006.0%
USD 25,000–USD 49,9996.0%
USD 50,000–USD 74,9997.0%
USD 75,000–USD 99,9995.0%
USD 100,000–USD 149,9996.5%
USD 150,000 or more9%
Unsure8.5%
Prefer not to say4.0%
Data not collected48.0%
Table 2. Correlation Matrix for the Motivation for Body Modification Scale (MBMS) and Related Measures.
Table 2. Correlation Matrix for the Motivation for Body Modification Scale (MBMS) and Related Measures.
12345678910111213141516
1. MBMS—Emotional Regulation1.0
2. MBMS—Expression/Autonomy0.58 ***1.0
3. MBMS—Social Identity0.23 **0.22 *1.0
4. Borderline Symptom List0.170.150.061.0
5. DERS Nonacceptance0.100.07−0.020.65 ***1.0
6. DERS Goal-directed Beh.0.110.21 *−0.130.59 ***0.53 ***1.0
7. DERS Impulse control0.160.110.030.65 ***0.64 ***0.56 ***1.0
8. DERS Awareness0.01−0.110.090.27 ***0.39 ***0.080.19 **1.0
9. DERS Strategies0.100.04−0.040.76 ***0.70 ***0.62 ***0.74 ***0.27 ***1.0
10 DERS Clarity0.07−0.020.000.50 ***0.55 ***0.32 ***0.44 ***0.64 ***0.54 ***1.0
11. DERS Global0.130.07−0.020.77 ***0.86 ***0.70 ***0.81 ***0.51 ***0.89 ***0.73 ***1.0
12. NSSI Craving0.24 **0.15−0.090.65 ***0.52 ***0.37 ***0.51 ***0.21 **0.55 ***0.36 ***0.57 **1.0
13. Body Modification Craving0.49 **0.55 **0.030.44 **0.30 **0.28 **0.36 **0.080.31 **0.23 **0.35 **0.56 **1.0
14. Percent Tattoos0.29 **0.190.37 **−0.10−0.15−0.16−0.0030.006−0.01−0.13−0.090.070.23 *1.0
15. Number Tattoos030 **0.23 *0.41 **−0.07−0.14−0.15−0.050.01−0.05−0.120.100.23 *0.23 *0.81 **1.0
16. Number Piercings0.26 **0.21 *−0.070.20 *0.100.060.090.140.100.160.150.20 *0.30 **0.030.201.0
M3.193.74 2.03 1.93 2.31 3.101.992.632.312.352.431.332.741.264.773.44
SD0.900.880.940.731.070.980.880.690.910.820.692.132.260.496.032.36
N1261261261991991991991991991991991991268181110
Note. MBMS = Motivations for Body Modification; DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; Nonacceptance = Nonacceptance of emotional responses; Goal-directed Beh. = Difficulty engaging in goal-directed behavior; Impulse = impulse control difficulties; Awareness = lack of emotional awareness; Strategies = Limited access to emotion regulation strategies; Clarity = Lack of emotional clarity; NSSI = Non-suicidal self-injury; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Multiple Linear Regression of Extent of Body Modification Variables on Borderline Symptom List Scores.
Table 3. Multiple Linear Regression of Extent of Body Modification Variables on Borderline Symptom List Scores.
VariablesBSEβpPart r
Number of tattoos−0.030.03−0.200.32−0.12
Percentage of tattoo body coverage 0.070.280.050.800.03
Number of piercings **0.100.030.420.0010.40
Note: Variables were analyzed in a multiple linear regression with borderline symptom list scores as the dependent variable. ** p < 0.01
Table 4. Linear Regression Analyses of Body Modification Motivations on Borderline Symptom List Scores.
Table 4. Linear Regression Analyses of Body Modification Motivations on Borderline Symptom List Scores.
Dependent VariableIndependent VariableBSEβpPart r
BSL scoreEmotion Regulation Motivation0.100.0930.120.290.10
Expression/Autonomy Motivation0.070.010.080.720.064
Social Identity Motivation0.010.070.020.160.014
Note. BSL = Borderline Symptom List.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Avon, V.; Gullo, N.; Walker, D.C. Borderline Personality Symptoms, Body Modification, and Emotional Regulation. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2025, 22, 89. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph22010089

AMA Style

Avon V, Gullo N, Walker DC. Borderline Personality Symptoms, Body Modification, and Emotional Regulation. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2025; 22(1):89. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph22010089

Chicago/Turabian Style

Avon, Victoria, Nathalie Gullo, and D. Catherine Walker. 2025. "Borderline Personality Symptoms, Body Modification, and Emotional Regulation" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 22, no. 1: 89. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph22010089

APA Style

Avon, V., Gullo, N., & Walker, D. C. (2025). Borderline Personality Symptoms, Body Modification, and Emotional Regulation. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 22(1), 89. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph22010089

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop