Next Article in Journal
Evaluating the Public Climate School—A School-Based Programme to Promote Climate Awareness and Action in Students: Protocol of a Cluster-Controlled Pilot Study
Previous Article in Journal
Exploring and Developing a Scale Using Item Response Theory for Sport Psychological Skills in Speed Skaters
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Association between the Sense of Coherence and the Self-Reported Adherence to Guidelines during the First Months of the COVID-19 Pandemic in Israel

Department of Health Promotion, School of Public Health, Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(13), 8041; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19138041
Submission received: 3 April 2022 / Revised: 7 June 2022 / Accepted: 8 June 2022 / Published: 30 June 2022

Abstract

:
(1) Background: Social distancing became a central strategy employed to limit the spread of the SARS-CoV-2 virus. We explore self-reported adherence (SRA) and factors associated with SRA among Israeli adults at the end of the first national lockdown in Israel. (2) Methods: We conducted a cross-sectional consumer panel survey of 820 Israeli adults aged 18 to 70 in May and June 2020. We collected data on the SRA to the social distancing measures, sociodemographic variables, perceptions of pandemic-related danger and of protection provided by the social distancing measures, as well as Sense of Coherence (SoC). (3) Results: 60% of respondents reported complying with 7 measures. Higher SoC was associated with higher SRA (p = 0.04), and was related to income, marital status, age, profession, and education. The SRA was higher among Jews than Arabs (Jews: Mean = 10.5, SD = 4.5; Arabs: Mean = 9.1, SD = 4.1, p < 0.001) and among males (Males: Mean = 10.8, SD = 4.7; Females: Mean = 9, SD = 4.1; p = 0.003). SoC, perception of protection and perception of danger were associated with higher SRA (p = 0.42, p < 0.001 and p = 0.005 respectively). Single people reported higher levels of SRA than people in relationships (Partnered: Mean = 9.7, SD = 4.2, Non-partnered: Mean = 10.9, SD = 4.7, p = 0.033). (4) Conclusions: At the time of exit from the first lockdown, compliance with social distancing measures was high, with Jewish, single and male Israelis more likely to adhere to the guidelines. We identified the populations at risk for non-adherence and associated factors, reporting for the first time the correlation between SoC and SRA. Further research is needed to assess the role of these factors in Jewish and Arab populations.

1. Introduction

As we enter the third year of the global pandemic, Omicron, the new variant of COVID-19 has shattered hopeful expectations and forced us to re-evaluate the prevention strategies that have been used so far. While new COVID-19 variants continue to emerge, the vaccine protection wanes, and many people around the world remain unvaccinated, social distancing prevails as one of the most effective protective strategies that limit the spread of the virus [1]. Due to the continuously high infection rates, social distancing should be and has been implemented as a legislative directive on a global scale [2]. However, turning the directive into reality is challenging, and we must obtain a deeper understanding of what drives people to adhere to the guidelines.
Since the outbreak of the pandemic, researchers have been vigorously studying the differences between people who adhere to social distancing and those who do not, or, in some cases, oppose the distancing [3,4,5,6]. The social-distance adherence continuum is revealed in literature as ranging from people who over-respond all the way to people who completely disregard the recommendation. On one end of the continuum is the “over-response”: the behavior of a distressed person who adopts an exaggerated form of social distancing that resembles self-isolation [7]. The “under-response” is understood as a disregard of the guidelines and of the dangers that come with COVID-19 [7].
To further our understanding of predictive indicators for adherence to the guidelines, we have reviewed relevant literature, including studies that have yet to be peer-reviewed. Due to the social desirability bias, the data collected must be analyzed with caution: a study that used the mobile data of 302 participants to track the differences between self-reported adherence to social distancing and real behavior found that the self-reports are consistently higher than the community average [8]. Among the factors identified as most highly associated with high adherence to the social distancing rules were one’s perception of their own risk, older age, recognition of one’s responsibility for their community, as well as the practical ability to do so, including the ability to work or study remotely [5,9,10]. One’s social circle appears to predict adherence to social distancing more than the individual’s own confidence in the guidelines [11]. A related predictor is the political standpoint of the individual. One of the studies on this matter investigated why the American conservatives are inclined to ignore the social-distancing guidelines [6]. The researchers found that adherence is related to the conservatives’ tendency to distrust science. Conservative people who reported trusting science were more inclined to socially distance themselves [6], and analytic thinkers tended to reject the conspiracy theories that have evolved regarding the COVID-19 outbreak [12]. A different study identified the impact of the politicization of the pandemic, including its representation in the liberal mainstream media, as what led the American conservatives to disregard the severity of the pandemic and doubt that social distancing can prevent its spread [13]. Further, boredom and self-control were identified as two important predictors of social-distancing adherence [14]. This high-powered cross-sectional study of 895 people revealed that individuals who scored high in the boredom trait found adherence to social distancing to be particularly challenging. The opposite was true for individuals with high self-control traits [14,15].
Research has been spreading out from merely looking into the traits that predict adherence to analyzing how public health promoters can call the public to action. One study found that messages to the public that emphasize one’s duty towards community and peers could be the key to promoting adherence [16]. Essentially, presenting social distancing as one’s moral duty adds an ethical dimension and meaning to the guidelines and frames the rejection of the adherence as immoral [17,18]. The urgency of such measures is clear, yet health promoters must use persuasion tools responsibly. Fear-based techniques have been shown to trigger emotional responses and are less effective than prosocial framing [19]. Eliciting fear is potentially dangerous as it can trigger extreme emotions and behaviors in populations predisposed to emotional disorders [19].
The current study, in conjunction with similar studies that are being conducted worldwide, was urgently needed because social distancing has been the crucial strategy for prevention of the collapse of medical systems [20]. Despite the calls for social distancing and legislation, the adherence has been far from perfect. A 2021 study conducted in London, England, revealed that 92.8% of 681 respondents did not adhere to all social-distancing rules, and 48.6% intentionally disregarded them [9]. The variability we are seeing in the adherence to the guidelines is one of the reasons why we need to identify the factors associated with adherence. Once identified, we will be more likely to address the issues successfully and lower the spread of COVID-19. Our role as health promoters is to inform policymakers on the most effective ways to prompt high adherence in their population and educate them on the populations that require special attention in this matter [21].
In the late 1970s, Aaron Antonovsky presented the theory of Salutogenesis, proposing that the way an individual copes with life is related to their health [22]. His Salutogenic model presents a paradigm shift that emphasizes the health end of the health-ease and dis-ease continuum—rather than the disease end [22]. The Sense of Coherence Scale (SoC) is the tool Antonovsky created in order to understand how one copes with life’s challenges. The 29-item and shortened 13-item SoC questionnaires measure three factors: comprehensibility, manageability, and meaningfulness of the perceived stressors [23]. As Hammond and Niedermann summarize, “the more a person is able to understand and integrate (comprehensibility), to handle (manageability) and to make sense (meaningfulness) of an experience or disease, the greater the individual’s potential to successfully cope with the situation or the disease” [24]. A large systematic review conducted in 2005 summarized 25 years of research and 458 studies on SoC [25], and found the scale to be valid, reliable, and cross-culturally applicable for measuring how people manage stressful situations and stay well.
One’s Sense of Coherence may be an essential source of resilience [23], defined as the capacity to adapt when faced with adversity and trauma [26]. Thus, SoC can partially explain the variability of the responses to stressors observed in society. The COVID-19 pandemic and the lockdowns have presented a unique, global challenge, stressor, and trauma, and tested individual, communal, and national resilience. While the national resilience declined due to the pandemic [27], many people were able to cope well thanks to their individual levels of resilience: 70% of the respondents in a large study in Italy displayed resilience, attributed by the authors mostly to the respondents’ trait resilience and conscientiousness [28]. An international study conducted simultaneously in seven countries in 2020 revealed that high SoC was correlated with better mental health during the pandemic and that it was mediated by perceived family support and trust in leaders and institutions [29]. These findings were confirmed by others, in relation to anxiety [30] and depression [31]. Thus, one’s ability to understand, manage and make sense of the crisis has been shown as crucial for their well-being. Following the findings regarding the trust in leaders and institutions as a mediator between SoC and mental health [29], we may theorize that high SoC will be associated with greater adherence to the imposed social distancing guidelines. While it has been proposed that strict adherence to social distancing may be a result of a negative fear-response to the crisis [32], a high SoC may result in the same outcome whilst maintaining the individual’s overall well-being. To the best of our knowledge, to date SoC has not been measured in relation to the adherence to guidelines.
Following the 3-component Salutogenic model, if an individual is able to comprehend the reason for social distancing (thanks to successful and trusting communication with leaders), manage the challenge it presents (thanks to their own and societal resources), and make meaning of it (see both the individual and the communal value of social distancing), we hypothesize that they will be more likely to adhere to the guidelines. We further hypothesize that higher perceptions of protection and danger will be associated with greater Self-reported Adherence (SRA), and that adherence and SoC will differ between people of different genders, nationalities, occupations, income, education, religion and religiosity and marital status. The hypothesized differences between representatives of the two main nationalities residing in Israel—Jews and Arabs—is based on previous studies that reported poorer health status in the Arab population and differences in health behaviors and well-being among the two groups [33]. Thus, the current study set out to collect and analyze data on the SRA to the social distancing guidelines and the factors associated with SRA, including socio-demographic factors, SoC, perceptions of protection and danger. Additionally, our goal was to identify the factors associated with SoC. The data were collected in Israel at the end of the first COVID-19 lockdown, in the spring and summer of 2020.

2. Materials and Methods

In Israel, the first case of COVID-19 was diagnosed on 21 February 2020. The social distancing regulations began on 11 March 2020 and escalated on 19 March 2020, when a national state of emergency was declared, followed by restrictions on movement: the population was restricted to a 100-m radius from home for any nonessential activities. The questionnaire used for this study was distributed during the gradual withdrawal from the first-wave lockdown, between 14 May and 22 June 2020. We conducted a cross-sectional survey, inviting the participants using the iPanel platform. iPanel is the largest panel in Israel and it strictly adheres to the European Society for Opinion and Marketing Research (ESOMAR) principles, while all results are filtered through multi-stage validation before final data analysis [34,35]. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee (Institutional Review Board) of Tel Aviv University (#0001338-2).
The quantitative questionnaire consists of previously validated questionnaires and items related specifically to COVID-19. All questions and explanations were written both in Hebrew and Arabic for the respective Israeli populations. The independent variables included standard social-demographics questions used to assess gender, age, religion, marital status, income, as previously described.
The dependent variables were 7 measures of adherence to social distancing, according to a self-report. The social distancing questions were based on the rapidly developed ones in the International Tobacco Control Policy Evaluation (ITC) project surveys. The social distancing measures were defined as follows: avoiding public places, avoiding contact with non-cohabitating family or friends, maintaining a 2-m distance from others, coughing into a tissue or elbow, handwashing practice, style of greeting, adherence to the measures during holidays, and remaining within a 100 m radius from one’s home, per national guidelines. We created a social distancing scale (SDS) that is based on these 7 measures. We used a Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The outcome of the SDS ranges from 7 to 35, with higher scores indicating the highest level of adherence to social distancing measures. The validity of questionnaires was tested via Pearson’s correlation coefficient, examining linear relationships between consecutive variables. Reliability was tested via Cronbach’s alpha measure (alpha = 0.68).
We collected data on the following explanatory variables: sociodemographic characteristics, sleep, health status, perceptions of pandemic-related danger and protection (provided by the social distancing measures), Sense of Coherence (SoC), and subjective well-being. For the analysis of demographic characteristics, we performed independent t-tests for continuous variables, and chi-square tests for categorical variables. Independent t-tests and ANOVA were utilized for the analysis of variables associated with the sense of coherence. Linear regression was performed to assess the association between SRA and explanatory variables.
The Sense of Danger Scale (SDS) was used to assess the risk perceptions. The respondents were asked how endangered they felt by the spread of the virus, using a Likert-like scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much) [36]. Similar questions were used in other studies on the risk perceptions during the pandemic [37,38,39]. The perception of protection provided by the social distancing measures was quantified using questions relating both to the individual and the societal level (“how personally protected from the COVID-19 do you feel by the governmental guidelines regarding social distancing?”; “how successful do you think that the government-advised social distancing is at preventing the spread of the virus on a societal level?”). The possible answers regarding the perceived protective value ranged from not at all through somewhat to very much, with an additional “don’t know” option. The questions were based on the ITC Project surveys [40].
The Sense of Coherence was measured using the shortened, 13-item questionnaire developed by A. Antonovsky [23]. The questionnaire addresses three components of the model: comprehensibility, manageability, and meaningfulness of the perceived stressors. The response scale to each question ranges from 1 to 7. The total score of the questionnaire ranges from 13 to 91 points, with higher scores indicating a greater sense of coherence and greater subjective well-being (alpha = 0.84).

3. Results

In total, 807 respondents completed the survey. The response rate stood at 25% for the Jewish population and 10% for the Arabic population. Distributions of gender, age (18–70), and nationality (Arab 20%, Jewish 80%) matched the national distributions. Table 1 presents the socio-demographic characteristics of the study population by nationality and as a whole.

3.1. Compliance with the Seven Social Distancing Measures

Figure 1 presents the self-reported adherence to the social distancing guidelines. The majority (60%) of respondents reported complying with the 7 measures all or most of the time (avoiding public places: 72.3%, avoiding people from outside of one’s household: 65.1%, maintaining 2-m distance outside the home: 82.1%, coughing in elbow or tissue: 83.4%, handwashing: 69.6%, greeting without physical contact: 81.5%, staying within 100 m of home: 80.6%). Just under half (46.2%) reported staying within 100 m of the home all of the time. Self-Reported Adherence among Jews was slightly higher than among Arabs (Jews:Mean = 10.5, SD = 4.5; Arabs:Mean = 9.1, SD = 4.1, p < 0.001) and higher among males than females (Males:Mean = 10.8, SD = 4.7; Females:Mean = 9.7, SD = 4.1, p = 0.003).

3.2. Multivariate Analysis of Variables Associated wih Social Distancing Adherence

Using both univariate and multivariate regressions, we found 6 independent variables significantly correlated with SRA: Perception of Protection (p < 0.001), nationality (p < 0.001), Sense of Coherence (p < 0.042), gender (p < 0.003), relationship status (p < 0.033), and Perception of Danger (p < 0.005). Those without partners reported significantly higher levels of Self-Reported Adherence than those with partners. Men were more likely to adhere to the guidelines than women, as was the Jewish population as compared to the Arabic population. Age, religiosity, family income and education did not have a significant correlation with SRA. These results are presented in Table 2.

3.3. Sense of Coherence

The sample mean of SoC levels stood at 61.1 out of the possible 91 points (SD = 13.8, p < 0.001). Out of the 3 components that make up the Sense of Coherence, manageability ranked the highest. The analysis of SoC according to demographic factors revealed that income, marital status, age, profession, and education were associated with it. As presented in Table 3, married respondents had greater SoC than non-married individuals (63.6 next to 57.9, t = −5.90, p < 0.001). Higher income and education had a significant impact on SoC, with higher levels observed in college graduates as opposed to those with no college degrees (62.6 next to 59.8, t = 2.85, p = 0.005), and highest SoC levels in those with graduate degrees (65.4, F = 6.30, p < 0.001). However, people with elementary and lesser education had higher SoC levels than those with high school diplomas (62.7 next to 57.1), at a lever similar to those with some college education (62.9). Career soldiers had the highest levels of SoC of all professional groups examined (69.5, F = 9.11, p < 0.001), followed closely by retirees (68.4), while the lowest levels of SoC were observed in soldiers undergoing their mandatory military service (52.8, p < 0.001). Older respondents had greater SoC than younger ones (t = 7.80, p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

Our study presents valuable data on the factors associated with adherence to the social distancing guidelines imposed during the COVID-19 pandemic in Israel. Thanks to the proximity of the study population’s socio-demographic characteristics to the national ones in Israel, its findings are reliable and possibly generalizable, if limited by the cross-sectional design of the study. Despite its effectiveness, social distancing has been a challenge: as revealed in this study, the adherence in Israel has been less than ideal already at the early stages, with approximately 60% of the respondents complying with all seven social-distancing measures at three months into the pandemic, defined as: avoiding public places, avoiding people from outside of one’s household, maintaining a 2-m distance from others while outside, coughing into elbow or tissue, proper handwashing, greeting people without physical contact, and staying within 100 m of one’s home. Coughing into the elbow or tissue, maintaining a 2-m distance from others when outside, greeting without physical contact, and remaining within a 100-m radius from one’s home, were the most commonly adhered to guidelines, at least most of the time, in the population analyzed for this study. However, about a third of the respondents did not avoid public places or people from outside of their households or followed the recommended handwashing routines. Thus, special attention needs to be devoted to promoting these aspects of the social distancing guidelines.
The analysis of collected data revealed six variables that were significantly correlated with one’s adherence to the social distancing guidelines: perception of protection, perception of danger, Sense of Coherence (SoC), gender, relationship status, and nationality. The perceptions of protection and danger depend on one’s understanding of the pandemic and the value of prevention measures, in addition to having trust in science and the government. If this trust erodes or the public health message does not reach the public, individuals are less likely to adhere to the guidelines, as was shown in large studies conducted in the United States [6,12]. In our study, greater perceptions of protection and danger were correlated with increased adherence to guidelines. Thus, we may hypothesize that adherence could increase if the perceptions of protection and danger are strengthened, possibly through successful and multi-faceted communication strategies between the leaders and the public. Age and sleep did not have a significant correlation with SRA. In other studies on this subject, older age was correlated with greater SRA [9], yet a multivariate analysis performed here showed that its effect became insignificant when other explanatory variables came into play.
In our study, men, single people, and Jews were more likely to adhere to the guidelines than their counterparts. It has been shown that one’s social circle predicts compliance with social distancing measures more than the individual’s confidence in the policy [11], explaining the correlation between the relationship status and compliance. Living with a partner might introduce opposing views on social distancing and widen the social circle. The finding that men in Israel were more likely to report adherence to guidelines stands in contrast with what was reported globally. A large study that combined data from eight countries showed a trend in the opposite direction, with women perceiving the pandemic as much more serious a threat than men and being more likely to adhere to the social-distancing advice [41]. Among the factors that might have contributed to this finding may be the child-rearing, cooking, and housekeeping burden that falls predominantly on women [42] and requires a greater reliance on others, inhibiting women’s ability to fully adhere to the guidelines. In the Israeli society, child-rearing is to a large extent a communal effort, and relies on the help of family members, including non-cohabiting ones like grandparents [43,44]. Further, if women are more responsible for grocery shopping than men, as is the case in the United States but has not been measured in Israel [45], they may be less likely to fully adhere to the social-distancing guidelines. Our finding that Jews were more likely to adhere to the guidelines is supported by previous studies that showed a greater toll of the pandemic in the Arabic population of Israel than in the country’s Jewish residents [46]. This outcome has been attributed to a myriad of factors, including their lower socio-economic status and education, lower than average trust in the government, unequal distribution of resources within the Israeli society, societal traditions and religious customs, and difficulty in maintaining health behaviors after the initial success of the first lockdown in Israel [46].
To the best of our knowledge, this is the only study conducted to date that examined the influence of one’s SoC on self-reported adherence to public health guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. It has been shown that SoC impacts one’s health status and chronic illness severity through its correlation with adherence to treatment guidelines: this mechanism has been described in people with Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease (COPD) [47], Human Immunodeficiency Virus (HIV) [48], and Inflammatory Bowel Disease (IBD) [49]. Our study confirms this finding in a population facing the threat of an illness rather than the illness itself: people with higher SoC were more likely to adhere to the national guidelines that were imposed in order to minimize the spread of the virus.
It is generally accepted that SoC is a significant predictor of Quality of Life (QoL), with higher SoC levels correlated with better QoL [50]. Our analysis supports this connection, revealing that the SoC levels were highest in people with higher incomes and college degrees. Age was correlated with one’s SoC, confirming previously reported findings on the continuous increase of SoC into old age [51]. One’s profession was associated with their SoC, with an interesting finding regarding the military: career soldiers had the highest SoC while the soldiers undergoing compulsory service had the lowest levels. This correlation might be in part related to the age of the soldiers—the Israeli Army drafts 18- and 19-year-olds—but it may also be connected to the young soldiers’ perceived lack of control over their lives. The locus of control and SoC are related constructs [52], and the COVID-19 pandemic appears to impact most negatively the younger population through decreasing their internal locus of control and SoC [53]. On the other hand, religiosity, gender, place of birth and the number of people in one’s household did not have a significant association with the respondents’ SoC.
Our findings support the use of the SoC model for successful communication between policymakers and the public. Coherent, precise, and straightforward communication will enable the residents to comprehend the reasons for adhering to the guidelines and comply with the social distancing policy, thus fulfilling the first element of the SoC model, comprehensibility. Further, the policymakers must make the measures manageable for people of all backgrounds, both through publicizing simple and attainable strategies for social distancing (such as tips on hygiene or alternative greetings) and through providing the citizens with resources and practical support they need (such as financial support, universal access to the Internet, telemedicine, well-organized online learning, online support groups, and more). Lastly, it is crucial to pinpoint the practical and ethical value of adhering to the guidelines, both on an individual and a communal level, thus fulfilling the final element of the SoC model, meaningfulness. Further research is needed on the factors that are associated with SoC and adherence to guidelines, including one’s trust in science and the government, as well as the differences in the Jewish and Arab populations in this context.
The study measures self-reported adherence to the social distancing guidelines. Thus, the social desirability bias comes into play, and the data collected must be interpreted with caution. It has been found that self-reports on adherence to social-distancing guidelines are consistently higher than the community average [8]. However, the study provides an important approximation of the adherence to guidelines, the reliability of which was strengthened by the anonymity of the data collection. Further, the bias does not negate this study’s findings regarding the factors associated with adherence. The cross-sectional nature of the study does not enable us to make causal inferences but only to identify correlations, some of which may have been muddied by unidentified confounders.

5. Conclusions

Compliance with the social distancing measures was generally high among Israeli respondents at the time of exit from the first lockdown. However, even at that time, close to a third of the respondents did not engage in the recommended handwashing routines, and nearly 20 percent did not maintain a 2-m distance when outside of the house. To contain COVID-19 in the absence of a lockdown, policies and education are needed to encourage ongoing, high compliance with social distancing. Identifying the populations least likely to adhere to the social distancing guidelines is crucial for health promoters and policymakers. We identified the populations at risk of non-adherence and some associated factors, beliefs and constructs. Women, Arabs, and people in relationships were identified as less likely to comply than their counterparts. Further, we showed the significant correlation of the perceptions of protection and danger with adherence, and reported for the first time its association with the Sense of Coherence. Thanks to identifying these factors, we may better fulfill our role as health promoters and inform the policymakers on effective ways to promote prevention and limit the spread of the disease.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.L.-A.; methodology, S.L.-A., A.K. and M.B.; formal analysis, M.B.; investigation, A.M.N. and S.L.-A.; writing—original draft preparation, S.L.-A., A.K., M.B. and A.M.N.; supervision, S.L.-A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Tel Aviv University (Approval 0001338-2).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Glogowsky, U.; Hansen, E.; Schächtele, S. How effective are social distancing policies? Evidence on the fight against COVID-19. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0257363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Ritchie, H.; Mathieu, E.; Rodés-Guirao, L.; Appel, C.; Giattino, C.; Ortiz-Ospina, E.; Hasell, J.; Macdonald, B.; Beltekian, D.; Roser, M. Coronavirus Pandemic (COVID-19). Our World Data. 2020. Available online: https://ourworldindata.org/coronavirus (accessed on 1 June 2022).
  3. Ingram, M.; Zahabian, A.; Hur, C. Prediction of COVID-19 Social Distancing Adherence (SoDA) on the United States county-level. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2021, 8, 87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Clark, C.; Davila, A.; Regis, M.; Kraus, S. Predictors of COVID-19 voluntary compliance behaviors: An international investigation. Glob. Transit. 2020, 2, 76–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Coroiu, A.; Moran, C.; Campbell, T.; Geller, A.C. Barriers and facilitators of adherence to social distancing recommendations during COVID-19 among a large international sample of adults. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0239795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Koetke, J.; Schumann, K.; Porter, T. Trust in science increases conservative support for social distancing. Group Processes Intergroup Relat. 2021, 24, 680–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Taylor, S.; Landry, C.A.; Paluszek, M.M.; Groenewoud, R.; Rachor, G.S.; Asmundson, G.J.G. A Proactive Approach for Managing COVID-19: The Importance of Understanding the Motivational Roots of Vaccination Hesitancy for SARS-CoV2. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 575950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Gollwitzer, A.; McLoughlin, K.; Martel, C.; Marshall, J.; Höhs, J.M.; Bargh, J.A. Linking Self-Reported Social Distancing to Real-World Behavior during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2021, 13, 656–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hills, S.; Eraso, Y. Factors associated with non-adherence to social distancing rules during the COVID-19 pandemic: A logistic regression analysis. BMC Public Health 2021, 21, 352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Eraso, Y.; Hills, S. Intentional and unintentional non-adherence to social distancing measures during COVID-19: A mixed-methods analysis. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0256495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Tunçgenç, B.; El Zein, M.; Sulik, J.; Newson, M.; Zhao, Y.; Dezecache, G.; Deroy, O. Social influence matters: We follow pandemic guidelines most when our close circle does. Br. J. Psychol. 2021, 112, 763–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Swami, V.; Barron, D. Rational thinking style, rejection of coronavirus (COVID-19) conspiracy theories/theorists, and compliance with mandated requirements. J. Pac. Rim Psychol. 2021, 15, 183449092110373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Rothgerber, H. Politicizing the COVID-19 Pandemic: Ideological Differences in Adherence to Social Distancing. PsyArXiv Preprints 2020. Available online: https://psyarxiv.com/k23cv/ (accessed on 1 June 2022).
  14. Bieleke, M.; Martarelli, C.S.; Wolff, W. If-then planning, self-control, and boredom as predictors of adherence to social distancing guidelines: Evidence from a two-wave longitudinal study with a behavioral intervention. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Wolff, W.; Martarelli, C.S.; Schüler, J.; Bieleke, M. High Boredom Proneness and Low Trait Self-Control Impair Adherence to Social Distancing Guidelines during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Everett, J.A.; Colombatto, C.; Chituc, V.; Brady, W.J.; Crockett, M. The Effectiveness of Moral Messages on Public Health Behavioral Intentions during the COVID-19 Pandemic. PsyArXiv Preprints 2020. Available online: https://psyarxiv.com/9yqs8/ (accessed on 1 June 2022).
  17. Pfattheicher, S.; Nockur, L.; Böhm, R.; Sassenrath, C.; Petersen, M.B. The Emotional Path to Action: Empathy Promotes Physical Distancing and Wearing of Face Masks during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 31, 1363–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Jordan, J.J.; Yoeli, E.; Rand, D.G. Don’t get it or don’t spread it: Comparing self-interested versus prosocial motivations for COVID-19 prevention behaviors. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 20222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Heffner, J.; Vives, M.-L.; FeldmanHall, O. Emotional responses to prosocial messages increase willingness to self-isolate during the COVID-19 pandemic. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2021, 170, 110420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Kissler, S.M.; Tedijanto, C.; Goldstein, E.; Grad, Y.H.; Lipsitch, M. Projecting the transmission dynamics of SARS-CoV-2 through the postpandemic period. Science 2020, 368, 860–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Van Bavel, J.J.; Baicker, K.; Boggio, P.S.; Capraro, V.; Cichocka, A.; Cikara, M.; Crockett, M.J.; Crum, A.J.; Douglas, K.M.; Druckman, J.N.; et al. Using Social and Behavioural Science to Support COVID-19 Pandemic Response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020, 4, 460–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Antonovsky, A. Health, Stress, and Coping; Jossey-Bass: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1979; ISBN 9780875894126. [Google Scholar]
  23. Antonovsky, A. Unraveling the Mystery of Health: How People Manage Stress and Stay Well (JOSSEY BASS SOCIAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE SERIES); Jossey-Bass: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hammond, A.; Niedermann, K. Patient education and self management. In Rheumatology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2010; pp. 77–97. ISBN 9780443069345. [Google Scholar]
  25. Eriksson, M.; Lindström, B. Validity of Antonovsky’s sense of coherence scale: A systematic review. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2005, 59, 460–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. APA Dictionary of Psychology. Available online: https://dictionary.apa.org/resilience (accessed on 11 March 2022).
  27. Kimhi, S.; Eshel, Y.; Marciano, H.; Adini, B. Fluctuations in National Resilience during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Panzeri, A.; Bertamini, M.; Butter, S.; Levita, L.; Gibson-Miller, J.; Vidotto, G.; Bentall, R.P.; Bennett, K.M. Factors impacting resilience as a result of exposure to COVID-19: The ecological resilience model. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0256041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Mana, A.; Bauer, G.F.; Meier Magistretti, C.; Sardu, C.; Juvinyà-Canal, D.; Hardy, L.J.; Catz, O.; Tušl, M.; Sagy, S. Order out of chaos: Sense of coherence and the mediating role of coping resources in explaining mental health during COVID-19 in 7 countries. SSM Ment. Health 2021, 1, 100001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Leung, A.Y.M.; Parial, L.L.; Tolabing, M.C.; Sim, T.; Mo, P.; Okan, O.; Dadaczynski, K. Sense of coherence mediates the relationship between digital health literacy and anxiety about the future in aging population during the COVID-19 pandemic: A path analysis. Aging Ment. Health 2022, 26, 544–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Schmuck, J.; Hiebel, N.; Rabe, M.; Schneider, J.; Erim, Y.; Morawa, E.; Jerg-Bretzke, L.; Beschoner, P.; Albus, C.; Hannemann, J.; et al. Sense of coherence, social support and religiosity as resources for medical personnel during the COVID-19 pandemic: A web-based survey among 4324 health care workers within the German Network University Medicine. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0255211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Barni, D.; Danioni, F.; Canzi, E.; Ferrari, L.; Ranieri, S.; Lanz, M.; Iafrate, R.; Regalia, C.; Rosnati, R. Facing the COVID-19 Pandemic: The Role of Sense of Coherence. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 578440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Daoud, N.; Soskolne, V.; Mindell, J.S.; Roth, M.A.; Manor, O. Ethnic inequalities in health between Arabs and Jews in Israel: The relative contribution of individual-level factors and the living environment. Int. J. Public Health 2018, 63, 313–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Constantini, K.; Markus, I.; Epel, N.; Jakobovich, R.; Gepner, Y.; Lev-Ari, S. Continued Participation of Israeli Adolescents in Online Sports Programs during the COVID-19 Pandemic Is Associated with Higher Resilience. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 4386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kimhi, S.; Eshel, Y.; Marciano, H.; Adini, B. A Renewed Outbreak of the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Longitudinal Study of Distress, Resilience, and Subjective Well-Being. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kimhi, S.; Dror, G. Resilience among Students from the Majority and Minority Group: The Israeli Case. JPBS 2017, 5, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Savadori, L.; Lauriola, M. Risk Perception and Protective Behaviors during the Rise of the COVID-19 Outbreak in Italy. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 577331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Franzen, A.; Wöhner, F. Coronavirus risk perception and compliance with social distancing measures in a sample of young adults: Evidence from Switzerland. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0247447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Xie, K.; Liang, B.; Dulebenets, M.A.; Mei, Y. The Impact of Risk Perception on Social Distancing during the COVID-19 Pandemic in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. About ITC—ITC Project. Available online: https://itcproject.org/about/ (accessed on 15 February 2022).
  41. Galasso, V.; Pons, V.; Profeta, P.; Becher, M.; Brouard, S.; Foucault, M. Gender differences in COVID-19 attitudes and behavior: Panel evidence from eight countries. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 27285–27291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Power, K. The COVID-19 pandemic has increased the care burden of women and families. Sustain. Sci. Pract. Policy 2020, 16, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Even-Zohar, A. Grandparent-Grandchild Relationships in Israel: A Comparison between Different Jewish Religious Groups. J. Intergener. Relatsh. 2015, 13, 75–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Attar-Schwartz, S.; Buchanan, A. Grandparenting and adolescent well-being: Evidence from the UK and Israel. Contemp. Soc. Sci. 2018, 13, 219–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Pew Research Center. Women Do More Cooking, Grocery Shopping than Men among U.S. Couples. Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2019/09/24/among-u-s-couples-women-do-more-cooking-and-grocery-shopping-than-men/ (accessed on 27 May 2022).
  46. Saban, M.; Myers, V.; Peretz, G.; Avni, S.; Wilf-Miron, R. COVID-19 morbidity in an ethnic minority: Changes during the first year of the pandemic. Public Health 2021, 198, 238–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Tsiligianni, I.; Sifaki-Pistolla, D.; Gergianaki, I.; Kampouraki, M.; Papadokostakis, P.; Poulonirakis, I.; Gialamas, I.; Bempi, V.; Ierodiakonou, D. Associations of sense of coherence and self-efficacy with health status and disease severity in COPD. NPJ Prim. Care Respir. Med. 2020, 30, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Cederfjäll, C.; Langius-Eklöf, A.; Lidman, K.; Wredling, R. Self-reported adherence to antiretroviral treatment and degree of sense of coherence in a group of HIV-infected patients. AIDS Patient Care STDS 2002, 16, 609–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Opheim, R.; Fagermoen, M.S.; Jelsness-Jørgensen, L.-P.; Bernklev, T.; Moum, B. Sense of coherence in patients with inflammatory bowel disease. Gastroenterol. Res. Pract. 2014, 2014, 989038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Eriksson, M.; Lindström, B. Antonovsky’s sense of coherence scale and its relation with quality of life: A systematic review. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2007, 61, 938–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Silverstein, M.; Heap, J. Sense of coherence changes with aging over the second half of life. Adv. Life Course Res. 2015, 23, 98–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Rothmann, S. Sense of coherence, locus of control, self-efficacy and job satisfaction. SAJEMS 2001, 4, 41–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Misamer, M.; Signerski-Krieger, J.; Bartels, C.; Belz, M. Internal Locus of Control and Sense of Coherence Decrease during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Survey of Students and Professionals in Social Work. Front. Sociol. 2021, 6, 705809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Compliance with the 7 social distancing measures.
Figure 1. Compliance with the 7 social distancing measures.
Ijerph 19 08041 g001
Table 1. Demographic statistics by nationality.
Table 1. Demographic statistics by nationality.
Characteristic Study Population Total N, (%) or Mean (SD)Jewish
N (%) or Mean (SD)
Arabic
N (%) or Mean (SD)
p Value
Age[18.0, 89.0]39.3 (14.7)40.4 (14.8)35.0 (13.7)<0.001
Number of People at Home[1.0, 13.0]3.8 (1.8)3.7 (1.8)4.5 (2.0)<0.001
Family IncomeBelow average321 (47.0)188 (35.2)18 (12.1)<0.001
Average156 (22.8)128 (24.0)28 (18.8)
Above average206 (30.2)218 (40.8)103 (69.1)
GenderMale404 (50.1)317 (49.2)87 (53.4)0.344
Female403 (49.9)327 (50.8)76 (46.6)
Marital StatusWithout partner357 (44.2)284 (44.1)73 (44.8)0.875
With partner450 (55.8)360 (55.9)90 (55.2)
ReligiousNo654 (81.2)535 (83.1)119 (73.9)0.008
Yes151 (18.8)109 (16.9)42 (26.1)
ReligiositySecular375 (46.6)335 (52.0)40 (24.8)<0.001
Traditional279 (34.7)200 (31.1)79 (49.1)
Religious131 (16.3)91 (14.1)40 (24.8)
Ultra-religious20 (2.5)18 (2.8)2 (1.2)
EducationElementary or less7 (0.9)5 (0.8)2 (1.2)<0.001
High school, no diploma76 (9.4)56 (8.7)20 (12.3)
High school with diploma172 (21.3)145 (22.5)27 (16.6)
College with no degree175 (21.7)144 (22.4)31 (19.0)
First degree272 (33.7)204 (31.7)68 (41.7)
Second degree and higher105 (13.0)90 (14.0)15 (9.2)
BirthplaceIsrael653 (88.0)555 (86.2)98 (100.0)<0.001
Abroad89 (12.0)89 (13.8)0 (0.0)
DistrictJerusalem64 (7.9)59 (9.2)5 (3.1)<0.001
North140 (17.3)63 (9.8)77 (47.2)
Haifa109 (13.5)78 (12.1)31 (19.0)
Center201 (24.9)181 (28.1)20 (12.3)
Tel Aviv145 (18.0)139 (21.6)6 (3.7)
South117 (14.5)93 (14.4)24 (14.7)
Judea and Samaria31 (3.8)31 (4.8)0 (0.0)
EducationNo430 (53.3)294 (45.7)83 (50.9)0.228
College degree377 (46.7)350 (54.3)80 (49.1)
EmploymentMandatory soldier84 (10.4)19 (3.0)65 (39.9)<0.001
Professional soldier6 (0.7)4 (0.6)2 (1.2)
Higher education student68 (8.4)51 (7.9)17 (10.4)
Part time employee61 (7.6)53 (8.2)8 (4.9)
Full time employee366 (45.4)320 (49.7)46 (28.2)
Independent 54 (6.7)49 (7.6)5 (3.1)
Retired with pension43 (5.3)41 (6.4)2 (1.2)
Unemployed77 (9.5)62 (9.6)15 (9.2)
Vacation without pay48 (5.9)45 (7.0)3 (1.8)
Table 2. Multivariate analysis of variables associated with social distancing adherence.
Table 2. Multivariate analysis of variables associated with social distancing adherence.
VariableLevelsMean (SD)UnivariableMultivariate
CoefficientpCoefficientp
Perception of Protection[1, 6]10.2 (4.5)0.84 (0.64, 1.03)<0.0010.69 (0.45, 0.93)<0.001
Perception of DangerNo10.7 (4.6) <0.001 0.005
Yes9.4 (4.1)−1.37
(−2.01, −0.72)
−1.10
(−1.86, −0.34)
Sense of Coherence[18, 91]10.2 (4.5)−0.03
(−0.05, −0.01)
0.016−0.03
(−0.06, −0.00)
0.042
GenderMale10.8 (4.7) <0.001 0.003
Female9.7 (4.1)−1.13
(−1.75, −0.52)
−1.09
(−1.80, −0.38)
NationalityArab9.1 (4.1) 0.001 <0.001
Jew10.5 (4.5)1.37 (0.60, 2.14) 1.98 (1.05, 2.91)
Relationship StatusPartnered9.7 (4.2) <0.001 0.033
Not partnered10.9 (4.7)1.18 (0.56, 1.80) 0.92 (0.07, 1.78)
ReligiousYes
No
10.1 (4.8)
10.3 (4.8)
0.18 (−0.63, 0.98)0.667−0.38 (−1.31, 0.54)0.418
Educationcollege
below
10.1 (4.4)
10.3 (4.5)
0.27 (−0.35, 0.89)0.397−0.19 (−0.91, 0.53)0.602
Family incomeBelow average
Average
Above average
10.0 (4.6)
10.7 (4.5)
10.4 (4.2)
0.71 (−0.16, 1.57)
0.37 (−0.42, 1.15)
0.2570.13 (−0.79, 1.05)
0.05 (−0.83, 0.93)
0.962
Age[18, 70]10.2 (4.5)−0.04
(−0.06, −0.02)
<0.001−0.03
(−0.06, 0.00)
0.094
Table 3. Demographic characteristics associated with sense of coherence.
Table 3. Demographic characteristics associated with sense of coherence.
LevelValueN (%)Value (t/F)p Value
GenderMale61.9 (13.0)404 (50.1)t = 1.730.083
Female60.3 (14.5)403 (49.9)
Marital StatusNo57.9 (13.9)357 (44.2)t = −5.90<0.001
Yes63.6 (13.1)450 (55.8)
ReligiousNo60.7 (13.7)654 (81.2)t = −1.760.075
Yes62.9 (14.0)151 (18.8)
Number of People in Household[1.0, 13.0]61.1 (13.8)806 (100.0)t = −0.420.676
College EducationCollege degree62.6 (13.2)377 (46.7)t = 2.850.005
None59.8 (14.1)430 (53.3)
Family Income Above average64.1 (13.6)206 (30.2)F = 10.04<0.001
Average61.8 (13.6)156 (22.8)
Below average58.7 (13.6)321 (47.0)
Age[18.0, 89.0]61.1 (13.8)807 (100.0)t = 7.80<0.001
BirthplaceIsraeli born61.5 (13.6)653 (88.0)t = 3.790.052
Born abroad64.4 (13.4)89 (12.0)
DistrictJerusalem64.3 (11.9)64 (7.9)F = 2.210.040
North59.3 (13.5)140 (17.3)
Haifa62.3 (13.7)109 (13.5)
Center61.2 (14.3)201 (24.9)
Tel Aviv60.8 (14.0)145 (18.0)
South59.3 (13.4)117 (14.5)
Judea and Samaria66.2 (14.3)31 (3.8)
ReligiositySecular61.4 (13.9)375 (46.6)F = 1.730.159
Traditional59.8 (13.4)279 (34.7)
Religious63.0 (13.9)131 (16.3)
Ultra-religious62.8 (15.1)20 (2.5)
EducationElementary or less62.7 (17.8)7 (0.9)F = 6.30<0.001
High school no diploma58.6 (14.2)76 (9.4)
High school with diploma57.1 (14.4)172 (21.3)
High school, no college degree62.9 (13.1)175 (21.7)
First degree61.5 (13.2)272 (33.7)
Second degree and higher65.4 (12.6)105 (13.0)
ProfessionMandatory soldier52.8 (13.1)84 (10.4)F = 9.11<0.001
Career soldier69.5 (11.7)6 (0.7)
Student—academia57.9 (12.4)68 (8.4)
Part time employee62.4 (12.4)61 (7.6)
Full time employee63.1 (13.7)366 (45.4)
Independent 64.2 (12.6)54 (6.7)
Retired with pension68.4 (13.4)43 (5.3)
Unemployed57.3 (13.8)77 (9.5)
Vacation without pay58.3 (11.7)48 (5.9)
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Novak, A.M.; Katz, A.; Bitan, M.; Lev-Ari, S. The Association between the Sense of Coherence and the Self-Reported Adherence to Guidelines during the First Months of the COVID-19 Pandemic in Israel. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 8041. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19138041

AMA Style

Novak AM, Katz A, Bitan M, Lev-Ari S. The Association between the Sense of Coherence and the Self-Reported Adherence to Guidelines during the First Months of the COVID-19 Pandemic in Israel. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(13):8041. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19138041

Chicago/Turabian Style

Novak, Anne Marie, Adi Katz, Michal Bitan, and Shahar Lev-Ari. 2022. "The Association between the Sense of Coherence and the Self-Reported Adherence to Guidelines during the First Months of the COVID-19 Pandemic in Israel" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 13: 8041. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19138041

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop