Next Article in Journal
COVID-19 Mortality in the Colorado Center for Personalized Medicine Biobank
Previous Article in Journal
Social Determinants and Prevalence of Antenatal Depression among Women in Rural Bangladesh: A Cross-Sectional Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Development and Validation of the Antisocial Preferences Scale

by
Bartłomiej Skowroński
Institute of Social Prevention and Resocialization, Faculty of Applied Social Sciences and Resocialization, University of Warsaw, ul. Podchorążych 20, 00-721 Warsaw, Poland
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20(3), 2366; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20032366
Submission received: 16 December 2022 / Revised: 25 January 2023 / Accepted: 26 January 2023 / Published: 29 January 2023

Abstract

:
The aim of this study was to create a new instrument measuring antisocial preferences based on the Theory of Social Derailment of Czesław Czapów, who indicated the role of antisocial preferences in predicting antisocial behavior. The measures used were the Antisocial Preferences Scale (APS), BPAQ, Mach-IV, and IVE questionnaires. The participants were 718 prisoners. CFA techniques were used to investigate the construct validity of the Antisocial Preferences Scale. Four alternative models of the APS were specified and tested, namely: one-factor, second-order, multi-factor, and bi-factor. CFA analyses revealed that the best-fitting model was the bifactor. This conceptualization contains seven subscales, namely: aggressiveness, lack of guilt or remorse, breaking legal norms, incapacity for mutually intimate relationships, impulsiveness, risk-taking, and egocentrism.

1. Introduction

1.1. Antisocial Behavior

The term “antisocial behavior” is used as “a description for all behaviors, attitudes, and personality traits that people engage in that appear to be dysfunctional, in that they often have negative interpersonal and societal outcomes” [1]. Such behavior is not only of interest to social sciences, public policy, and public services but also poses a conceptual challenge [2]. As noted by dos Santos et al. [3], this type of behavior is variously defined and known under various terms: juvenile delinquency [4], antisocial personality disorder [5], conduct problems [6], disruptive behavior [7], and psychopathy [8,9,10].
Dos Santos et al. [3] observe that “despite the theoretical and methodological advances observed in the last few years, there is no consensus on the nature and dimensionality of antisocial behavior”. They explain: “Although these constructs may appear to be synonymous, most research in the area was carried out from a variety of conceptual perspectives and different approaches where the definition and the identification of the target participants were based on different criteria and methodological procedures, which makes the integration of the results problematic and suggests that the constructs are different” [3].
However, at least two perspectives can be highlighted. The first perspective is juridical, with the antisocial label applied to individuals who break the law [11]. Some authors give examples of antisocial behaviors, such as: shoplifting, bicycle theft, breaking into a house, using threats or force, bullying, fighting, and carrying a weapon [12,13,14,15].
The other perspective is related to classification systems such as the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.
There are different approaches to the assessment of antisocial behavior, too. The first approach is associated with personality measurement (interviews, rating scales, self-report tests/personality inventories, e.g., the Jesness Inventory). Another one is the behavioral checklist, an example of which is the Psychopathy Checklist—Revised [16]. Finally, the third approach is DSM-oriented measures, an example of which is the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA) [17], or the categorical classification. Focusing largely on the behavioral features while ignoring the underlying psychopathology, this approach has arbitrary cut-offs to classify the disorder. Moreover, some of the information obtained through a personality profile is lost in a categorical system [18].
A better understanding of what antisociality is and the development of new measures of antisociality have significant implications for prevention and intervention efforts.

1.2. Affective Component of Attitude

The classic model of attitude offered by Rosenberg and Hovland, known more widely as the multicomponent model, posits that attitude consists of affective, cognitive, and behavioral components [19,20]. Empirical studies have demonstrated that both the cognitive and affective components of attitude are associated with the overall attitude evaluation [21,22]. The components pointed out by Rosenberg and Hovland are also included in newer concepts [23]. Ajzen and Fishbein [24] recently postulated that, when measuring attitudes within the framework offered by the theory of reasoned action and the theory of planned behavior, one should take both cognitive and affective components into account. Attitude researchers exploring attitude–behavior relations are increasingly interested in the affective component of attitude [25,26,27]. A few recent studies have shown that in the case of some behaviors, affective attitudes are a stronger predictor of intentions and actions than cognitive attitudes [28].
There is no consensus on the definition of the affective/emotional component of attitude. Conner and colleagues [27] pointed out that affective attitude was conceptualized in different ways. According to these authors, the measures used in the research were “usually labeled anticipated affective reactions (AAR)” [27]. Moreover, affective assessment can be dichotomized in three ways: (1) affect that is expected to follow after the performance or nonperformance of a behavior vs. affect that is expected to occur while the behavior is being performed; (2) anticipated affective reactions, focused on self-conscious emotions (e.g., guilt), vs. affective attitudes, focused on hedonic emotions (e.g., excitement); (3) AAR have tended to examine the negative affect associated with the nonperformance of a behavior rather than the positive affect associated with the performance of a behavior [27]. Other authors, Breckler and Wiggins [29], define the affective aspect of attitude as the emotions and drives that are engendered by a specific attitude object.
A Polish scholar Czesław Czapów was probably one of the first researchers to indicate the role of emotional components in explaining antisocial behavior based on his concept of attitude being part of his Social Derailment Theory. Czapów believed that attitude was a mental system consisting of an external function and an internal one. According to him, activities are the external function, while motivation is the internal function [30]. Motivation comprises perceptive and preferential dispositions. The former is related to the cognitive belief system, while the latter is related to the emotional system [30]. Moreover, Czapów postulated that the affective component of attitudes was related to preferential dispositions (“I like seeing people fear me”) as opposed to cognitive dispositions (“There is nothing wrong with beating up a stealer”) [30].

1.3. Behavioral Prediction Based on Antisocial Attitudes

Antisocial attitude is a concept underlying antisocial behavior [23,30,31,32,33]. Antisocial attitudes are also one of the Central Eight risk/need factors in the Risk-Need-Responsivity Model [23]. There is strong evidence that antisocial attitudes, including emotional factors, are among the factors that predict criminal behavior [23,33,34,35,36,37,38,39].
Currently, two models of diagnosis dominate in psychology and psychiatry, namely categorical and dimensional. An example of the former is the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), being a formal classification of mental health disorders and features symptoms (i.e., ASPD criteria). The example of the latter is a reliable and valid instrument measuring antisociality. Some authors argue that categorical assessment is associated with the loss of clinically relevant information [40]. There is strong evidence that dimensional models are more effective in terms of predictive validity [40,41,42,43].

1.4. The Present Study

Several instruments measuring criminal/antisocial attitudes are available in the literature, inter alia, the Psychological Inventory of Criminal Thinking Styles (PICTS) by Walters [44]; the Criminal Attribution Inventory (CRAI) by Kroner and Mills [32]; the Avoidance Responsibility Scale (ARS) by Powell, Rosen, and Huff [45]; The How I Think Questionnaire (HIT) [46]; The Pride Delinquency Scale (PID) [47]; The Criminal Sentiments Scale-Modified (CSS-M) [48]; The Measures of Criminal Attitudes and Associates (MCAA) [31]. The instruments listed above have limitations. Some of the scales of the Psychological Inventory of Criminal Thinking Styles (PICTS) are not reliable; this instrument is also not parsimonious. The next tool, namely the Criminal Attribution Inventory (CRAI), seems to be limited in its usefulness for predicting criminal behavior. In turn, the How I Think Questionnaire (HIT) is limited as it was designed for adolescents; the Avoidance Responsibility Scale (ARS) is limited in its usefulness for predicting antisocial behavior; it has not been found that the Pride Delinquency Scale (PID) predicts antisocial behavior.
The extent of antisocial content should be significantly different from non-offenders if attitudes have to be useful for the prediction of offending [49]. It seems that the DSM-V diagnostic criteria for Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD) may be taken into account as criteria of the new tool predicting antisocial behavior. There is strong evidence that ASPD predicts crime [50,51,52]. Moreover, people in correctional mental health settings have higher rates of antisocial personality disorder than people in the general community [53].
Antisocial attitude [30,54] comprises perceptive (cognitive belief system) and preferential dispositions (emotional system). While the Antisocial Beliefs Scale exists [55], there is a lack of a tool for measuring antisocial preferences.
Despite the growing number of instruments measuring antisocial attitudes, there is a lack of new tools assessing antisocial preferences and measuring the affective component of attitudes based on the antisocial personality disorder criteria. The goal of the study was to design a new instrument that would measure antisocial preferences. Additionally, there is a need to make new instruments measuring antisocial preferences, which could be used before and after intervention programs aimed at offenders. The next aim of the study was to develop and validate the construct validity and divergent criterion validity of a new measure of antisocial attitudes.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample and Procedure

A group of 800 individuals serving prison sentences and 339 non-offenders were approached. The non-offenders group was the only comparison group.

2.1.1. Sample 1

The research was conducted in penal institutions administrated by the District Inspectorate of the Prison Service in Warsaw, Poland. Self-report questionnaires delivered to all prisons were anonymous. Before starting the research study, the prison staff was instructed about the procedure of the study. All questionnaires were distributed among prison inmates by the prison personnel, who also explained the nature of the study and provided a summary of the informed consent. This was necessary because prison inmates are a vulnerable population who might otherwise have felt compelled to take part in this study. Individuals were instructed to place completed questionnaires in unmarked envelopes and return them to the data collector. The prison staff monitored data collection, and the questionnaires were returned to the research team. 752 prison inmates returned completed questionnaires. Response rate was 89.75%. Due to significant missing data, some participants were excluded. Finally, data from 718 prisoners were included in the further analysis.
The survey revealed that 32 prisoners (4.45%) had committed murder (Article 148 of the Penal Code), 147 (20.47%) were serving a sentence under Article 207 (mistreatment), and 66 (9.19%) were in prison for offenses under Article 286 (fraud, ransom).

2.1.2. Sample 2

Data were collected from non-offenders, too. Students of social prevention and resocialization distributed the questionnaires among adult men who were without a criminal record. In addition, in this case, informed consent was obtained from participants, and training for students was provided by the research team prior to data collection. As regards the control group, 400 individuals were approached; 366 of them returned completed surveys. Ultimately, 339 questionnaires were included in the analysis, as some of those returned were excluded due to missing data. The oldest and the youngest respondents were born in 1979 and 1998, respectively. The demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1 below.

2.2. Measures

Antisocial Preferences. To measure antisocial preferences, the Antisocial Preferences Scale (APS) was used. The APS is a 35-item self-report tool measuring antisocial preferences in forensic populations. It consists of seven subscales, namely: (1) Aggressiveness (AG), (2) Lack of Guilt or Remorse (LSGR), (3) Breaking Legal Norms (BLN), (4) Incapacity for Mutually Intimate Relationships (IMIR), (5) Impulsiveness (IMP), (6) Risk-Taking (RT), and (7) Egocentrism (EG). All items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree).

Development of the Antisocial Preferences Scale

The theoretical basis for the APS is Czesław Czapów’s concept of attitude [30]. A Polish scholar indicated the role of the emotional component in explaining antisocial behavior and focused on two elements: cognitive beliefs and emotional preferences. These components are related to behavior [30,54,56]. The APS consists of emotional preferences. The antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) criteria of the alternative model showed in the “Emerging Measures and Models” chapter of the DSM–5 manual, section III; [57] were taken into account in the development of the new instrument. Criterion A of the DSM–5 alternative model refers to impairment in self and interpersonal functioning (e.g., egocentricity and absence of internal prosocial standards and failure to conform to lawful behavior; lack of concern for others, lack of remorse, exploitativeness, use of deceit, coercion, dominance, and intimidation to fulfill interpersonal needs [57]. Criterion B for personality pathology refers to the presence of maladaptive personality traits, namely: manipulativeness, deceitfulness, callousness, and hostility (the antagonism) combined with irresponsibility, impulsivity, and risk-taking (the disinhibition) [57,58].
Based on the ASPD criteria, seven dimensions of antisocial preferences were extracted and discussed by a panel of 11 experts (specialists in the field of social rehabilitation). In the first stage of the construction of the measure, 140 items were extracted, each of them rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). The procedure proposed by Lawshe was used to assess the content validity of the new measure [59]. The initial item pool was reduced to 35 items (five per dimension). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the dimensions of APS are: (1) Aggressiveness = 0.81, (2) Lack of Guilt or Remorse = 0.76, (3) Breaking Legal Norms = 0.79, (4) Incapacity for Mutually Intimate Relationships = 0.82, (5) Impulsiveness = 0.79, (6) Risk Taking = 0.74, and (7) Egocentrism = 0.79. Moreover, McDonald’s omega coefficients for the subscales are very similar: 0.81 for Aggressiveness, 0.77 for Lack of Guilt or Remorse, 0.79 for Breaking Legal Norms, 0.81 for Incapacity for Mutually Intimate Relationships, 0.80 for Impulsiveness, 0.74 for Risk Taking, and 0.80 for Egocentrism. The Antisocial Preferences Scale had good internal reliability.
I predicted that the APS factors should correlate significantly with external variables: aggression [60], impulsiveness [61], and manipulativeness [62]. To test the correlations between the APS dimensions and the external variables listed above, three measures were used: the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ) measuring aggression, the Mach-IV measuring Machiavellianism, and the IVE Questionnaire.
Aggression. To measure aggression, the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ) was administered. The questionnaire consists of 29 items and measures four factors: physical aggression, verbal aggression, anger, and hostility. Its items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = extremely uncharacteristic, 2 = somewhat uncharacteristic, 3 = neither uncharacteristic nor characteristic, 4 = somewhat characteristic, 5 = extremely characteristic). Cronbach’s α coefficient for the whole scale was 0.80; as far as the subscales are concerned, it ranged from 0.77 for Hostility to 0.85 for Physical Aggression [63].
Machiavellianism. To measure Machiavellianism, the Mach-IV was used. The questionnaire measures three dimensions: (1) interpersonal tactics, (2) cynical views of human nature, and (3) utilitarian morality. The instrument consists of 20 self-report items, each of them scored on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strong disagreement to 7 = strong agreement). Cronbach’s α for the whole scale was 0.73 [64].
Impulsivity. The IVE Questionnaire was used to measure the personality traits of impulsivity, venturesomeness, and empathy. This instrument consists of 63 items using a yes/no response format. Cronbach’s α coefficients are the following: for Impulsivity, 0.86 for women and 0.76 for men, for Venturesomeness 0.90 for women and 0.85 for men; and for Empathy, 0.77 for both genders [65].

2.3. Analytical Procedure

The four models were tested using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA): one latent factor model (M1), a multifactor model (M2), a second-order model (M3), and a bifactor model (M4).
Kline recommends that at least four indicators should be used to test which model is the best fit for the data: (1) RMSEA (root mean square error of approximation) and (2) SRMR (standardized root mean square residual; [66,67], (3) TLI (Tucker–Lewis index) and (4) CFI (comparative fit index; [66,68]. RMSEA value should be close to 0.06 [69]. Kline [66] recommends that the CFI value should be higher than or equal to 0.90, and TLI should be higher than 0.95. Moreover, the cut-off for SRMR indicating a good fit is 0.08 [66].
All calculations were carried out using Mplus 8.2 [70]. Descriptive statistics, the Mann–Whitney U test, Pearson correlation coefficient, discrimination, and threshold parameters for the Graded Response Model were calculated using SPSS version 28. Additionally, the reliability of APS was provided by examining the composite reliability for a total score.
Graded Response Model (GRM) was used to examine the likelihood of responding in a particular response category. GRM is a type of polytomous IRT model [71]. It is an extension of the 2PL model for ordered scored items like a Likert-type rating.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

All descriptive statistics for the seven factors of the Antisocial Preferences Scale are provided in Table 2.
Descriptive statistics for BPAQ, MACH-IV, and IVE for the prisoners group are shown in Table 3.

3.2. Confirmatory Factor Analysis Results and Correlations between APS Dimensions

Table 4 presents the fit indices of all alternative models of the APS. The bifactor model has the best fit when compared with other estimated models; it showed slightly lower RMSEA and SRMR values and slightly higher TLI and CFI values than the remaining models.
All of the standardized factor loadings for the general factor and the factors representing seven antisocial preferences are shown in Table 5 below.
The seven factors of the Antisocial Preferences Scale were found to be intercorrelated (see Table 6). Most of the correlations were moderate and positive. The correlation between risk-taking and incapacity for mutually intimate relationships was lowest and positive but above 0.40.

3.3. Criterion Validity

Table 7 presents the correlations between the seven APS factors and external variables. Aggressiveness (AG) correlated positively with the BPAQ total score, the remaining dimension of aggression, namely: physical, verbal aggression, anger and hostility (BPAQ), impulsiveness and venturesomeness (IVE), cynical views of human nature, interpersonal tactics, utilitarian morality (MACH-IV) and negatively with empathy (IVE). The correlation between the lack of guilt or remorse and empathy (IVE) was negative. On the other hand, the association between the lack of guilt or remorse and the remaining variables was positive. Breaking legal norms was a strong correlate of impulsivity. Moreover, impulsiveness correlated positively with all dimensions of the BPAQ, MACH-IV, and IVE. Risk-taking correlates strongly with impulsivity. Egocentrism and incapacity for mutually intimate relationships correlate negatively with empathy (IVE). The associations listed above were all expected.

3.4. Sample Comparisons

Table 8 presents the differences between prisoners and non-offenders (comparison group). All of them are statistically significant at p < 0.001. The mean scores of prisoners were significantly higher than the means of controls without a criminal record.

3.5. Composite Reliability

In order to assess the internal reliability of the Antisocial Preferences Scale, a composite reliability analysis was performed. The antisocial preferences total score demonstrated very good internal reliability (0.96).

3.6. Items Discrimination

Discrimination and the threshold parameters for each item of the Antisocial Preferences Scale are shown in Table 9. Item information curves for the all dimensions and Kernel Density estimation for ability estimates are available at Supplementary Materials.
In the first factor (aggressiveness), item discrimination varies between 1.902 (item_22) and 2.589 (item_1), and the average discrimination is 2.23.
When the threshold parameters are examined, with a 50% probability, then the average ability level required for persons to mark categories higher than 1 instead of 1 is −0.440; the average ability level required to mark categories higher than 2 instead of 2 is 0.501; the average ability level required for to mark categories higher than 3 instead of 3 is 1.223, and finally, the average ability level required for individuals to mark categories for 5 instead of 4 is 1.847. In the second dimension (lack of guilt or remorse), item discrimination varies between 1.536 (item_30) and 2.166 (item_16), and the average discrimination is 1.959.
In the next dimension (breaking legal norms), item discrimination varies between 1.230 (item_31) and 3.926 (item_3), and the average discrimination is 2.410.
In turn, in the fourth dimension (incapacity for mutually intimate relationships item discrimination varies between 1.846 (item_11) and 2.873 (item_4), and the mean of discrimination is 2.293.
In the fifth dimension, namely impulsiveness, item discrimination varies between 1.492 (item_5) and 2.756 (item_19), and the average discrimination is 2.214.
In the next dimension (risk taking), item discrimination varies between 1.531 (item_20) and 2.271 (item_27). The average discrimination is 1.769.
Finally, in the last dimension (egocentrism), item discrimination varies between 1.754 (item_35) and 2.640 (item_21), and the average discrimination is 2.202.
To sum up, the average discrimination for all dimensions is above 1.7. According to Baker [72], when the value of the discrimination parameter is 1.7 or above, then it is classified as very high.

4. Discussion

The current study was carried out with the primary aim of introducing the Antisocial Preferences Scale and evaluating its dimensionality and construct validity. Moreover, this paper sought to determine its internal reliability.
The fit indices for all alternative measurement models for the Antisocial Preferences Scale are acceptable, and the differences between those indices are slight. However, the bi-factor model was indeed found to offer the best explanation of the data. The parameters of the bi-factor model show that the factor loadings are lower for grouping factors than for general factors. This suggests that the Antisocial Preferences Scale is not multidimensional but rather a unidimensional measure. Similar results were found in a recent study of antisocial beliefs in prison inmates [55].
There is no consensus on the construct of antisociality. Some authors suggest that antisociality is a syndrome of problem behavior best structured in terms of one factor [73,74]. Other authors believe that antisocial behavior has a multidimensional nature [3,75,76]. The results presented in this article provide empirical evidence on the nature of antisociality, which seems to be unidimensional. This seems to be in line with the social derailment theory. The author of this approach perceived antisocial preferences as unidimensional rather than multidimensional [30,54,56].
The results of the present study also indicate that antisociality, which has been conceptualized in various ways, can be defined as an attitudinal inclination and successfully measured as such. Moreover, the previously defined factorial structure of the Antisocial Preferences Scale, based on the DSM-5 Antisocial Personality Disorder Criteria, was confirmed. This means it is a new promising tool to assess one of the components of antisocial attitudes, namely the affective component.
Composite reliability was calculated for the antisocial preferences total score, which is very high. This indicates that all items constantly measure the same construct. The Antisocial Preferences Scale is a promising alternative to the existing scales assessing antisocial attitudes. It demonstrates very high internal reliability of the whole scale measured by the composite reliability coefficient [77,78] and good internal consistency of the seven dimensions (<0.7) of the instrument measured by Cronbach’s Alpha coefficient.
The correlations between the latent factors were above 0.50, except for the association between risk-taking and incapacity for mutually intimate relationships (0.43). If the latent factors are highly intercorrelated, some researchers recommend tests to verify whether the dimensions correlate differentially with external criteria. [79,80].
Aggressiveness and lack of guilt or remorse correlated highly with each other. On the other hand, these factors correlated differently with BPAQ dimensions and empathy (IVE). Breaking legal norms and aggressiveness correlated differently with anger, physical aggression, and utilitarian morality too. The next two dimensions of the APS, namely lack of guilt or remorse and egocentrism, correlated differently with utilitarian morality and cynical views of human nature. Breaking legal norms and risk-taking correlated highly with each other but on the other hand, these variables correlated differently with empathy, interpersonal tactics, and utilitarian morality. The correlation between aggressiveness and risk-taking was high too, but the association between aggressiveness and empathy was negative and significant; in turn, the association between risk-taking and empathy was not statistically significant. Moreover, aggressiveness is highly correlated with impulsiveness (0.69). These variables correlated differently with anger, verbal aggression, and similar construct measured by IVE (impulsivity). Aggressive and impulsive preferences are correlates of aggression measured by the BPAQ. This seems to be consistent with the conclusion that aggression is a frequent manifestation of antisocial personality disorder [61].
The differences between the group of prisoners and the group of men without a criminal record are statistically significant in the case of all variables measured by the Antisocial Preferences Scale. The means were significantly higher for prisoners than for the control group.
This study has important practical implications. The social rehabilitation process has been a challenge for social scientists [23]. The Antisocial Preferences Scale can be used in testing antisocial preferences among adults representing forensic populations, both before and after intervention programs (pretest vs. posttest). The theoretical underpinnings of the tool are the antisocial personality disorder criteria of the DSM-5 alternative model [81]. Persons with ASPD were found to be more prone to recidivism [82]. However, the recidivism-predicting power of the APS is an issue for future research to investigate.
The use of a self-report measure of antisocial preferences in various populations (including nonincarcerated samples) might allow for a more effective exploration of the relationship between criminal behavior and antisocial attitudes.
Future research should investigate the relationship of the Antisocial Preferences Scale to other established measures of antisocial behavior and antisocial attitudes in order to further elucidate its criterion validity. If this measure is found to map well onto antisociality, it might be useful in reducing the time of measuring antisociality in forensic and correctional samples. Moreover, future research should extend and replicate the findings of the present study in broader community-based samples and in any other setting where the assessment of antisociality can be conducted (e.g., forensic inpatient psychiatric settings).
The study has several methodological limitations. The analysis was based on data from a Polish prison population only. Different linguistic and cultural contexts should be included in future studies. Another limitation of the project was its cross-sectional design, which made it impossible to draw conclusions about the causal relationships between the factors of the Antisocial Preferences Scale and external criteria.
According to Hare and Neumann [83], the latest generation of risk assessment instruments has largely dispelled the belief that useful predictions cannot be made about antisocial behavior. Testing the predictive value of the Antisocial Preferences Scale is a task for future research.

5. Conclusions

The presented instrument can be used among individuals with a criminal history. Additionally, this instrument measuring antisocial preferences may be used before and after intervention programs aimed at offenders.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph20032366/s1.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. The research project has the ethical approval of the Institutional Review Board of The Institute of Social Prevention and Resocialization, Faculty of Applied Social Sciences and Resocialization, University of Warsaw (2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Hashmani, T.; Jonason, P.K. Antisocial Behavior. In Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science; Shackelford, T.K., Weekes-Shackelford, V.A., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  2. Van Lier, P.A.C.; Vitaro, F.; Wanner, B.; Vuijk, P.; Crijnen, A.A.M. Gender Differences in Developmental Links among Antisocial Behavior, Friends’ Antisocial Behavior, and Peer Rejection in Childhood: Results from Two Cultures. Child. Dev. 2005, 76, 841–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. dos Santos, W.; Holanda, L.; Meneses, G.; Luengo, M.A.; Gómez-Fraguela, J.A. Antisocial Behaviour: A Unidimensional or Multidimensional Construct? Av. En Psicol. Latinoam. 2019, 37, 13–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Ryan, J.P.; Williams, A.B.; Courtney, M.E. Adolescent Neglect, Juvenile Delinquency and the Risk of Recidivism. J. Youth Adolesc. 2013, 42, 454–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Ogloff, J.R.; Campbell, R.E.; Shepherd, S.M. Disentangling Psychopathy from Antisocial Personality Disorder: An Australian Analysis. J. Forensic. Psychol. Pract. 2016, 16, 198–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. White, S.F.; Tyler, P.M.; Erway, A.K.; Botkin, M.L.; Kolli, V.; Meffert, H.; Pope, K.; Blair, J.R. Dysfunctional Representation of Expected Value Is Associated with Reinforcement-Based Decision-Making Deficits in Adolescents with Conduct Problems. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 2016, 57, 938–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Bubenzer-Busch, S.; Herpertz-Dahlmann, B.; Kuzmanovic, B.; Gaber, T.J.; Helmbold, K.; Ullisch, M.G.; Zepf, F.D. Neural Correlates of Reactive Aggression in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Comorbid Disruptive Behaviour Disorders. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2016, 133, 310–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Hare, R.D.; Neumann, C. Structural Models of Psychopathy. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2005, 7, 57–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Skilling, T.A.; Harris, G.T.; Rice, M.E.; Quinsey, V.L. Identifying Persistently Antisocial Offenders Using the Hare Psychopathy Checklist and DSM Antisocial Personality Disorder Criteria. Psychol. Assess. 2002, 14, 27–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Widiger, T.A. Psychopathy and DSM-IV Psychopathology. In Handbook of Psychopathy; Patrick, C., Ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 156–171. [Google Scholar]
  11. Liu, W.; Wang, W.; Xia, L.; Lin, S.; Wang, Y. Left-Behind Children’s Subtypes of Antisocial Behavior: A Qualitative Study in China. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. McGauran, A.; Brooks, M.; Khan, R. The Role of Emotional Resilience, Childhood Parentification, and Attachment Style on Antisocial Behaviour in Adulthood: A Comparison of an Offender and Normative Population. J. Crim. Psychol. 2019, 9, 75–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Drukker, M.; Kaplan, C.D.; Feron, F.J.M.; Van Os, J.; Korebrits, A. Delinquency in Context; Neighbourhood and Gender Interactions among Adolescents. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 2010, 19, 148–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Neumann, A.; Barker, E.D.; Koot, H.M.; Maughan, B. The Role of Contextual Risk, Impulsivity, and Parental Knowledge in the Development of Adolescent Antisocial Behavior. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2010, 119, 534–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Vaughan, E.; Dennehy, E.; Kelly, C.; Gabhainn, S.N. Understanding and Addressing Anti-Social Behaviour a Rapid Evidence Review; Health Promotion Research Centre NUI: Galway, Ireland, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  16. Hare, R.D. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist—Revised, 2nd ed.; Multi-Health Systems: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  17. Achenbach, T.M.; Bernstein, A.; Dumenci, L. DSM-Oriented Scales and Statistically Based Syndromes for Ages 18 to 59: Linking Taxonomic Paradigms to Facilitate Multi-Taxonomic Approaches. J. Pers. Assess. 2005, 84, 47–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Banerjee, P.J.M.; Gibbon, S.; Huband, N. Assessment of Personality Disorder. Adv. Psychiatr. Treat. 2009, 15, 389–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Haddock, G.; Maio, G.R. Attitudes: Content, Structure and Functions. In Introduction to Social Psychology a European Perspective; Hewstone, M., Stroebe, W., Jonas, K., Eds.; Blackwell Publishing: Oxford, UK, 2008; pp. 112–133. [Google Scholar]
  20. Rosenberg, M.J.; Hovland, C.I. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Components of Attitudes. In Attitude Organization and Change: An Analysis of Consistency among Attitude Components; Rosenberg, M.J., Hovland, C.I., Eds.; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 1960; pp. 1–14. [Google Scholar]
  21. Abelson, R.P.; Kinder, D.R.; Peters, M.D.; Fiske, S.T. Affective and Semantic Components in Political Person Perception. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 42, 619–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Weiss, H.M.; Nicholas, J.P.; Daus, C.S. An Examination of the Joint Effects of Affective Experiences and Job Beliefs on Job Satisfaction and Variations in Affective Experiences over Time. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 78, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Andrews, D.A.; Bonta, J. The Psychology of Criminal Conduct; Taylor & Francis: London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  24. Ajzen, I.; Fishbein, M. The Influence of Attitudes on Behavior. In Handbook of Attitudes and Attitude Change: Basic Principles; Albarracin, D., Johnson, B.T., Zanna, M.P., Eds.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 2005; pp. 173–221. [Google Scholar]
  25. Hofmann, W.; Friese, M.; Wiers, R.W. Impulsive versus Reflective Influences on Health Behavior: A Theoretical Framework and Empirical Review. Health Psychol. 2008, 2, 111–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Sheeran, P.; Gollwitzer, P.M.; Bargh, J.A. Nonconscious Processes and Health. Health Psychol. 2013, 32, 460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Conner, M.; Godin, G.; Sheeran, P.; Germain, M. Some Feelings Are More Important: Cognitive Attitudes, Affective Attitudes, Anticipated Affect, and Blood Donation. Health Psychol. 2013, 32, 264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lawton, R.; Conner, M.; McEachan, R. Desire or Reason: Predicting Health Behaviors from Affective and Cognitive Attitudes. Health Psychol. 2009, 28, 56–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Breckler, S.J.; Wiggins, E.C. Affect versus Evaluation in the Structure of Attitudes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 25, 253–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Czapów, C. Wychowanie Resocjalizujące; PWN: Warsaw, Poland, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  31. Mills, J.F.; Kroner, D.G.; Forth, A.E. Measures of Criminal Attitudes and Associates (MCAA) Development, Factor Structure, Reliability, and Validity. Assessment 2002, 9, 240–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Kroner, D.G.; Mills, J.F. The Criminal Attribution Inventory. A Measure of Offender Perceptions. J. Offender Rehabil. 2008, 39, 15–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Gendreau, P.; Goggin, C.; Chanteloupe, F.; Andrews, D.A. The Development of Clinical and Policy Guidelines for the Prediction of Criminal Behaviour in Criminal Justice Settings; Corrections Branch, Ministry of the Solicitor General: Ottawa, ON, Canada, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  34. Simourd, L.; Andrews, D.A. Correlates of Delinquency: A Look at Gender Differences. Forum Correct. Res. 1994, 6, 26–31. [Google Scholar]
  35. Gendreau, P.; Little, T.; Goggin, C. A Meta-Analysis of the Predictors of Adult Offender Recidivism: What works. Criminology 1996, 34, 401–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Andrews, D.A.; Bonta, J.; Wormith, S.J. The Level of Service/Case Management Inventory (LSI/CM): User’s Manual; Multi-Health Systems: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hanson, R.K.; Morton-Bourgon, K. Predictors of Sexual Recidivism: An Up-Dated Meta-Analysis; Public Safety and Emergency Preparedness: Ottawa, ON, Canada, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  38. Dowden, C.; Andrews, D.A. What Works for Female Offenders: A Meta-Analytic Review. Crime Delinq. 1999, 45, 438–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Dowden, C.; Andrews, D.A. What Works in Young Offender Treatment: A Meta-Analysis. Forum Correct. Res. 1999, 11, 21–24. [Google Scholar]
  40. Fowler, J.C.; Sharp, C.; Kalpakci, A.; Madan, A.; Clapp, J.; Allen, J.G.; Frueh, B.C.; Oldham, J.M. A Dimensional Approach to Assessing Personality Functioning: Examining Personality Trait Domains Utilizing DSM-IV Personality Disorder Criteria. Compr. Psychiatry 2015, 56, 75–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Morey, L.C.; Hopwood, C.J.; Markowitz, J.C.; Gunderson, J.G.; Grilo, C.M.; McGlashan, T.H.; Shea, M.T.; Yen, S.; Sanislow, C.A.; Ansell, E.B.; et al. Comparison of Alternative Models for Personality Disorders, II: 6-, 8- and 10-Year Follow-Up. Psychol. Med. 2012, 42, 1705–1713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Samuel, D.B.; Widiger, T.A. A Meta-Analytic Review of the Relationships between the Five-Factor Model and DSM-IV-TR Personality Disorders: A Facet Level Analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2008, 28, 1326–1342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Hopwood, C.J.; Zanarini, M.C. Borderline Personality Traits and Disorder: Predicting Prospective Patient Functioning. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2010, 78, 585–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Walters, G.D. The Psychological Inventory of Criminal Thinking Styles (PICTS): A Review and Meta-Analysis. Assessment 2002, 9, 278–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Powell, K.M.; Rosén, L.A.; Huff, M.E. Disruptive Behavior Disorders and the Avoidance of Responsibility. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1997, 23, 549–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Barriga, A.Q.; Gibbs, J.C. Measuring Cognitive Distortion in Anitsocial Youth: Development and Preliminary Validation of the “How I Think” Questionnaire. Aggress. Behav. 1996, 22, 333–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Shields, I.W.; Whitehall, G.C. The Pride in Delinquency Scale. In Proceedings of the Eastern Ontario Correctional Psychologist’s Winter Conference, Ontario, ON, Canada, December 1991. [Google Scholar]
  48. Simourd, D.J. The Criminal Sentiments Scale-Modified and Pride in Delinquency Scale: Psychometric Properties and Construct Validity of Two Measures of Criminal Attitudes. Crim. Justice Behav. 1997, 24, 52–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Cargill, R. Antisocial Attitudes and Antisocial Behaviour: An Investigation of Antisocial Attitudes in a New Zealand Non-Offender Sample. Master’s Thesis, Massey University, Albany, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  50. Fridell, M.; Hesse, M.; Jaeger, M.E.; Kühlhorn, E. Antisocial Personality Disorder as a Predictor of Criminal Behaviour in a Longitudinal Study of a Cohort of Abusers of Several Classes of Drugs: Relation to Type of Substance and Type of Crime. Addict. Behav. 2008, 33, 799–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Kingston, D.; Olver, M.; Harris, M.; Wong, C.; Bradford, J. The Relationship between Mental Disorder and Recidivism in Sexual Offenders. Int. J. Forensic Ment. Health 2015, 14, 10–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Hanson, K. Will They Do It Again? Predicting Sex-Offense Recidivism. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2000, 9, 106–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Fazel, S.; Danesh, J. Serious Mental Disorder in 23,000 Prisoners: A Systematic Review of 62 Surveys. Lancet 2002, 359, 545–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Pytka, L. Pedagogika Resocjalizacyjna. Wybrane Zagadnienia Teoretyczne, Diagnostyczne i Metodyczne; Akademia Pedagogiki Specjalnej: Warsaw, Poland, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  55. Skowroński, B. Introduction and Validation of the Antisocial Beliefs Scale in a Sample of Polish Prisoners. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 991687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Skowroński, B. Typologie Wadliwej Integracji Postaw w Świetle Teorii Wadliwej Integracji Postaw Czesława Czapówa. Studium Teoretyczno-Empiryczne; Wydawnictwa Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego: Warsaw, Poland, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  57. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  58. Wygant, D.B.; Sellbom, M.; Sleep, C.E.; Wall, T.D.; Applegate, K.C.; Krueger, R.F.; Patrick, C.J. Examining the DSM–5 Alternative Personality Disorder Model Operationalization of Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy in a Male Correctional Sample. Pers. Disord. 2016, 7, 229–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Lawshe, C. A Quantitative Approach to Content Validity. Pers. Psychol. 1975, 28, 563–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Lobbestael, J.; Cima, M.; Arntz, A. The Relationship between Adult Reactive and Proactive Aggression, Hostile Interpretation Bias, and Antisocial Personality Disorder. Pers. Disord. 2013, 27, 53–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Azevedo, J.; Vieira-Coelho, M.; Castelo-Branco, M.; Coelho, R.; Figueiredo-Braga, M. Impulsive and Premeditated Aggression in Male Offenders with Antisocial Personality Disorder. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0229876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Black, D.W. The Natural History of Antisocial Personality Disorder. Can. J. Psychiatry 2015, 60, 309–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  63. Aranowska, E.; Rytel, J. Struktura Czynnikowa Kwestionariusza Agresji Buss’a i Perry’ego (BPQA) w Populacji Polskiej. Stud. Psychol. 2012, 12, 135–151. [Google Scholar]
  64. Pilch, I. Osobowość Makiawelisty i Niemakiawelisty w Ocenie Własnej i Partnerów. Czas. Psychol. 2006, 12, 265–273. [Google Scholar]
  65. Jaworowska, A. Kwestionariusz Impulsywności IVE; Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych Polskiego Towarzystwa Psychologicznego: Warsaw, Poland, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  66. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modelling, 4th ed.; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  67. Steiger, J.H. Structural Model Evaluation and Modification: An Interval Estimation Approach. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1990, 25, 173–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Bentler, P.M. Comparative Fit Indexes in Structural Models. Psychol. Bull. 1990, 107, 238–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Hu, L.T.; Bentler, P.M. Cut off Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus New Alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Muthén, L.K.; Muthén, B.O. Mplus User’s Guide, 7th ed.; Muthén & Muthén: Los Angles, CA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  71. Samejima, F. The Graded Response Model. In Handbook of Modern Item Response Theory; van der Linden, W.J., Hambleton, R.K., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 85–100. [Google Scholar]
  72. Baker, F.B. The Basics of Item Response Theory, 2nd ed.; ERIC Clearinghouse on Assessment and Evaluation: Washington, DC, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  73. Jessor, R. Risk Behavior in Adolescence: A Psychosocial Framework for Understanding and Action. J. Adolesc. Health 1991, 12, 597–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Farrell, A.D.; Kung, E.M.; White, K.S.; Valois, R.F. The Structure of Self-Reported Ag gression, Drug Use, and Delinquent Behaviors during Early Adolescence. J. Clin. Child. Psychol. 2000, 29, 282–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Loeber, R.; Stouthamer-Loeber, M. The Development of Juvenile Aggression and Violence: Some Common Misconcep tions and Controversies. Am. Psychol. 1998, 53, 242–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Thornberry, T.P.; Huizinga, D.; Loeber, R. The Causes and Correlates Studies: Findings and Policy Implications. Juv. Just. 2004, 9, 3–19. [Google Scholar]
  77. Raykov, T. Estimation of Composite Reliability for Congeneric Measures. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1997, 21, 173–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Raykov, T. Coefficient Alpha and Composite Reliability with Interrelated Nonhomogeneous Items. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1998, 22, 375–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Boduszek, D.; Dębowska, A. Critical Evaluation of Psychopathy Measurement (PCL-R and SRP-III/SF) and Recommendations for Future Research. J. Crim. Justice 2016, 44, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Carmines, E.G.; Zeller, R.A. Reliability and Validity Assessment; Sage: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  81. American Psychiatric Association. Desk Reference to the Diagnostic Criteria from DSM.-5 (R.); American Psychiatric Association Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  82. Bonta, J.; Law, M.; Hanson, K. The Prediction of Criminal and Violent Recidivism among Mentally Disordered Offenders: A Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Bull. 1998, 123, 123–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Hare, R.D.; Neumann, C. Psychopathy: Assessment and Forensic Implications. Can. J. Psychiatry 2009, 54, 791–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for demographic variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for demographic variables.
Prisoners (N = 718)Non-Offenders (N = 339)
Age in years (M/SD)37/10.828.6/5.4
Residence (N/%)
Village119/16.6175/51.6
Town (≤50,000)167/23.3118/34.8
City (50,000–150,000)106/14.89/2.7
City (>150,000)311/43.337/10.9
Total703/97.9339/100
No data15/2.10
Education level (N/%)
Elementary166/23.10
Middle school88/12.30
Basic vocational195/27.20
Secondary vocational114/15.938/11.2
General Secondary90/12.5197/58.1
Bachelor’s degree25/3.536/10.6
Master’s degree25/3.568/20.1
Total703/97.9339/100
No data15/2.10
Number of sentences (N/%)
1–5480/66.85
6–10155/21.58
11–1519/2.64
16–2012/1.67
20<10/1.39
Total676/94.15
No data42/5.85
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for APS factors: prisoners (N = 718); non-offenders (N = 339).
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for APS factors: prisoners (N = 718); non-offenders (N = 339).
PrisonersNon-Offenders
VariablesMSDSkewnessKurtosisMin.Max.MSDSkewnessKurtosisMin.Max.
1.Aggressiveness11.094.540.348−0.7285247.331.860.626−0.766511
2.Lack of guilt or remorse11.744.240.120−0.6555238.472.350.4860.128515
3.Breaking legal norms11.154.390.301−0.6985256.981.640.340−1.096510
4.Incapacity for mutually intimate relationships9.804.260.597−0.5835247.011.670.120−1.388510
5.Impulsiveness11.784.440.213−0.5865258.382.170.210−1.107512
6.Risk-taking12.444.20−0.101−0.6385248.361.740.079−0.967512
7.Egocentrism11.334.330.269−0.57352510.423.890.241−0.802519
8.APS total score79.3525.270.08−0.5503515956.7712.170.30−1.203878
Table 3. Descriptive statistics for BPAQ, MACH-IV, and IVE for the prisoners group.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics for BPAQ, MACH-IV, and IVE for the prisoners group.
VariablesMSDMin.Max.SkewnessKurtosis
BPAQAnger18.825.297330.228−0.460
Hostility22.356.388400.049−0.191
Physical aggression226.799450.391−0.230
Verbal aggression15.043.80525−0.034−0.034
BPQA global score78.2218.97351360.1880.188
MACH-IVInterpersonal tactics31.937.15952−0.1010.023
Cynical views of human nature34.316.32957−0.3361.474
Utilitarian morality7.982.57214−0.1720.626
IVEImpulsivity9.724.561190.041−0.870
Venturesomeness9.572.85216−0.168−0.567
Empathy10.342.653190.3020.087
Note: BPAQ—The Buss–Perry Aggression Questionnaire; MACH-IV—Machiavellianism Test; IVE—Eysenck’s Impulsivity Inventory.
Table 4. Fit indices for alternative measurement models for the Antisocial Preferences Scale.
Table 4. Fit indices for alternative measurement models for the Antisocial Preferences Scale.
ModelTest χ2 (df)CFITLISRMRRMSEARMSEA CI 90%
M1: ONE-FACTOR3196.154 (560) *0.9130.9080.0560.081[0.078, 0.084]
M2: MULTIFACTOR2395.947 (553) *0.9390.9350.0480.068[0.065, 0.071]
M3: SECOND-ORDER2268.087 (539) *0.9430.9370.0450.067[0.064, 0.070]
M4: BIFACTOR2126.687 (525) *0.9470.9400.0450.065[0.062, 0.068]
Note: * p < 0.001.
Table 5. Standardized factor loadings for the general factor and the factors representing seven antisocial preferences.
Table 5. Standardized factor loadings for the general factor and the factors representing seven antisocial preferences.
Original Item NumbersAGLSGRBLNIMIRIMPRTEGGeneral Factor
(1)I often get into fights when someone irritates me.0.091 * 0.764 **
(2)I don’t like people who cry. 0.106 * 0.724 **
(3)I could steal a petty thing only to experience a thrill of excitement. 0.449 ** 0.743 **
(4)I treat relationships with women as an unwanted obligation. 0.411 ** 0.732 **
(5)I am annoyed by people looking at me on the bus. 0.077 * 0.641 **
(6)I am capable of doing many crazy things if someone persuades me to. 0.379 ** 0.602 **
(7)There is no place for sentiment in life: I only do what benefits me. 0.519 **0.637 **
(8)I like to see people fear me.0.271 ** 0.717 **
(9)I don’t care if someone feels bad because of my behavior. 0.336 ** 0.662 **
(10)I could steal only the smallest things for fun. 0.432 ** 0.724 **
(11)I have never been in love with a woman. 0.306 ** 0.656 **
(12)Sometimes I flare up over nothing for no reason (at all). 0.377 ** 0.692 **
(13)I avoid engaging in something that does not give me “a thrill of excitement”. 0.068 * 0.585 **
(14)I am only guided by my own good. 0.267 **0.660 **
(15)I sometimes humiliate others.0.247 ** 0.698 **
(16)I am not in the habit of apologizing. 0.703 ** 0.676 **
(17)I could commit theft to experience a surge of adrenaline (“a thrill of excitement”). 0.392 ** 0.677 **
(18)I do not enter into relationships with women because they demand too much. 0.545 ** 0.603 **
(19)I very easily lose my temper. 0.521 ** 0.655 **
(20)I need constant changes in my life. 0.277 ** 0.548 **
(21)I try to care mostly for myself. 0.362 **0.690 **
(22)I wouldn’t be a real man if I didn’t want to fight others.0.073 * 0.713 **
(23)I do not inquire into whether I am doing the right thing. 0.081 * 0.706 **
(24)I do not treat agreements with other people as something that must always be fulfilled. 0.117 ** 0.675 **
(25)I avoid entering into relationships with women because of the inevitable problems. 0.457 ** 0.608 **
(26)I quickly lose my patience when someone criticizes me. 0.267 ** 0.626 **
(27)I often feel the need for thrills. 0.519** 0.589 **
(28)I don’t care much about other people’s fate. 0.277 **0.642 **
(29)I am excited by fist fighting.0.074 * 0.715 **
(30)There are times when I remain indifferent to the pain and suffering of others. 0.178 ** 0.590 **
(31)I only keep written agreements. 0.152 ** 0.667 **
(32)I avoid intimate relationships with women. 0.490 ** 0.560 **
(33)I am quarrelsome. 0.304 ** 0.687 **
(34)Being in danger is also exciting. 0.257 ** 0.673 **
(35)I rarely help other people. 0.164 **0.676 **
Note: AG = aggressiveness; LSGR = lack of guilt or remorse; BLN = breaking legal norms; IMIR = incapacity for mutually intimate relationships; IMP = impulsiveness; RT = risk-taking; EG = egocentrism. Factor loadings are statistically significant at * p < 0.05 ** p < 0.001.
Table 6. Correlations between APS factors.
Table 6. Correlations between APS factors.
FactorAGLSGRBLNIMIRIMPRTEGAPTS
Aggressiveness (AG)1
Lack of guilt or remorse (LSGR)0.728 **1
Breaking legal norms (BLN)0.702 **0.666 **1
Incapacity for mutually intimate relationships (IMIR)0.614 **0.621 **0.627 **1
Impulsiveness (IMP)0.699 **0.645 **0.639 **0.580 **1
Risk-taking (RT)0.684 **0.627 **0.702 **0.435 **0.628 **1
Egocentrism (EG)0.638 **0.709 **0.663 **0.582 **0.640 **0.572 **1
Antisocial preferences total score (APTS)0.873 **0.858 **0.860 **0.766 **0.832 **0.798 **0.826 **1
Note: ** p < 0.001.
Table 7. Associations between the seven APS factors and external variables.
Table 7. Associations between the seven APS factors and external variables.
AggressivenessLack of Guilt or RemorseBreaking Legal NormsIncapacity for Mutually Intimate RelationshipsImpulsivenessRisk-TakingEgocentrismAntisocial Preferences Total Score
BPQAAnger0.334 **0.265 **0.263 **0.145 **0.449 **0.271 **0.299 **0.350 **
Hostility0.318 **0.278 **0.293 **0.162 **0.386 **0.307 **0.285 **0.350 **
Physical aggression0.480 **0.387 **0.375 **0.253 **0.490 **0.349 **0.381 **0.469 **
Verbal aggression0.262 **0.193 **0.227 **n.s.0.311 **0.237 **0.228 **0.258 **
BPQA: total score0.425 **0.345 **0.352 **0.193 **0.493 **0.351 **0.362 **0.435 **
MACH-IVInterpersonal tactics0.284 **0.278 **0.311 **0.199 **0.240 **0.265 **0.294 **0.322 **
Cynical views of human nature0.156 **0.121 *0.168 **0.109 *0.207 **0.120 *0.232 **0.192 **
Utilitarian morality0.148 **0.123 *0.218 **n.s.0.185 **0.167 **0.224 **0.193 **
IVEImpulsivity0.431 **0.362 **0.402 **0.219 **0.514 **0.446 **0.404 **0.479 **
Venturesomeness0.265 **0.224 **0.217 **n.s.0.227 **0.329 **0.212 **0.259 **
Empathy−0.182 **−0.280 **−0.141 **−0.268 **−0.102 *n.s.−0.295 **−0.225 **
Note: AG = aggressiveness; LSGR = lack of guilt or remorse; BLN = breaking legal norms; IMIR = incapacity for mutually intimate relationships; IMP = impulsiveness; RT = risk-taking; EG = egocentrism. * p < 0.01. ** p < 0.001.
Table 8. Differences between prisoners and non-offenders on the dimensions of the Antisocial Preferences Scale.
Table 8. Differences between prisoners and non-offenders on the dimensions of the Antisocial Preferences Scale.
MrankUZ
PrisonersNon-Offenders
Aggressiveness608.26361.1264,791−12.327 *
Lack of guilt or remorse609.47358.5763,925.5−12.531 *
Breaking legal norms624.29327.1853,282.5−14.853 *
Incapacity for mutually intimate relationships589.73400.3878,097.5−9.512 *
Impulsiveness607.13363.5365,607−12.151 *
Risk-taking627.80319.7450,762−15.363 *
Egocentrism552.16479.95105,072−3.603 *
Antisocial preferences total score619.85336.5856,471.5−14.083 *
Note: * p < 0.001.
Table 9. Discrimination and threshold parameters for the Graded Response Model.
Table 9. Discrimination and threshold parameters for the Graded Response Model.
DimensionsItemsItem Parameters
ab1b2b3b4
AggressivenessItem_12.589−0.3640.5711.2331.900
Item_82.350−0.3200.5341.3041.867
Item_152.324−0.4990.4681.1211.782
Item_221.902−0.7110.4481.2961.884
Item_292.000−0.3060.4861.1631.805
Mean2.233−0.4400.5011.2231.847
Lack of guilt or remorseItem_22.130−0.6860.4051.3112.076
Item_92.042−0.6460.5001.3362.074
Item_162.166−0.5000.5541.2341.870
Item_231.925−0.8480.2901.1361.887
Item_301.536−0.8740.3851.1562.202
Mean1.959−0.7100.4261.2342.021
Breaking legal normsItem_33.926−0.2900.3790.9801.509
Item_103.207−0.3020.5181.1361.713
Item_172.387−0.4020.4471.0471.803
Item_241.300−0.7940.6031.6442.946
Item_311.230−0.7520.6581.7832.797
Mean2.410−0.5080.5211.3182.153
Incapacity for mutually intimate relationshipsItem_42.8730.0230.8171.6562.337
Item_111.8460.2120.8811.7002.466
Item_182.626−0.1460.8751.6952.405
Item_252.125−0.3200.7571.5932.261
Item_321.9980.0360.8861.5452.249
Mean2.293−0.0390.8431.6372.343
ImpulsivenessItem_51.492−0.7750.3751.3362.243
Item_122.595−0.6050.3800.9471.604
Item_192.756−0.4310.5091.1451.877
Item_261.910−0.9820.2551.1061.930
Item_332.317−0.5230.5211.2242.066
Mean2.214−0.6630.4081.1511.944
Risk-takingItem_61.854−0.7940.3451.0192.033
Item_131.176−0.9770.3921.7792.939
Item_201.531−1.1080.1671.2022.249
Item_272.271−0.8470.1360.9991.654
Item_342.015−0.7990.1390.9531.901
Mean1.769−0.9050.2351.1902.155
EgocentrismItem_72.605−0.5790.4761.2211.670
Item_142.054−0.7100.4631.2372.039
Item_212.640−0.5330.5601.0841.887
Item_281.960−0.6030.4961.3112.126
Item_351.754−0.6750.5901.3272.040
Mean2.202−0.6200.5171.2361.952
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Skowroński, B. The Development and Validation of the Antisocial Preferences Scale. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 2366. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20032366

AMA Style

Skowroński B. The Development and Validation of the Antisocial Preferences Scale. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(3):2366. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20032366

Chicago/Turabian Style

Skowroński, Bartłomiej. 2023. "The Development and Validation of the Antisocial Preferences Scale" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 3: 2366. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20032366

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop