Next Article in Journal
Mapping Building-Based Spatiotemporal Distributions of Carbon Dioxide Emission: A Case Study in England
Previous Article in Journal
Application of Integrated Fixed-Film Activated Sludge in a Conventional Wastewater Treatment Plant
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Long-Term Effects of Early Childhood Resilience Profiles on School Outcomes among Children in the Child Welfare System

1
College of Social Work, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
2
School of Social Work, Falk College, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244, USA
3
Quantitative Research, Evaluation and Measurement, College of Education and Human Ecology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
4
Department of Psychology, The University of the South, Sewanee, TN 37383, USA
5
Life Paths Research Center, Sewanee, TN 37375, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(10), 5987; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19105987
Submission received: 13 April 2022 / Revised: 10 May 2022 / Accepted: 12 May 2022 / Published: 14 May 2022

Abstract

:
This study aimed to examine the association between early childhood resilience profiles and later school outcomes (academic achievement and school involvement) among children in the U.S. child welfare system. This study compared 827 children aged 3–5 years in three latent profile groups (poor emotional and behavioral resilience, low cognitive resilience, and multi-domain resilience) to their baseline profiles using data from the National Survey of Child and Adolescent Well-Being (NSCAW-II). At the three-year follow-up, children with low emotional and behavioral resilience profiles and children with the multi-domain resilience profile had significantly higher basic reading skills, reading comprehension, and math reasoning compared to children with low scores on the cognitive resilience profile. Furthermore, children with the multi-domain resilience profile had significantly higher levels of emotional school engagement than did those with the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile and considerably higher levels of behavioral school engagement compared to those with the low cognitive resilience profile. The findings highlight the persistent effects of early resilience into the later childhood years. Moreover, our results suggest the need for early identification of and intervention for children with low cognitive or emotional/behavioral resilience during the preschool years to promote academic success and school engagement during the school-age years.

1. Introduction

Building resilience during early childhood has long-lasting positive effects on an individual’s well-being over the life course. There is a growing body of research on resilience among children who have experienced childhood maltreatment. Recently, an emerging line of research has utilized person-centered approaches (e.g., cluster analysis, latent class analysis, latent profile analysis, or growth mixture modeling) to explore patterns of resilience in populations with a history of childhood trauma and maltreatment. Although recent research has made remarkable headway towards identifying resilience patterns in maltreated children, how these patterns affect later developmental functioning, such as school outcomes, remains unclear and understudied. Examining school outcomes related to early childhood resilience (defined here as a process of achieving positive adaptation across multiple domains of functioning despite exposure to trauma [1] during the preschool years) is crucial in maintaining and strengthening early developmental assets and milestones. This paper builds upon our previous work that identified three distinct profiles of early childhood resilience (i.e., low cognitive resilience, low emotional and behavioral resilience, and multi-domain resilience) among child-welfare-involved children [2]. In this paper, we seek to investigate the extent to which these resilience profiles predict later school outcomes, including academic achievement and school engagement, after three years.

1.1. Resilience in Children with a History of Child Maltreatment

Children who have experienced child maltreatment often face adverse psychosocial and behavioral outcomes in later life [3,4]. Specifically, there is a known association between maltreatment and poor social adjustment, juvenile delinquency, psychiatric conditions, low education attainment, and substance abuse [5,6,7]. Several challenges persist for this population as they experience continued exposure to violence, separation from parents/caregivers, multiple out-of-home placements, or re-traumatization [8].
Fortunately, there is a plethora of research on resiliency and protective factors that help support healthy development [9,10]. These studies provide empirical evidence that these children can achieve positive adaptation despite their maltreatment [11,12,13,14,15]. Moreover, a robust body of research has found that certain protective factors help mitigate the harmful effects of childhood maltreatment [16,17]. Among these are personal attributes such as self-efficacy, emotion regulation, temperament, or future orientation; family and cultural supports such as parental emotional support, cognitive stimulation, or parent–child attachment; school/community resources and characteristics such as community cohesion, schools, or peer support [4,16,17,18,19]. Taken cumulatively, these studies recognize the importance of employing a strengths-based perspective to examine resilience and protective factors among children with a history of child maltreatment, as well as moving beyond deficit models that focus on risks and adverse outcomes.

1.2. Measuring Resilience and Identifying Patterns of Resilience

Resilience has been historically difficult to measure due to the dynamic nature of the changes and development across the various domains of human function [9]. The literature includes disparate methods and instruments to assess an individual’s resilience and adaptation to hardship, particularly in studies that focus on adults and children with a history of maltreatment [20]. Studies involving children frequently measure functioning across developmental domains (social, cognitive, emotional, behavioral, and occupational) and achievement milestones [9]. In contrast, studies that measure resilience in adults often include the preclusion of psychopathology, measurements of well-being, and social competency [21]. However, studies remain varied throughout both groups.
Although measurement differences remain a challenge in research, multipoint resilience indicators remain viable for comprehensively assessing resilience across different developmental domains [16]. Researchers have argued for considering multiple domains of child functioning when examining resilience among children with a history of maltreatment [9,16,22], as competence and resilience within one domain do not guarantee competence and resilience in another domain. Altogether, child maltreatment and resilience researchers have suggested that resilience measures should be comprehensive and expansive to capture the many facets of resilience and adaptation among children with maltreatment histories.
Recently, an emerging line of research has explored different patterns of resilience, using person-centered approaches rather than variable-centered approaches. While a variable-centered approach focuses on examining the relationship among variables, a person-centered approach identifies heterogeneous subgroups of individuals who share distinct characteristics and attributes [23]. Thus, a person-centered approach is useful in identifying unique and distinct configurations of resilience. One study used a latent profile analysis with a sample of 164 emancipated foster youth and identified four resilience profiles, including maladaptive (16.5%), resilient (47%), internally resilient (30%), and externally resilient (6.5%) [24]. Another study conducted a latent profile analysis with 12-year-old children who had been involved with child protective services (CPS). The study identified five profiles of adaptation/competence, including consistent resilience (12.7%), consistent maladaptation (11.6%), posttraumatic stress problems (8.9%), school maladaptation/family protection (36.2%), and low socialization skills (30.6%) [25]. Other studies, not specifically focused on child maltreatment, explored resilience patterns among children who have experienced early adversities and trauma (such as exposure to intimate partner violence, poverty, or parental psychopathology), and these studies likewise identified four or five patterns of resilience [15,26].

1.3. Early Childhood Resilience Profiles and Later School Outcomes

Our team’s prior work focused on the identification of profiles of resilience, specifically during early childhood (the ages of 3 to 5 years) among child-welfare-involved children as this important developmental period remained overlooked in the maltreatment and resilience profile literature [2]. Using a latent profile analysis, we identified three distinct profiles of resilience: low emotional and behavioral resilience (20%), low cognitive resilience (24%), and multi-domain resilience (56%). The low emotional and behavioral resilience profile had children who showed the lowest emotional and behavioral adaptations yet above-average levels of cognitive and social functioning. The low cognitive resilience profile included children who showed the lowest levels of cognitive ability and lower levels of social functioning, yet average levels of emotional and behavioral functioning. Finally, the multi-domain resilience profile included children who demonstrated above-average levels of competence across all domains of functioning (Figure 1 provides a visual representation of the three latent profiles).
Despite emerging research exploring distinct configurations of resilience among children with maltreatment histories, including our work of three early childhood resilience profiles, little research has examined how resilience profiles in early childhood are associated with distal outcomes such as school outcomes at a later developmental stage. School context and outcomes are critical to examine as they have important short- and long-term implications for elements of quality of life, including child self-esteem/self-worth, mental and behavioral well-being, and later employment status and job satisfaction during adulthood [27,28,29,30,31]. For example, school engagement is a protective factor for maltreated children [32]. Specifically, researchers have found that children with adverse childhood experiences demonstrate high levels of well-being, including higher self-esteem, when they have high levels of high school engagement [33,34]. Youth acquire a sense of belongingness and purpose when they feel connected to their school and the staff [35,36]. Unfortunately, however, research has also shown that although school is an important change agent, children with a history of maltreatment are likely to quickly disengage from school for reasons such as placement or school instability and peer victimization [35]. Similarly, children with maltreatment histories often experience academic difficulties and poorer academic achievement, including lower scores on standardized reading and math tests [37,38].
To date, most studies have examined resilience profiles as an outcome and have focused on identifying predictors or characteristics associated with resilience profiles [15,25,26]. Consequently, there is a dearth of research on the relationship between early childhood resilience profiles and distal outcomes such as later school outcomes (e.g., academic achievement or school engagement) among children involved with the child welfare system. Understanding how early childhood resilience affects later development and outcomes is vital to developing intervention strategies that will maximize the likelihood of ongoing, uninterrupted resilient development. Although not directly focused on children with maltreatment histories, one study found that children who grew up in poverty but achieved a high threshold of resilience by the time they entered kindergarten showed academic achievement during the elementary school years comparable to that of children not in poverty [39], providing preliminary evidence that resilience in early childhood predicts later school success.

1.4. The Current Study

Building upon our prior work on early childhood resilience profiles [2], we examined the resilience profiles’ long-term relations to academic achievement and school engagement during the school-age years. Understanding the long-term influence of early childhood resilience on school outcomes is vital to providing effective support and interventions that foster continued resilient development, including success in school, for this vulnerable population. The following research question guided the study: how are different profiles of resilience at baseline associated with academic achievement (e.g., basic reading skills, reading comprehension, or math reasoning) and school engagement (emotional or behavioral) when measured after three years? It was hypothesized that children with the multi-domain resilience profile at baseline would have greater academic achievement and school engagement compared to those with the low cognitive resilience profile and those with the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile at baseline.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Data and Sample

This secondary data analysis used the data from the National Survey of Child and Adolescent Well-Being [40], which collected longitudinal data from children and families involved in the child protective services system. From 2008 to 2021, NSCAW-II collected three waves of data among 5872 children up to the age of 17.5: Wave 1 was the baseline, Wave 2 was a 1.5-year follow-up, and Wave 3 was a 3-year follow-up. Since we sought to investigate how early childhood resilience was associated with later school outcomes, the present study analyzed data from Waves 1 and 3. Our analytic sample included children who were 3–5 years old at Wave 1 (N = 827).
Descriptive statistics of all study variables are shown in Table 1. In terms of sample characteristics, children’s mean age in Wave 1 was 3.96 (SD = 0.82). Just under half of the children (46.1%) were girls; 39.6% were non-Hispanic White, 31.4% were non-Hispanic Black, 24.0% were Hispanic, and 5.0% were other races, including American Indian, Asian, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, and multiple races. Regarding caregivers’ characteristics, 25.8% of primary caregivers had not completed high school, and about 78.1% reported household income below the 200% federal poverty line.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Resilience

Resilience was conceptualized as a multidimensional construct and measured using multiple instruments in Wave 1 (baseline). Specifically, cognitive resilience, social resilience, emotional resilience, and behavioral resilience were assessed and operationalized as competence and positive functioning in the domains of cognitive, social, emotional, and behavioral development, respectively, after exposure to adversity [4,41]. Receptive language skills and verbal ability (expressive language skills) in the domain of cognitive resilience were measured using two standardized, validated scales, the Expressive Communication subscale and the Auditory Comprehension subscale, within the Preschool Language Scale—3 [42]. PLS-3, which showed strong validity [43], is a standardized tool to assess children’s overall language development from birth to age 6. The standard sum scores were calculated for these two subscales, with higher scores indicating better receptive language skills and greater verbal ability. Internal consistency of these two subscales was good at α = 0.87 for the Expressive Communication scale and α = 0.85 for the Auditory Comprehension scale. The domain of social resilience was measured using two scales: the 39-item Social Skills Rating System [44] and the 15-item Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale Screener [45]. The SSRS assessed children’s prosocial skills, and the VABSS measured children’s functioning in social situations (socialization). Standard scores were calculated for both the SSRS and the VABSS, with higher scores indicating higher levels of social functioning. Internal consistencies of the two scales were acceptable in this sample (SSRS α = 0.91; VABSS α = 0.75). For the domain of emotional resilience, emotional regulation and anxiety/depression were assessed using two subscales of the Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 1.5–5 (CBCL/1.5–5; Achenbach and Ruffle, 2000): the Emotionally Reactive scale (8 items; α = 0.78) and the Anxious/Depressed scale (8 items; α = 0.63), respectively. Finally, for the domain of behavioral resilience, child aggression and attention were measured using the 8-item CBCL Aggression scale (α = 0.82) and the 8-item CBCL Attention Problem scale (α = 0.91).

2.2.2. Academic Achievement

At the 3-year follow-up, the Woodcock–Johnson III Tests of Achievement (WJ-III; Woodcock et al., 2003) was employed to evaluate children’s academic achievement. Three subscales of WJ-III, the Letter–Word Identification scale, the Passage Comprehension scale, and the Applied Problems scale, were used to capture children’s basic reading skills, reading comprehension, and math reasoning, respectively. For each subscale, the standard sum scores of the items were used, with higher scores representing better achievement/performance.

2.2.3. School Engagement

At the 3-year follow-up, children’s school engagement was evaluated using the Drug-Free Schools Outcome Study Questions (DFSCA). This scale has been widely used to measure school engagement among children involved in the child welfare system (α = 0.72). Two aspects of school engagement were assessed based on a study that found the two-factor structure of the scale to fit the data well [46]. Specifically, the scale measured emotional engagement (3 items, such as “enjoy being in school” and “find classes interesting”) and behavioral engagement (6 items, such as “do best work in school,” “get along with other students,” and “get sent to the office because misbehaved”). The youth’s responses to the items were summed (1 = never, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often, 4 = almost always) to create emotional engagement and behavioral engagement scores. Negatively worded items were reverse-coded, so higher scores meant better school engagement.

2.2.4. Covariates

Demographic variables, including children’s age, sex (0 = male; 1 = female), and race/ethnicity, were controlled for in the present study. Child race/ethnicity was a categorical variable and was dummy coded into White, Hispanic, and Other, using Black as a reference group. Other information was added as covariates, including child abuse (0 = no exposure to child abuse; 1 = at least one CPS report for alleged child abuse), neglect (0 = no exposure to child neglect; 1 = at least one alleged incident of child neglect), poverty (0 = household income ≥ federal poverty level; 1 = household income < federal poverty level), and caregiver’s highest education level (0 = high school or more; 1 = less than high school).

2.3. Data Analysis

A latent profile analysis (LPA) was previously conducted to examine heterogeneity in resilience among children involved with the child welfare system and revealed three distinct resilience profiles (i.e., low emotional and behavioral resilience, low cognitive resilience, and multi-domain resilience) [2]. Building upon these findings, we performed a three-step LPA to examine the relationship between the three resilience profiles and the 3-year follow-up school outcomes (distal outcomes). The three-step LPA method is generally considered more robust and rigorous than the one-step LPA approach [47,48,49,50]. In the first step, unconditional LPA models were estimated to determine the optimal number of classes (this step overlaps with the previous analysis whereby we identified three resilience profiles) [2]. The second step involved assigning each child to a latent class based on the posterior probability obtained in the first step. Finally, in the third step, the conditional LPA model examined the mean differences in the five distal outcomes (three academic achievement outcomes and two school engagement outcomes) across the identified resilience profiles while controlling covariates. Missing data were handled by the full maximum likelihood method. SPSS Version 27.0 [51] was used for data management and descriptive analysis, and Mplus Version 8.0 [23] was used to conduct the three-step LPA.

3. Results

To address our research question, we examined how the three resilience profiles at baseline—low emotional and behavioral resilience (20%), low cognitive resilience (24%), and multi-domain resilience (56%) [2] were associated with children’s academic achievement and school engagement measured three years after. Table 2 shows the results of pairwise mean comparisons for the five school outcomes across the three resilience profiles. Children with the multi-domain resilience profile showed significantly higher levels of basic reading skills (mean difference = 13.18, p < 0.001), reading comprehension (mean difference = 12.60, p < 0.001), and math reasoning (mean difference = 14.82, p < 0.001), compared to children with the low cognitive resilience profile. Similarly, children with the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile showed significantly higher levels of basic reading skills (mean difference = 10.81, p < 0.01), reading comprehension (mean difference = 10.13, p < 0.001), and math reasoning (mean difference = 12.16, p < 0.001), compared to children with the low cognitive resilience profile. There were no significant mean differences in academic achievement between children with the multi-domain resilience profile and children with the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile.
Regarding school engagement, children with the multi-domain resilience profile at baseline showed significantly higher levels of emotional engagement (mean difference = 0.80, p = 0.041) compared to children with the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile. Additionally, children with the multi-domain resilience profile at baseline showed significantly higher levels of behavioral school engagement (mean difference = 1.17, p = 0.033) compared to children with the low cognitive resilience profile. There were no other significant mean differences in school engagement among the three resilience profiles. The effects of covariates on academic achievement and school engagement distal outcomes are shown in Table 3. Girls had showed significantly higher levels of basic reading skills, reading comprehension, and behavioral engagement compared to boys. Older age at baseline was associated with lower levels of basic reading skills and reading comprehension but with higher levers of behavioral engagement during the school-age years. Poverty, child abuse, and child neglect were all significantly associated with lower levels of basic reading skills and reading comprehension during the school-age years.

4. Discussion

The primary aim of this study was to examine the extent to which unique profiles of resilience (low emotional and behavioral resilience, low cognitive resilience, multi-domain resilience) in early childhood predict future school outcomes, including academic achievement and school engagement, three years later, in children involved with the child welfare system. The study findings provide new insight into the long-term effects of early childhood resilience on later development among children involved with the child welfare system.
Our hypothesis that children with the multi-domain resilience profile would have greater academic achievement compared to those with the low cognitive or emotional/behavioral resilience profile was partially supported. Children with the multi-domain resilience profile had greater academic achievement compared to those with the low cognitive resilience profile but did not do any better or worse than those with the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile. Interestingly, not just children with the multi-domain resilience profile, but also children with the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile showed significantly higher levels of basic reading skills, reading comprehension, and math reasoning compared to those who had the low cognitive resilience profile at baseline. In other words, children who showed low cognitive competence during early childhood continued to show those lower skills as they entered school. These findings corroborate the broader literature that documents a link between early cognitive skills and later academic performance among high-risk children, including children in low-income families [52] and children with a history of child maltreatment [53]. Collectively, our findings may suggest that these children could benefit from more targeted support and highlight the potential importance of early intervention to support the successful development of cognitive skills during early childhood.
Overall, resilience profiles during the preschool years were not a strong predictor of the level of school engagement at the three-year follow-up. It should be noted that there is still some ambiguity about the factor structure of the school engagement measure (the Drug-Free Schools and Communities Act scale) used in this study [46]. Some studies have used it as a unidimensional scale, whereas others have argued for the two-factor (emotional and behavioral) or the three-factor (emotional, behavioral, and cognitive–behavioral) structure [54,55,56,57]. We opted to use the two-factor structure to assess emotional engagement and behavioral engagement in school because this structure has consistently shown robust evidence for construct validity in prior research [46], but it is possible that other factor structures might have fit the data better.
Nonetheless, there were two significant findings for school engagement. First, children with the multi-domain resilience profile showed significantly higher levels of emotional school engagement compared to those with the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile. Our first finding is partially consistent with prior studies that measured emotional school engagement and other areas associated with child internalizing and externalizing behaviors [58,59,60]. These studies found anger, measured in preschool [58] and kindergarten [60], to be negatively associated with emotional and behavioral engagement in kindergarten. Although it is somewhat surprising and inconsistent with prior research that children exhibiting low emotional engagement would still be behaviorally engaged and doing well academically, these findings are consistent in part with the study of Bryce et al. (2018) in which academic achievement was positively associated with behavioral engagement, but not emotional engagement. Our finding suggest that the strong cognitive skills among the children with the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile may have allowed them to participate and do well academically, despite negative feelings towards school. While these children are behaviorally engaged and doing well academically, it is possible that emotional engagement among children in the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile may be hindered by poor relationships with peers and/or teachers. While research specifically measuring emotional engagement among early school-age children remains relatively limited, existent literature has found positive peer relationships to be connected to high emotional engagement [61,62]. Conversely, lower emotional engagement has been connected to higher teacher–student and peer conflict [62,63]. Children lower in emotional and behavioral functioning may struggle to make positive relational connections, diminishing their sense of belonging and emotional connectedness to school.
Our second significant finding related to school engagement revealed that children with the multi-domain resilience profile showed significantly higher levels of behavioral school engagement than those with the low cognitive resilience profile did. This finding is aligned in part with previous literature that has found that language and behavior problems often co-occur [64] and that children with language delays are more likely to develop later behavior problems [65]. Considering children with low cognitive resilience profiles displayed average levels of behavioral functioning during the preschool period, later issues with behavioral school engagement during the school-age years may be connected to their lower cognitive skills. As evidenced by their academic performance, children with the low cognitive resilience profile displayed lower levels of basic reading skills and reading comprehension three years later. This supports elements of Stanovich’s (1986) Matthew effects model, in which difficulty with reading early on “results in unrewarding early reading experiences that lead to less involvement in reading-related activities” (p. 364). Our findings are consistent with other research that found that, after controlling for prior behavior and attention problems as well other SES and demographic confounds, poor reading ability in first grade significantly predicted children’s behavior in third grade, as these children were more likely to be task avoidant, act out, withdraw from classroom activities, and display poor self-control [66].
Given their struggles in academic performance and behavioral engagement, it is somewhat surprising that children with the low cognitive resilience profile did not display significantly lower emotional engagement, especially considering they displayed lower social functioning at baseline. This could be explained by the significant change in demands from preschool to school age [67]. While lower receptive and expressive language skills during preschool age may have inhibited social functioning, they likely had little impact on behavioral functioning, given that the behavioral demands at preschool age are relatively low. However, once children entered school age, their lower cognitive skills may have affected their ability to meet increased academic needs, leading to disengagement. At the same time, social demands may have not been as challenging to achieve, likely allowing children to meet these demands despite their lower cognitive skills.

4.1. Limitations

Like other empirical studies, this study is not without its limitations. First, the use of a sample comprising children involved with the child welfare system due to child maltreatment may limit the generalizability of the study results. Second, despite the temporal order of measuring resilience at baseline and school outcomes 3 years later, any firm causal inference may not be made, because other, unobserved factors may play a role in the associations between these variables. Third, although it is a strength that we utilized multiple indicators and measures of resilience to identify resilience profiles, our measures of cognitive resilience were heavily focused on language ability and did not assess other aspects of cognitive ability, such as reasoning, remembering, problem solving, or decision making. Future research may benefit from including more comprehensive measures of cognitive resilience.

4.2. Implications

The study’s findings offer important insights into practice and policy development to promote the educational well-being of children with a history of CPS involvement. Considering that our results indicated less optimal school outcomes among children who had low cognitive resilience or low emotional and behavioral resilience during early child-hood, it is important to target these children for early identification and intervention to ensure their academic success and school engagement during the school-age years. For instance, it is crucial to test whether interventions, such as Dialogic Reading [68], designed to improve language ability among young children with low cognitive resilience (i.e., those experiencing challenges with language development) provide long-term benefits for children’s academic achievement and behavioral engagement in school during the school-age period. Similarly, given the lower levels of emotional school engagement among children who had low emotional and behavioral resilience during early childhood, targeted interventions, such as trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy (TF-CBF) [69], may be helpful in addressing emotional and behavior problems. Further, it may be particularly important to implement interventions during early school age that help children gain a positive image, thoughts, and emotions about school, especially considering the decline in school engagement trajectories among many students as they progress through elementary school [63,70]. Utilizing school-based interventions that use integrative strategies to target the various dimensions of school engagement may be most appropriate. Such integration may increase effectiveness while also addressing a wide variety of different students’ engagement needs. Considering that children in this study exhibited generally moderate levels of emotional and behavioral school engagement regardless of their early resilience profiles, it seems essential to devise more effective strategies to boost school interest and engagement among children involved with the child welfare system.

5. Conclusions

We examined the longitudinal associations between early childhood resilience profiles (low emotional and behavioral resilience, low cognitive resilience, multi-domain resilience) and school outcomes (academic achievement; emotional and behavioral school engagement) among children involved with the U.S. child welfare system. Overall, the study results indicated the lasting effects of early resilience into the later childhood years, demonstrating the virtuous cycles of resilience. Compared to children with the multi-domain resilience profile, children with the low emotional and behavioral resilience profile had comparable academic achievement and were behaviorally engaged in school but displayed lower emotional engagement. In contrast, children with the low cognitive resilience profiles had poorer academic achievement and were not behaviorally engaged, yet still displayed high levels of emotional engagement. Collectively, our findings may suggest that while academic achievement and behavioral engagement may be closely connected, emotional engagement may depend on other factors outside of how well a child is doing in school or how behaviorally engaged they are. It should be noted that multiple mechanisms that were not examined in the study, such as social ecological strengths and resources available during the preschool years, may have contributed to better school outcomes [71]. Future research is needed to further determine the factors that influence emotional school engagement among early school-age children, the extent to which interpersonal relationships, such as teacher and peer relationships, influence such engagement, and whether such connections are unique to this age group. Further illumination in these areas will allow for more targeted and effective interventions to bolster school engagement and academic achievement among children involved with the child welfare system as they progress throughout their academic careers. Finally, it might be interesting to investigate whether the enduring effects of early childhood resilience observed during the school-age period persist throughout later developmental stages, such as adolescence and young adulthood.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.Y.; methodology, F.P. and J.L.; formal analysis, F.P.; writing—original draft preparation, S.Y., J.L.B., A.P. and F.P.; writing—review and editing, J.L.B., A.P., S.H., J.L., F.P. and S.Y.; supervision, S.H.; project administration, S.Y. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) of the National Institutes of Health, Grant # R03HD100603. S.Y. was supported by the National Institute of Drug Abuse through K01DA050778 (Yoon, PI). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of The Ohio State University (protocol ID: 2019B0246).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

This document includes data from the National Survey of Child and Adolescent Well-Being that was developed under contract with the Administration on Children, Youth, and Families, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (ACYF/DHHS). Restrictions apply to the availability of these data. The data were provided by the National Data Archive on Child Abuse and Neglect (NDACAN).

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Natasha Slesnick of The Ohio State University for her valuable comments and advice.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Luthar, S.S.; Cicchetti, D.; Becker, B. The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Dev. 2000, 71, 543–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Yoon, S.; Pei, F.; Logan, J.; Helsabeck, N.; Hamby, S.; Slesnick, N. Early childhood maltreatment and profiles of resilience among child welfare-involved children. Dev. Psychopathol. 2022, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Lansford, J.E.; Malone, P.S.; Stevens, K.I.; Dodge, K.A.; Bates, J.E.; Pettit, G.S. Developmental trajectories of externalizing and internalizing behaviors: Factors underlying resilience in physically abused children. Dev. Psychopathol. 2006, 18, 35–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Sattler, K.M.; Font, S.A. Resilience in young children involved with child protective services. Child Abus. Negl. 2018, 75, 104–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Farnia, V.; Salemi, S.; Mordinazar, M.; Khanegi, M.; Tatari, F.; Golshani, S.; Jamshidi, P.; Alikhani, M. The effect of child-abuse on the behavioral problems in the children of the parents with substance use disorder: Presenting a model of structural equations. J. Ethn. Subst. Abus. 2020, 21, 730–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Garbarino, J.; Plantz, M.C. Child abuse and juvenile delinquency: What are the links? In Troubled Youth. Troubled Families; Routledge: London, UK, 2017; pp. 27–40. [Google Scholar]
  7. Halpern, S.C.; Schuch, F.B.; Scherer, J.N.; Sordi, A.O.; Pachado, M.; Dalbosco, C.; Fara, L.; Pechansky, F.; Kessler, F.; Von Diemen, L. Child maltreatment and illicit substance abuse: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Child Abus. Rev. 2018, 27, 344–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Filippelli, J.; Fallon, B.; Truelsen, S.; Carradine, J. An Initial Evaluation of a Resource Parent Training Curriculum for Child Welfare-Involved Children Who Have Experienced Trauma and Loss. Adopt. Q. 2022, 25, 27–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Walsh, W.A.; Dawson, J.; Mattingly, M.J. How are we measuring resilience following childhood maltreatment? Is the research adequate and consistent? What is the impact on research, practice, and policy? Trauma Violence Abus. 2010, 11, 27–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Wekerle, C. Resilience in the context of child maltreatment: Connections to the practice of mandatory reporting. Child Abus. Negl. 2013, 37, 93–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Cicchetti, D.; Rogosch, F.A. Gene × Environment interaction and resilience: Effects of child maltreatment and serotonin, corticotropin releasing hormone, dopamine, and oxytocin genes. Dev. Psychopathol. 2012, 24, 411–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Dubowitz, H.; Thompson, R.; Proctor, L.; Metzger, R.; Black, M.M.; English, D.; Poole, G.; Magder, L. Adversity, maltreatment, and resilience in young children. Acad. Pediatr. 2016, 16, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Flores, E.; Cicchetti, D.; Rogosch, F.A. Predictors of resilience in maltreated and nonmaltreated Latino children. Dev. Psychol. 2005, 41, 338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Ungar, M. Resilience after maltreatment: The importance of social services as facilitators of positive adaptation. Child Abus. Negl. 2013, 37, 110–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Yoon, S.; Helsabeck, N.; Wang, X.; Logan, J.; Pei, F.; Hamby, S.; Slesnick, N. Profiles of resilience among children exposed to non-maltreatment adverse childhood experiences. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 10600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Afifi, T.O.; MacMillan, H.L. Resilience following child maltreatment: A review of protective factors. Can. J. Psychiatry 2011, 56, 266–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Meng, X.; Fleury, M.-J.; Xiang, Y.-T.; Li, M.; D’arcy, C. Resilience and protective factors among people with a history of child maltreatment: A systematic review. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2018, 53, 453–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Edmond, T.; Auslander, W.; Elze, D.; Bowland, S. Signs of resilience in sexually abused adolescent girls in the foster care system. J. Child Sex. Abus. 2006, 15, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Tlapek, S.M.; Auslander, W.; Edmond, T.; Gerke, D.; Schrag, R.V.; Threlfall, J. The moderating role of resiliency on the negative effects of childhood abuse for adolescent girls involved in child welfare. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2017, 73, 437–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Cicchetti, D. Resilience under conditions of extreme stress: A multilevel perspective. World Psychiatry 2010, 9, 145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Stine-Morrow, E.A.; Chui, H. Cognitive resilience in adulthood. Annu. Rev. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2012, 32, 93–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Domhardt, M.; Münzer, A.; Fegert, J.M.; Goldbeck, L. Resilience in survivors of child sexual abuse: A systematic review of the literature. Trauma Violence Abus. 2015, 16, 476–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Muthén, B.; Muthén, L.K. Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: Growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2000, 24, 882–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Yates, T.M.; Grey, I.K. Adapting to aging out: Profiles of risk and resilience among emancipated foster youth. Dev. Psychopathol. 2012, 24, 475–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Martinez-Torteya, C.; Miller-Graff, L.E.; Howell, K.H.; Figge, C. Profiles of adaptation among child victims of suspected maltreatment. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2017, 46, 840–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. McDonald, S.E.; Graham-Bermann, S.A.; Maternick, A.; Ascione, F.R.; Williams, J.H. Patterns of adjustment among children exposed to intimate partner violence: A person-centered approach. J. Child Adolesc. Trauma. 2016, 9, 137–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Bjerk, D. Re-examining the impact of dropping out on criminal and labor outcomes in early adulthood. Econ. Educ. Rev. 2012, 31, 110–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Henry, K.L.; Knight, K.E.; Thornberry, T.P. School disengagement as a predictor of dropout, delinquency, and problem substance use during adolescence and early adulthood. J. Youth Adolesc. 2012, 41, 156–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Pietarinen, J.; Soini, T.; Pyhältö, K. Students’ emotional and cognitive engagement as the determinants of well-being and achievement in school. Int. J. Educ. Res. 2014, 67, 40–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Winefield, A.H.; Tiggemann, M. Employment status and psychological well-being: A longitudinal study. J. Appl. Psychol. 1990, 75, 455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Yang, Q.; Tian, L.; Huebner, E.S.; Zhu, X. Relations among academic achievement, self-esteem, and subjective well-being in school among elementary school students: A longitudinal mediation model. Sch. Psychol. 2019, 34, 328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Gartland, D.; Riggs, E.; Muyeen, S.; Giallo, R.; Afifi, T.O.; MacMillan, H.; Herrman, H.; Bulford, E.; Brown, S.J. What factors are associated with resilient outcomes in children exposed to social adversity? A systematic review. BMJ Open 2019, 9, e024870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Bethell, C.D.; Newacheck, P.; Hawes, E.; Halfon, N. Adverse childhood experiences: Assessing the impact on health and school engagement and the mitigating role of resilience. Health Aff. 2014, 33, 2106–2115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Khambati, N.; Mahedy, L.; Heron, J.; Emond, A. Educational and emotional health outcomes in adolescence following maltreatment in early childhood: A population-based study of protective factors. Child Abus. Negl. 2018, 81, 343–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Leiter, J. School performance trajectories after the advent of reported maltreatment. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 2007, 29, 363–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sesma, A.; Mannes, M.; Scales, P.C. Positive adaptation, resilience and the developmental assets framework. In Handbook of Resilience in Children; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2013; pp. 427–442. [Google Scholar]
  37. McGuire, A.; Jackson, Y. A multilevel meta-analysis on academic achievement among maltreated youth. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 21, 450–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Haskett, M.E.; Nears, K.; Ward, C.S.; McPherson, A.V. Diversity in adjustment of maltreated children: Factors associated with resilient functioning. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 26, 796–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Sattler, K.; Gershoff, E. Thresholds of resilience and within-and cross-domain academic achievement among children in poverty. Early Child. Res. Q. 2019, 46, 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Dowd, K.; Dolan, M.; Smith, K.; Day, O.; Keeney, J.; Wheeless, S.; Biemer, P. National Survey of Child and Adolescent Well-Being (NSCAW II): Combined Waves 1–3 Restricted Release Version Data File User’s Manual; National Data Archive on Child Abuse and Neglect, Cornell University: Ithaca, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  41. Haft, S.L.; Myers, C.A.; Hoeft, F. Socio-emotional and cognitive resilience in children with reading disabilities. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2016, 10, 133–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Zimmerman, B.J.; Bandura, A.; Martinez-Pons, M. Self-motivation for academic attainment: The role of self-efficacy beliefs and personal goal setting. Am. Educ. Res. J. 1992, 29, 663–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Qi, C.H.; Marley, S.C. Validity study of the preschool language Scale-4 with English-speaking hispanic and european american children in head start programs. Top. Early Child. Spec. Educ. 2011, 31, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Gresham, F.M.; Elliott, S.N. Social Skills Rating System (SSRS); American Guidance Service: Washington, DC, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  45. Sparrow, S.S.; Carter, A.S.; Cicchetti, D.V. Vineland Screener: Overview, Reliability, Validity, Administration and Scoring; Yale University Child Study Center: New Haven, CT, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  46. Cage, J.; Coles, D.C.; McGinnis, H.A.; Yoon, S.; Pitts, B.E. Validating the factor structure of the drug free schools and communities scale for child welfare involved youth. J. Public Child Welf. 2022, 16, 95–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Bakk, Z.; Tekle, F.B.; Vermunt, J.K. Estimating the association between latent class membership and external variables using bias-adjusted three-step approaches. Sociol. Methodol. 2013, 43, 272–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Lanza, S.T.; Tan, X.; Bray, B.C. Latent class analysis with distal outcomes: A flexible model-based approach. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2013, 20, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Nylund-Gibson, K.; Grimm, R.P.; Masyn, K.E. Prediction from latent classes: A demonstration of different approaches to include distal outcomes in mixture models. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2019, 26, 967–985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Vermunt, J.K. Latent class modeling with covariates: Two improved three-step approaches. Polit. Anal. 2010, 18, 450–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Released, I.C. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0; IBM Corp: Armonk, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  52. Welsh, J.A.; Nix, R.L.; Blair, C.; Bierman, K.L.; Nelson, K.E. The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. J. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 102, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Coohey, C.; Renner, L.M.; Hua, L.; Zhang, Y.J.; Whitney, S.D. Academic achievement despite child maltreatment: A longitudinal study. Child Abus. Negl. 2011, 35, 688–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Font, S.; Maguire-Jack, K. Academic engagement and performance: Estimating the impact of out-of-home care for maltreated children. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 2013, 35, 856–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Merritt, D.H.; Snyder, S.M. Correlates of optimal behavior among child welfare-involved children: Perceived school peer connectedness, activity participation, social skills, and peer affiliation. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 2015, 85, 483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Mihalec-Adkins, B.P.; Christ, S.L.; Day, E. An exploration of placement-related psychosocial influences on school engagement among adolescents in foster care. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 2020, 108, 104616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Snyder, S.M.; Gwaltney, A.Y.; Landeck, E. What social bonds have the greatest influence on patterns of substance use among child-welfare-involved youth? J. Psychoact. Drugs 2015, 47, 308–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Bryce, C.I.; Goble, P.; Swanson, J.; Fabes, R.A.; Hanish, L.D.; Martin, C.L. Kindergarten school engagement: Linking early temperament and academic achievement at the transition to school. Early Educ. Dev. 2018, 29, 780–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Diaz, A.; Eisenberg, N.; Valiente, C.; VanSchyndel, S.; Spinrad, T.L.; Berger, R.; Hernandez, M.M.; Silva, K.M.; Southworth, J. Relations of positive and negative expressivity and effortful control to kindergarteners’ student–teacher relationship, academic engagement, and externalizing problems at school. J. Res. Pers. 2017, 67, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Valiente, C.; Swanson, J.; Lemery-Chalfant, K. Kindergartners’ temperament, classroom engagement, and student–teacher relationship: Moderation by effortful control. Soc. Dev. 2012, 21, 558–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Furrer, C.; Skinner, E. Sense of relatedness as a factor in children’s academic engagement and performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 95, 148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Hosan, N.E.; Hoglund, W. Do Teacher–Child Relationship and Friendship Quality Matter for Children’s School Engagement and Academic Skills? Sch. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 46, 201–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Archambault, I.; Kurdi, V.; Olivier, E.; Goulet, M. The joint effect of peer victimization and conflict with teachers on student engagement at the end of elementary school. Merrill Palmer. Q. 2016, 62, 207–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Chow, J.C.; Wehby, J.H. Associations between language and problem behavior: A systematic review and correlational meta-analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 30, 61–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Yew, S.G.K.; O’Kearney, R. Emotional and behavioural outcomes later in childhood and adolescence for children with specific language impairments: Meta-analyses of controlled prospective studies. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2013, 54, 516–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Morgan, P.L.; Farkas, G.; Tufis, P.A.; Sperling, R.A. Are reading and behavior problems risk factors for each other? J. Learn. Disabil. 2008, 41, 417–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Rimm-Kaufman, S.E.; Pianta, R.C. An ecological perspective on the transition to kindergarten: A theoretical framework to guide empirical research. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2000, 21, 491–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Whitehurst, G.J.; Lonigan, C.J. Child development and emergent literacy. Child Dev. 1998, 69, 848–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Cohen, J.A.; Mannarino, A.P.; Berliner, L.; Deblinger, E. Trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy for children and adolescents: An empirical update. J. Interpers. Violence 2000, 15, 1202–1223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Skinner, E.A.; Pitzer, J.R.; Steele, J.S. Can student engagement serve as a motivational resource for academic coping, persistence, and learning during late elementary and early middle school? Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 2099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Hamby, S.; Grych, J.; Banyard, V. Resilience portfolios and poly-strengths: Identifying protective factors associated with thriving after adversity. Psychol. Violence 2018, 8, 172–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Radar Chart of Early Childhood Resilience Profiles. Note. Adapted from [2]. z-scores (M = 0, SD = 1) of the resilience indicators were used for the radar chart for interpretability purposes.
Figure 1. Radar Chart of Early Childhood Resilience Profiles. Note. Adapted from [2]. z-scores (M = 0, SD = 1) of the resilience indicators were used for the radar chart for interpretability purposes.
Ijerph 19 05987 g001
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Key Study Variables (N = 827).
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Key Study Variables (N = 827).
%M (SD)
Age at baseline (in years) 3.96 (0.82)
Sex (girls)46.1%
Race/Ethnicity
White; Non-Hispanic39.6%
Black; Non-Hispanic31.4%
Hispanic24.0%
Other5.0%
Caregiver’s education (less than high school)25.8%
Household income ≤ 200% poverty level78.1%
Academic Performance
Letter–Word Identification standard score 98.81 (16.22)
Passage Comprehension standard score 91.21 (14.54)
Applied Problems standard score 97.00 (15.36)
School Engagement
Emotional engagement 9.04 (2.29)
Behavioral engagement 20.25 (3.23)
Note. Adapted from [2]. Other race included American Indian, Asian, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, and Multiple race categories.
Table 2. Distal Mean Differences between Four Latent Classes.
Table 2. Distal Mean Differences between Four Latent Classes.
School Distal OutcomeClassDistal MeanLow Cognitive Resilience Multi-Domain Resilience
Letter–Word IdentificationLow emotional, behavioral resilience121.6810.81 ***−2.37
Low cognitive resilience110.87-−13.18 ***
Multi-domain resilience124.06--
Passage ComprehensionLow emotional, behavioral resilience104.7710.13 ***−2.47
Low cognitive resilience94.64-−12.60 ***
Multi-domain resilience107.23--
Applied ProblemsLow emotional, behavioral resilience102.6012.16 ***−2.66
Low cognitive resilience90.44-−14.82 ***
Multi-domain resilience105.26--
Emotional EngagementLow emotional, behavioral resilience7.07−6.66−0.80 *
Low cognitive resilience7.74-−0.13
Multi-domain resilience7.87--
Behavioral EngagementLow emotional, behavioral resilience16.280.40−0.77
Low cognitive resilience15.88-−1.17 *
Multi-domain resilience17.05--
Note. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. The Effects of the Covariates on the Distal Outcomes.
Table 3. The Effects of the Covariates on the Distal Outcomes.
Letter–Word
Identification
Passage
Comprehension
Applied ProblemsEmotional
Engagement
Behavioral
Engagement
EstimateS.E.EstimateS.E.EstimateS.E.EstimateS.E.EstimateS.E.
Child sex (girls)0.43 **0.130.38 **0.120.160.120.310.230.88 **0.31
Child age at baseline−0.56 ***0.09−0.32 ***0.09−0.160.090.250.150.43 *0.21
Child race/ethnicity
White/Non-Hispanic−0.110.16−0.140.140.010.14−0.140.270.420.37
Hispanic−0.200.18−0.240.180.000.170.230.290.460.42
Other a−0.230.26−0.020.23−0.110.23−0.240.400.690.58
Parent education b0.190.150.160.140.260.14−0.050.240.540.36
Poverty c−0.28 *0.14−0.26 *0.12−0.190.130.090.23−0.200.3
Child neglect−0.33 *0.14−0.28 *0.120.020.13−0.160.23−0.200.32
Child abuse−0.42 **0.15−0.40 **0.13−0.28 *0.13−0.180.23−0.230.34
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001; a other race included American Indian, Asian, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, and Multiple race categories; b parental education more than high school; c household income below the federal poverty level.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yoon, S.; Pei, F.; Benavides, J.L.; Ploss, A.; Logan, J.; Hamby, S. The Long-Term Effects of Early Childhood Resilience Profiles on School Outcomes among Children in the Child Welfare System. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 5987. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19105987

AMA Style

Yoon S, Pei F, Benavides JL, Ploss A, Logan J, Hamby S. The Long-Term Effects of Early Childhood Resilience Profiles on School Outcomes among Children in the Child Welfare System. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(10):5987. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19105987

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yoon, Susan, Fei Pei, Juan Lorenzo Benavides, Alexa Ploss, Jessica Logan, and Sherry Hamby. 2022. "The Long-Term Effects of Early Childhood Resilience Profiles on School Outcomes among Children in the Child Welfare System" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 10: 5987. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19105987

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop