1. Introduction
In 2021, the World Health Organization released a global report on ageism [
1]. Although the original definition of ageism focused on prejudice between age groups [
2], the WHO expanded this definition to include age-based stereotypes (i.e., beliefs), age-based prejudices (i.e., feelings), and age-based discrimination (i.e., behaviors) [
1]. The WHO report reflects the large and growing area of research showing the prevalence of ageism worldwide, as manifested in negative attitudes toward older adults (e.g., [
3,
4,
5,
6,
7,
8,
9]) and older people’s own reported experiences of being the targets of ageist comments and behavior (e.g., [
10,
11]). A recent systematic review detailed the numerous harms arising from ageism, including worsened mental, physical, and cognitive health; poor quality of life; lower well-being; poor social relationships and social isolation; and reduced longevity [
12]. Given the negative impacts of ageism on older people, it is vital to understand how ageism is experienced in order to develop interventions to counteract it.
A recent example of how ageism is experienced came from a national poll of American older adults, which found that 82% experienced one or more forms of ageism (ageist messages in media, internalized ageism, and ageism in interpersonal interactions) in their daily lives [
13]. Of those reporting ageism experiences, 45% encountered ageism in interpersonal interactions, which took the form of age-based assumptions about their physical, sensory, and cognitive abilities as well as beliefs that they needed assistance in tasks they could complete independently [
13].
Similar themes emerged in a study by Chasteen et al. [
14], which provided a relatively detailed picture of ageism experienced by a mostly American sample of young, middle-aged, and older adults. Participants’ written descriptions of an ageism incidents were coded for the life domain (e.g., work, public space), type of perpetrator (e.g., employer/co-worker, stranger), and type of act (e.g., unwanted help, lack of respect). They found that older adults experienced ageism when receiving goods and services, at work, and in public spaces, whereas younger age groups reported experiencing ageism most often at work. Consistent with that pattern, the perpetrators of ageist acts tended to match the domain in which the prejudiced behavior occurred (e.g., by a stranger in a public space). In terms of different types of ageist acts, older adults encountered incorrect assumptions about themselves, unwanted help, and a lack of respect, and this differed from the younger age groups, who also reported experiencing some incorrect assumptions and a lack of respect, but no unwanted help.
Although the findings of Chasteen et al. [
14] provide a more nuanced understanding of the shape and form that ageism experiences can take, more research is needed to determine whether the patterns observed can be generalized to other older adults. Moreover, research on ageist attitudes towards older people as well as on older adults’ own experiences of ageism tends to rely on surveys or polls to capture individuals’ experiences. The present study aimed to address these issues by using informal interviewing techniques developed in sociolinguistics [
15] in a sample of middle-aged and older Canadians in order to determine whether similar patterns of ageism experiences would be found when individuals had great latitude to describe an ageism incident. In doing so, this study provides insight into ageism both in the context of extended story-telling and in a less-studied North American population—namely, Canadians. As indicated in the Chang et al. review, the number of studies on ageism from the United States far outpaces those from other nations [
12]. Broadening the scope of ageism research is needed in order to develop tailored interventions. Moreover, it has been suggested that ageism experiences may differ between Americans and Canadians [
16]. Thus, the present study represents a conceptual replication designed to ascertain the degree to which ageism experiences assessed previously in one country (United States) using a survey method can also be observed in another country (Canada) using an interview method. We note that this study was conducted prior to the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic, which has been shown to have exacerbated ageism [
17,
18,
19,
20]. Although it is important to capture acts of bias such as ageism during a time of crisis, we believe it is also vital to ascertain the shape and form of age prejudice during less extraordinary times.
Based on the earlier findings of Chasteen et al. [
14], we anticipated the following results. Middle-aged adults would report experiencing ageism most often at work, whereas older adults would indicate goods and services as the most common domain. Perpetrators would match the domain in terms of frequency (e.g., at work by an employer/co-worker). As for the type of ageist act, we anticipated that both age groups would report experiencing incorrect assumptions and a lack of respect, but that only older adults would indicate receiving unwanted help.
3. Results
3.1. Analytic Approach
Since the participants were asked to describe an ageism event that they experienced themselves, we excluded responses that did not describe an ageism event (n = 20) and that were not aimed at the participants themselves (n = 10). Frequency percentages of the remaining sample (n = 58; 21 middle-aged, 37 older adults) were then assessed via Chi-square tests of independence, unless otherwise noted.
3.2. Valence of Ageism Experiences
An independent samples t-test showed no difference in the valence of middle-aged (M = 2.67, SD = 1.11) and older adults’ (M = 2.92, SD = 1.48) descriptions of their experiences of ageism, t(56) = −0.679, p = 0.50. Both age groups’ descriptions on average were somewhat negative to neutral in valence. Below is an example of an experience from an individual, 049, (age 71) that was rated as very negative. Note that for all examples, numbers in square brackets are unique numerals assigned to each participant. Excerpts have been edited slightly for readability:
[049] Oh yeah. Yeah. I get called ‘dearie’. Interviewer: Really? [049] ‘Honey’ you know. You know salespeople. Interviewer: Oh. [049] “Oh dear- oh sweetie can I help you? Dearie, can I help you?” And I’ll go, “No darling, you can’t.” (both laugh) And they look at me and I think- at one point I went and I said- I called a manager and said, “You have to get your staff to stop calling older people ‘dearie’ and ‘sweetie’. We are not their dearie and we are not their sweetie.” Interviewer: Yeah. [049] “We’re not their grandmother.” Interviewer: Yeah. [049] And they looked at me like, “So sorry.” or ‘Hon’. “Hon, can I help you?” “No Hon, you can’t.”
3.3. Domain of Ageism Experience
There was a remarkable similarity between the two age groups in terms of the reported domain of the ageist experience (
X2 (4,
N = 53) = 2.65,
p = 0.62). As shown in
Table 1, Panel A, middle-aged adults reported ageism most frequently in public spaces, at work, and when receiving goods and services. A similar pattern occurred for older adults, with an even greater percentage reporting ageism most frequently in public spaces, followed by at work and then in goods and services settings.
3.4. Perpetrators of Ageist Act
Mirroring the pattern for domain, middle-aged and older adults reported similar perpetrators of ageism (
X2 (5,
N = 57) = 6.22,
p = 0.29).
Table 1, Panel B shows that middle-aged adults most often reported ageism perpetrated by strangers, followed by employer/co-workers and service workers. This pattern was also observed in older adults’ reports, whereby an even greater percentage indicated ageism perpetrated by strangers, followed by employer/co-workers and then a smaller percentage of service workers. The two age groups differed regarding reports of friends and authority figures as perpetrators. Middle-aged adults indicated more instances of friends as perpetrators, whereas older adults indicated more instances perpetrated by authority figures (i.e., someone such as a teacher, coach, doctor, or another person from whom the target takes and obeys instruction).
3.5. Type of Ageist Experience
Middle-aged and older adults also experienced similar types of ageist actions (
X2 (4,
N = 54) = 3.25,
p = 0.52).
Table 1, Panel C shows that middle-aged adults reported experiencing social/physical assumptions most frequently, followed by a lack of respect and unwanted help/special treatment. Older adults also reported experiencing social/physical assumptions the most, followed by a lack of respect. Whereas those two experiences constituted the majority of middle-aged responses, there was greater diversity in older adults’ experiences, with some older participants also reporting instances of unwanted help/special treatment, social exclusion, and cognitive assumptions. An example of a middle-aged adult’s, [064, age 56], report of experiencing ageism in a public space in the form of social/physical assumptions and unwanted help/special treatment is:
When I’m on the bus there are some people now offering me their seats. And I kind of- sometimes I’m kind of taken aback by that like what are you asking me for? I’m- I’m not that old. I’m- I’m- do I look it, you know? But then I appreciate it and I say thank you but you know it- it kind of also on the other side makes me feel old.
Whereas, for an older adult [026, age 73], an example is:
I think, when you get old, on the street you become invisible. Because it seems like everybody who is not old, wants to bang into you because they don’t see you. Well, there’s a lot of this going on, too. But it- you know, like I just thought- like, back in- way back in our day, you know, you would step aside for an older person and that kind of- but now, that’s the only thing I notice. Other than that, I don’t think anybody treats you any differently. Um, I do get offered a seat on the subway, to which I scowl at them.
4. Discussion
The goal of the present study was to examine ageism experiences described by middle-aged and older adults. We used a conversational interview method to allow for the greater expansion of people’s experiences. In addition, we captured ageism experiences among Canadian individuals, an understudied group. Thus, the current work provides a conceptual replication by using different forms of assessment in a less investigated population.
Unlike in previous findings [
14], there were no age differences in the shape or form in which ageism experiences occurred. Middle-aged and older participants showed similar patterns for the domain in which the incident occurred, the perpetrator of the ageist act, and for the type of ageist act. In the case of the domain, both age groups most often recalled ageism incidents that occurred in public spaces. This contrasts with earlier findings [
14], in which middle-aged adults most often reported ageism experiences in the workplace and older adults most often reported experiences when receiving goods and services. As in our earlier study, the perpetrators of ageism largely mapped onto the domain in which the incident occurred. In the present study, this meant strangers were reported as the most common transgressor, consistent with recollections of ageist acts in public spaces. Notably, strangers expressing ageism in public spaces are considered to be among the least acceptable sources of ageism [
24]. It appears that such normative beliefs, however, are not a strong enough deterrent to strangers engaging in ageist communication and acts in the public sphere.
In terms of types of ageist acts, social/physical assumptions were reported by both middle-aged and older adults far more than any other type of act, followed by a lack of respect. Although these two forms of ageist behavior were the most common in Chasteen et al. [
14], other forms of ageist behavior were also recalled, whereas in the present study very few or no participants reported social exclusion or cognitive assumptions. Lack of respect was experienced more by both age groups in this study compared to past findings. Both groups also reported experiencing some unwanted help, although that was less frequent than in the earlier study. When it did occur, unwanted help sometimes took the form of offering seats on public transportation. While that might appear to be a courteous act, some participants viewed such offers as a questioning of their strength, vitality, and independence, thus making them feel older. This suggests that, when offering help, it is important to consider the recipient’s perspective.
There are some potential reasons why we did not observe age differences in the present study. First, the middle-aged adults in this sample were older on average (
M age = 53.94 years) compared with those in the study of Chasteen et al. [
14] (
M age = 42.15 years). It could be that the middle-aged participants in the current study appeared somewhat older and thus encountered more ageist behavior in public spaces. It should be noted, however, that the vast majority (93.5%) of the middle-aged participants were still working, so it is somewhat surprising that the workplace was not mentioned more frequently as a domain in which ageism was occurring. Second, the sample in the present study was smaller than that examined by Chasteen et al. [
14]. This was due to the fact that these data were part of a larger sociolinguistic and social psychological study that included the interactive and extensive interviewing format of sociolinguistics; thus, the sample size was determined by the overall project. Third, it may be that because the prompt regarding ageism experiences was at the end of the interview, following a lengthy conversation on other topics, the participants focused on different ageism experiences than they would have when completing a survey that directly and solely inquired about ageism encounters (e.g., Chasteen et al. [
14]).
Overall, the present data provide valuable insight into the ageism experiences of middle-aged and older Canadians. Moreover, parallel findings to earlier research were obtained, despite the fact that the ageism comments emerged out of stories of personal experience rather than from self-report surveys. As such, this study provides a necessary extension of past findings to an understudied population using a different method.
Although the contributions of this work are notable, there are limitations to this study. The constraints of the smaller sample size were likely a factor in the lack of age differences, both in terms of statistical power and also in terms of the age range. Indeed, there were only nine participants aged 80+, and although their reported ageism experiences were similar to those aged 60–79, a larger sample would be needed for statistical comparisons. Given the heterogeneity among older adults [
17,
20], it is conceivable that comparisons between younger and older segments of the older adult age group might reveal differences. In order to conduct a conceptual replication, we used the same prompt as in our earlier work in which we asked participants to report instances in which they were treated differently because of their age. Future studies may wish to use more specific queries about experiences of age-based prejudice. Additional directions for future research include considering how possible individual differences (e.g., moral or political views, need for care or assistance) might moderate ageism experiences.