Next Article in Journal
Meaningful Work, Well-Being, and Health: Enacting a Eudaimonic Vision
Previous Article in Journal
Flourishing and Self-Control in Adolescence: The Role of Perceived Parenting
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Unconscious Bias among Health Professionals: A Scoping Review

School of Health Sciences, Zurich University of Applied Sciences, Katharina-Sulzer-Platz 9, 8400 Winterthur, Switzerland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20(16), 6569; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20166569
Submission received: 15 June 2023 / Revised: 31 July 2023 / Accepted: 2 August 2023 / Published: 12 August 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Health Care Sciences & Services)

Abstract

:
Background: Unconscious biases are one of the causes of health disparities. Health professionals have prejudices against patients due to their race, gender, or other factors without their conscious knowledge. This review aimed to provide an overview of research on unconscious bias among health professionals and to investigate the biases that exist in different regions of the world, the health professions that are considered, and the research gaps that still exist. Methods: We conducted a scoping review by systematically searching PubMed/MEDLINE, CINAHL, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, and AMED. All records were double-screened and included if they were published between 2011 and 2021. Results: A total of 5186 records were found. After removing duplicates (n = 300), screening titles and abstracts (n = 4210), and full-text screening (n = 695), 87 articles from 81 studies remained. Studies originated from North America (n = 60), Europe (n = 13), and the rest of the world (n = 6), and two studies were of global scope. Racial bias was investigated most frequently (n = 46), followed by gender bias (n = 11), weight bias (n = 10), socio-economic status bias (n = 9), and mental illness bias (n = 7). Most of the studies were conducted by physicians (n = 51) and nurses (n = 20). Other health care professionals were rarely included in these studies. Conclusions: Most studies show that health professionals have an implicit bias. Racial biases among physicians and nurses in the USA are well confirmed. Research is missing on other biases from other regions and other health professions.

1. Introduction

Although the professional ethos of health care providers includes treating all people equally regardless of their physical and mental characteristics, there are inequalities between groups of people in health care [1,2]. There are numerous factors that explain health disparities between groups of people, such as poverty, low health literacy, and harmful health behaviors [3]. However, disparities remain when confounding variables related to patient access, such as income and health insurance, are accounted for [3,4]. One factor contributing to these disparities may be explained by unconscious biases in provider perceptions [5,6,7].
The term “bias” is used to describe a tendency to favor one group over another [8]. It often involves associating physical features or characteristics with a particular behavior [2,9,10], i.e., categorization. The ability to quickly categorize a person or phenomenon that we encounter is likely an evolutionary development that ensured survival [11]. Such categorizations are effectively a shortcut in the brain and, therefore, they relieve the individual of the burden of making repetitive decisions [8,12]. Related terms in the literature include stereotype and prejudice [9]. Stereotypes are often negative and overgeneralized views held toward individuals of certain disadvantaged groups, e.g., poor people are lazy [8,13,14]. Prejudices are negative attitudes toward another person or group formed in-advance of any experience, and they comprise three components: affective (e.g., approach/avoid predilection), cognitive (e.g., assumptions and beliefs), and behavioral (e.g., propensity) [15].
The literature distinguishes between explicit and implicit biases. An explicit bias is based on the conscious thoughts and beliefs that an individual can report [16]. Implicit or unconscious bias, on the other hand, is not conscious of an individual. Often, implicit bias is described as involuntary associations or attitudes that influence our perceptions, and thus our behaviors, decisions, and interactions in an unconscious manner (e.g., [1,8,10,12,13]). It is believed that implicit biases are learned through cultural immersion and socialization [10]. These unconscious associations or attitudes are expressed, for example, in nonverbal behaviors toward others, such as eye contact or physical proximity [10].
A person’s explicit and implicit biases can be different or even contradictory to each other. For example, negative views and beliefs associated with disadvantaged groups may be consciously rejected and inevident in self-reported questionnaires [16], but may still exhibit response–time patterns [10,17] on implicit measures, such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT) [18] or the Go/No-Go Association Task (GNAT) [19]. For example, Green et al. showed that physicians who did not explicitly report bias against African American patients and did not classify them as less cooperative still exhibited an implicit belief that these patients were less cooperative compared to white patients [7].
Even when health professionals are aware of their biases and consciously try to suppress them [17], high cognitive loads, such as fatigue, heavy workload, distraction, or time pressure, can still activate implicit biases [2,9,16,20]. This is because cognitive load makes it more difficult to process social information thoroughly and increases the susceptibility to responses based on learned categorizations [3,16]. Bias, then, can unconsciously influence the way information about a person is processed, leading to unintended inequalities. The consequences of these unconscious biases can affect medical school admissions, patient care, faculty hiring, promotion, and advancement opportunities [8]. In health-sciences education, biases and stereotypes may shape education when students learn with typical examples based on prevalence data [8]. These prevalences can lead to premature conclusions and missed diagnoses when applied to heterogeneous patients who are not perceived as individuals, but as demographic characteristics [8].
For health professionals, inequalities that work at the disadvantage of those who are already at-risk are particularly significant. Examples include ethnic minorities, immigrants, the impoverished, those with low health literacy, sexual minorities, children, women, the elderly, the mentally ill, the obese, and the disabled [10]. Unconscious bias can influence providers’ decisions about treatment, screening, and other procedures they recommend to these patients [3].
Over the last two decades, much research has been conducted on unconscious bias. However, an up-to-date overview is missing. Therefore, this research aims to provide a current overview of the research on unconscious bias and to identify the remaining knowledge gaps. In a prior literature review, Fitzgerald and Hurst [10] examined whether trained health care professionals demonstrate implicit/unconscious biases toward certain types of patients and the unconscious biases they exhibit toward patients. As they only included data through March 2013, we are interested in how the research field has evolved and which biases have been examined. In addition, because most of the research has been conducted in the United States, we are interested in how research on this topic has developed in Europe. Thus, our question is as follows: What biases are prevalent in which countries? Moreover, a review by Fitzgerald and Hurst [10] showed that previous research focused on two health professions: physicians and nurses. In this review, we examine whether other health professions have been studied in the last 10 years. So, our question is as follows: Which health professions are included in these studies? Finally, we want to identify the substantive research gaps that remain in this area.

2. Materials and Methods

We chose the method of a scoping review [21] as our aim was to create an overview of the research conducted on unconscious bias during the last 10 years, to include a broad range of studies, to evaluate the scope of available research, and to identify research gaps [22]. In doing so, we followed the JBI International Scientific Committee’s guide on conducting scoping reviews that builds on the PRISMA-ScR guideline [22] and published a research protocol on OFS before performing the search [23].
To operationalize our research questions, we used the Population, Construct, and Context (PCC) mnemonic for our search components. As the population, we selected trained health professionals such as physicians, nurses, midwives, occupational therapists, and physiotherapists (see Appendix A Table A1). The construct was unconscious or implicit bias. For context, we focused on the health care delivery system. From these components, we derived the following three inclusion and exclusion criteria: 1. Studies were included in which participants were trained health professionals. Studies with untrained health care workers, students, or administrative staff were excluded. Studies with mixed populations were included if 2/3 of participants were trained health professionals or if outcomes were presented separately rather than combined. 2. For the construct, we included empirical studies that used methods that elicit unconscious bias towards patients. Studies investigating explicit bias, structural discrimination, and other forms of overt discrimination by health care workers against patients were excluded. Regarding the measurement, we included studies that used implicit measures [24] such as the “Implicit Association Test” (IAT) [18] or the “Go/No-Go Association Task” (GNAT) [19] and other implicit measures [25]. In addition, we included vignette studies. Vignettes are short descriptions of a person or situation, which can be used to elicit subjects’ attitudes [26,27]. 3. The setting is the health care system. Studies conducted in home or community settings were excluded. Furthermore, we included studies published between 1 January 2011 and 31 December 2021 in English, German, or French.
A sensitive search approach was used. We selected the databases PubMed/MEDLINE, CINAHL, PsycINFO/PsycARTICLES, and AMED. Following the PCC-mnemonic (Population, Construct, and Context). For the population, this was “health professionals” and specific health professions (e.g., “nurse”, “therapist”, and “physician”); for the construct, this was “unconscious bias”, “implicit bias”, and related terms (e.g., “stereotype”, “prejudice”, and “racism“); and for the context we used “health care delivery” or similar terms (e.g., “patient care”). To these search terms we added relevant subject headings and MeSH terms from the relevant databases and combined them with the Boolean operators “AND” and “OR”. For detailed search strings see Appendix A. We then tested the search string, and it was additionally checked by a second researcher using the Peer Review of Electronic Search Strategies (PRESS) checklist [28]. The search was conducted on the 14 January 2022 through to the 18 January 2022.
All identified records were imported into the literature management program Zotero [29] and from there into the web application “Covidence” [30] (see Figure 1). After the removal of duplicate records, we conducted a title and abstract screening, in which each record was double screened by two of the three independent reviewers from our team (U.M., C.D., and T.B.) using the established inclusion and exclusion criteria.
Disagreements regarding inclusion or exclusion were resolved through discussion until an agreement was reached. In a second step, the full texts were screened by two independent reviewers following the same procedure. They also documented the reasons for exclusion. With review articles we proceeded as follows: we extracted all studies that met our inclusion and exclusion criteria and included them in our analysis. Data were extracted and summarized using a structured extraction table.

3. Results

3.1. Search Results

The database search resulted in 5186 records (see Figure 1). After the removal of 300 duplicates, 4210 records were removed during the title and abstract screening, resulting in 676 records. From the reviews, 19 additional studies were extracted at this point of the review process, leading to 695 records. Of these 695 records, 608 records were excluded for the following reasons: 549 studies predominantly used self-report questionnaires with explicit measures, 26 records addressed the wrong population, measuring patients’ bias either in students of health professions or untrained health professionals, 24 records were excluded as they were not empirical studies, 6 records were written in another language than English, French or German, 2 studies used an implicit measure (IAT) but did not publish the results, and 1 study was conducted in a community setting. As a result, 87 articles that were based on 81 studies were included. For six studies, more than one article was published reporting different aspects but using the same data.

3.2. Study Characteristics

Of the 87 records included in this review, 86 were peer-reviewed and published as articles; only one record [31] was a dissertation published as a monograph. Of the 81 studies that are covered in these records, most studies were cross-sectional (n = 68). A few studies were re-analyses of previously collected data (n = 5), used repeated measures of unconscious bias (n = 5), had a prospective design or longitudinal design (n = 1 and n = 1, respectively), or were a cohort study (n = 1). The sample sizes of the studies ranged from 11 to 25,006 participants with a total of 58,908 participants and a mean of 727 participants.
Regarding the measures, 55 of the 81 included studies used an implicit measure: 53 used one or several versions of the IAT [18], 1 study [32] the GNAT [19], 1 study [33] the Affect Misattribution Procedure (AMP) [34], and 1 study [35] used a reaction time task with subliminal priming. Vignettes in which at least one relevant variable (e.g., gender, Body Mass Index) was changed and treatment recommendations were assessed were used in 46 studies. Of these, 25 studies used vignettes only, and 21 studies combined an IAT and vignettes so that the unconscious bias could be analyzed in combination with the individual treatment recommendations using the vignette method.

3.3. Results of the Studies

Table 1 provides information on all included studies and their findings related to unconscious bias. In the 81 studies included, 70 studies found some type of unconscious bias against minorities, while 11 studies found no bias. Regarding the 56 studies that used an IAT or another implicit measure, 54 studies found at least one unconscious bias, while 2 found no bias at all. Looking at the level of individual implicit test measures, 89 tests were conducted in total. Thereof, 85 found an unconscious bias and 2 did not find any unconscious bias; from 2 further studies, the results were not applicable as they were not published. Of the 84 tests showing an unconscious bias, 16 weak, 35 moderate, and 22 strong unconscious biases were found. For 14 tests, the strength of the unconscious bias was not applicable as the results did not follow the recommended form or a classification in terms of strength was not provided for the instruments.
Regarding the 45 studies that used one or several vignettes, 38 studies found at least one unconscious bias in terms of a difference in treatment recommendations or treatment decisions according to the bias assessed, whereas 21 did not find any differences in treatment recommendations or decisions.
Regarding the 20 studies that used both an IAT and a vignette, 13 studies found a bias in the IAT and some difference in the vignettes according to the bias assessed, whereas 7 studies found a bias in the IAT but no differences in the vignettes. One study found some differences in the vignettes and in the IAT for one bias (race) but not for another (SES) [36]. One study [37] used treatment outcomes from medical records prior to assessing unconscious racial bias with the IAT, finding no difference in treatment between African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian patients treated for hypertension depending on the level of implicit bias.
There were six studies that tested interventions to reduce or adjust unconscious bias. Each of these interventions are briefly detailed further below. In study [38], participants engaged in an empathy-inducing perspective-taking intervention that instructed them to imagine how patients’ pain affected patients’ lives, which resulted in a 55% reduction in pain treatment bias in comparison to controls. Study [39] found a similar effect in a subset analysis of white participants who demonstrated a significantly decreased level of implicit bias against Latino people in terms of pleasantness in the intervention group compared to the control group when looking at and reflecting on a photographic story of a migrant family. In study [40], an educational module on patient-centered counseling served as an intervention, resulting in the correlation between personal preferences for pre-natal testing and patient-recommended testing being reduced to non-significance. Thus, the relationship between implicit bias and the propensity to recommend genetic testing was reduced. In study [41], one group had the possibility to exchange real-time information in structured peer networks, which resulted in significantly improved clinical accuracy and showed no bias compared to the control group, which showed significant disparities in treatment recommendations. Two studies were identified that were not successful in reducing unconscious bias. Specifically, study [42] involved clinical residents exposed to schizophrenic patients as an intervention, which resulted in an increased implicit association of illness with the word “criminal”. Finally, study [43], focused on the uncontrollable aspects of obesity, found no change in weight-related IAT as a result of the intervention.
Table 1. Overview of included studies, with information on country, type of bias, method, sample, results, and effect size.
Table 1. Overview of included studies, with information on country, type of bias, method, sample, results, and effect size.
ReferenceCountryType of BiasMethodDescriptionNHPs Results +/++/+++/0
Afulani et al., 2021 [44]KenyaSES biasIAT and vignettesCross-sectional mixed-methods study with SES-IAT, vignettes varying by SES and follow-up qualitative interviews101Nurses, midwives, physicians, support staffModerate negative UB against women with low SES in IAT, differences in expectations towards patients according to stereotypes for both low- and high-SES women in vignettes++/n.a.
Al Alwan et al., 2019 [45]Saudi ArabiaSES biasVignettesCross-sectional vignette study varying by SES (low/high/neutral)45PhysiciansNo difference in accuracy or time for diagnosis between low, high, or neutral SES0
Anastas et al., 2020 [46]USARacial bias, SES biasIAT and vignettes Cross-sectional study with race IAT (black/white) and re-analysis of computer-simulated vignette study varying by race (black/white) and SES (high/low) 436PhysiciansWeak negative UB for black people and strong negative UB for low SES in IATs. Effects between race and SES IAT scores and pain judgement. Different treatment decisions for race and SES in vignettes+/+++/n.a.
Aweidah et al., 2016 [47]AustraliaWeight biasIATCross-sectional mixed-methods pilot-study with weight IAT and qualitative interviews 37Diagnostic radiographersNegative weight bias n.a.
Barnato et al., 2011 [48]USARacial biasVignettesCross-sectional study with randomized trial with standard ized patients varying by race (black/white)33PhysiciansNo difference in treatment decisions0
Bartley et al., 2015 [49]USAAgeism, gender bias, racial biasVignettesCross-sectional online vignette study with videos of virtual humans varying by age, gender, and race154Dentists, physiciansDifferences in ratings of pain and pain treatment by age, sex and race of patient and by provider age, sex and race n.a.
Bernardes & Lima 2011 [50]PortugalGender biasVignettesCross-sectional vignette study varying by gender 126NursesDifferences in attribution of pain according to gender in the absence of diagnosisn.a.
Blair et al., 2013, 2013 and 2014 [37,51,52] USARacial biasIATCross-sectional mixed-methods study with race IATs (black/white and Hispanic/white), patients’ perception of treatment and electronic medical records210Health care providersStrong negative UB against black and Hispanic people. The stronger the physicians’ UB, the lower the rating of the black patient of treatment (not for Hispanic). No difference in treatment according to medical records+++/+++/0
Bøker Lund et al., 2018 [53]DenmarkWeight biasIAT and vignettesCross-sectional study with IAT on attitude and stereotypes for obesity and vignettes varying by gender and weight240PhysiciansStrong negative UB against over weight people in IAT on attitudes and stereo-types. No differences in treatment options by weight, but in recommen dations by sex and weight in vignettes+++/0/n.a.
Bous et al., 2021 [54]USADisability biasIATCross-sectional online study with disability IAT (cleft lips/normal lips)52DentistsModerate negative UB against people with disability (cleft lips)++
Breathett et al., 2019 [55]USARacial biasVignettesCross-sectional mixed-methods study with vignette varied by race (black/white) and think-aloud interviews422/44Health care providersNo differences in recommendations for treatment in vignettes according to race0
Brener et al., 2013 [56]AustraliaMental illness biasIATCross-sectional study with IAT on mental illness74Mental health providersWeak negative UB bias against people with mental illness +
Burgess et al., 2014 [20]USARacial biasVignettesCross-sectional web-based randomized vignette study varying by race (black/white) and cognitive load 99PhysiciansDifferences in drug prescription according to physicians’ gender, cognitive load, and patient race n.a.
Cassell 2015 [31]USARacial biasVignettes and IATCross-sectional online-study with race IAT (black/white) and vignettes varying by race216PhysiciansModerate negative UB on IAT against black people. Difference in diagnosis but no difference in treat ment recommendation in vignettes++/n.a./0
Centola et al., 2021 [41]USARacial bias, gender biasVignettesCross-sectional online-study with video-based vignettes manipulated by gender and race (black/white) 120PhysiciansNegative UB against black women with unsafe treatment recommen dations compared to white male patientn.a.
Chapman et al., 2018 [33]USARacial biasAMPSequential cohort online study with race AMP (Hispanic/ white) with pre- and post-intervention measure69PhysiciansNegative racial bias against Hispanic peoplen.a.
Claréus & Renström 2019 [57]SwedenGender biasVignettesCross-sectional online study with vignette varying by gender 90PhysiciansNegative UB on women in diagnosis related to back painn.a.
Colón-Emeric et al., 2017 [58]USARacial bias, gender biasVignettesRepeated-measures study (pre- and post-intervention) with randomized vignettes differing by race, gender, and age541Nurses, rehabilitation staffSmall degree of negative UB against black people, no UB on gender+
Cooper et al., 2012 [39]USARacial biasIATCross-sectional study with race IAT (black/white) on attitudes and racial stereotyping on compliance 40PhysiciansModerate negative UB on race attitude and race compliance stereotyping++/++
Crapanzano et al., 2018 [59]USAMental illness biasIATCross-sectional online-study with 4 IATs on attitude (good/bad), permanence, controllability, and ethology of mental illness (depression/ physical illness) 86Physicians, psychiatristsWeak–moderate negative UB on attitude, permanence, and controllability against mentally ill people amongst physicians not in psychiatrists. Moderate negative UB on ethology for both+-++/+-++/+-++/0/++
Daugherty et al., 2017 [60]USAGender biasIAT and vignettes Cross-sectional study with 2 gender IATs on stereotypes (strength and risk taking) and vignettes diffing by gender 503PhysiciansModerate negative UB on gender stereotype in risk-taking and strong UB on strength IAT against women. No difference for diagnosis but differences in recommendations for testing in vignettes by gender++/+++/n.a.
Drwecki et al., 2011 [38]USARacial biasVignettesRandomized experimental intervention study with vignettes varying by race (black/white)40NursesDifferences in pain treatment recommendations by racen.a.
Dy et al., 2015 [61]USARacial bias, gender biasVignettesCross-sectional computerized vignette study varied by race (black/white) and gender113PhysiciansNo difference in recommendations for surgery in vignettes for race and gender0
Enea-Drapeau et al., 2012 [62]FranceDisability biasIATCross-sectional study with 2 disability IAT (Trisomy 21, typical or weakly typical)55Health care providersImplicit negative bias against people with Trisomy 21n.a.
Fiscella et al., 2021 [63]USARacial biasIAT and vignettes Randomized field experiment with standardised patient (black/white) and race IAT (black/white)90PhysiciansNegative UB on race against black people in IAT. Physicians with stronger UB prescribed less frequently opioids to black patients and those with lower UB less frequently to white patients n.a.
Galli et al., 2015 [13]ItalyDisability biasIATCross-sectional study with disability IAT (wheelchair users/no wheelchair users) 45Physio-therapists, other health care providersNo UB against wheelchair users0
Gould et al., 2019 [64]USADisability biasIATCross-sectional study with disability IAT290Genetic counsellorsStrong negative UB against people with disability+++
Graetz et al., 2021 [65]USARacial bias, SES biasIAT and vignettes Prospective study with IAT on race (black/white) and SES (high/low) and case vignettes105Health care providersStrong negative UB against low-SES patients and a moderate negative bias on race (black people). No bias in vignettes+++/++/0
Guedj et al., 2021 [66]USARacial bias, weight bias IAT Cross-sectional online-study with weight IAT and 2 race IATs (black/white and Hispanic/white) 101PhysiciansStrong negative UB against black, Hispanic people and overweight people+++/+++/+++
Guillermo & Barre-Hemingway 2020 [67]USARacial biasVignettesCross-sectional study with randomized vignettes varying by race (black/white)116Health care providersNo race-based differences in pain estimates nor treatment recommendationsn.a.
Hagiwara et al., 2013, 2016, 2017 [68,69,70]USARacial biasIATRe-analysis of cross-sectional study with race IAT and of racially discordant medical interactions14PhysiciansWeak negative UB against black people. UB has influence in communi cation style of physicians and inter action when patients reported prior discrimination+
Haider et al., 2014 [71]USARacial bias, SES biasIAT and vignettesCross-sectional online study with race and SES IATs and vignettes varying by race (black/white) and SES (low/high)251PhysiciansModerate negative UB against black and strong negative UB against low SES people in IATs. No differences in treatment decisions in vignettes++/+++/0
Haider et al., 2015 [72] USARacial bias, SES biasIAT and vignettes Cross-sectional online study with race and SES IATs and vignettes varying by race (black/white) and SES (low/high)215PhysiciansModerate negative UB against black people, strong UB against people with low SES in IATs. Differences on treatment decisions by race in 3 out of 27 decisions in vignettes++/+++/n.a.
Haider et al., 2015 [73]USARacial bias, SES biasIAT and vignettes Cross-sectional online study with race and SES IATs and vignettes varying by race (back/white) and SES (high/low)245NursesModerate negative UB against black people and a strong negative UB against people with low SES in IATs. Differences in treatment decisions in vignettes according to race and SES++/+++/n.a.
Halvorson et al., 2019 [74]USAWeight biasIATCross-sectional mixed-methods study with weight IAT and semi-structured key informant interviews 28Physicians, nursesModerate to strong negative UB against overweight people++-+++
Hausmann et al., 2015 [75]USARacial biasIATCross-sectional online pilot study with race IAT (black/white)14PhysiciansModerate negative UB against black people++
Hirsh et al., 2015 [76]USARacial biasIAT and vignettes Cross-sectional study with race IAT and virtual human vignettes varying by race (black/white) and ambiguity of pain129PhysiciansModerate negative UB against black people in IAT. No difference in treat-ment options in vignettes with low ambiguity, with high ambiguity decisions varied for white patients not for black++/n.a.
Hirsh et al., 2020 [77]USARacial bias IAT and vignettesCross-sectional online-study with race IAT and virtual vignettes varying by race (black/white) and addiction history135PhysiciansModerate negative UB against black people in IAT. Differences in perceptions about patients’ risks for misuse/abuse by race and past opioid misuse in vignettes++/n.a.
Hirsh et al., 2014 [78]USAGender biasVignettesCross-sectional study with computer simulated patient vignettes varying by gender98Health care providersDifferences in treatment recommendations by gendern.a.
Hull et al., 2021 [79]USARacial biasVignettesCross-sectional online-study with vignette varying by race (black/white)174Health care providersNegative UB against black people in consultation and prescribing behaviour depending on perceived ability of adherencen.a.
Johnson et al., 2016 [80] and 2017 [81]USARacial biasIATCross-sectional study with repeated measures (pre- and post- shift) with 2 race IATs (black/ white) (adult and child versions)91PhysiciansModerate negative UB against black people both for adults and children pre- and post-shift++
Kopera et al., 2015 [32]PolandMental illness biasGNATCross-sectional study with GNAT on mental illness29Psychiatrists, psycho-therapistsNegative UB toward people with mental illnessn.a.
Lepièce et al., 2014 [82]BelgiumRacial biasVignettesCross-sectional vignette study varying by race (migrant status/ local)171PhysiciansNo differences in medical decisions by ethnicity except prescription of drugs0/n.a.
Liang et al., 2019 [83]USADiagnosis bias IAT Cross-sectional study with 2 IATs on prejudice and stereotypes on cervical cancer vs. ovarian cancer patients176Physicians, nursesWeak negative unconscious prejudice and stereotyping toward cervical cancer patients. Physicians had no UB while nurses did+/+/0/0
Londono Tobon et al., 2021 [84]USARacial biasIATCross-sectional online study with 3 race IATs (black/white) related to diagnosis, compliance, and medication171PsychiatristsWeak to moderate negative UB against black people on IATs on dia gnosis, compliance, and medication+-++/+-++/+-++
Lowe et al., 2020
[85]
USARacial bias(IAT) vignettesRe-analysis of cross-sectional study with 2 race IATs (black/white and Hispanic/white) of videotaped counselling sessions with simulated patients60Genetic counsellorsResults of IATs see Schaa et al., 2015 Slight difference in communication strategies according to race in vignettes no association between communication style and IATsn.a.
Moskowitz et al., 2012 [35]USARacial biasReaction time task with priming Cross-sectional study with computerized reaction time task with subliminal exposure to black/white photographs 11PhysiciansUnconscious association of certain diseases to black people compared to white peoplen.a.
Nash et al., 2014 [86]UKAgeismIATCross-sectional study with ageism IAT49NursesStrong negative UB towards elderly people+++
Nymo et al., 2018 [87]NorwaySES biasVignettesCross-sectional online-study with vignettes varying by SES (low/neutral).107PhysiciansDifference in priority of referrals by SES in one of 3 vignettes giving low-SES patients low priorityn.a.
Oliver et al., 2014 [88]USARacial biasIAT and vignettesCross-sectional online study with randomized vignettes varying by race (black/white). Race and cooperativeness IATs 543PhysiciansModerate negative UB against black people, weak negative UB on their cooperativeness in IATs. No difference in treatment decisions in vignettes according to race ++/+/0
Omori et al., 2012 [42]JapanMental illness biasIATRepeated-measure sstudy before and after contact with mentally ill patients with 2 IATs with different expressions for mental illness and association to “criminal”51PhysiciansNegative UB against people with mental illness (schizophrenia)n.a.
Patel et al., 2019 [89]ItalyRacial biasIAT and vignettesCross-sectional study with brief race IAT and randomized vignettes varying by race (black/white)57DentistsStrong negative UB against black people in IAT. Differences in recommendations for treatment options by race in vignettes+++/n.a.
Penner et al., 2016 [90]USARacial biasIATMixed-methods study with race IAT, recorded physician–patient interactions and follow-up interviews 18PhysiciansWeak negative UB against black people+
Puumala et al., 2016 [91]USARacial biasIAT and vignettes Cross-sectional online study with vignettes. Two race IATs (American Native/white), child and adult versions101Physicians, nursesModerate negative UB against American Native people on IAT on child and adult versions. Only little differences in treatment recommen-dations in vignettes++/++/n.a.
Robstad et al., 2018 [92]NorwayWeight biasIAT and vignettesCross-sectional online pilot study with 2 weight IATs on attitudes and stereotypes and vignettes varying by weight30NursesStrong negative UB on attitude and moderate on stereotypes against overweight people, no difference in behavioural intention in vignettes+++/++/0
Robstad et al., 2019 [93]NorwayWeight biasIAT and vignettesCross-sectional study with two weight IATs on attitudes and stereotypes and vignettes varying by weight159NursesStrong negative UB against overweight people in both IAT, no difference in behavioural intention in vignettes+++/+++/0
Rojas et al., 2017 [94]USARacial biasVignettes Cross-sectional online study with vignettes varying by race (black/white)342PhysiciansNo statistically significant differences in suspicion for abuse-related injury based on race of child0
Sabin & Greenwald 2012 [95]USARacial biasIAT and vignettesCross-sectional online study with 3 race IATs on attitude, compliance, and stereotypes and vignettes of child patients varying by race (black/white)86PhysiciansWeak negative UB on attitude, moderate on compliance and stereo type against black people on IATs. Differences in treatment recommen dations by race in 1 out of 4 vignettes+/++/++/n.a.
Sabin et al., 2012 [96]USA/GlobalWeight biasIATRe-analysis of data from weight IAT from Project Implicit (2006–2010) 2284PhysiciansStrong negative UB against overweight people+++
Sabin et al., 2015 [97]USAWeight bias, racial biasIATCross-sectional online pilot study with weight and race IATs on (Native American/white) 75Physicians, nurses, physician assistantsStrong negative UB against overweight people, weak negative UB against Native Americans+++/+
Sabin et al., 2015 [98]USALGBTQ biasIATRe-analysis of data from LGB IAT from Project Implicit (2006–2012)18,983Physicians, nurses, mental health providers, other health care providersWeak to moderate negative UB against homosexual people+-++
Sandhu et al., 2019 [99]CanadaMental illness biasIATCross-sectional online study with IAT on mental illness538PsychiatristsNo UB against people with mental illness0
Schaa et al., 2015 [100]USARacial biasIATMixed-methods study with cross-sectional online survey with IAT on race (black/white) and with re-analysis of data on patient–physician interaction60Genetic counsellorsModerate negative UB against black people. Differences in communi cation style during counselling sessions according to race++/n.a.
Schoenberg et al., 2019 [101]USARacial bias, gender biasVignettesCross-sectional online pilot study with vignettes varying by race (skin tone) and gender80PhysiciansTreatment options differed by skin colour and gender according to stereotypes n.a.
Schroyen et al., 2016 [102]BelgiumAgeismVignettesCross-sectional study with ran domized vignettes varying by age76NursesNegative UB by age that increases as age of patient increases n.a.
Setchell et al., 2014 [103]AustraliaWeight biasVignettesCross-sectional online survey with vignettes differing in body mass index265Physio-therapistsMinimal statistically not significant differences in treatment options by weight of patients 0
Shapiro et al., 2018 [36]USARacial bias, SES biasIAT and vignettesCross-sectional study with race and SES IATs and vignettes varying by race (black/white) and SES. 971/549/530 PhysiciansModerate negative UB against black women, strong negative UB against women with low SES in IATs. No differences of treatment recommen dations by race but by SES in vignettes++/+++/0/ n.a.
Siegelman et al., 2016 [104]USARacial biasIAT and vignettesCross-sectional study with race IAT and vignettes varying by race (black/white)57PhysiciansNo results for IAT published and some difference in pain treatment by race in vignettes n.a
Stepanikova 2012 [105]USARacial biasVignettesCross-sectional online vignette study with/without racial priming varying by race (black/Hispanic/ white) and cognitive load.81PhysiciansUB in diagnosis and referral against black people and less so towards Hispanic people under time pressure, less so if there is no time pressuren.a.
Sukhera et al., 2018 [106] and 2019 [17]CanadaMental illness biasIATCross-sectional mixed-methods study with IAT on dangerous ness of mental illness and semi-structured interviews about results31Psychiatrists, Nurses32% had a negative UB on dangerousness of mentally ill people and 55% on physically ill people, and 13% had no UBn.a.
Tajeu et al., 2018 [107]USARacial biasIATCross-sectional online-study with race IAT (black/white)107Physicians, nurses, other health care providersModerate negative UB against black people ++
Tucker Edmonds et al., 2017 [108]USARacial biasVignettesCross-sectional pilot study with self-administered vignettes varying by race (black/white)77Physicians, nursesDifferences in treatment options by racen.a.
Vaimberg et al., 2021 [40]USADisability biasIAT and vignettesRepeated measure online pilot study (pre- and post-intervention) with disability IAT (pre-intervention) and vignettes (with/without physical disability) pre- and post-intervention335Physicians, nurses, genetic counsellors, other health care providersNegative UB on disabled people (84% of respon-dents), UB influencing clinical recommen dations. After intervention UB decreased, recommendations changedn.a.
VanPuymbrouck et al., 2020 [109]GlobalDisability biasIAT Re-analysis of data from disability IAT from Project Implicit (2004–2017)25,006Physicians, dentists, nurses, occupational- and physio-therapists, other health care providersModerate negative UB against disabled people++
Walden et al., 2020 [110]USADisability biasIATCross-sectional study with disability IAT (stuttering/normal speech) 15Speech–language pathologistsModerate negative UB against people that stutter++
Wandner et al., 2014 [111]USARacial bias, gender bias, ageismVignettesCross-sectional online vignette study with human virtual profiles varying by race, gender, and age193Physicians, nursesDifferences in pain assessment by race and gender, not by age. Male and black people were rated to have more painn.a./0
Welch et al., 2012 [112]USAGender biasVignettesCross-sectional mixed-methods study with video vignette varying by gender with/without cognitive priming, think-aloud interviews256PhysiciansDifferences in treatment options by gendern.a.
Welch et al., 2015 [113]USAMental illness biasVignettesCross-sectional video-based vignette study varying by symptoms of mental and physical illness256PhysiciansNegative stereotypes against people with mental illness n.a.
Wijayatunga et al., 2021 [43]USAWeight biasIATRandomized online study with repeated measures (pre-/post-intervention, 1-month follow-up) with weight IAT taken 3 times147DieticiansNegative UB against overweight people at all 3 measurement pointsn.a.
Wittlin et al., 2019 [114]USALGBTQ biasIATLongitudinal study with 3 repeated measures with IAT on lesbian and gay people1155PhysiciansWeak negative UB against lesbian and gay people at both time points+/1
Zestcott et al., 2021 [115] USARacial biasIATCross-sectional online-study with 2 IAT on race (American Native/ white) on prejudice and stereotypes111PhysiciansModerate negative UB toward American Natives in attitudes and stereotypes++/++
Legend: + weak, +-++: weak to moderate, ++ moderate, ++-+++ moderate to strong, +++ strong unconscious bias in IAT; n.a., strength of bias not applicable; 0, no unconscious bias found.

3.4. Biases

Of the 81 studies included, 67 assessed one bias, 12 studies two biases, and 2 studies three biases. In total, 97 biases were investigated (see Table 2). Racial bias was assessed against African Americans or “black” people in 41 studies, against Hispanic or Latino Americans in 5 studies, and against Native American people in three studies relative to “white” people. Four studies assessed more than one race. One study [82] assessed the biases of local (European) general practitioners against individuals with migrant status compared to local people using a foreign (Moroccan) vs. a typical local (Belgian) name. No study investigated bias against Asian people or people from other racial or ethnic backgrounds. The studies on racial bias were conducted predominantly in the United States. Only two studies that addressed racial bias were conducted in Europe: one on race (black/white) [89] and one on migrants (migrant/local) [82].
Racial bias was assessed with 14 studies with implicit measures, in 16 studies with vignettes and in 16 studies with an implicit measure and vignettes. Amongst the 30 studies using an implicit measure, 28 used an IAT, and 1 each used a reaction time task [35] and the AMP [33]. All showed an unconscious racial bias against the minority group assessed, even though participants from minority groups might have had a lower unconscious bias, no unconscious bias, or an unconscious bias against white people (e.g., [36]). The 32 studies using a vignette showed in 22 cases a difference in treatment decisions or recommendations, while in ten cases there were no differences. In five studies, implicit measures found an unconscious bias while vignettes showed none.
While in almost all studies bias was against a minority, two studies found a bias against a majority: One study using the vignette method [108] found that white patients were more suspected to divert pain medication compared to black patients. Another vignette study [76] found that white patients received less consistent care when the vignette was manipulated in such a way that clinical ambiguity (pain etiology, congruence of facial expression, and pain report) was high.
The racial background of study participants reflected the composition of health professionals in the respective countries: In the United States, they were predominantly Caucasian/white with minorities of African American/black, Asian/Pacific Islander, Hispanic/Latino, and Native American people, as well as people identifying as mixed race. In the European studies, participants were Europeans.
The second-most studied bias was gender bias, with 11 studies, all of which used vignettes to assess gender bias. In addition, one study also used an IAT [60]. Nine studies showed an unconscious bias against women, while two studies found no differences according to gender in vignettes. The IAT showed a strong unconscious bias against women and a moderate negative unconscious bias for the stereotype of women and risk-taking.
Weight bias was examined in ten studies; seven studies used an IAT, one study used vignettes only, and two studies used both methods. Of the ten studies, eight showed an unconscious bias while two studies found no [53] or only a minimal [103] difference in treatment decisions. Six studies using an IAT showed a moderate to strong unconscious bias while two studies did not report on the strength of unconscious bias [43,47]. The vignette study showed only a minimal difference in treatment options [103], and of the two studies using an IAT and vignettes, one found a difference in treatment recommendation but not in treatment options between normal-weight patients and overweight patients. The other study found no difference between the two groups in treatment intentions [93], while both IAT studies showed a strong unconscious bias.
Socio-economic status (SES) bias was addressed in nine studies. Of these, seven studies used both an IAT and vignettes, while two studies were based on vignettes only. Of the seven studies using both an IAT and vignettes, all showed an unconscious bias in the IAT ranging from moderate to strong. In the vignettes, however, two studies did not find a difference in treatment decisions, and five did. Of the two studies which used vignettes, only one showed a difference in treatment decisions [87] and one did not find a difference in time spent on diagnosis or in accuracy [45].
Mental illness bias was addressed in seven studies using the general term “mental illness” three times, schizophrenia twice, depression one time, and the combination of mentally ill and dangerous once. Six studies assessed the unconscious bias against mentally ill people with an implicit measure: the IAT was used six times and the GNAT was used once. One study used vignettes. While five studies using an implicit measure found an unconscious bias, one did not [93]. The vignette study found an unconscious bias.
In seven studies, disability bias was examined. In three studies, the term “disability” was used, and one study each assessed the bias against people with Trisomy 21, people in wheelchairs, people with cleft lips, and people who stutter. In six studies an IAT was used. Of these, only one study found no unconscious bias (against wheelchair users) [13]. The study which used both an IAT and vignettes showed an unconscious bias in the IAT and a difference in recommendations according to disability [40].
Ageism was examined in four studies. The IAT was used once [86], showing a strong unconscious bias against elderly people. In three studies, vignettes were used, all of which found an implicit bias against the elderly, two of which showed differences in pain ratings [49,111], and one showed decreasing support for oncology treatment as the age of patients increased [102].
Little research was conducted on LGBTQ bias, which was assessed in two studies, both using the IAT and both showing a weak to moderate implicit bias against lesbian and gay people [98,114].
One study [83] tested whether there was a bias against certain diagnoses. For this study, they compared cervical cancer with ovarian cancer in two IATs. Both prejudice and stereotypes were found against patients with cervical cancer.
While most studies assessed unconscious bias towards adults, four studies tested unconscious bias against children. Two studies compared the IAT child version to the adult version. In one of these studies [91], two versions of the race IAT were compared using Caucasian and Hispanic pictures of adults for one test and pictures of children for the other. The other study [81] did the same using pictures of Caucasian and African American people. In both studies, there was an unconscious bias against Hispanic and African American people, but there was no significant difference for the child vs. adult versions of the IAT. The residual two studies used vignettes with children or youth. One study [95] assessed treatment recommendation using child vignettes and the other [67] used vignettes with adolescents to estimate pain ratings. In both studies, differences in treatment decisions between races were found. However, no comparisons to adults were made.

3.5. Geographical Distribution

Of the 81 studies included, 79 were from 15 different countries and two studies were global in scope and used large datasets from Project Implicit [116]. Most studies were conducted in North America (n = 60), followed by Europe (n = 13), Australia (n = 3), Asia (n = 2) and Africa (n = 1) (see Table 3).

3.6. Involved Health Professions

As Table 4 shows, the majority of studies involved physicians (n = 51), followed by nurses (n = 20). Other health professions were studied to a much lesser extent. They were not of focal interest but were specified as components under “health care providers” or “other health care providers”.
In Table 5, we combine the information on health profession with that on bias types. It shows that bias on race, weight, and disability were studied amongst a wider range of health professionals than other biases, such as ageism or gender bias.

4. Discussion

The current review retrieved 87 records from 81 studies on unconscious bias. The findings indicate that there are 87 studies from around the globe that addressed different types of biases and used different methods and samples. The results suggest that unconscious biases seem to be widespread amongst health professionals. Whereas almost all implicit measure studies observed implicit biases in health professionals (53 out of 55), 75% of vignette studies reported implicit biases (33 out of 44). The implicit biases thereby include race/ethnicity, gender, SES, age, mental illness, LGBTQ people, weight, and disability.
Most studies in this review examined and found racial biases. Even though racial bias was assessed in 46 studies, most of these studies examined racial bias against African American/black people compared to Caucasian/white people. Other races or ethnic groups have rarely been assessed. Therefore, it is unclear how widespread or how strong racial bias against other ethnic groups is and how this racial bias and stereotypes manifest themselves in other regions of the world. Apart from racial bias, other biases like gender bias, ageism, LGBTQ bias, disability bias, and diagnosis bias have received little attention, even though these biases concern a considerable part of the population.
For most biases, there have been too few studies to claim strong evidence. Moreover, it would be relevant to compare the size, patterns, and interactions of biases against groups and outcomes to better understand how they work and how they can be overcome. Some stereotypes might be more salient and more relevant than others or some stereotypes might be an indicator of further stereotypes. For instance, all studies in our review that assessed both SES bias and racial bias showed that the bias against low SES is greater than that against belonging to an ethnic minority [36,46,65,71,72,73]. In future research, the phenomenon of intersectionality should be given more attention. Intersectionality is when an individual belongs to more than one minority group and might be subject to several unconscious biases, e.g., being an African American woman. Even though 14 studies included in this review examined more than one bias, and in several studies, intersectionality might have been observed, the results are inconclusive as they either did not find or report on intersectionality. Therefore, further research on intersectionality is needed.
Considering where the research has been conducted, almost 75% of the studies in our review were conducted in the United States (n = 58). In contrast, the topic has been as widely examined by researchers in Europe or elsewhere in the world. Evidence in this review shows that there is also unconscious bias in other parts of the world. However, studies are too sparse and too heterogeneous concerning other biases to claim strong evidence. This is especially true because there are major structural and cultural differences between the regions, and the results are therefore not easily transferable to other countries or regions. More research is needed in other world regions to find out which unconscious biases prevail to what extent and how they can be addressed under the given circumstances to prevent adverse outcomes.
Although implicit biases were found in all health professions, most studies focused on physicians (n = 51) or nurses (n = 20), while other health professionals were only sporadically studied or subsumed under “health care providers” or “other health care providers”. Two studies in our review found differences between professions. Crapanzano and colleagues [59] observed that physicians in general had an unconscious bias towards mentally ill people, whereas psychiatrists did not. Moreover, Liang and colleagues [83] found that nurses had an unconscious bias towards patients with cervical cancer, while physicians did not. These findings suggest that further research on potential differences between health professions would be promising, as it might provide insights into the origins of implicit biases and their perpetuation. For example, it could help to identify differences in the socialization of the profession and perhaps the passing on of stereotypes and biases, or differences in professional training and practice that are relevant to biases. It would also be interesting to compare care models and differences in patient-centeredness, as well as institutional factors, in relation to unconscious bias to find out whether care practices and institutional settings can mitigate unconscious bias.
In this respect, the three intervention studies [33,38,40] that used empathy-including methods produced interesting results. They showed a reduction in implicit bias when health professionals were encouraged to consider the perspectives of their patients. Additionally, the study [41] that used real-time information exchange resulted in unbiased treatment recommendations. Further intervention studies should explore how unconscious bias can be reduced. Furthermore, evidence-based interventions should be part of any educational program for health professionals.
In conclusion, the present review confirms the persistence of an unconscious bias amongst health professionals that was observed in earlier research [10]. Health professionals seem to apply stereotypes to patients’ superficial characteristics. They then derive their expectations towards patients from those characteristics. These expectations in turn influence their choice of recommendations for treatments and prescriptions. In the study of Hull et al. [79], the expectation of the physicians that a black patient would adhere less to an HIV pre-exposure prophylaxis (PrEP) medication compared to a white patient led them to be less willing to discuss the prophylaxis in a consultation. In the study of Lund [53], no difference in treatment decisions was observed in a vignette study that manipulated body mass index and sex. However, general practitioners expected less compliance from obese male patients than obese female patients or normal-weight patients and therefore would not recommend a headache diary to male patients. In line with this assumed process, by which implicit biases impact expectations and behavior, are the results of Cooper et al. [39] who investigated compliance stereotyping in a race and compliance IAT. They found that while black people were strongly associated with words like reluctant, apathetic, and lax, white people were more strongly associated with words like willing and reliable, thus depicting the latter as more adherent. The same study also found a link between implicit bias and communication style. The greater the implicit bias, the greater the verbal dominance in recorded visits of black patients, and the more negative ratings from patients compared to white patients. These findings can also be seen as evidence of how unconscious stereotypes form expectations, and of how they translate into communication and possible consequences for patients.
We found that multiple disciplines have contributed to this area of research. Often, these disciplines do not recognize each other, as has been pointed out in an earlier review by Fitzgerald and Hurst [10]. Furthermore, different terminologies are used to discuss the same or very similar concepts, such as “attitudes”, “stigmatizing attitudes”, “prejudice”, “stereotypes”, or “bias”, among others. When reading the abstract, it was often unclear what phenomenon was being studied and how it was operationalized. It was therefore difficult to conclude whether it was an unconscious or explicit bias that was being studied. In addition, many articles lacked a definition of terminology or concepts used, which makes it difficult to compile previous research results on unconscious bias. More uniform definitions would facilitate the synopsis of evidence and the progression of the research field.
We found that there were many different approaches to exploring conscious and unconscious bias. For this review, many studies had to be excluded as their used measures used did not consider unconscious bias. Rather, explicit bias and structural discrimination were measured, or explicit and implicit bias could not be distinguished. In this review, we included only empirical studies that used instruments that to our knowledge can measure unconscious bias. This was either vignette studies or implicit measures.
Regarding implicit measures, 55 out of 81 studies included in this review used an implicit measure. The most often (n = 52) used approach was the “Implicit Association Test” (IAT) [18]. Other implicit measures, each used once, were the Go/No-Go Association Task (GNAT) [19], the Misattribution Procedure [34], and a reaction time task. These have the advantage of internal validity but have been criticized for their lack of external validity [117]. The IAT has shown validity in predicting bias (prejudice and stereotyping) more accurately than self-reports [118]. However, there is a debate regarding whether these implicit measures can predict behavior [119,120,121] and that in situations in which deliberate decisions are made, explicit measures of attitudes and behavioral intentions are more reliable than implicit measures, which in turn are better at predicting spontaneous reactions [122]. This might explain the discrepancy between the findings of unconscious bias using implicit measures and vignettes. In the present review, studies that used an IAT and vignettes arrived in 1/3 of cases at contradictory results. Both deliberate as well as spontaneous decisions are common in the daily life of health professionals.
Regarding vignette studies, they manipulate the variable of relevance, such as race or gender, and measure responses to fictional patients, while the rest of the portrayed clinical case is identical. This is a rather indirect approach to measure unconscious bias and may seem imprecise. However, the method has external validity, as vignettes are frequently used in the training of health professionals and in patient case descriptions, and may be similar to case reports in medical practice [27]. The results of these vignette studies included in this review are somewhat complex and heterogeneous because the study design often provided several different vignettes or vignettes with multiple options to choose from. In total, 33 out of 44 studies showed that a change in one variable in a vignette, such as gender or weight, leads to at least some differences in judgements, treatment recommendations, and decisions, some of which were considered as unsafe for the patient (e.g., [41]). Other differences were minor, such as the communication methods used (e.g., [85]). In many cases, there was no difference at all. These differences may reflect real unconscious biases but may also reflect learned practices built on stereotypes. However, tests with vignettes are a good opportunity to compare, reflect on, and question attitudes, stereotypes, intentions, and treatment routines.
Future research should address the differences between the different measures and their distinct predictability in greater detail and find out which situations in the everyday life of health professionals are particularly prone to unconscious bias.
This review has several limitations. It only covers the period of 2011 to 2021, and studies published earlier were not included in this review. Thereby, we may have excluded studies that would have provided more conclusive insight into the unconscious bias in other health professions and regions, or which might have given more insight on those biases studied only seldomly (e.g., ageism). However, as the present review builds on the one by Fitzgerald et al. [10], prior research is indirectly considered in our discussion.
Because we included only studies that, to our knowledge, measured unconscious bias, we had to exclude a great number of studies. These included studies with a qualitative approach and studies in which explicit and unconscious bias could not be separated. These studies might have provided more insight into how unconscious bias is perceived by patients or how health professionals try to mitigate their unconscious bias. The focus of our review, however, was different. A qualitative review could provide further insights in this regard to better understand the mechanisms and consequences of unconscious biases. However, we encounter the difficulty that implicit and explicit bias are two distinct phenomena that must be considered separately from each other.
Publication bias most likely occurred amongst studies evaluating unconscious bias in health professionals. Studies that found unconscious bias in health professionals might have been more likely to be published than if their results were inconclusive or indifferent. Therefore, it is difficult to estimate the prevalence of unconscious bias among health professionals. The combined results show that unconscious bias is widespread among health professionals.
As we did not systematically assess the quality of the studies in our review, the quality of the included studies should be interpreted with caution. In some cases, studies did not provide results for the IAT with the d-scores suggested by Greenwald and colleagues [123]. It is, therefore, difficult to compare or compile the results. More rigor in the reporting of results would be beneficial for the research field.
This review shows that, apart from in the United States, there has been only little research on unconscious bias. Given that most studies have been conducted in the United States, the transferability to Europe or other regions of the world is limited due to cultural differences concerning minorities and the awareness of health disparities and unconscious bias, in addition to differences in the health care systems and society. This is particularly the case with racial bias, which has been investigated almost exclusively in the United States in the context of black people in comparison to white people. Research on other minorities or bias towards immigrants, asylum seekers, and refugees is very sparse. It would be important to learn which minority groups are specifically concerned in which country, how different biases are generally associated with each other, and how they are linked within health care professionals.
A further research gap lays in the type of biases assessed: research on gender bias, SES bias, disability bias, ageism, and weight bias is sparse, even though these biases concern most societies around the world.
The unconscious bias of physicians and nurses has been studied most frequently. Physical and occupational therapists, midwives, dieticians, physician associates, pharmacists, and other health care professionals are other decision makers in terms of patient care. Therefore, these professionals should also be included in further studies of unconscious bias.
In this review, only five studies tested an intervention. Further studies are needed to better understand how unconscious bias can be mitigated and to ensure good health care for all people.

5. Conclusions

The majority of health professionals assessed in the studies included in this review have an unconscious bias against certain groups of patients. There are some challenges in the conceptualization of unconscious bias, the terminology used, and the use of instruments. Despite these challenges, methods to examine unconscious bias are available and can be used. The relationship between unconscious bias and health disparities remains somewhat unclear. There are considerable research gaps, with little research conducted outside the United States and on other biases besides racial bias. Additionally, health care professionals other than physicians and nurses have been seldom studied. Given the pervasiveness of unconscious biases, and the potential discrimination that might arise as a consequence, more research on the consequences and generalizability of biases, the emergence processes, and interventions to reduce unconscious biases is strongly called for.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, U.M., T.B. and A.G.-G.; methodology, U.M.; formal analysis, U.M., G.D. and T.B.; writing—original draft preparation, U.M. and G.D.; writing—review and editing, T.B., F.W. and A.G.-G.; visualization, U.M. and F.W.; supervision, F.W. and A.G.-G.; project administration, U.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created or analyzed in this study. Data sharing is not applicable to this article.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Michael J. Shanahan and Matthew Kerry-Krause for revision of the article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Search strategy.
Table A1. Search strategy.
Component/DatabasePubMed/MEDLINECINAHLPsycINFO and PsycARTICLESAMED
Population“Health personnel”[MESH] OR “Health personnel” OR “Health professional*” OR “Health personnel” OR Nurs* OR Physiotherap* OR “Physical therap*” OR “Occupational Therap*” OR Midwif* OR “Midwifery” [MESH] OR Logoped* OR “Speech therap*” OR Dietician* OR Paramedic* OR “Medical orderl*” OR Doctor* OR Osteopath*(MH “Health Personnel+”) OR “Health professional*” OR “Health personnel” OR Nurs* OR Physiotherap* OR “Physical Therap*” OR “Occupational therap*” OR Midwife* OR Logoped* OR “Speech Therap*” OR Dietician* OR Paramedic* OR “Medical orderly*” OR Physician* OR Doctor* OR Osteopath*exp clinicians/or exp health personnel/or exp therapists/OR Health personnel OR Health Professional* OR Nurs* OR Physiotherap* OR Occupational therap* OR Midwife* OR Logoped* OR Speech therap* OR Dietician* OR Paramedic* OR Medical orderly* OR Physician* OR Doctor* OR Osteopath* exp health personnel OR Health Professional*.mp OR Nurs*.mp OR Physiotherap*.mp OR Physical therap*.mp” OR Occupational therap*.mp OR Midwife*.mp OR Logoped*.mp OR Speech therap*.mp OR Dietician*.mp OR Paramedic*.mp OR Medical orderly*.mp OR Physician*.mp OR Doctor*-mp OR Osteopath*.mp
(mp=abstract, heading words, title)
ANDANDANDAND
Concept (Phenomenon of Interest): Unconscious Bias/Implicit Bias“Prejudice”[Mesh] OR “Stereotyping”[Mesh] OR “Social Discrimination”[Mesh] OR “unconscious Bias” OR “Implicit Bias” OR “Bias, implicit” OR “automatic bias” OR stereotyp* OR Prejudice*(MM “Race Relations”) OR (MM “Attitude to AIDS”) OR (MM “Race Relations”) (MM “Prejudice+”) OR (MM “Attitude to ageing”) OR (MM “Attitude to disability”) OR (MM “Attitude to sexuality”) OR (MM “Attitude to obesity”) OR (MM “Cultural bias”) OR (MM “Cultural sensitivity”) OR (MM “Gender bias”) OR (MM “Attitude to mental illness”) (MM “Unconscious (Psychology))” OR (MM “Stereotyping”) OR (MM “Discrimination+”) OR “Implicit bias” OR “Unconscious bias”exp *Implicit attitudes or exp *Prejudice or exp *Racial and ethnic attitudes or exp *Racial bias OR exp *Aged (attitudes toward) *Obesity (attitudes toward) OR exp *Disabled (attitudes toward) or exp *Health personnel attitudes or exp *physical illness (attitudes toward) or exp *Sexual attitudes OR “Unconscious bias”exp Attitude of health personnel OR Exp Attitude to disability OR Exp Prejudice OR Implicit bias.mp OR Gender bias.mp OR Weight bias.mp OR Obesity bias.mp OR Ageism.mp OR Racial bias.mp OR Racial attitude.mp OR Health status disparities.mp (mp=abstract, heading words, title)
ANDANDANDAND
Context: Health Care Delivery“Delivery of Health Care”[Mesh] OR “Health Services”[Mesh] OR “Delivery of healthcare” OR “health care service*”(MH “Health Care Delivery+”) OR (MH “Patient Care+”) OR (MH “Quality of Health Care+”) OR (MH “Professional-Patient Relations+”) OR “delivery of health care”exp Treatment or “Delivery of health care” or “delivery of healthcare”.mp. [mp=ti, ab, hw, tc, id, ot, tm, mf, tx, ct]exp “Delivery of health care”/or exp Health services/or exp Patient care/OR Health Care delivery.mp
Filter1 January 2011–31 December 2021 1 January 2011–31 December 2021
without Medline records and peer-reviewed
1 January 2011–31 December 2021 peer-reviewed1 January 2011–31 December 2021
*: symblol for truncation

References

  1. Maina, I.W.; Belton, T.D.; Ginzberg, S.; Singh, A.; Johnson, T.J. A Decade of Studying Implicit Racial/Ethnic Bias in Healthcare Providers Using the Implicit Association Test. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018, 199, 219–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. James, S.A. The Strangest of All Encounters: Racial and Ethnic Discrimination in US Health Care. Cad. Saúde Pública 2017, 33 (Suppl. S1), e00104416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Ho, I.K.; Lawrence, J.S. The Role of Social Cognition in Medical Decision Making with Asian American Patients. J. Racial Ethn. Health Disparities 2021, 8, 1112–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Artiga, S.; Orgera, K.; Pham, O. Disparities in Health and Health Care: Five Key Questions and Answers. Kais. Fam. Found. Policy Brief. 2020. Available online: https://files.kff.org/attachment/Issue-Brief-Disparities-in-Health-and-Health-Care-Five-Key-Questions-and-Answers (accessed on 8 May 2023).
  5. Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Institutional and Policy-Level Strategies for Increasing the Diversity of the US Health Care Workforce, Board on Health Sciences Policy. In The Nation’s Compelling Interest: Ensuring Diversity in the Health-Care Workforce; Smedley, B.D.; Butler, A.S.; Bristow, L.R. (Eds.) National Academies Press (US): Washington, DC, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  6. Nelson, A. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Health Care. J. Natl. Med. Assoc. 2002, 94, 666. [Google Scholar]
  7. Green, A.R.; Carney, D.R.; Pallin, D.J.; Ngo, L.H.; Raymond, K.L.; Iezzoni, L.I.; Banaji, M.R. Implicit Bias among Physicians and Its Prediction of Thrombolysis Decisions for Black and White Patients. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2007, 22, 1231–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Marcelin, J.R.; Siraj, D.S.; Victor, R.; Kotadia, S.; Maldonado, Y.A. The Impact of Unconscious Bias in Healthcare: How to Recognize and Mitigate It. J. Infect. Dis. 2019, 220, S62–S73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Groves, P.S.; Bunch, J.L.; Sabin, J.A. Nurse Bias and Nursing Care Disparities Related to Patient Characteristics: A Scoping Review of the Quantitative and Qualitative Evidence. J. Clin. Nurs. 2021, 30, 3385–3397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. FitzGerald, C.; Hurst, S. Implicit Bias in Healthcare Professionals: A Systematic Review. BMC Med. Ethics 2017, 18, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Banaji, M.R.; Greenwald, A.G. Blindspot: Hidden Biases of Good People; Bantam: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  12. Macrae, C.N.; Milne, A.B.; Bodenhausen, G.V. Stereotypes as Energy-Saving Devices: A Peek inside the Cognitive Toolbox. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 66, 37–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Galli, G.; Lenggenhager, B.; Scivoletto, G.; Molinari, M.; Pazzaglia, M. Don’t Look at My Wheelchair! The Plasticity of Longlasting Prejudice. Med. Educ. 2015, 49, 1239–1247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Fiske, S.T. Stereotyping, Prejudice, and Discrimination. In The Handbook of Social Psychology, 4th ed.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1998; Volume 1–2, pp. 357–411. ISBN 978-0-19-521376-8. [Google Scholar]
  15. APA Dictionary of Psychology. Available online: https://dictionary.apa.org/ (accessed on 8 May 2023).
  16. Hall, W.J.; Chapman, M.V.; Lee, K.M.; Merino, Y.M.; Thomas, T.W.; Payne, B.K.; Eng, E.; Day, S.H.; Coyne-Beasley, T. Implicit Racial/Ethnic Bias Among Health Care Professionals and Its Influence on Health Care Outcomes: A Systematic Review. Am. J. Public Health 2015, 105, e60–e76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Sukhera, J.; Wodzinski, M.; Milne, A.; Teunissen, P.W.; Lingard, L.; Watling, C. Implicit Bias and the Feedback Paradox: Exploring How Health Professionals Engage With Feedback While Questioning Its Credibility. Acad. Med. 2019, 94, 1204–1210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Greenwald, A.G.; McGhee, D.E.; Schwartz, J.L.K. Measuring Individual Differences in Implicit Cognition: The Implicit Association Test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 74, 1464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Nosek, B.A.; Banaji, M.R. The Go/No-Go Association Task. Soc. Cogn. 2001, 19, 625–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Burgess, D.J.; Phelan, S.; Workman, M.; Hagel, E.; Nelson, D.B.; Fu, S.S.; Widome, R.; van Ryn, M. The Effect of Cognitive Load and Patient Race on Physicians’ Decisions to Prescribe Opioids for Chronic Low Back Pain: A Randomized Trial. Pain Med. 2014, 15, 965–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Munn, Z.; Peters, M.D.J.; Stern, C.; Tufanaru, C.; McArthur, A.; Aromataris, E. Systematic Review or Scoping Review? Guidance for Authors When Choosing between a Systematic or Scoping Review Approach. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2018, 18, 143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Peters, M.D.J.; Marnie, C.; Tricco, A.C.; Pollock, D.; Munn, Z.; Alexander, L.; McInerney, P.; Godfrey, C.M.; Khalil, H. Updated Methodological Guidance for the Conduct of Scoping Reviews. JBI Evid. Implement. 2021, 19, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Meidert, U.; Dönnges, G.; Bucher, T. Unconscous Bias in Health Professionals a Systematic Review. OSF Cent. Open Sci. 2022. Available online: https://osf.io/gjr9z/?view_only=a34d0ee2685b447eaad94b107928355f (accessed on 8 May 2023).
  24. Gawronski, B.; Hahn, A. Implicit Measures: Procedures, Use, and Interpretation. In Measurement in Social Psychology; Routledge: London, UK, 2018; pp. 29–55. [Google Scholar]
  25. Gawronski, B.; De Houwer, J. Implicit Measures in Social and Personality Psychology. In Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology; Reis, H.T., Judd, C.M., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 283–310. ISBN 978-0-511-99648-1. [Google Scholar]
  26. Gould, D. Using Vignettes to Collect Data for Nursing Research Studies: How Valid Are the Findings? J. Clin. Nurs. 1996, 5, 207–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Evans, S.C.; Roberts, M.C.; Keeley, J.W.; Blossom, J.B.; Amaro, C.M.; Garcia, A.M.; Stough, C.O.; Canter, K.S.; Robles, R.; Reed, G.M. Vignette Methodologies for Studying Clinicians’ Decision-Making: Validity, Utility, and Application in ICD-11 Field Studies. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2015, 15, 160–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. McGowan, J.; Sampson, M.; Salzwedel, D.M.; Cogo, E.; Foerster, V.; Lefebvre, C. PRESS Peer Review of Electronic Search Strategies: 2015 Guideline Statement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2016, 75, 40–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Corporation for Digital Scholarship Zotero. Available online: https://www.zotero.org/ (accessed on 8 May 2023).
  30. Veritas Health Innovation. Covidence Systematic Review Software. Available online: www.covidence.org (accessed on 8 May 2023).
  31. Cassell, N. Medical Treatment Decisions: Impact of Source of Motivation to Control Prejudice; University of Maryland, Baltimore County: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kopera, M.; Suszek, H.; Bonar, E.; Myszka, M.; Gmaj, B.; Ilgen, M.; Wojnar, M. Evaluating Explicit and Implicit Stigma of Mental Illness in Mental Health Professionals and Medical Students. Community Ment. Health J. 2015, 51, 628–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Chapman, M.V.; Hall, W.J.; Lee, K.; Colby, R.; Coyne-Beasley, T.; Day, S.; Eng, E.; Lightfoot, A.F.; Merino, Y.; Simán, F.M.; et al. Making a Difference in Medical Trainees’ Attitudes toward Latino Patients: A Pilot Study of an Intervention to Modify Implicit and Explicit Attitudes. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018, 199, 202–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Payne, B.K.; Cheng, C.M.; Govorun, O.; Stewart, B.D. An Inkblot for Attitudes: Affect Misattribution as Implicit Measurement. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 89, 277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Moskowitz, G.B.; Stone, J.; Childs, A. Implicit Stereotyping and Medical Decisions: Unconscious Stereotype Activation in Practitioners’ Thoughts about African Americans. Am. J. Public Health 2012, 102, 996–1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Shapiro, N.; Wachtel, E.V.; Bailey, S.M.; Espiritu, M.M. Implicit Physician Biases in Periviability Counseling. J. Pediatr. 2018, 197, 109–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Blair, I.V.; Steiner, J.F.; Hanratty, R.; Price, D.W.; Fairclough, D.L.; Daugherty, S.L.; Bronsert, M.; Magid, D.J.; Havranek, E.P. An Investigation of Associations between Clinicians’ Ethnic or Racial Bias and Hypertension Treatment, Medication Adherence and Blood Pressure Control. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2014, 29, 987–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Drwecki, B.B.; Moore, C.F.; Ward, S.E.; Prkachin, K.M. Reducing Racial Disparities in Pain Treatment: The Role of Empathy and Perspective-Taking. Pain 2011, 152, 1001–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Cooper, L.A.; Roter, D.L.; Carson, K.A.; Beach, M.C.; Sabin, J.A.; Greenwald, A.G.; Inui, T.S. The Associations of Clinicians’ Implicit Attitudes about Race with Medical Visit Communication and Patient Ratings of Interpersonal Care. Am. J. Public. Health 2012, 102, 979–987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Vaimberg, E.; Demers, L.; Ford, E.; Sabatello, M.; Stevens, B.; Dasgupta, S. Project Inclusive Genetics: Exploring the Impact of Patient-Centered Counseling Training on Physical Disability Bias in the Prenatal Setting. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0255722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Centola, D.; Guilbeault, D.; Sarkar, U.; Khoong, E.; Zhang, J. The Reduction of Race and Gender Bias in Clinical Treatment Recommendations Using Clinician Peer Networks in an Experimental Setting. Nat. Commun. 2021, 12, 6585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Omori, A.; Tateno, A.; Ideno, T.; Takahashi, H.; Kawashima, Y.; Takemura, K.; Okubo, Y. Influence of Contact with Schizophrenia on Implicit Attitudes towards Schizophrenia Patients Held by Clinical Residents. BMC Psychiatry 2012, 12, 205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Wijayatunga, N.N.; Bailey, D.; Klobodu, S.S.; Dawson, J.A.; Knight, K.; Dhurandhar, E.J. A Short, Attribution Theory-Based Video Intervention Does Not Reduce Weight Bias in a Nationally Representative Sample of Registered Dietitians: A Randomized Trial. Int. J. Obes. 2021, 45, 787–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Afulani, P.A.; Ogolla, B.A.; Oboke, E.N.; Ongeri, L.; Weiss, S.J.; Lyndon, A.; Mendes, W.B. Understanding Disparities in Person-Centred Maternity Care: The Potential Role of Provider Implicit and Explicit Bias. Health Policy Plan. 2021, 36, 298–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Al Alwan, I.; Magzoub, M.E.; Al Haqwi, A.; Badri, M.; Al Yousif, S.M.; Babiker, A.; Mamede, S.; Schmidt, H.G. Do Poor Patients Suffer from Inaccurate Diagnoses More than Well-to-Do Patients? A Randomized Control Trial. BMC Med. Educ. 2019, 19, 386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Anastas, T.M.; Miller, M.M.; Hollingshead, N.A.; Stewart, J.C.; Rand, K.L.; Hirsh, A.T. The Unique and Interactive Effects of Patient Race, Patient Socioeconomic Status, and Provider Attitudes on Chronic Pain Care Decisions. Ann. Behav. Med. 2020, 54, 771–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Aweidah, L.; Robinson, J.; Cumming, S.; Lewis, S. Australian Diagnostic Radiographers’ Attitudes and Perceptions of Imaging Obese Patients: A Study of Self, Peers and Students. Radiography 2016, 22, e258–e263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Barnato, A.E.; Mohan, D.; Downs, J.; Bryce, C.L.; Angus, D.C.; Arnold, R.M. A Randomized Trial of the Effect of Patient Race on Physicians’ Intensive Care Unit and Life-Sustaining Treatment Decisions for an Acutely Unstable Elder with End-Stage Cancer. Crit. Care Med. 2011, 39, 1663–1669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Bartley, E.J.; Boissoneault, J.; Vargovich, A.M.; Wandner, L.D.; Hirsh, A.T.; Lok, B.C.; Heft, M.W.; Robinson, M.E. The Influence of Health Care Professional Characteristics on Pain Management Decisions. Pain Med. 2015, 16, 99–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Bernardes, S.F.; Lima, M.L. A Contextual Approach on Sex-Related Biases in Pain Judgements: The Moderator Effects of Evidence of Pathology and Patients’ Distress Cues on Nurses’ Judgements of Chronic Low-Back Pain. Psychol. Health 2011, 26, 1642–1658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Blair, I.V.; Havranek, E.P.; Price, D.W.; Hanratty, R.; Fairclough, D.L.; Farley, T.; Hirsh, H.K.; Steiner, J.F. Assessment of Biases against Latinos and African Americans among Primary Care Providers and Community Members. Am. J. Public Health 2013, 103, 92–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Blair, I.V.; Steiner, J.F.; Fairclough, D.L.; Hanratty, R.; Price, D.W.; Hirsh, H.K.; Wright, L.A.; Bronsert, M.; Karimkhani, E.; Magid, D.J.; et al. Clinicians’ Implicit Ethnic/Racial Bias and Perceptions of Care among Black and Latino Patients. Ann. Fam Med. 2013, 11, 43–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Bøker Lund, T.; Brodersen, J.; Sandøe, P. A Study of Anti-Fat Bias among Danish General Practitioners and Whether This Bias and General Practitioners’ Lifestyle Can Affect Treatment of Tension Headache in Patients with Obesity. Obes. Facts 2018, 11, 501–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Bous, R.M.; Lyamichev, A.; Kmentt, A.; Valiathan, M. Bias in a Blink: Shedding Light on Implicit Attitudes toward Patients with a Cleft Lip. Am. J. Orthod. Dentofac. Orthop. 2021, 160, 200–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Breathett, K.; Yee, E.; Pool, N.; Hebdon, M.; Crist, J.D.; Knapp, S.; Larsen, A.; Solola, S.; Luy, L.; Herrera-Theut, K.; et al. Does Race Influence Decision Making for Advanced Heart Failure Therapies? J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2019, 8, e013592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Brener, L.; Wilson, H.; Rose, G.; Mackenzie, A.; de Wit, J. Challenging Stereotypes and Changing Attitudes: Improving Quality of Care for People with Hepatitis C through Positive Speakers Programs. Psychol. Health Med. 2013, 18, 242–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Claréus, B.; Renström, E.A. Physicians’ Gender Bias in the Diagnostic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms and Its Effect on Patient-Physician Relations. Scand. J. Psychol. 2019, 60, 338–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Colón-Emeric, C.S.; Corazzini, K.; McConnell, E.; Pan, W.; Toles, M.; Hall, R.; Batchelor-Murphy, M.; Yap, T.L.; Anderson, A.L.; Burd, A.; et al. Study of Individualization and Bias in Nursing Home Fall Prevention Practices. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2017, 65, 815–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Crapanzano, K.; Fisher, D.; Hammarlund, R.; Hsieh, E.P.; May, W. An Exploration of Residents’ Implicit Biases Towards Depression—A Pilot Study. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2018, 33, 2065–2069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Daugherty, S.L.; Blair, I.V.; Havranek, E.P.; Furniss, A.; Dickinson, L.M.; Karimkhani, E.; Main, D.S.; Masoudi, F.A. Implicit Gender Bias and the Use of Cardiovascular Tests Among Cardiologists. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2017, 6, e006872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Dy, C.J.; Lyman, S.; Boutin-Foster, C.; Felix, K.; Kang, Y.; Parks, M.L. Do Patient Race and Sex Change Surgeon Recommendations for TKA? Clin. Orthop. Relat. Res. 2015, 473, 410–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Enea-Drapeau, C.; Carlier, M.; Huguet, P. Tracking Subtle Stereotypes of Children with Trisomy 21: From Facial-Feature-Based to Implicit Stereotyping. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e34369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Fiscella, K.; Epstein, R.M.; Griggs, J.J.; Marshall, M.M.; Shields, C.G. Is Physician Implicit Bias Associated with Differences in Care by Patient Race for Metastatic Cancer-Related Pain? PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0257794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Gould, H.; Hashmi, S.S.; Wagner, V.F.; Stoll, K.; Ostermaier, K.; Czerwinski, J. Examining Genetic Counselors’ Implicit Attitudes toward Disability. J. Genet. Couns. 2019, 28, 1098–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Graetz, D.E.; Madni, A.; Gossett, J.; Kang, G.; Sabin, J.A.; Santana, V.M.; Russo, C.L. Role of Implicit Bias in Pediatric Cancer Clinical Trials and Enrollment Recommendations among Pediatric Oncology Providers. Cancer 2021, 127, 284–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Guedj, R.; Marini, M.; Kossowsky, J.; Berde, C.; Mateo, C.; Fleegler, E.W. Explicit and Implicit Bias Based on Race, Ethnicity, and Weight among Pediatric Emergency Physicians. Acad. Emerg. Med. 2021, 28, 1073–1076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Guillermo, S.; Barre-Hemingway, M. Pain Perception and Treatment for Adolescents in Racially Concordant Versus Discordant Patient–Provider Scenarios. J. Adolesc. Health 2020, 66, 589–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Hagiwara, N.; Penner, L.A.; Gonzalez, R.; Eggly, S.; Dovidio, J.F.; Gaertner, S.L.; West, T.; Albrecht, T.L. Racial Attitudes, Physician-Patient Talk Time Ratio, and Adherence in Racially Discordant Medical Interactions. Soc. Sci Med. 2013, 87, 123–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Hagiwara, N.; Slatcher, R.B.; Eggly, S.; Penner, L.A. Physician Racial Bias and Word Use during Racially Discordant Medical Interactions. Health Commun. 2017, 32, 401–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Hagiwara, N.; Dovidio, J.F.; Eggly, S.; Penner, L.A. The Effects of Racial Attitudes on Affect and Engagement in Racially Discordant Medical Interactions between Non-Black Physicians and Black Patients. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 2016, 19, 509–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Haider, A.H.; Schneider, E.B.; Sriram, N.; Dossick, D.S.; Scott, V.K.; Swoboda, S.M.; Losonczy, L.; Haut, E.R.; Efron, D.T.; Pronovost, P.J.; et al. Unconscious Race and Class Bias: Its Association with Decision Making by Trauma and Acute Care Surgeons. J. Trauma Acute Care Surg. 2014, 77, 409–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Haider, A.H.; Schneider, E.B.; Sriram, N.; Dossick, D.S.; Scott, V.K.; Swoboda, S.M.; Losonczy, L.; Haut, E.R.; Efron, D.T.; Pronovost, P.J.; et al. Unconscious Race and Social Class Bias among Acute Care Surgical Clinicians and Clinical Treatment Decisions. JAMA Surg. 2015, 150, 457–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Haider, A.H.; Schneider, E.B.; Sriram, N.; Scott, V.K.; Swoboda, S.M.; Zogg, C.K.; Dhiman, N.; Haut, E.R.; Efron, D.T.; Pronovost, P.J.; et al. Unconscious Race and Class Biases among Registered Nurses: Vignette-Based Study Using Implicit Association Testing. J. Am. Coll. Surg. 2015, 220, 1077–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Halvorson, E.E.; Curley, T.; Wright, M.; Skelton, J.A. Weight Bias in Pediatric Inpatient Care. Acad. Pediatr. 2019, 19, 780–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Hausmann, L.R.M.; Myaskovsky, L.; Niyonkuru, C.; Oyster, M.L.; Switzer, G.E.; Burkitt, K.H.; Fine, M.J.; Gao, S.; Boninger, M.L. Examining Implicit Bias of Physicians Who Care for Individuals with Spinal Cord Injury: A Pilot Study and Future Directions. J. Spinal Cord Med. 2015, 38, 102–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Hirsh, A.T.; Hollingshead, N.A.; Ashburn-Nardo, L.; Kroenke, K. The Interaction of Patient Race, Provider Bias, and Clinical Ambiguity on Pain Management Decisions. J. Pain 2015, 16, 558–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Hirsh, A.T.; Anastas, T.M.; Miller, M.M.; Quinn, P.D.; Kroenke, K. Patient Race and Opioid Misuse History Influence Provider Risk Perceptions for Future Opioid-Related Problems. Am. Psychol. 2020, 75, 784–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Hirsh, A.T.; Hollingshead, N.A.; Matthias, M.S.; Bair, M.J.; Kroenke, K. The Influence of Patient Sex, Provider Sex, and Sexist Attitudes on Pain Treatment Decisions. J. Pain 2014, 15, 551–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Hull, S.J.; Tessema, H.; Thuku, J.; Scott, R.K. Providers PrEP: Identifying Primary Health Care Providers’ Biases as Barriers to Provision of Equitable PrEP Services. J. Acquir. Immune Defic. Syndr. 2021, 88, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Johnson, T.J.; Hickey, R.W.; Switzer, G.E.; Miller, E.; Winger, D.G.; Nguyen, M.; Saladino, R.A.; Hausmann, L.R.M. The Impact of Cognitive Stressors in the Emergency Department on Physician Implicit Racial Bias. Acad. Emerg. Med. 2016, 23, 297–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Johnson, T.J.; Winger, D.G.; Hickey, R.W.; Switzer, G.E.; Miller, E.; Nguyen, M.B.; Saladino, R.A.; Hausmann, L.R.M. Comparison of Physician Implicit Racial Bias Toward Adults Versus Children. Acad. Pediatr. 2017, 17, 120–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Lepièce, B.; Reynaert, C.; van Meerbeeck, P.; Lorant, V. General Practice and Ethnicity: An Experimental Study of Doctoring. BMC Fam. Pract. 2014, 15, 89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Liang, J.; Wolsiefer, K.; Zestcott, C.A.; Chase, D.; Stone, J. Implicit Bias toward Cervical Cancer: Provider and Training Differences. Gynecol. Oncol. 2019, 153, 80–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Londono Tobon, A.; Flores, J.M.; Taylor, J.H.; Johnson, I.; Landeros-Weisenberger, A.; Aboiralor, O.; Avila-Quintero, V.J.; Bloch, M.H. Racial Implicit Associations in Psychiatric Diagnosis, Treatment, and Compliance Expectations. Acad. Psychiatry 2021, 45, 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Lowe, C.L.; Beach, M.C.; Roter, D.L. Genetic Counselor Implicit Bias and Its Effects on Cognitive and Affective Exchanges in Racially Discordant Simulations. J. Genet. Couns. 2020, 29, 332–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Nash, P.; Stuart-Hamilton, I.; Mayer, P. The Continuation of Prejudice: Addressing Negative Attitudes in Nurse Training and Continuing Professional Education. Educ. Gerontol. 2014, 40, 53–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Nymo, L.S.; Aabakken, L.; Lassen, K. Priority and Prejudice: Does Low Socioeconomic Status Bias Waiting Time for Endoscopy? A Blinded, Randomized Survey. Scand. J. Gastroenterol. 2018, 53, 621–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Oliver, M.N.; Wells, K.M.; Joy-Gaba, J.A.; Hawkins, C.B.; Nosek, B.A. Do Physicians’ Implicit Views of African Americans Affect Clinical Decision Making? J. Am. Board Fam Med. 2014, 27, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Patel, N.; Patel, S.; Cotti, E.; Bardini, G.; Mannocci, F. Unconscious Racial Bias May Affect Dentists’ Clinical Decisions on Tooth Restorability: A Randomized Clinical Trial. JDR Clin. Transl. Res. 2019, 4, 19–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Penner, L.A.; Dovidio, J.F.; Gonzalez, R.; Albrecht, T.L.; Chapman, R.; Foster, T.; Harper, F.W.K.; Hagiwara, N.; Hamel, L.M.; Shields, A.F.; et al. The Effects of Oncologist Implicit Racial Bias in Racially Discordant Oncology Interactions. J. Clin. Oncol 2016, 34, 2874–2880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Puumala, S.E.; Burgess, K.M.; Kharbanda, A.B.; Zook, H.G.; Castille, D.M.; Pickner, W.J.; Payne, N.R. The Role of Bias by Emergency Department Providers in Care for American Indian Children. Med. Care 2016, 54, 562–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Robstad, N.; Siebler, F.; Söderhamn, U.; Westergren, T.; Fegran, L. Design and Psychometric Testing of Instruments to Measure Qualified Intensive Care Nurses’ Attitudes toward Obese Intensive Care Patients. Res. Nurs. Health 2018, 41, 525–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Robstad, N.; Westergren, T.; Siebler, F.; Söderhamn, U.; Fegran, L. Intensive Care Nurses’ Implicit and Explicit Attitudes and Their Behavioural Intentions towards Obese Intensive Care Patients. J. Adv. Nurs. 2019, 75, 3631–3642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Rojas, M.; Walker-Descartes, I.; Laraque-Arena, D. An Experimental Study of Implicit Racial Bias in Recognition of Child Abuse. Am. J. Health Behav. 2017, 41, 358–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Sabin, J.A.; Greenwald, A.G. The Influence of Implicit Bias on Treatment Recommendations for 4 Common Pediatric Conditions: Pain, Urinary Tract Infection, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, and Asthma. Am. J. Public Health 2012, 102, 988–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Sabin, J.A.; Marini, M.; Nosek, B.A. Implicit and Explicit Anti-Fat Bias among a Large Sample of Medical Doctors by BMI, Race/Ethnicity and Gender. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e48448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Sabin, J.A.; Moore, K.; Noonan, C.; Lallemand, O.; Buchwald, D. Clinicians’ Implicit and Explicit Attitudes about Weight and Race and Treatment Approaches to Overweight for American Indian Children. Child. Obes. 2015, 11, 456–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Sabin, J.A.; Riskind, R.G.; Nosek, B.A. Health Care Providers’ Implicit and Explicit Attitudes toward Lesbian Women and Gay Men. Am. J. Public Health 2015, 105, 1831–1841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Sandhu, H.S.; Arora, A.; Brasch, J.; Streiner, D.L. Mental Health Stigma: Explicit and Implicit Attitudes of Canadian Undergraduate Students, Medical School Students, and Psychiatrists. Can. J. Psychiatry 2019, 64, 209–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Schaa, K.L.; Roter, D.L.; Biesecker, B.B.; Cooper, L.A.; Erby, L.H. Genetic Counselors’ Implicit Racial Attitudes and Their Relationship to Communication. Health Psychol. 2015, 34, 111–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Schoenberg, E.; Wang, J.V.; Duffy, R.; Keller, M. Uncovering Unconscious Bias and Health Disparities in the Practice of Dermatology. Skinmed 2019, 17, 367–368. [Google Scholar]
  102. Schroyen, S.; Missotten, P.; Jerusalem, G.; Gilles, C.; Adam, S. Ageism and Caring Attitudes among Nurses in Oncology. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2016, 28, 749–757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Setchell, J.; Watson, B.; Jones, L.; Gard, M.; Briffa, K. Physiotherapists Demonstrate Weight Stigma: A Cross-Sectional Survey of Australian Physiotherapists. J. Physiother. 2014, 60, 157–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Siegelman, J.N.; Woods, C.; Salhi, B.; Heron, S. Health Care Disparities Education Using the Implicit Association Test. Med. Educ. 2016, 50, 1158–1159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Stepanikova, I. Racial-Ethnic Biases, Time Pressure, and Medical Decisions. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2012, 53, 329–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Sukhera, J.; Wodzinski, M.; Teunissen, P.W.; Lingard, L.; Watling, C. Striving While Accepting: Exploring the Relationship Between Identity and Implicit Bias Recognition and Management. Acad. Med. 2018, 93, S82–S88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Tajeu, G.S.; Halanych, J.; Juarez, L.; Stone, J.; Stepanikova, I.; Green, A.; Cherrington, A.L. Exploring the Association of Healthcare Worker Race and Occupation with Implicit and Explicit Racial Bias. J. Natl Med. Assoc. 2018, 110, 464–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Tucker Edmonds, B.; McKenzie, F.; Austgen, M.B.; Ashburn-Nardo, L.; Matthias, M.S.; Hirsh, A.T. Obstetrical Providers’ Management of Chronic Pain in Pregnancy: A Vignette Study. Pain Med. 2017, 18, 832–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. VanPuymbrouck, L.; Friedman, C.; Feldner, H. Explicit and Implicit Disability Attitudes of Healthcare Providers. Rehabil. Psychol. 2020, 65, 101–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Walden, T.A.; Lesner, T.A.; Jones, R.M. Is What I Think I Think Really What I Think? Implicit and Explicit Attitudes toward Stuttering among Practicing Speech-Language Pathologists. J. Commun. Disord. 2020, 83, 105965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Wandner, L.D.; Heft, M.W.; Lok, B.C.; Hirsh, A.T.; George, S.Z.; Horgas, A.L.; Atchison, J.W.; Torres, C.A.; Robinson, M.E. The Impact of Patients’ Gender, Race, and Age on Health Care Professionals’ Pain Management Decisions: An Online Survey Using Virtual Human Technology. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 2014, 51, 726–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Welch, L.C.; Lutfey, K.E.; Gerstenberger, E.; Grace, M. Gendered Uncertainty and Variation in Physicians’ Decisions for Coronary Heart Disease: The Double-Edged Sword of “Atypical Symptoms”. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2012, 53, 313–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Welch, L.C.; Litman, H.J.; Borba, C.P.C.; Vincenzi, B.; Henderson, D.C. Does a Physician’s Attitude toward a Patient with Mental Illness Affect Clinical Management of Diabetes? Results from a Mixed-Method Study. Health Serv. Res. 2015, 50, 998–1020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Wittlin, N.M.; Dovidio, J.F.; Burke, S.E.; Przedworski, J.M.; Herrin, J.; Dyrbye, L.; Onyeador, I.N.; Phelan, S.M.; van Ryn, M. Contact and Role Modeling Predict Bias against Lesbian and Gay Individuals among Early-Career Physicians: A Longitudinal Study. Soc. Sci Med. 2019, 238, 112422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Zestcott, C.A.; Spece, L.; McDermott, D.; Stone, J. Health Care Providers’ Negative Implicit Attitudes and Stereotypes of American Indians. J. Racial Ethn Health Disparities 2021, 8, 230–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. IAT Corporation. Project Implicit. Available online: www.implicit.harvard.edu (accessed on 8 May 2023).
  117. Lee, D.J. Racial Bias and the Validity of the Implicit Association Test; The United Nations University World Institute for Development Economics Research (UNU-WIDER): Helsinki, Finland, 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Greenwald, A.G.; Poehlman, T.A.; Uhlmann, E.L.; Banaji, M.R. Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-Analysis of Predictive Validity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 97, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Karpinski, A.; Steinman, R.B.; Hilton, J.L. Attitude Importance as a Moderator of the Relationship between Implicit and Explicit Attitude Measures. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2005, 31, 949–962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Rezaei, A.R. Validity and Reliability of the IAT: Measuring Gender and Ethnic Stereotypes. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2011, 27, 1937–1941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Meissner, F.; Grigutsch, L.A.; Koranyi, N.; Müller, F.; Rothermund, K. Predicting Behavior with Implicit Measures: Disillusioning Findings, Reasonable Explanations, and Sophisticated Solutions. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Friese, M.; Hofmann, W.; Wänke, M. When Impulses Take over: Moderated Predictive Validity of Explicit and Implicit Attitude Measures in Predicting Food Choice and Consumption Behaviour. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 47, 397–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Greenwald, A.G.; Nosek, B.A.; Banaji, M.R. Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: I. An Improved Scoring Algorithm. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram of scoping review.
Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram of scoping review.
Ijerph 20 06569 g001
Table 2. Number of assessed bias by type (focus).
Table 2. Number of assessed bias by type (focus).
BiasNumber of Studies
Racial bias46
Gender bias11
Weight bias10
SES bias9
Mental illness bias7
Disability bias7
Ageism4
LGBTQ bias2
Diagnosis bias (cervical cancer)1
Total97
Note that in some studies, multiple biases were assessed in the same study.
Table 3. Type of bias, number of biases assessed per bias, and total number of biases assessed per country.
Table 3. Type of bias, number of biases assessed per bias, and total number of biases assessed per country.
CountryType of BiasNumber of Biases Assessed per BiasTotal Number of Biases Assessed per Country
Australia (n = 3)Weight bias
Mental illness bias
2
1

3
Belgium (n = 2)Ageism
Racial bias
1
1

2
Canada (n = 2)Mental illness bias22
Denmark (n = 1)Weight bias11
France (n = 1)Disability bias11
Italy (n = 2)Disability bias
Racial bias
1
1

2
Japan (n = 1)Mental illness bias11
Kenya (n = 1)SES bias11
Norway (n = 3)Weight bias
SES bias
2
1

3
Poland (n = 1)Mental illness bias11
Portugal (n = 1)Gender bias11
Saudia Arabia (n = 1)SES bias11
Sweden (n = 1)Gender bias11
UK (n = 1)Ageism11
USA (n = 58)Racial bias 44
Gender bias9
SES bias6
Weight bias4
Disability bias4
LGBTQ bias2
Mental illness bias2
Ageism 2
Diagnosis bias174
Global (n = 2)Disability bias1
Weight bias12
Table 4. Number of biases by profession.
Table 4. Number of biases by profession.
BiasNumber of Studies
Physicians51
Nurses20
Health care providers (not specified)12
Psychiatrists5
Dentists4
Genetic counsellors4
Physiotherapists3
Mental health providers (not specified)2
Physician assistants 1
Speech–language pathologists1
Dieticians1
Diagnostic radiographers1
Occupational therapists1
Midwives1
Psychotherapists 1
Other support staff1
Note that in some studies, multiple professions were assessed.
Table 5. Number and type of biases assessed by health profession.
Table 5. Number and type of biases assessed by health profession.
PhysiciansNursesHealth Care Providers (Not Specified)DentistsPsychiatristsGenetic CounselorsPhysiotherapistsRehabilitation Staff (Not Specified)Physician AssistantsMental Health Providers (Not Specified)DietitiansOccupational TherapistsSpeech–Language PathologistsDiagnostic RadiographersMidwivesPsychotherapistsOther Support Staff
Race3486212 11
Gender8311 1
SES721 1 1
Weight54 1 1 1 1
Mental illness31 4 1 1
Disability2242 22 11
LGBTQ211 1
Ageism23 1
Diagnosis11
Total64251365432221111111
Note that in some studies, multiple professions and multiple biases were assessed.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Meidert, U.; Dönnges, G.; Bucher, T.; Wieber, F.; Gerber-Grote, A. Unconscious Bias among Health Professionals: A Scoping Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 6569. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20166569

AMA Style

Meidert U, Dönnges G, Bucher T, Wieber F, Gerber-Grote A. Unconscious Bias among Health Professionals: A Scoping Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(16):6569. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20166569

Chicago/Turabian Style

Meidert, Ursula, Godela Dönnges, Thomas Bucher, Frank Wieber, and Andreas Gerber-Grote. 2023. "Unconscious Bias among Health Professionals: A Scoping Review" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 16: 6569. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20166569

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop