Next Article in Journal
Does Institutional Social Insurance Cause the Abandonment of Cultivated Land? Evidence from Rural China
Previous Article in Journal
Can We Identify Patients in Danger of Complications in Retrograde Intrarenal Surgery?—A Retrospective Risk Factors Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Brief Report

Attentive Processes and Blood Lactate in the Sambo

1
Department of Biomedical and Biotechnological Sciences, University of Catania, 95123 Catania, Italy
2
Study Center of Italian Osteopathy and Horus Social Cooperative, 95100 Catania, Italy
3
Department of Social and Humanistic Sciences in Sport, Faculty of Sport, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
4
Department of Sport Sciences, Kore University, Cittadella Universitaria, 94100 Enna, Italy
5
Polisportiva City Gym, 97100 Ragusa, Italy
6
Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, Human Physiology Section, University of Milan, 20122 Milan, Italy
7
Department of Educational Sciences, University of Catania, 95100 Catania, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(3), 1113; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19031113
Submission received: 1 December 2021 / Revised: 14 January 2022 / Accepted: 16 January 2022 / Published: 20 January 2022

Abstract

:
Background: Sambo is a martial art and combat sport that originated in the Soviet Union. There are two main stiles, Sport Sambo and Combat Sambo which resembles modern mixed martial arts. Very little literature is available about physiological aspects of Sambo and, in particular, on the possible effects on cognitive domains. The purpose of the present research was to determine if there is a correlation between a blood lactate increase and the intensity and/or selectivity of attentions. Methods: Sixteen male athletes practicing Sambo for at least 5 years participated voluntarily in the study. Each athlete had to sustain, with an interval of one week, both a Sport Sambo match and a Combat Sambo match, each lasting 5 min. Blood lactate levels as well as attentive capacities were evaluated at three different times: at rest, i.e., 5 min before the start of the session (pre), at end of the session and 15 min after its conclusion. Reaction time protocol was used to evaluate the intensity of attention, whereas divided attention was assessed for analyzing the selectivity of attention together with errors and omissions. Results: Concerning Sport Sambo, blood lactate was 1.66 mmol/L (±0.55 SD) before the session, reached a mean value of 3.40 mmol/L (±0.45 SD) at the end of the session (end) and returned to values similar to initial ones (a mean value of 1.98 mmol/L (±0.37 SD) after 15 min (15-end). None of the attentive parameters examined, showed statistically significant differences. Conversely, for Combat Sambo, it was found a significant increase in blood lactate levels that went from 1.66 mmol/L (±0.55 SD) before the session (pre), to 4.76 mmol/L (±0.60 SD) at the end (end) and then back to values similar to those observed before the session 15 min after its conclusion (15-end), i.e., 1.97 mmol/L (±0.37 SD); however, after a Combat Sambo session increases in blood lactate were associated with significant worsening of attentional mechanisms. Conclusions: In conclusion, in all the participants, the worsening of attentional mechanisms was observed only after the Combat Sambo session in which blood lactate values exceeded 4 mmol/L. This figure, also known as the Onset of Blood Lactate Accumulation (OBLA), is commonly used to determine the anaerobic threshold.

1. Introduction

Sambo is a martial art as well as a combat sport that originated in the Soviet Union; the word Sambo is an acronym of Russian “Samozashchita Bez Oruzhiya” (self-defense without weapons). Sambo roughly resembles Japanese judo, but uses a greater variety of moves such as ground grappling techniques and leg locks. Its development began after World War I by the Soviet Red Army to improve hand-to-hand combat abilities, thanks to the efforts of two Soviet athletes: Viktor Spiridonov (1882–1944) and Vasili Oshchepkov (1893–1938). While Viktor Spiridonov privileged a style based on strength, Vasili Oshchepkov, who had studied judo in Japan with Jigoro Kano (1860–1938), preferred a softer style. Anatoly Kharlampiyev (1906–1972), a pupil of Vasili Oshchepkov, helped to transform Sambo from a martial art into a sporting discipline.
The International Sambo Federation (FIAS) recognizes three main styles [1], sport Sambo, combat Sambo, and beach Sambo. Sport Sambo is mainly characterized by specific throws, holds, arm and leg locks. In addition to the technique of sports Sambo in combat Sambo, rules allow blows with the hands, legs and head, as well as choking. Beach Sambo bouts are held only in the standing position.
Very little in literature is present about physiological aspects of Sambo, e.g., [2,3], in particular, on lactate production during performance and on possible effects on cognitive domains.
Although about two hundred years have passed since the studies carried out by the Swedish chemist Carl Wilhelm Scheele and the subsequent discovery of lactic acid, lactate continues to be a much-studied molecule. Lactic acid was considered for a long time a waste product of metabolism and the main cause of muscle fatigue [4].
However, the depletion of muscle glycogen seems to be critic for the peripheral fatigue, also considering that, fatigue-related metabolites (such as ADP, Pi, and H+) do not accumulate as exercise progresses, in spite of the fact that peripheral fatigue gradually develops [5].
Over the last few decades, it has been observed that brain cells, in particular astrocytes, produce lactate which they transfer to neurons. These latter use it as a metabolite, like glucose, in order to cope with the increase in energy needs, both in physiological conditions that it does not.
The lactate produced by the muscular system during physical exercise is released and later absorbed by muscles, heart, liver and brain, to be oxidized as a useful metabolite [4,6].
Brooks was the first to introduce the concept of lactate shuttle mechanism by which lactate, with appropriate MCT transporters, is conducted in different parts in order to be used as a metabolite [7].
It is generally recognized that after an intense exercise, blood lactate levels rise, indicating the intensity of the performance instead of muscular fatigue [4]. High levels of blood lactate, resulting from an intense exercise or an intravenous infusion of lactate solution, were found to have a negative correlation with two aspects of the attention, intensity and selectivity [8,9].
Why the attention? Attention is a cognitive domain that has been studied extensively in sports, as it is known that attention significantly influences sports performance [10,11,12,13].
The studies of Mackworth [14] have clarified the existence of two different types of attention: one due to the experience of the physical and social environment (regulated by the activity of the frontal lobes) and an involuntary one (regulated by the flow of external stimuli) independently of experience.
Attention can be considered as a set of processes, through which information processing and decision-making activities are controlled [15].
These processes involve specific parameters such as intensity (vigilance and sustained attention) and selectivity (broad, concentrated and divided).
Furthermore, it can be observed that general attention is characterized by the evaluation of a large amount of information, while selective attention involves the discrimination of stimuli and/or the selection of a small number of them [16].
Attention represents, for almost the multitude of sporting disciplines, a feature of considerable importance. Being ready and vigilant in order to respond to an offense, to shoot for a basket, to go off to shoot, etc. in addition to specific muscle characteristics, can make a difference [17,18,19,20,21,22,23].
The authors Furfley and Wood [24] even claim that an athlete’s attention is the key to success in sports performance.
This research’s goal was to determine if there is a correlation between a blood lactate increase and the intensity and/or selectivity of attentions. To this end, we measured both blood lactate levels and attentional abilities of participants before, at the end, and after 15 min of a Sambo session.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The study included sixteen volunteer male athletes who had been practicing Sambo for at least 5 years. The volunteers were fully informed about the study’s goal and signed an informed consent that was written in accordance with the ethical standards outlined in the Helsinki Declaration (revision 2013) and approved by our Institution’s Ethical Committee.
In the three days leading up to the experimental session, all participants were requested not to make any substantial changes in their habits (food intake, stimulants, physical activity, etc.). All competitors were obliged to have a certificate of suitability for competitive sports given by a Sports Medicine professional.

2.2. Procedure

The experimental protocol required that each athlete had to sustain, with an interval of one week, both a Sambo sport match and a Sambo combat match. Half of the participants, randomly selected, had to take first he Sambo sport match and then the Sambo combat match, while the other half had to do the opposite.
The victory can be: (a) total; (b) by superiority; (c) on points; (d) technical; (e) on warnings; (f) by the opponent’s elimination for passivity.
In the present study, both a Sport Sambo and Combat Sambo session last 5 min.
All measurements were made in three different moments: at rest, i.e., five minutes before the start of the session (pre), at the end of the session, and 15 min after it ended.
Blood lactate levels were measured using a portable analyzer called “Lactate Pro 2” (Arkray, Japan) on a drop of blood obtained by the subject’s index finger.
Two tests were used to evaluate attention, according to the Zimmermann and Fimm [25] attentional test procedure. The Attention and Concentration Tasks (ACT) [26] were used with the validated Italian version.
The ACT is composed of a set of tasks that were created specifically for evaluating different attentional abilities using a simple computer.
The main issue in this sort of experiment is that attentional tests must be completed in a short period of time (less than 4 min in total), so that the evaluation can actually occur with higher blood lactate levels. Indeed, blood lactate levels have been found to be halved 7 min after an exhausting workout [27].
The intensity of attention was measured using a reaction time (RT) methodology. The RT technique requires the participant to answer as rapidly as possible by pushing the spacebar when the symbol on the monitor target “star” appears at random intervals.
Divided attention was used to analyze attention selectivity. A dual activity was conducted, which allowed for the parallel identification of auditory and visual sort. An object would appear on the computer screen and then the name of an object would be spoken from a loudspeaker. Only if the name coming from the loudspeaker was that of the object presented on the screen the subject had to press the space-bar as fast as possible. The test allows to evaluate the role of the interferences on the divided attention through three variables: number of errors, the number of omissions and performances duration (execution time, ET).

2.3. Statistical Analysis

For the purpose of this study, we have chosen 16 Sambo male athletes with anthropometric characteristics very similar (see Table 1). However, such a limited number of subjects did not allow to apply the classic formula for determining the sample size. In that meaning, non-parametric statistics were preferred to parametric statistics.
Data were gathered and averaged before being contrasted with a one-way repeated measure ANOVA and the Dunn’s Multiple Comparison Test. The significance level was set at p = 0.05, and all data were provided as mean standard deviation (SD). All analyses were carried out using the GraphPad Prism 6:00 version for Windows (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA, USA, www.graphpad.com, accessed on 21 October 2021).

3. Results

Figure 1 shows the results observed after a Sport Sambo and Combat Sambo session. As can be seen, both Sambo sessions result in a considerable increase in blood lactate levels. The observed blood lactate values in the Sport Sambo, that were 1.66 mmol/L (±0.55 SD) before the session, exhibited a significant increase at the end of the session (end), reaching a mean value of 3.40 mmol/L (±0.45 SD), and then returning to values similar to initial ones (a mean value of 1.98 mmol/L (±0.37 SD) after 15 min (15-end). It can be seen that, during the session of sport Sambo, none of the parameters examined, i.e., RT, ET errors and omissions, showed statistically significant differences.
Also for the Combat Sambo it was found that the session causes a significant increase in blood lactate levels that went from 1.66 mmol/L (±0.55 SD) before the session (pre), to 4.76 mmol/L (± 0.60 SD) at the end (end) and then back to values similar to those observed before the session 15 min after its conclusion (15-end), i.e., 1.97 mmol/L (±0.37 SD). However, in contrast to what was observed after a Sport Sambo session, after a Combat Sambo session increases in blood lactate were associated with significant worsening of attentional mechanisms, particularly RT, ET, and number of omissions.

4. Discussion

It is known that, during maximal exercise, at the level of the CNS, an increase in the demand for oxygen, glucose and lactate occurs; as Dalsgaard suggests, this indicates that increased brain activation is associated with the increase in physical activity [28,29].
Gonzalez-Alonso et al. observed that a marked increase in glucose, O2, and lactate utilization by the brain is associated with impaired tissue oxygenation in the frontal cortex [30].
It has been suggested that a maximal exercise induces an increase in plasma catecholamines which, in turn, influences central catecholamines neurotransmitters via the vagal/nucleus of the solitary tract pathway capable, by inducing in this way changes on cognitive domains [26,27]. However, in a previous study carried out on a subject completely immobile, it has been observed that the same changes that an exhaustive exercise produced on attentional mechanisms were also induced by intravenous infusion of lactate. This led to the conclusion that, regardless of the catecholamine theory, a rise in blood lactate is capable of worsening attentional processes on its own.
Attention, in this regard, represents that cognitive domain that can allow us to verify the incoming information [31], allowing us to focus only on the information that is relevant at that given moment, allowing us to realize the goals we had previously set and to ignore information that is irrelevant or even potentially interfering with it. The frontal lobe is the portion of our brain that plays a critical role in attentional processes.
Iidaka and collaborators observed that during the performance of physical exercise, there is a bilateral increase of cerebral blood flow in the dorsolateral prefrontal areas, region involved in selectivity rather than intensity of attention [32].
An interesting aspect of the current research was that in all the participants the worsening of attentional mechanisms was observed only after the Combat Sambo session in which blood lactate values exceeded 4 mmol/L. This value, called OBLA (Onset of Blood Lactate Accumulation), is commonly used to express the anaerobic threshold.
Clearly, Combat Sambo has more anaerobic activities than Sport Sambo such as to justify the greater increase in lactate production during exercise, the decrease of both the intensity and selectivity of attentional capacity seen in the current study appears to confirm what has previously been observed in both maximal [33] and submaximal [34] activities.
The capacity to pay attention is an essential requirement during the development of sport performance in some cases, it turns out to be a necessary component of sporting success. It is important to note that these participants should have pretty limited attentional performances when undertaking Combat but not Sport Sambo.
The main limitations of the present study are firstly the small number of participants (n = 16) and, furthermore, the fact that they were all male and with very close values of age and weight.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the current study’s findings confirm that the decrease of both selectivity and intensity of attentional capacity reported in the current research appears to back up what has previously been seen in the highest level of performance, as well as submaximal exercises [34,35].
It can be concluded that the role played by physical exercise in reducing the blood flow of the frontal cortex, and the concomitant increase in lactate in the blood, could modify both the selectivity and the intensity of attentional mechanisms.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.C., A.B., M.T. and D.D.C.; methodology, M.C.; investigation, A.B. and M.C.; resources, A.B., D.D.C. and M.C.; data curation, M.C., M.T., V.P. (Valentina Perciavalle); writing—original draft preparation, M.C., A.B., V.P. (Vincenzo Perciavalle), P.C. and D.D.C.; writing—review and editing M.C.; visualization, A.N., P.C. and M.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Milan (number 15/16).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. The International Sambo Federation (FIAS) Sambo Ranking System. Approved on XVI FIAS Congress in Astana, Kazakhstan. 2005. Available online: https://web.archive.org/web/20140219181101/http://Sambo.com/ranking (accessed on 15 December 2021).
  2. Drid, P.; Kondratijeva, A.; Tabakov, S.; Stojanović, M.; Trivić, T.; Pasternak, J.; Ostojić, S. Salivary hormones are markedly affected by sambo exercise in female athletes. Med. Sport 2017, 70, 328–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Drid, P.; Figlioli, F.; Lakicevic, N.; Gentile, A.; Stajer, V.; Raskovic, B.; Vojvodic, N.; Roklicer, R.; Trivic, T.; Tabakov, S.; et al. Patterns of rapid weight loss in elite sambo athletes. BMC Sports Sci. Med. Rehabil. 2021, 13, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Proia, P.; Di Liegro, C.M.; Schiera, G.; Fricano, A.; Di Liegro, I. Lactate as a Metabolite and a Regulator in the Central Nervous System. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2016, 17, 1450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Burnley, M.; Jones, A.M. Power-duration relationship: Physiology, fatigue, and the limits of human performance. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2018, 18, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Adeva-Andany, M.; López-Ojén, M.; Funcasta-Calderón, R.; Ameneiros-Rodríguez, E.; Donapetry-García, C.; Vila-Altesor, M.; Rodríguez-Seijas, J. Comprehensive review on lactate metabolism in human health. Mitochondrion 2014, 17, 76–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Brooks, G.A. The lactate shuttle during exercise and recovery. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1986, 18, 360–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Hall, M.M.; Rajasekaran, S.; Thomsen, T.W.; Peterson, A.R. Lactate: Friend or Foe. PMR 2016, 8, S8–S15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Coco, M.; Di Corrado, D.; Calogero, R.A.; Perciavalle, V.; Maci, T.; Perciavalle, V. Attentional processes and blood lactate levels. Brain Res. 2009, 1302, 205–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Perciavalle, V.; Maci, T.; Perciavalle, V.; Massimino, S.; Coco, M. Working memory and blood lactate levels. Neurol. Sci. 2015, 36, 2129–2136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Perciavalle, V.; Alagona, G.; De Maria, G.; Rapisarda, G.; Costanzo, E.; Perciavalle, V.; Coco, M. Somatosensory evoked potentials and blood lactate levels. Neurol. Sci. 2015, 36, 1597–1601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Coco, M. The brain behaves as a muscle? Neurol. Sci. 2017, 38, 1865–1868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Coco, M.; Di Corrado, D.; Ramaci, T.; Di Nuovo, S.; Perciavalle, V.; Puglisi, A.; Cavallari, P.; Bellomo, M.; Buscemi, A. Role of lactic acid on cognitive functions. Phys. Sportsmed. 2019, 7, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Vaughan, R.S.; Laborde, S. Attention, working-memory control, working-memory capacity, and sport performance: The moderating role of athletic expertise. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2021, 21, 240–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mackworth, A.K. Model-driven interpretation in intelligent vision systems. Perception 1976, 5, 349–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Parasuraman, R. The attentive Brain: Issues and Prospects. In The Attentive Brain; MIT: Cambridge, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  17. van Zomeren, A.H.; Brouwer, W.H. Clinical Neuropsychology of Attention; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  18. Coco, M.; Alagona, G.; De Maria, G.; Rapisarda, G.; Costanzo, E.; Perciavalle, V.; Perciavalle, V. Relationship of high blood lactate levels with latency of visual evoked potentials. Neurol. Sci. 2015, 36, 541–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Coco, M.; Caggia, S.; Musumeci, G.; Perciavalle, V.; Graziano, A.C.; Pannuzzo, G.; Cardile, V. Sodium L-lactate differently affects brain-derived neurothrophic factor, inducible nitric oxide synthase, and heat shock protein 70 kDa production in human astrocytes and SH-SY5Y cultures. J. Neurosci. Res. 2013, 91, 313–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Coco, M.; Buscemi, A.; Perciavalle, V.; Maci, T.; Galvano, G.; Scavone, A.M.F.; Perciavalle, V.; Di Corrado, D. Cognitive Deficits and White Matter Alterations in Highly Trained Scuba Divers. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Coco, M.; Buscemi, A.; Sagone, E.; Pellerone, M.; Ramaci, T.; Marchese, M.; Perciavalle, V.; Perciavalle, V.; Musumeci, G. Effects of Yoga Practice on personality, body image and lactate. Pilot study on a group of women from 40 years. Sustainability 2020, 12, 6719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Coco, M.; Buscemi, A.; Cavallari, P.; Massimino, S.; Rinella, S.; Tortorici, M.M.; Maci, T.; Perciavalle, V.; Tusak, M.; Di Corrado, D.; et al. Executive functions during submaximal exercises. Front. Psychol. 2020, 30, 537922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Di Corrado, D.; Quartiroli, A.; Coco, M. Editorial: Psychological and Motor Associations in Sports Performance: A Mental Approach to Sports. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 629944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Coco, M.; Di Corrado, D.; Cirillo, F.; Iacono, C.; Perciavalle, V.; Buscemi, A. Effects of General Fatigue Induced by Exhaustive Exercise on Posture and Gait Stability of Healthy Young Men. Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Furley, P.; Wood, G. Working memory, attentional control, and expertise in sports: A review of current literature and directions for future research. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2016, 5, 415–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Zimmermann, P.; Fimm, B. Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP); Psytest, Würselen (software); Herzogenrath: Köln, Germany, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  27. Di Nuovo, S. Attenzione e Concentrazione; Erikson, Trento (software); Erikson: Chicago, IL, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  28. Coco, M.; Perciavalle, V.; Cavallari, P.; Perciavalle, V. Effects of an Exhaustive Exercise on Motor Skill Learning and on the Excitability of Primary Motor Cortex and Supplementary Motor Area. Medicine 2016, 95, e2978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Dalsgaard, M.K.; Ide, K.; Cai, Y.; Quistorff, B.; Secher, N.H. The intent to exercise influences the cerebral O2/carbohydrate uptake ratio in humans. J. Physiol. 2002, 540, 681–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Dalsgaard, M.K.; Quistorff, B.; Danielsen, E.R.; Selmer, C.; Vogelsang, T.; Secher, N.H. A reduced cerebral metabolic ratio in exercise reflects metabolism and not accumulation of lactate within the human brain. J. Physiol. 2004, 554, 571–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Gonzalez-Alonso, J.; Dalsgaard, M.K.; Osada, T.; Volianitis, S.; Dawson, E.A.; Yoshiga, C.C.; Secher, N.H. Brain and central haemodynamics and oxygenation during maximal exercise in humans. J. Physiol. 2004, 557, 331–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Posner, M.I.; Snyder, C.R.; Davidson, B.J. Attention and the detection of signals. J. Exp. Psychol. 1980, 109, 160–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Iidaka, T.; Anderson, N.D.; Kapur, S.; Cabeza, R.; Craik, F.I. The effect of divided attention on encoding and retrieval in episodic memory revealed by positron emission tomography. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000, 12, 267–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. el-Sayed, M.S.; George, K.P.; Dyson, K. The influence of blood sampling site on lactate concentration during submaximal exercise at 4 mmol.l-1 lactate level. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 1993, 67, 518–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Cox, R.H. Sport Psychology. Concepts and Applications; Mc Graw Hill: London, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Changes of attentional processes in relation to blood lactate after a Sport Sambo session (left) and a Combat Sambo session (right). Mean values (±standard deviation) of blood lactate, reaction time, execution time, number of errors, and number of omissions measured at rest (pre), at the conclusion of the session (end) as well as 15 min after its conclusion (15 end). Symbols: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001; **** p< 0.0001.
Figure 1. Changes of attentional processes in relation to blood lactate after a Sport Sambo session (left) and a Combat Sambo session (right). Mean values (±standard deviation) of blood lactate, reaction time, execution time, number of errors, and number of omissions measured at rest (pre), at the conclusion of the session (end) as well as 15 min after its conclusion (15 end). Symbols: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001; **** p< 0.0001.
Ijerph 19 01113 g001
Table 1. Sambo athletes’ anthropometric characteristics. Mean ± s (range).
Table 1. Sambo athletes’ anthropometric characteristics. Mean ± s (range).
SubjectAge (Years)Height (cm)Weight (kg)BMI
mean24.69172.7574.3124.89
SD3.074.164.320.90
BMI: body mass index; SD: standard deviation.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Coco, M.; Buscemi, A.; Tušak, M.; Perciavalle, V.; Nifosì, A.; Cavallari, P.; Di Corrado, D.; Perciavalle, V. Attentive Processes and Blood Lactate in the Sambo. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19031113

AMA Style

Coco M, Buscemi A, Tušak M, Perciavalle V, Nifosì A, Cavallari P, Di Corrado D, Perciavalle V. Attentive Processes and Blood Lactate in the Sambo. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(3):1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19031113

Chicago/Turabian Style

Coco, Marinella, Andrea Buscemi, Matej Tušak, Vincenzo Perciavalle, Alfio Nifosì, Paolo Cavallari, Donatella Di Corrado, and Valentina Perciavalle. 2022. "Attentive Processes and Blood Lactate in the Sambo" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 3: 1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19031113

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop