Next Article in Journal
Analysis of the 2007–2018 National Health Interview Survey (NHIS): Examining Neurological Complications among Children with Sickle Cell Disease in the United States
Next Article in Special Issue
The Role of Career Adaptability and Academic Engagement in College Student’s Life Satisfaction
Previous Article in Journal
Exclusive Breastfeeding at Discharge in Regional New South Wales, Australia: The Role of Antenatal Care (2011–2020)
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Dark Personality Traits and Online Behaviors: Portuguese Versions of Cyberstalking, Online Harassment, Flaming and Trolling Scales

1
Centre for Philosophical and Humanistic Studies (CEFH), Universidade Católica Portuguesa, 4710-362 Braga, Portugal
2
Research Center in Sports Sciences and Human Development, CIDESD, Universidade de Trás-os-Montes e Alto Douro, 5000-801 Vila Real, Portugal
3
Department of Social Sciences and Behavior, University of Maia, Av. Carlos Oliveira Campos, 4475-690 Maia, Portugal
4
Departamento de Educação e Psicologia, Universidade de Trás-os-Montes e Alto Douro, 5000-801 Vila Real, Portugal
5
CIIE—Center for Research and Intervention in Education, University of Porto, 4200-135 Porto, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20(12), 6136; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20126136
Submission received: 12 April 2023 / Revised: 28 May 2023 / Accepted: 13 June 2023 / Published: 15 June 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Personality and Trauma: A Pathway to Health Status)

Abstract

:
The main objective of this study is to assess moderation effects of online behaviors between personality traits and addiction to Internet. To this end, four instruments were validated for Portuguese version through confirmatory factor analysis and exploratory factor analysis (Study 1) Multiple regression analysis was applied to examine the personality predictors of specific online behaviors while controlling for gender and age; and moderation effects were assessed (Study 2). Results showed good psychometric properties for the four validated scales. Machiavellianism is positively associated with all the dimensions of this study. Psychopathy is positively associated with total Cyberstalking, Cyberstalking Control, Flaming and Trolling. Narcissism is positively associated with all the dimensions, except Online Harassment and Flaming. Machiavellianism is positively associated with Addiction to Internet through Cyberstalking, Flaming and Trolling. Psychopathy is positively associated with Addiction to Internet through Cyberstalking Control and Flaming. Narcissism is also positively associated with Addiction to Internet through Cyberstalking and Trolling. This study demonstrates that dimensions of the dark triad of personality play an important role in Internet addiction through online behaviors. The results of this study have theoretical and practical implications: on the one hand, they reinforces the findings of other studies showing that dimensions of the dark personality triad play an important role in Internet and social network addition, contributing to the literature; and, on the other hand, on a practical level, they allow to conduct awareness campaigns in communities, schools, and work to understand that one can be exposed to unpleasant situations due to behaviors that some people with personality traits of Machiavellianism, narcissism and/or psychopathy that may cause problems affecting the mental, emotional and psychological health of others.

1. Introduction

It is almost impossible to live nowadays without the Internet and hundreds of online applications [1]. Although certainly beneficial, they can also represent a source of problems when used problematically or additively [1,2,3], and/or the emergence of various forms of abuse [4,5] and unwanted and problematic digital contact [6,7].
The addition to the Internet is an extensive concept that encompasses a multiplicity of behaviors and impulse control problems in its use [3], with characteristics similar to other types of dependence, namely, salience, tolerance, mood modification, conflict, abstinence and relapse [2,8].
Cyberstalking, also known in the literature as electronic or virtual stalking, includes the use of the Internet (or other computerized device), to harassing or chasing other persons, by systematic and undesirable actions, causing suffering to the targets of these behaviors [5]. Cyberstalking can include behaviors such as maintaining remote surveillance, constant contact with and/or direct threats against the victim [9]. In fact, online harassment may become a public health issue because of the Internet’s globalization [6,10]. Similarly, online harassment is considered a form of cyberbullying that consists of repeatedly sending offensive, threatening or intimidating messages to someone via email, short message (or messaging) service (SMS), multimedia messaging service (MMS) or others [11]. This can also arise in the form of offensive sexual messages [12].
Literature has reported a number of harmful consequences for mental health of victims of cyberstalking and/or harassment. The negative psychological impact of the cyberstalking and/or online harassment manifests itself through psychopathological symptoms such as anxiety, depression, posttraumatic stress disorder and panic attacks; through negative emotions such as sadness, anger, fear, shame, embarrassment, isolation and low self-esteem; through physical symptoms such as stomach pains and heart palpitation; and even through inappropriate behaviors such as harmful self-behavior [13,14].
In turn, Trolling can be considered a type of online harassment [10]; this behavior is intended to intentionally deceive, destabilize a discussion, and provoke others creating conflict, and evoking hostility and anguish, which causes pleasure in online offenders [4,10]. Although there is no consensus in the literature regarding the definition of flaming, most studies consider that flaming includes sending hostile, aggressive, intimidating, insulting and offensive messages through uninhibited and sarcastic language [7]. These messages and comments do not contribute to the discussion in question, but instead attempt to hurt another person socially or psychologically and impose power on others [15]. According to Hinduja and Patchin [15], trolling is directed to the subject of discussion, while flaming is directed at another participant or other participants in the discussion. Trolls try to cause maximum disruption, annoyance through the arguments presented on the subject online and their words and actions have no sincere basis. In turn, in flaming, the transmission of what the individual thinks is true or correct is done in an aversive and harmful way [15].
It is possible to classify the above-mentioned behaviors (cyberstalking, online harassment, flaming, trolling) as anti-social behavior and online; according to Moor and Anderson [16], antisocial online behavior is any deviant behavior or the deliberate absence of proper behavior that is committed online and has negative online and or offline consequences to whom it is directed (including self-directed behavior).
It should be noted that online antisocial behavior represents a more complex phenomenon and with more serious consequences because it is practiced online or through any technological devices. Thus, the possible anonymity of the perpetrator, the public potential to become uncontrollable, the constant nature of victimization through the permanent presence of information and communications technology (ICT) in everyday life, the increased difficulty in identifying the perpetrator and the power imbalance created by anonymity are perceived by victims as weakening and strengthening the perpetrators [17,18].
Research has shown that the personality traits that make up dark triad play a crucial role in adding to the Internet and social networks [19,20,21]. These traits also appear in the literature as an attempt to explain the problems related to Internet misconduct/online antisocial behaviors. Manuoğlu and Öner-Özkan [10] highlighted the importance of analyzing the role of the dark triad of personality in online behavior, considering that these personality traits can have a negative impact and with serious consequences on these behaviors.
The dark triad assesses aversive personality traits: Machiavellianism, psychopathy and narcissism [22,23]. These traits share some characteristics in common, such as manipulation, selfishness, insensitivity, lack of empathy and affection [24]. Dark triad has been the subject of several studies in the last decade, which has recently led to its extension being suggested to introduce Sadism, thus becoming the dark tetrad [4]. Such insertion is still not consensual [25], although Bonfá-Araujo et al. [26] found that Sadism is the best predictor of aversive online behaviors. Also, Pineda et al. [27] found that Sadism was the strongest predictor of online sexual victimization perpetration. Thus, Machiavellianism is characterized by cunning, cynicism, selfish ambition, difficulties in terms of empathy, manipulation and strategic exploitation of others [25,28]. Regarding narcissism, the traits associated with it are the feeling of grandiosity, individualism and an unrealistic self-image that reflects an idea of superiority, dominance and entitlement in relation to others. It is also characterized by a lack of empathy and feelings of envy [25,29]. It also presents a generalized and cynical view of the world and people [30]. In turn, psychopathy is characterized by insensitivity, low level of empathy, impulsiveness, imprudence, disdain for others, tendency to tease and deception, and also by loquacity and superficial charm [25,28,31].
Several researches reveal that dark triad’s personality traits seem to be positively associated with the problematic addition to the Internet [32] and with online antisocial behaviors, particularly cyberstalking [9,16], online trolling [10,33,34] and cyberbullying [35]. Machiavellianism has been associated with problematic use of social media, trolling in online games, and online self-promotion. Individuals high in Machiavellian traits use social media or online gaming platforms to engage in manipulative interpersonal behavior or deceptive self-promotion, in part, due to their fear of social rejection. Considering that Machiavellianism is negatively associated with positive humor, it is expected that people with a high level of this trait will end up becoming problematic Internet users, since using the Internet corresponds to a (maladaptive) coping strategy to deal with negative feelings [32,36]. Machiavellian individuals can use Internet games, especially violent games, not only for entertainment, but also to satisfy the need to explore and control others [37]. Narcissism is associated with a great involvement with problematic use of the Internet and social networks [20,32,38,39]. People who have high levels of this trait tend to exhibit online self-promotion behaviors (sometimes deceptively), through selfies, posts and video clips. Self-promotion behaviors and the attempt to present a more popular self than the real ones are risk factors for the development of problematic Internet use [38,39]. In turn, psychopathy is associated with emotional dysregulation and a low level of positive mood. Thus, psychopathic individuals tend to seek online activities as a form of coping, but also to seek in these activities a way to obtain greater sensations. They also tend to engage in antisocial online behaviors such as cyberbullying, trolling, cyberstalking intimate partners, and playing violent games [9,20,32].
Sindermann et al. [37] stated that there are associations, in females, between Machiavellianism and psychopathy, on the one hand, and tendencies toward Internet-shopping disorder, on the other. The authors also found an association between psychopathy, on the one hand, and tendencies toward Internet-pornography-use disorder, on the other. Finally, they showed an association between Machiavellianism, on the one hand, and tendencies toward Internet-communication disorder, on the other. In turn, the associations found between the dark triad traits and unspecified Internet use disorder are similar in males and females.
Smoker and March [9] examined the influence of Dark Tetrad on intimate partner cyberstalking and the results revealed moderate associations with psychopathy, Machiavellianism and Sadism, while narcissism was weakly correlated. Also, high scores in the dark triad of personality are related to high levels of perpetration of cyber dating abuse [40].
In the study by Lopes and Yu [34], 135 participants evaluated two fake Facebook profiles regarding their agreement with trolling comments and with the social acceptance of the fake profiles. The authors found that narcissism was related to a tendency to see oneself as superior to the popular profile, while psychopathy was positively associated with trolling behaviors; in addition, individuals with higher scores on psychopathy were more likely to spy a profile that was considered popular. In turn, the results of the systematic review of the literature conducted by Moor and Anderson [16] suggest that psychopathy is the darkest of traits, based on its ability to predict behaviors of “high severity”, namely cyberaggression and technology facilitated sexual violence.
The main objective of this study is to assess the moderation effects of online behaviors between personality traits and addiction to Internet through two studies. To achieve this objective, specific objectives were outlined: (1) to evaluate the psychometric qualities of the instruments used in this study through (1a) assessment of the adjustment to our sample of instruments previously validated for the Portuguese population (Internet Addiction Test and Dirty Dozen Dark Triad); (1b) and the validation for the Portuguese population of the Cyberstalking scale, Online Harassment scale, Flaming scale and Trolling scale; (2) to establish associations between Internet addiction, Machiavellianism, psychopathy and narcissism, cyberstalking, online harassment, flaming and trolling, on the one hand, and the sociodemographic variables, o the another; (3) to establish associations between personality and online behaviors; and (4) to understand if online behaviors moderates the relation between the dark triad of personality and Internet addiction.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Procedures

All procedures followed the Declaration of Helsinki and later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Also, the Scientific Committee of Universidade Católica Portuguesa approved this study that was conducted from July to September 2022. Participants were recruited through social media and assessed through Google Forms, having been informed about the study’s purpose and assured of confidentiality and anonymity of the data. All participants signed the informed consent. The criteria used to select participants were: to be 18 years old or older, and have Portuguese nationality and residence. Exclusion criteria include being fewer than 18, not understanding the content of the questions asked and not using social media. The translation of the four instruments (Cyberstalking Scale, Online Harassment Scale, Flaming Behaviors Scale and Trolling Behavior Scale) was performed according to the International Test Commission (ITC) guidelines for translating and adapting tests [41] and the translation back-translation procedure [42]. The original versions of the instruments were translated from English to Portuguese by two bilingual translators, one psychologists and another from social sciences field. A third bilingual translator (psychologist) carried out a reconciliation of the two translations. A native English speaker from other religious field performed the reconciled version’s back-translation. The first translator compared the back-translated version with the original English versions to achieve linguistic and cultural equivalence consistency. No differences were found between the back-translated and the original versions. A convenience sample of 15 people over 18 years old, with Portuguese nationality and residence were invited to evaluate the items’ relevance to the scales and cultural context. Original and Portuguese versions of the scales can be found in Appendix A.

2.2. Instruments

2.2.1. Sociodemographic Questionnaire

The sociodemographic questionnaire includes questions related to gender (man—0; woman—1), age, years of education and if participants are in a romantic relationship (no—0; yes—1).

2.2.2. Psychological Variables

Internet Addiction Test

The Internet Addiction Test (IAT) was conceived by Young [43] to measure the extent of a person’s involvement with the Internet; it classifies addictive behavior as mild, moderate or severe impairment. IAT comprises 20 items, rated on a six-point Likert scale (0—‘does not apply’, 1—‘rarely’, 2—‘occasionally’, 3—‘frequently’, 4—‘often’, and 5—‘always’). The sum of all items allows obtaining a global score [43]. Young [43,44] proposed two different cut-off points criteria: the first one (1998) [44] stated that values between 20 and 39 correspond to the average user; between 40 and 69 correspond to a person that has frequent problems because of Internet usage; and between 70 and 100 correspond to Internet addicts. The second cut-off points criteria [43] proposed that values between 0 and 30 correspond to a normal range; between 31 and 49 correspond to mildly addicted; between 50 and 79 correspond to moderately addicted; and between 80 and 100 correspond to severely addicted. Factor analysis of the IAT by Widyanto and McMurran [45] yielded six factors (salience, excessive use, neglecting work, anticipation, lack of control, and neglecting social life) with all factors showing good internal consistency and concurrent validity. Several studies reported reliability coefficients and internal consistency for the IAT, ranging between α = 0.88 and α = 0.93 [46,47]. Pontes et al. [48] validated the IAT for the Portuguese population and found a uni-factorial model for the IAT and also found that IAT is a valid and reliable instrument for measuring IA among Portuguese young adults as demonstrated by its satisfactory psychometric properties (e.g., α = 0.90). In this study it was also found a value of α = 0.90.

Dirty Dozen Dark Triad

Jonason and Webster [49] developed and validated a concise, 12-item measure of the Dark Triad: narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism because an exponential interest in the dark side of human nature lasts. The authors first created 22 candidate items inspired by the original Dark Triad measure; the four items with the strongest loadings on the primary factor were retained from each of the three Dark Triad measures, being that, together, these 12 items constituted the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen and the three factors emerged [49]. Participants answered the 12 items by agreeing or not (1—‘strongly disagree’, 5—‘strongly agree’). Overall, the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen achieved good internal consistency α = 0.83, for psychopathy α = 0.63, Machiavellianism α = 0.72, and narcissism α = 0.79. Pechorro et al. [50] validated this instrument for the Portuguese population and found a three-factor structure invariant across the genders and adequate psychometric properties (internal consistency, convergent validity, discriminant validity, criterion-related validity, and know-groups validity (boys versus girls). Pechorro et al. [50] reported alpha values for psychopathy α = 0.63, Machiavellianism α = 0.72, and narcissism α = 0.79. In this study, it was found a value of psychopathy α = 0.68, Machiavellianism α = 0.73, and narcissism α = 0.81.

Cyberstalking Scale

Silva Santos et al. [51] developed a cyberstalking measure for the Brazilian population. Initially, this scale had 15 items, but after exploratory factor analysis and Cronbach’s alpha, five items were removed, remaining 10 items. These items were answered on a five-point Likert-type scale (from 1—‘totally disagree’ to 5—‘totally agree’). The ten items solution presents a Cronbach alpha value of 0.82. A confirmatory factor analysis proved that the one-factor model was adequate [51]. This instrument has not been validated for the Portuguese population, so, its measurement will be carried out in this study.

Online Harassment Scale

Lewis et al. [52] conceived nine items to assess online harassment using a seven-point Likert-type scale measuring the frequency of harassment (from 1—‘never’ to 7—‘all the time’) personally experienced by the respondent in the course of their work as journalists. The first item measured general online harassment, with the subsequent eight items measuring particular forms of it. However, in this study, it will only be used 8 items; the first item was excluded as its content is specifically aimed at journalists and not the general public. The authors do not report psychometric properties of the instrument. Again, this instrument has not been validated for the Portuguese population, so, its measurement will be carried out in this study.

Flaming Behaviors Scale

The items used to measure flaming behavior were based on scales developed by Turnage [7]. Aggression, intimidation, insults, uninhibited language, and sarcasm are characteristics of flaming behaviors [7]. Each item was rated on a seven-point Likert scale from 1—‘strongly disagree’ to 7—‘strongly agree’. The author [7] found a Cronbach alpha value of 0.87. Hwang et al. [53] also found a value of 0.87. This instrument has not been validated for the Portuguese population, so, its measurement will be carried out in this study.

Trolling Behaviors Scale

Howard et al. [54] developed the 3-item scale that measures the extent to which the participant debated with others online and had intentions to aggravate/irritate others online. These items were created by a focus group. “The items asking, ‘To what extent do you enjoy the following: Debating various topics with the intention to irritate/upset others’ and ‘To what extent do you enjoy the following: ‘Trolling’ on public forums’ were measured on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1—‘not at all’ to 5—‘very much’. The item asking, ‘Please indicate how much you agree with the following statement: I like to post memes and comments with the intent to aggravate or annoy others’ was measured on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1—strongly disagree to 5—strongly agree” [54] (p. 310). The authors do not report the value of the Cronbach alpha. This instrument has not been validated for the Portuguese population, so, its measurement will be carried out in this study.

2.3. Data Analysis

Data analysis includes procedures related to descriptive statistics: for continuous data, measures of centrality (mean), dispersion (standard deviation and range) and shape (skewness and kurtosis) were used; frequencies and percentages are also displayed. The normality of the items was assessed by skewness (SI < 3) and kurtosis (KI < 10) indexes suggesting non-severe violations of normality [55], utilizing SEM, and multicollinearity was assessed by tolerance (>0.100) and variance inflation factor (VIF) (<10) [56]. Pearson correlations were established for continuous variables and Spearman correlations when at least one of the variables was ordinal or nominal. Correlations between 0 and 0.3 are weak, between 0.3 and 0.5 are moderate, between 0.5 and 0.7 are strong and between 0.7 and 1 are very strong either positive or negative [57].
A Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) with Maximum Likelihood Estimation (MLE) was carried out to confirm the models proposed by the authors of the original versions of the instruments. The model fit evaluation was based on Kline’s [58] thresholds concerning statistics tests and approximate fit indexes. A statistically non-significant model chi-square statistic, χ2, shows that the model fits the data acceptably; the higher the probability related to χ2, the closer the fit to the perfect one. A value of the parsimony-corrected index Steiger–Lind root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) close to 0 and non-significant at the 0.05 level represents a good fit. Values of incremental fit index (IFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) and the Bentler incremental comparative fit index (CFI), close to 1 (0.95 or better), are indicators of best fit, as well as goodness of fit index (GFI). Re-specification of the models allows to analyzing path estimates, standardized residuals of items, and the modification indices. Regarding construct validity, items with low individual standardized factor loadings (regression weights below 0.50) may eventually be removal. The modifications indices (MI) provide information about potential cross-loadings and error term correlations (modifications theoretically meaningful and MI > 11 were taken into account). Concerning the parsimony-adjusted index Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) (over 0.10 suggests fit problems), the smallest the values the most parsimonious is the model, they allow to compare the fit across models.
Concerning Cyberstalking scale, it was not possible to confirm the model proposed by the authors through the CFA. Therefore, it was decided to carry out an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with maximum likelihood factoring. Parallel analysis—principal components analysis with raw data permutation [59]—was performed to determine the number of components to extract. Extracted factors were rotated by varimax rotation and the number of factors was decided in consideration of the scree-plot, cumulative variance explained, interpretability, and Kaiser’s criterion. Before, the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) test (measures sampling adequacy) and Bartlett’s test of sphericity were conducted to evaluate the factorability. Significance of Bartlett’s test of sphericity should be less than 0.001, meaning that EFA can be applied to the obtained data.
To assess the model reliability, convergent and discriminant validity, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients, composite reliability (CR, 0.70 or higher suggests good model reliability), average variance extracted (AVE, 0.50 or higher suggests adequate convergence) and square root of AVE (higher than the highest correlation with any other latent variable) were used; if AVE is less than 0.50 and CR higher than 0.60, the convergent validity of the model is adequate [60].
To compare the means of two groups, the independent means t-test (assumes the normality and homoscedasticity of the distribution variable) was applied. To compare the means of more than two groups, the F-test was applied (it assumes that the variable is normally and independently distributed, with equal variances among groups). Chi-squared test compares the distribution of categorical variables. Three measures of the effect-size, Cohen’s d, phi and eta squared were used accordingly to the level of measurement of the variables; interpretation followed Cohen et al. [57] guidelines. Hierarchical regression analysis was applied to examine the personality predictors of specific online behaviors while controlling for gender and age. To assess mediation effects of online behavior between personality traits and addiction to Internet, a multiple mediation model was tested with dark personality traits as independent variables, specific online behaviors as mediators, addiction to Internet as the outcome variable, and gender and age as control variables. A moderation analysis was carried out. The statistical significance level was set at 0.05. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS version 28 and AMOS version 28 (Armonk, NY: IBM Corp, 2021).

3. Results

3.1. Descriptives

The sample consists of 773 participants, of which 467 (60.4%) are women; the sample presents a mean age of 27.39 (SD = 11.93; ranging from 19 to 78 years). Most of the sample is in a romantic relationship (N = 442; 57.2%). The mean age of the years of educative is 13.06 (SD = 2.47).
Most of the sample spends between two and six hours a day on the Internet, in non-academic or professional tasks. The main activities carried out on the Internet are social networking, communicate and relate to others, listen to music online and search for information and news in general. Less common activities on the Internet are betting sites and looking for new friends (Table 1).
In Appendix B, there is a description of all items of all instruments used in this study. Regarding the Internet Addiction Test, item 1 (Do you find that you stay online longer than you intended?) is the one with the highest mean value and item 3 (Do you prefer the excitement of the Internet to intimacy with your partner?) the lowest. Concerning Dirty Dozen Dark Triad, item 1 (I tend to want others to pay attention to me.) is the one with the highest mean value and items 4 and 6 (I tend to exploit others towards my own end; I tend to not be too concerned with morality or the morality of my actions.) the lowest. With regard to the Cyberstalking Scale, item 10 (When you’re interested in someone, it’s not wrong to look at their acquaintances’ social media, in order to get to know them better.) is the one with the highest mean value and item 6 (If I had my partner’s social media passwords, my life would be easier.) the lowest. In what it refers to Online Harassment Scale, item 6 (Hurt emotionally or psychologically.) is the one with the highest mean value and item 4 (Threats of physical sexual violence.) the lowest. Regarding Flaming Behaviors Scale, item 5 (In the community, I tend to make sarcastic remarks about others opinion. (Sarcasm)) is the one with the highest mean value and item 2 (In the community, I tend to intimidate people who get on my nerves. (Intimidation)) the lowest. At last, concerning Trolling Behaviors Scale, item 1 (Debating various topics with the intention to irritate/upset others.) is the one with the highest mean value and item 2 (Trolling’ on public forums) the lowest (Appendix B).
Based on the normative values of skewness and kurtosis, utilizing SEM, proposed by Brown [55], data is considered to have a normal distribution if skewness is between −3 and +3 and kurtosis between −10 and +10. Item 4 of the Online Harassment Scale is slightly above the recommended values for skewness and well above recommended values for kurtosis (Appendix B). The literature suggests several options for dealing with skewness and kurtosis, including (1) doing nothing (because many statistical tests, such as t tests, ANOVAs, and linear regressions, aren’t very sensitive to skewed data, especially if the skew is mild or moderate); (2) use another model (such as non-parametric tests or generalized linear models); and (3) transform the variable if it becomes less skewed [61]. At this point of the analysis, it was decided not to make any changes to the data, waiting to see how these items behave in subsequent analyses.

3.2. Study 1

3.2.1. Specific Objective (1) to Evaluate the Psychometric Qualities of the Instruments Used in This Study through (1a) Assessment of the Adjustment to Our Sample of Instruments Previously Validated for the Portuguese Population (Internet Addiction Test and Dirty Dozen Dark Triad)

The Internet Addiction Test was validated for the Portuguese population as being unifactorial. We proceeded to carry out a confirmatory factorial analysis to evaluate the model in our population, but the fit was unacceptable. After consulting the modification indices, two correlations between errors were established until a good model was found [χ2(149) = 3.30; IFI = 0.938; TLI = 0.921; CFI = 0.938; GFI = 0.936; SRMR = 0.046; RMSEA = 0.055 (LO90 = 0.049; HI90 = 0.060); AIC = 612.98]. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients (0.90) composite reliability (0.91), average variance extracted (AVE, 0.55) and square root of AVE (0.74) were calculated, being the values within the reference range. Mean (1.78) and standard deviation (0.64) were also calculated.
The Dirty Dozen Dark Triad was validated for the Portuguese population as tri-factorial. We proceeded to carry out a confirmatory factorial analysis to evaluate the model in our sample, but the fit was not good. After consulting the modification indices, two correlations between errors were established until a good model was found [χ2(41) = 3.66; IFI = 0.961; TLI = 0.937; CFI = 0.961; GFI = 0.970; SRMR = 0.046; RMSEA = 0.059 (LO90 = 0.049; HI90 = 0.069); AIC = 223.87]. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients (Machiavellianism α = 0.73; Psychopathy α = 0.68; Narcissism α = 0.81); composite reliability (Machiavellianism CR = 0.83; Psychopathy CR = 0.81; Narcissism CR = 0.88); average variance extracted (AVE) (Machiavellianism AVE = 0.56; Psychopathy AVE = 0.51; Narcissism AVE = 0.64); square root of AVE (Machiavellianism AVEsr = 0.75; Psychopathy AVEsr = 0.71; Narcissism AVEsr = 0.80); mean and standard deviation were calculated (Machiavellianism M ± SD = 2.27 ± 0.81; Psychopathy M ± SD = 1.81 ± 0.73; Narcissism M ± SD = 2.38 ± 0.92). All the values were within the reference range.

3.2.2. Specific Objective (1) to Evaluate the Psychometric Qualities of the Instruments Used in this Study through (1b) the Validation for the Portuguese Population of the Cyberstalking Scale, Online Harassment Scale, Flaming Scale and Trolling Scale

Cyberstalking Scale

A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was carried out (N = 773) in order to confirm the unidimensional model proposed by the original authors. However, the model was very poor [χ2(35) = 17.45; IFI = 0.783; TLI = 0.720; CFI = 0.782; GFI = 0.841; SRMR = 0.083; RMSEA = 0.147 (0.147-0.157); AIC = 657.39]; the estimated values of standardized regression weights are within the reference values (between 0.44 and 0.70). Cronbach’s alpha of the total is 0.84 and no item, if removed, increases Cronbach’s alpha. After consulting the modification indices, several correlations between errors were established but, in spite of that, a good model was not found. The authors decided to perform an Exploratory Factorial Analysis (EFA). To this end, the sample was randomly divided into two groups with almost the same number of participants: one group assigned to EFA (N = 387) and the other to subsequent CFA (N = 386). Factors were extracted by the maximum likelihood method and rotated by varimax rotation. The number of factors was decided in consideration of the parallel analysis, scree-plot, cumulative variance explained, interpretability, and Kaiser’s criterion. In the parallel analysis, 3 components had eigenvalues that were above the 95th percentile of eigenvalues of 1000 random datasets of the same dimension. The EFA resulted in a solution with ten items distributed by three factors (Table 2) that explains more than 60% of the variance, with good psychometric indicators. The structure obtained in EFA was confirmed by CFA, using the other half of the sample. While most of the indicators are within the benchmarks, some, notably, were not. We consulted the modification indices and found that they suggested three correlations between errors; once these correlations between errors were established (theoretically supported by the proximity of the content), the model turned out to be very good [χ2(31) = 2.54; IFI = 0.963; TLI = 0.946; CFI = 0.963; GFI = 0.959; SRMR = 0.083; RMSEA = 0.063 (0.046–0.081); AIC = 126.73]. Cronbach’s alpha (Table 2), composite reliability, average variance extracted (AVE) and AVE square roots of the Portuguese version of Cyberstalking Scale are within the recommended values (Table 3).

Online Harassment Scale

A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was carried out (N = 773) to confirm the unidimensional model proposed by the original authors. However, the model was not good [χ2(20) = 14.00; IFI = 0.875; TLI = 0.824; CFI = 0.874; GFI = 0.917; SRMR = 0.061; RMSEA = 0.130 (0.117–0.143); AIC = 312.02]; the estimated values of standardized regression weights are within the reference values (between 0.54 and 0.71). Cronbach’s alpha of the total is 0.83 and no item, if removed, increases Cronbach’s alpha. Also, in the parallel analysis, only 1 component had an eigenvalue above the 95th percentile of eigenvalues of 1000 random datasets of the same dimension. After consulting the modification indices, three correlations between errors were suggested and established and a very good model was found [χ2(17) = 4.11; IFI = 0.975; TLI = 0.958; CFI = 0.974; GFI = 0.977; SRMR = 0.030; RMSEA = 0.063 (0.048–0.079); AIC = 107.91]. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients (0.83) composite reliability (0.88), average variance extracted (AVE, 0.50) and square root of AVE (0.71) were calculated, being the values within the reference range. Mean (1.85) and standard deviation (0.87) were also calculated.

Flaming Behaviors Scale

A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was carried out (N = 773) to confirm the unidimensional model proposed by the original authors. However, the model was not good [χ2(5) = 17.77; IFI = 0.957; TLI = 0.913; CFI = 0.957; GFI = 0.953; SRMR = 0.038; RMSEA = 0.147 (0.121–0.145); AIC = 108.84]; the estimated values of standardized regression weights are within the reference values (between 0.65 and 0.83). Cronbach’s alpha of the total is 0.87 and no item, if removed, increases Cronbach’s alpha. Also, in the parallel analysis, only 1 component had an eigenvalue above the 95th percentile of eigenvalues of 1000 random datasets of the same dimension. After consulting the modification indices, two correlations between errors were suggested and established and a very good model was found [χ2(3) = 3.03; IFI = 0.997; TLI = 0.989; CFI = 0.997; GFI = 0.995; SRMR = 0.010; RMSEA = 0.051 (0.015–0.091); AIC = 33.10]. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients (0.87) composite reliability (0.91), average variance extracted (AVE, 0.67) and square root of AVE (0.82) were calculated, being the values within the reference range. Mean (2.10) and standard deviation (1.25) were also calculated.

Trolling Behaviors Scale

A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was carried out (N = 773) to confirm the unidimensional model proposed by the original authors. It was found a very good model [χ2(1) = 1.64; IFI = 0.999; TLI = 0.997; CFI = 0.999; GFI = 0.999; SRMR = 0.001; RMSEA = 0.029 (0.000–0.105); AIC = 11.64]; the estimated values of standardized regression weights are within the reference values (between 0.70 and 0.75). Cronbach’s alpha of the total is 0.76 and no item, if removed, increases Cronbach’s alpha. Also, in the parallel analysis, only 1 component had an eigenvalue above the 95th percentile of eigenvalues of 1000 random datasets of the same dimension. Composite reliability (0.86), average variance extracted (AVE, 0.68) and square root of AVE (0.82) were calculated, being the values within the reference range. Mean (1.56) and standard deviation (0.82) were also calculated.

3.2.3. Correlations between Psychological Variables

All psychological variables are positively and significantly correlated ranging from 0.150 (between Dirty Dozen Psychopathy and Cyberstalking Time) and 0.535 (between Flaming and Trolling); besides, all variables have a normal distribution and absence of multicollinearity (Table 4).

3.3. Specific Objective (2) to Establish Associations between the Variables under Study and the Sociodemographic Variables

Age correlates negatively and significantly with almost all the psychological variables: Internet Addiction (r = −0.420; p < 0.001), Dirty Dozen Dark Triad Machiavellianism (r = −0.217; p < 0.001), psychopathy (r = −0.171; p < 0.001), narcisism (r = −0.116; p < 0.001), cyberstalking total (r = −0.176; p < 0.001), cyberstalking justification (r = −0.181; p < 0.001), cyberstalking time (r = −0.243; p < 0.001), online harassment (r = −0.354; p < 0.001), flaming behaviors (r = −0.322; p < 0.001), and trolling behaviors (r = −0.196; p < 0.001). Years of education correlate positively and significantly with Dirty Dozen Dark Triad narcisism (r = 0.103; p < 0.001) and with cyberstalking justification (r = −0.072; p = 0.045).
Machiavellianism, psychopathy and narcissism are significantly higher in men than in women. The same goes for flaming and trolling. Cyberstalking justification and time, as well as online harassment, show higher values in women than in men (Table 5). Also, those who are not in a romantic relationship show higher values in Internet addiction, psychopathy, cyberstalking time, online harassment and flaming (Table 6).

3.4. Study 2

Specific Objective (3) to Look for Relationships between Personality Traits and Online Behaviors

Hierarchical regression analyses were applied to examine the personality predictors of specific online behaviors while controlling for gender and age (Table 7). Being female is associated with cyberstalking total, cyberstalking justification, cyberstalking time and online harassment; being male is associated with flaming and trolling. Age is positively associated with cyberstalking control and negatively with all the other dimensions. Machiavellianism is positively associated with all the dimensions of this study. Psychopathy is positively associated with cyberstalking total, cyberstalking control, flaming and trolling. At last, narcissism is positively associated with all the dimensions, except online harassment and flaming.

3.5. Specific Objective (4) to Determine Whether Online Behaviors Moderate the Relation between the Dimensions of the Dark Triad of Personality and Internet Addiction

To achieve this moderation, an index of online behaviors (cyberstalking, online harassment, flaming and trolling) was developed. The results of the moderation (Figure 1) showed that online behaviors moderate the relation between the dimensions of the dark triad of personality and internet addiction [χ2(2) = 2.30; IFI = 0.997; TLI = 0.984; CFI = 0.997; GFI = 0.998; SRMR = 0.013; RMSEA = 0.041 (0.000–0.092); AIC = 30.60]. Also, Machiavellianism, Psychopathy and Narcissism were indirectly associated with addiction to Internet through online behaviors.

4. Discussion

The main objective of this study was to assess the moderation effects of online behaviors between personality traits and Internet addiction. Thus, several specific objectives were delimited. The first objective was to evaluate the psychometric qualities of the instruments used in this study by assessing the fit to our sample of instruments previously validated for the Portuguese population (Internet Addiction Test and Dirty Dozen Dark Triad), and the validation for the Portuguese population of the Cyberstalking scale, Online Harassment scale, Flaming scale and Trolling scale. The results reveal good psychometric properties for the four validated scales, which allowed us to continue with our objectives.
As for the Cyberstalking scale, in the present study the one-dimensional structure was not verified, and it was found three factors were named cyberstalking control (includes items that monitor the other’s online behavior); cyberstalking justification (includes items that justify or normalize the acts committed) and cyberstalking time (includes items that refer to the time spent online in the practice of cyberstalking). The alpha values found are close to the original scale. One of the advantages of this scale is that it considered not only romantic relationships (past, current, and desired), but also included persecution of acquaintances and people that the perpetrator suspects/dislikes [51].
Relatively to the Online Harassment scale model found, it is in line with the proposal of the original authors [52]. As for the Flaming Behaviors Scale, the alpha value obtained was identical to the original author Turnage [7] and the study by Hwang et al. [53]. Confirmatory factor analysis revealed good scale adjustment indices, allowing confirmation of the unifactorial model of the original instrument. In turn, the Trolling Behaviors Scale in the present sample proved to be a reliable measure to evaluate this online malicious behavior.
The adaptation of these instruments may constitute a contribution to research in the context of these online antisocial behaviors (cyberstalking, online harassment, flaming and trolling) in a Portuguese context; the results that individuals obtain in the analyzed behavior can be related to other variables that help to understand the complexity of it.
The second objective was to establish associations between the variables under study and the sociodemographic variables. The results show that age correlates negatively and significantly with most of the psychological variables, namely: Internet addiction, Dirty Dozen Dark Triad Machiavellianism, psychopathy, narcissism, cyberbullying total, cyberbullying justification, cyberbullying time, online harassment, flaming behaviors, and trolling behaviors. That is, as a person age they exhibit fewer problematic online behaviors. This can be explained due to exposure to technology from a very young age, it may be that older people have less technical competencies to use the Internet and especially social networks. In this sense, most investigations indicate that younger individuals tend to be more Internet addicts [1], with age being considered in the literature as a risk factor. Young people are more directed to the use of technology (e.g., communicating, playing), and are more likely to have problems related to its use unlike older people, who use it mainly for work purposes [62].
Concerning the years of education, they are positively and significantly correlated with the Dirty Dozen Dark Triad narcissism and with the justification of cyberbullying. With respect to the sex variable, Machiavellianism, psychopathy and narcissism are significantly higher in men than in women. The same is true for flaming and trolling. Cyberstalking justification and time, as well as online harassment show higher values in women than in men. These results contrast with other results that find similar values for men and women [9]. Although traditional stalking is mostly practiced by male perpetrators, women are more likely to engage in covert stalking behaviors such as cyberstalking and online harassment [51].
The third objective was to search for relationships between personality traits and online behaviors. The results of our study show that Machiavellianism is positively associated with all dimensions of the studied variables. Psychopathy is positively associated with cyberbullying total, cyberbullying control, flaming and trolling; and narcissism was positively associated with most dimensions, except with online harassment and flaming. These results are in agreement with other studies showing similar results [9,16,20]. For example, Machiavellianism has been associated with problematic network use, trolling with online games, and online self-promotion, which is consistent with the explanation given by other studies indicating that this correlation is expected, as Machiavellianism manipulative behavior can be shown to be problematic on the Internet, being violent and controlling [2,20,37].
For its part, narcissism is associated with online self-promotion behaviors and trying to be more popular, which can cause problematic Internet use [20,32,38,39]; this coincides with our results. Given that individuals with a high level of narcissistic trait tend to focus extensively on themselves; and to be characterized by the dominance, grandeur and devaluation of others, it is expected that they use the possibilities of social media to promote and exalt themselves [25]. This process can occur through the search for approval and admiration of others on social networks leading to the employment of excessive time on the Internet [32].
Psychopathy has been associated with cyberbullying, trolling, intimate partner cyberbullying, and violent gaming [9,20,32]. This is in line with our findings, which can be explained by emotional dysregulation and the search for greater sensations, as referred to above. The involvement of individuals with psychopathic traits in online antisocial behaviors can be facilitated by the characteristics of the Internet itself, namely anonymity and difficulty in identifying the aggressor [17].
And finally, the last objective was to determine whether dimensions of the dark triad of personality explain online behaviors and these last moderates the relation between dak traid and Internet addiction. Our results reveal that Machiavellianism, Psychopathy and Narcissism are associated with Internet addiction through online behaviors. These associations reinforce the behavioral characteristics of these individuals and how they may behave on the Internet, allowing us to suggest that dimensions of the dark triad of personality explain online behaviors [4,9,10,16,20,32,34,35,38,39,63]. Cyberstalking also requires excessive time to investigate public posts from different social media accounts and victim profiles [20]. This compulsive impulse to persecute others gives rise to excessive social media use, gaining the Internet great importance in the lives of individuals and tolerance for the time spent online, characteristic symptoms of addiction to the Internet [2,8].

5. Conclusions

The results of this study have theoretical and practical implications, since, on the one hand, they reinforces the findings of other studies showing that dimensions of the dark personality triad play an important role in Internet and social network addition [20,21], contributing to the literature; and, on the other hand, on a practical level, they allow to conduct awareness campaigns in communities, schools, and work to understand that one can be exposed to unpleasant situations due to behaviors that some people with personality traits of Machiavellianism, narcissism and/or psychopathy can cause problems affecting the mental, emotional and psychological health of others.
Despite these results, some limitations should be addressed, namely, the use of self-report instruments that may somehow influence the data obtained, being susceptible to arbitrariness of responses, subjectivity of participants in the perception of content and tendency to give socially desirable answers. Additionally, the cross-sectional nature of the study is highlighted, which leads to the impossibility of establishing cause and effect relationships between the variables.
Future studies may investigate the issue of Internet dependencies versus Internet dependency, to understand whether the addition, when occurs, is originated in relation to activities on the Internet (e.g., social networks, gambling, shopping) or if it is in relation to the Internet itself. Future investigations should continue to study the addition to the Internet and online antisocial behavior including specific samples of clinical/forensic context, involving individuals with diagnoses of personality disorders that constitute the Dark Triad.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Â.L. and A.P.M.; methodology, Â.L.; software, Â.L.; validation, Â.L.; formal analysis, Â.L.; investigation, Â.L. and A.P.M.; resources, Â.L.; data curation, Â.L.; writing—original draft preparation, Â.L., S.C. and A.P.M.; writing—review and editing, Â.L., S.C. and A.P.M.; visualization, Â.L., S.C. and A.P.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Universidade Católica Portuguesa (protocol code AL202134 in November 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data used in this study are available upon request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Original and Portuguese versions of the instruments validated in this study
Cyberstalking Scale [51]Escala de Cybertalking
1. I usually find the social media of someone I’m interested in, even if it takes hours. Costumo encontrar as redes sociais de alguém que me suscite interesse, mesmo que demore horas.
2. It’s ok to check who likes and comments on the posts of your partner. Acho aceitável verificar quem “gosta” e comenta os posts de um(a) parceiro(a) amoroso(a).
3. I lose track of time searching for information about my acquaintances on the Internet. Perco a noção do tempo à procura de informações acerca dos meus conhecidos na Internet.
4. It is normal to “keep an eye” on the social media of someone who frequently interacts with your partner. É normal estar atenta às redes sociais de alguém que interage frequentemente com o meu (minha) parceiro(a) amoroso (a).
5. If a person hides their messages, I look for other ways to find out the content of them.Se uma pessoa esconde muito as suas mensagens, procuro outras formas de as descobrir.
6. If I had my partner’s social media passwords, my life would be easier.Se eu tivesse as passwords das redes sociais do(a) meu(minha) parceiro(a), a minha vida seria mais fácil.
7. If I could I would look at my love partner’s browsing history.Se eu pudesse, veria o histórico de navegação do(a) meu(minha) parceiro(a) amoroso(a).
8. I prefer to form relationships with people that I can investigate on social media.Prefiro relacionar-me com pessoas que possa investigar nas redes sociais.
9. I check what kind of apps my partner uses on their phone.Verifico que tipo de aplicações o(a) meu (minha) parceiro(a) amoroso(a) utiliza no seu telemóvel.
10. When you’re interested in someone, it’s not wrong to look at their acquaintances’ social media, in order to get to know them better.Quando se está interessado em alguém, não tem mal ver as redes sociais das pessoas próximas dele(a) para conhecê-lo(a) melhor.
Online Harassment Scale [52]Escala de Assédio Online
1. Embarrassed on purpose.Envergonhado(a) de propósito.
2. Called offensive names.Alvo de nomes ofensivos.
3. Unwanted sexual messages.Mensagens sexuais indesejadas.
4. Threats of physical sexual violence.Ameaças de violência sexual física.
5. Threats of physical non-sexual violence.Ameaças de violência física não sexual.
6. Hurt emotionally or psychologically.Ferido emocionalmente ou psicologicamente.
7. Used information from your social media profile in a way that made you uncomfortable.Utilizaram informações do seu perfil das redes sociais de uma forma que o(a) deixou desconfortável.
8. Repeatedly contacted in a way that made you feel afraid or unsafe.Repetidamente contactado de uma maneira que o fez sentir com medo ou inseguro(a).
Flaming Behaviours Scale [7] Escala de Comportamentos de Flaming
1. In the community, I tend to use swear or harsh words. (Aggression)Na comunidade, costumo usar palavrões ou palavras duras. (Agressão)
2. In the community, I tend to intimidate people who get on my nerves. (Intimidation)Na comunidade, costumo intimidar as pessoas que me enervam. (Intimidação)
3. In the community, I tend to insult people who I hate. (Insults)Na comunidade, costumo insultar as pessoas que odeio. (Insultos)
4. In the community, I tend to make vulgar jokes. (Uninhibited language)Na comunidade, costumo fazer piadas ordinárias. (Linguagem desinibida)
5. In the community, I tend to make sarcastic remarks about others opinion. (Sarcasm)Na comunidade, costumo fazer comentários sarcásticos sobre a opinião dos outros (Sarcasmo)
Trolling Behaviors Scale [54]Escala de Comportamentos de Trolling
1. Debating various topics with the intention to irritate/upset others.Debater vários tópicos com a intenção de irritar/perturbar os outros.
2. Trolling’ on public forums.Trolling em fóruns públicos.
3. I like to post memes and comments with the intent to aggravate or annoy others.Gosto de postar memes e comentários com a intenção de aborrecer ou irritar os outros.

Appendix B

Items description (N = 773)
MSDSk
(SD = 0.088)
Kr
(SD = 0.176)
Internet Addiction Test Items(0–5)
1. Do you find that you stay on-line longer than you intended?3.281.05−0.53−0.09
2. Do you neglect household chores to spend more time on-line?2.191.150.12−0.48
3. Do you prefer the excitement of the Internet to intimacy with your partner?0.810.921.563.49
4. Do you form new relationships with fellow on-line users?1.720.980.570.14
5. Do others in your life complain to you about the amount of time you spend on-line?1.771.110.70−0.04
6. Do your grades or school work suffer because of the amount of time you spend on-line?1.371.160.810.33
7. Do you check your e-mail before something else that you need to do?2.921.20−0.20−0.56
8. Does your job performance or productivity suffer because of the Internet?1.821.150.56−0.27
9. Do you become defensive or secretive when anyone asks you what you do on-line?1.460.981.091.54
10. Do you block out disturbing thoughts about your life with soothing thoughts of the Internet?1.331.121.321.73
11. Do you find yourself anticipating when you will go on-line again?1.451.021.131.50
12. Do you fear that life without the Internet would be boring, empty, and joyless?2.021.250.61−0.32
13. Do you snap, yell, or act annoyed if someone bothers you while you are on-line?1.380.911.041.63
14. Do you lose sleep due to late-night log-ins?2.111.280.43−0.69
15. Do you feel preoccupied with the Internet when off-line, or fantasize about being on-line?1.400.971.201.71
16. Do you find yourself saying “just a few more minutes” when on-line?2.491.330.06−0.95
17. Do you try to cut down the amount of time you spend on-line and fail?2.021.170.44−0.37
18. Do you try to hide how long you’ve been on-line?1.290.871.292.31
19. Do you choose to spend more time on-line over going out with others?1.551.090.890.43
20. Do you feel depressed, moody, or nervous when you are off-line, which goes away once you are back on-line?1.310.911.091.48
Dirty Dozen Dark Triad Items(1–5)
1. I tend to manipulate others to get my way.2.431.090.33−0.77
2. I have used deceit or lied to get my way.2.581.210.13−1.29
3. I have use flattery to get my way.2.471.200.25−1.22
4. I tend to exploit others towards my own end.1.620.811.291.36
5. I tend to lack remorse.1.861.051.210.84
6. I tend to not be too concerned with morality or the morality of my actions.1.620.951.732.67
7. I tend to be callous or insensitive.1.951.080.91−0.19
8. I tend to be cynical.1.821.001.180.74
9. I tend to want others to admire me.2.611.210.10−1.16
10. I tend to want others to pay attention to me.2.681.190.05−1.08
11. I tend to seek prestige or status.2.421.230.40−0.95
12. I tend to expect special favors from others.1.810.971.160.86
Cyberstalking Scale Items(1–5)
1. I usually find the social media of someone I’m interested in, even if it takes hours.2.301.260.64−0.78
2. It’s ok to check who likes and comments on the posts of your partner.2.451.170.24−0.97
3. I lose track of time searching for information about my acquaintances on the Internet.1.901.001.050.33
4. It is normal to “keep an eye” on the social media of someone who frequently interacts with your partner.2.181.130.61−0.57
5. If a person hides their messages, I look for other ways to find out the content of them.1.901.101.110.30
6. If I had my partner’s social media passwords, my life would be easier.1.380.701.883.11
7. If I could I would look at my love partner’s browsing history.1.510.861.873.32
8. I prefer to form relationships with people that I can investigate on social media.1.490.781.632.23
9. I check what kind of apps my partner uses on their phone.1.540.871.722.62
10. When you’re interested in someone, it’s not wrong to look at their acquaintances’ social media, in order to get to know them better.2.651.20−0.03−1.13
Online Harassment Scale Items(1–7)
1. Embarrassed on purpose.1.911.211.501.93
2. Called offensive names.2.091.441.300.89
3. Unwanted sexual messages.2.301.671.110.01
4. Threats of physical sexual violence.1.200.694.5023.47
5. Threats of physical non-sexual violence.1.400.942.888.98
6. Hurt emotionally or psychologically.2.451.580.93−0.06
7. Used information from your social media profile in a way that made you uncomfortable.1.641.182.063.70
8. Repeatedly contacted in a way that made you feel afraid or unsafe.1.801.271.832.93
Flaming Behaviours Scale Items(1–7)
1. In the community, I tend to use swear or harsh words. (Aggression)2.201.691.300.63
2. In the community, I tend to intimidate people who get on my nerves. (Intimidation)1.671.222.083.98
3. In the community, I tend to insult people who I hate. (Insults)1.751.332.013.57
4. In the community, I tend to make vulgar jokes. (Uninhibited language)2.101.621.461.22
5. In the community, I tend to make sarcastic remarks about others opinion. (Sarcasm)2.761.840.71−0.66
Trolling Behaviors Scale Items(1–5)
1. Debating various topics with the intention to irritate/upset others1.661.051.641.89
2. Trolling’ on public forums1.380.862.445.44
3. I like to post memes and comments with the intent to aggravate or annoy others1.651.061.591.53
M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Sk = skweness; Kr = kurtosis.

References

  1. Monteiro, A.P.; Sousa, M.; Correia, E. Adição à Internet e Caraterísticas de Personalidade: Um Estudo com Estudantes Universitários Portugueses. Revista Portuguesa de Educação 2020, 33, 159–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Kuss, D.J.; Griffiths, M.D.; Karila, L.; Billieux, J. Internet addiction: A systematic review of epidemiological research for the last decade. Curr. Pharm. Design. 2014, 20, 4026–4052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Young, K.; Pistner, M.; O’Mara, J.; Buchanan, J. Cyber disorders: The mental health concern for the new millennium. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 1999, 2, 475–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Buckels, E.E.; Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. Behavioral confirmation of everyday Sadism. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 2201–2209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Sani, A.I.; Valquaresma, J. Cyberstalking: Prevalência e estratégias de coping em estudantes portugueses do ensino secundário. Avances en Psicología Latinoamericana 2020, 38, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Lindsay, B.; Megan; Krysik, J. Online Harassment Among College Students: A Replication Study Incorporating New Internet Trends. 2011. Available online: http://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1941065 (accessed on 30 March 2023).
  7. Turnage, A.K. Email flaming behaviors and organizational conflict. J. Comput. Mediat. Commun. 2007, 13, 43–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Griffiths, M.D. A “components” model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. J. Subst. Use 2005, 10, 191–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Smoker, M.; March, E. Predicting perpetration of intimate partner cyberstalking: Gender and the Dark Tetrad. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 72, 390–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Manuoğlu, E.; Öner-Özkan, B. Sarcastic and Deviant Trolling in Turkey: Associations with Dark Triad and Aggression. Soc. Media Soc. 2022, 8, 20563051221126053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Willard, N.; An Educator’s Guide to Cyberbullying. Center for Responsible Internet Use. 2005. Available online: http://www.cyberbully.org/docs/cbct.educators.pdf (accessed on 30 March 2023).
  12. Nova, F.F.; Rifat, M.R.; Saha, P.; Ahmed, S.I.; Guha, S. Online sexual harassment over anonymous social media in Bangladesh. In Proceedings of the Tenth International Conference on Information and Communication Technologies and Development, Ahmedabad, India, 4–7 January 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Pashang, S.; Khanlou, N.; Clarke, J. The mental health impact of cyber sexual violence on youth identity. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2019, 17, 1119–1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Stevens, F.; Nurse, J.R.; Arief, B. Cyber stalking, cyber harassment, and adult mental health: A systematic review. Cyberpsychology. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2021, 24, 367–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Hinduja, S.; Patchin, J.W. Bullying Beyond the Schoolyard: Preventing and Responding to Cyberbullying, 2nd ed.; Corwin, A Sage Company: London, UK, 2015; p. 65. [Google Scholar]
  16. Moor, L.; Anderson, J.R. A systematic literature review of the relationship between dark personality traits and antisocial online behaviours. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2019, 144, 40–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. O’Higgins Norman, J. Tackling Bullying from the Inside Out: Shifting Paradigms in Bullying Research and Interventions. Int. J. Bullying Prev. 2020, 2, 161–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Smith, P.K. School bullying. Sociologia Problemas e Práticas 2013, 71, 81–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Bogolyubova, O.; Panicheva, P.; Tikhonov, R.; Ivanov, V.; Ledovaya, Y. Dark personalities on Facebook: Harmful online behaviors and language. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 78, 151–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Kircaburun, K.; Griffiths, M. Instagram addiction and the Big Five of personality: The mediating role of self-liking. J. Behav. Addict. 2018, 7, 158–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kircaburun, K.; Demetrovics, Z.; Tosuntaş, Ş.B. Analyzing the links between problematic social media use, dark triad traits, and self-esteem. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2019, 17, 1496–1507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Paulhus, D.L.; Williams, K.M. The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. J. Res. Pers. 2002, 36, 556–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Paulhus, D.L. Toward a taxonomy of dark personalities. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 23, 421–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Furnham, A.; Richards, S.C.; Paulhus, D.L. The Dark Triad of personality: A 10 year review. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2013, 7, 199–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Salessi, S.; Omar, A. Propriedades psicométricas de uma escala para medir o lado escuro da personalidade. Estudos de Psicologia 2018, 35, 159–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Bonfá-Araujo, B.; Lima-Costa, A.R.; Hauck-Filho, N.; Jonason, P.K. Considering sadism in the shadow of the Dark Triad traits: A meta-analytic review of the Dark Tetrad. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2022, 197, 111767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Pineda, D.; Martínez-Martínez, A.; Galán, M.; Rico-Bordera, P.; Piqueras, J.A. The Dark Tetrad and online sexual victimization: Enjoying in the distance. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2023, 142, 107659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Wai, M.; Tiliopoulos, N. The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 794–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Di Blasi, M.; Giardina, A.; Coco, G.L.; Giordano, C.; Billieux, J.; Schimmenti, A. A compensatory model to understand dysfunctional personality traits in problematic gaming: The role of vulnerable narcissism. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2020, 160, 109921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Czibor, A.; Szabo, Z.P.; Jones, D.N.; Zsido, A.N.; Paal, T.; Szijjarto, L.; Carre, J.R.; Bereczkei, T. Male and female face of Machiavellianism: Opportunism or anxiety? Pers. Individ. Differ. 2017, 117, 221–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hare, R.D. Without Conscience: The Disturbing World of the Psychopaths Among Us; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999; p. 6. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kircaburun, K.; Griffiths, M.D. The dark side of the Internet: Preliminary evidence for the associations of dark personality traits with specific online activities and problematic Internet use. J. Behav. Addict. 2018, 7, 993–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Buckels, E.E.; Trapnell, P.D.; Paulhus, D.L. Trolls just want to have fun. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2014, 67, 97–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Lopes, B.; Yu, H. Who do you troll and Why: An investigation into the relationship between the Dark Triad Personalities and online trolling behaviours towards popular and less popular Facebook profiles. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 77, 69–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Chui, X.; Li, Y.; Wang, P.; Zeng, P.; Lei, L. Social support and cyberbullying for university students: The mediating role of Internet addiction and the moderating role of stress. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 42, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Ko, C.H.; Liu, T.L.; Wang, P.W.; Chen, C.S.; Yen, C.F.; Yen, J.Y. The exacerbation of depression, hostility, and social anxiety in the course of Internet addiction among adolescents: A prospective study. Compr. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 1377–1384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Sindermann, C.; Sariyska, R.; Lachmann, B.; Brand, M.; Montag, C. Associations between the dark triad of personality and unspecified/specific forms of Internet-use disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 2018, 7, 985–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Alloway, T.; Runac, R.; Quershi, M.; Kemp, G. Is Facebook linked to selfishness? Investigating the relationships among social media use, empathy, and narcissism. Soc. Netw. 2014, 3, 150–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Pantic, I.; Milanovic, A.; Loboda, B.; Błachnio, A.; Przepiorka, A.; Nesic, D.; Mazic, S.; Dugalic, S.; Ristic, S. Association between physiological oscillations in self-esteem, narcissism and Internet addiction: A cross-sectional study. Psychiatry Res. 2017, 258, 239–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Carton, H.; Egan, V. The dark triad and intimate partner violence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2017, 105, 84–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gregoire, J. ITC guidelines for translating and adapting tests. Int. J. Test. 2018, 18, 101–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Brislin, R.W. Cross-cultural research methods. In Environment and Culture; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1980; pp. 47–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Young, K. Clinical assessment of Internet-addicted clients. In Internet Addiction: A Handbook and Guide to Evaluation and Treatment; Young, K., Abreu, C., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2011; pp. 19–34. [Google Scholar]
  44. Young, K. Caught in the Net: How to Recognize the Signs of Internet Addiction; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1998; p. 55. [Google Scholar]
  45. Widyanto, L.; McMurran, M. The psychometric properties of the Internet addiction test. Cyberpsychology Behav. 2004, 7, 443–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Lee, K.; Lee, H.K.; Gyeong, H.; Yu, B.; Song, Y.M.; Kim, D. Reliability and validity of the Korean version of the Internet Addiction Test among college students. J. Korean Med. Sci. 2013, 28, 763–768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Osada, H. Internet addiction in Japanese college students: Is Japanese version of Internet Addiction Test (JIAT) useful as a screening tool. Bull. Senshu Univ. Sch. Hum. Sci. 2013, 3, 71–80. [Google Scholar]
  48. Pontes, H.M.; Patrão, I.M.; Griffiths, M.D. Portuguese validation of the Internet Addiction Test: An empirical study. J. Behav. Addict. 2014, 3, 107–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Jonason, P.K.; Webster, G.D. The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad. Psychol. Assess 2010, 22, 420–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Pechorro, P.; Jonason, P.K.; Raposo, V.; Maroco, J. Dirty Dozen: A concise measure of Dark Triad traits among at-risk youths. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 40, 3522–3531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Silva Santos, I.L.; Pimentel, C.E.; Mariano, T.E. Cyberstalking scale: Development and relations with gender, FOMO and social media engagement. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 4802–4810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Lewis, S.C.; Zamith, R.; Coddington, M. Online harassment and its implications for the journalist–audience relationship. Digit. J. 2020, 8, 1047–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Hwang, J.; Lee, H.; Kim, K.; Zo, H.; Ciganek, A.P. Cyber neutralisation and flaming. Behav. Inf. Technol. 2016, 35, 210–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Howard, K.; Zolnierek, K.H.; Critz, K.; Dailey, S.; Ceballos, N. An examination of psychosocial factors associated with malicious online trolling behaviors. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2019, 149, 309–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Brown, T.A. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  56. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S.; Ullman, J.B. Using Multivariate Statistics; Pearson: Boston, MA, USA; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013; Volume 6, pp. 497–516. [Google Scholar]
  57. Cohen, I.; Huang, Y.; Chen, J.; Benesty, J.; Benesty, J.; Chen, J.; Cohen, I. Pearson Correlation Coefficient. Noise Reduction in Speech Processing; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2009; pp. 1–4. [Google Scholar]
  58. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  59. O’connor, B.P. SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 2000, 32, 396–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Fornell, C.; Larcker, D.F. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Turney, S.; Skewness | Definition, Examples & Formula. Scribbr. 2022. Available online: https://www.scribbr.com/statistics/skewness/ (accessed on 2 February 2023).
  62. Buckner, J.; Castille, C.; Sheets, T. The five-factor model of personality and employees’ excessive use of technology. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2012, 28, 1947–1953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Molenda, Z.A.; Marchlewska, M.; Rogoza, M.; Michalski, P.; Górska, P.; Szczepańska, D.; Cislak, A. What makes an Internet troll? On the relationships between temperament (BIS/BAS), Dark Triad, and Internet trolling. Cyberpsychol. J. Psychosoc. Res. Cyberspace 2022, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Final model.
Figure 1. Final model.
Ijerph 20 06136 g001
Table 1. Questionnaire about Internet use on mobile phone, computer or other devices (N = 773).
Table 1. Questionnaire about Internet use on mobile phone, computer or other devices (N = 773).
N%
How much time do you dedicate to the Internet per day, in non-academic or professional tasks?1Up to 2 h18523.9
2Between 2 to 4 h25332.7
3Between 4 to 6 h23430.3
4More than 6 h10113.1
Identify how you use the Internet (excluding academic or professional tasks).
NoYes
N%N%
1—Consult email10213.267186.8
2—Social networks233.075097.0
3—Listen to music online8511.068889.0
4—Looking for new friends62280.515119.5
5—Search for information and news in general9812.767587.3
6—Play online47561.429838.6
7—Communicate and relate to others8110.569289.5
8—Online shopping26334.051066.0
9—Digress on the Internet22328.855071.2
10—Watch movies/series15920.661479.4
11—Betting sites67987.89412.2
12—Search for adult content59877.417522.6
N = frequencies; % = percentages.
Table 2. Cyberstalking Scale: Exploratory Factorial Analysis.
Table 2. Cyberstalking Scale: Exploratory Factorial Analysis.
(N = 387) (10 Items)
h2F1F2F3
ControlJustificationTime
1. I usually find the social media of someone I’m interested in, even if it takes hours.0.7040.0340.2800.791
2. It’s ok to check who likes and comments on the posts of your partner.0.7410.0930.8370.177
3. I lose track of time searching for information about my acquaintances on the Internet.0.7380.2280.1510.814
4. It is normal to “keep an eye” on the social media of someone who frequently interacts with your partner.0.7470.1690.8410.107
5. If a person hides their messages, I look for other ways to find out the content of them.0.5150.3500.6170.108
6. If I had my partner’s social media passwords, my life would be easier.0.7500.8390.2060.070
7. If I could I would look at my love partner’s browsing history.0.7110.7910.2910.019
8. I prefer to form relationships with people that I can investigate on social media.0.7020.702−0.0150.457
9. I check what kind of apps my partner uses on her/his phone.0.5620.6780.2820.153
10. When you’re interested in someone, it’s not wrong to look at their acquaintances’ social media, in order to get to know them better.0.5370.2710.6200.281
Eigenvalues 4.321.301.10
Total variance explained (67.07%) 43.2312.9810.87
Cronbach’s alfa (α)0.840.810.790.65
Correlation matrix range [0.30–0.90]0.19–0.69
Determinant score [above 0.00001]0.023
Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (df); p < 0.051441.77 (45); <0.001
Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure (KMO) (above 0.50)0.846
Diagonal element anti-correlation matrix (above 0.50)0.81–0.91
h2 = communalities; F = factor.
Table 3. Correlations, Cronbach’s alpha, composite reliability, average variance extracted (AVE), AVE square roots, mean and standard deviation of the Dirty Dozen Dark Triad (N = 773).
Table 3. Correlations, Cronbach’s alpha, composite reliability, average variance extracted (AVE), AVE square roots, mean and standard deviation of the Dirty Dozen Dark Triad (N = 773).
Pearson’s Correlations
1234αCRAVEMean
(Standard Deviation)
1. Cyberstalking Total0.71 0.840.880.501.93 (0.66)
2. Cyberstalking Control0.80 **0.75 0.810.840.571.48 (0.64)
3. Cyberstalking Justification0.89 **0.55 **0.73 0.790.820.542.30 (0.89)
4. Cyberstalking Time0.69 **0.38 **0.45 **0.800.650.780.642.10 (0.97)
Note: ** p < 0.001; α = Cronbach’s alpha; CR = composite reliability; AVE = average variance extracted; bold (diagonal) = AVE square roots.
Table 4. Pearson correlations between variables.
Table 4. Pearson correlations between variables.
1234567891011
1. Adiction Internet Total
2. Dirty Dozen Machiavellianism0.336 **
3. Dirty Dozen Psychopathy0.168 **0.300 **
4. Dirty Dozen Narcissism0.254 **0.510 **0.278 **
5. Cyberstalking Total0.358 **0.399 **0.241 **0.430 **
6. Cyberstalking Control0.234 **0.307 **0.254 **0.327 **0.803 **
7. Cyberstalking Justification0.290 **0.343 **0.181 **0.383 **0.892 **0.554 **
8. Cyberstalking Time0.372 **0.318 **0.150 **0.322 **0.690 **0.384 **0.452 **
9. Online Harassment Total0.319 **0.330 **0.176 **0.183 **0.309 **0.176 **0.276 **0.307 **
10. Flaming Total0.291 **0.374 **0.343 **0.272 **0.247 **0.219 **0.204 **0.173 **0.378 **
11. TrollingTotal0.221 **0.310 **0.278 **0.247 **0.224 **0.188 **0.176 **0.188 **0.278 **0.535 **
Skweness0.2120.1880.9830.2120.6401.4240.4570.6881.5471.4041.744
Kurtosis0.100−0.7150.632−0.712−0.0531.827−0.297−0.3542.9181.6582.650
Tolerance 0.6260.8130.6620.8770.6390.5870.7130.7610.6060.686
VIF 1.5981.2301.5111.1401.5641.7041.4021.3141.6491.457
Note: ** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Differences in psychological variables concerning sex.
Table 5. Differences in psychological variables concerning sex.
SexNMSDt(771) pd
1. Adiction Internet TotalMan3061.780.67−0.110.9100.64
Woman4671.790.62
2. Dirty Dozen MachiavellianismMan3062.360.822.470.0140.80
Woman4672.220.80
3. Dirty Dozen PsychopathyMan3061.930.783.48<0.0010.72
Woman4671.740.68
4. Dirty Dozen NarcissismMan3062.500.952.920.0040.92
Woman4672.300.90
5. Cyberstalking TotalMan3061.850.62−2.870.0040.65
Woman4671.980.68
6. Cyberstalking ControlMan3061.510.630.990.3220.64
Woman4671.460.64
7. Cyberstalking JustificationMan3062.150.79−3.89<0.0010.89
Woman4672.390.94
8. Cyberstalking TimeMan3061.920.88−4.11<0.0010.96
Woman4672.211.01
9. Online Harassment TotalMan3061.780.76−1.970.0490.87
Woman4671.900.94
10. Flaming TotalMan3062.481.426.66<0.0011.22
Woman4671.851.06
11. TrollingTotalMan3061.780.935.70<0.0010.80
Woman4671.420.70
Note. N = frequencies; M = Mean; SD = Standard deviation; t = t test; p = significance; d = Cohen’s d.
Table 6. Differences in psychological variables concerning romantic relationship.
Table 6. Differences in psychological variables concerning romantic relationship.
Romantic
Relationship
NMSDt(771)pd
1. Adiction Internet TotalNo3311.970.637.19<0.0010.62
Yes4421.650.61
2. Dirty Dozen MachiavellianismNo3312.290.800.600.5480.81
Yes4422.260.81
3 Dirty Dozen PsychopathyNo3311.890.772.410.0160.73
Yes4421.760.69
4. Dirty Dozen NarcissismNo3312.380.930.040.9690.93
Yes4422.380.93
5. Cyberstalking TotalNo3311.950.610.890.3710.66
Yes4421.910.69
6. Cyberstalking ControlNo3311.480.620.030.9750.64
Yes4421.480.65
7. Cyberstalking JustificationNo3312.250.78−1.300.1930.89
Yes4422.330.97
8. Cyberstalking TimeNo3312.310.985.35<0.0010.96
Yes4421.940.93
9. Online Harassment TotalNo3311.940.852.630.0090.87
Yes4421.780.88
10. Flaming TotalNo3312.211.242.070.0391.25
Yes4422.021.26
11. TrollingTotalNo3311.610.841.360.1760.82
Yes4421.530.81
Note. N = frequencies; M = Mean; SD = Standard deviation; t = t test; p = significance; d = Cohen’s d.
Table 7. Summary of the hierarchical regression analyses predicting different online behaviors.
Table 7. Summary of the hierarchical regression analyses predicting different online behaviors.
β (t)
Cyberstalking
Total
Cyberstalking
Control
Cyberstalking
Justification
Cyberstalking
Time
Online
Harassment
FlamingTrolling
Block 1Gender0.163 (5.222) ***0.023 (0.679) ns0.185 (5.734) ***0.182 (5.579) ***0.095 (2.913) **−0.200 (−6.491) ***−0.169 (−5.119) ***
Age−0.075 (−2.353) *0.075 (2.185) *−0.098 (−2.974) **−0.172 (−5.160) ***−0.286 (−8.594) ***−0.238 (−7.590) ***−0.123 (−3.632) ***
Block 2Machiavellianism0.214 (5.766) ***0.173 (4.347) ***0.175 (4.585) ***0.174 (4.486) ***0.250 (6.472) ***0.216 (5.901) ***0.180 (4.587) ***
Psychopathy0.101 (3.049 **0.161 (4.532) ***0.055 (1.617) ns0.029 (0.832) ns0.060 (1.720) ns0.195 (5.976) ***0.159 (4.538) ***
Narcissism0.301(8.287) ***0.205 (5.250) ***0.286 (7.630) ***0.225 (5.907) ***0.016 (0.422) ns0.059 (1.655) ns0.079 (2.048) *
R2adj0.2640.1540.2170.1950.1980.2850.174
F(3, 767)79.87447.90156.57538.81523.94544.12224.591
p<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
β = standardized Beta; t = t test; *** = p < 0.001; ** = p < 0.010; * = p < 0.050; ns = non significance.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Leite, Â.; Cardoso, S.; Monteiro, A.P. Dark Personality Traits and Online Behaviors: Portuguese Versions of Cyberstalking, Online Harassment, Flaming and Trolling Scales. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 6136. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20126136

AMA Style

Leite Â, Cardoso S, Monteiro AP. Dark Personality Traits and Online Behaviors: Portuguese Versions of Cyberstalking, Online Harassment, Flaming and Trolling Scales. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(12):6136. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20126136

Chicago/Turabian Style

Leite, Ângela, Susana Cardoso, and Ana Paula Monteiro. 2023. "Dark Personality Traits and Online Behaviors: Portuguese Versions of Cyberstalking, Online Harassment, Flaming and Trolling Scales" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 12: 6136. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20126136

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop