The Effectiveness of Hearing Protection Devices: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Strategy for Systematic Search
2.2. Article Selection
2.3. Study Quality and Potential Sources of Study Bias
2.4. Meta-Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Study Quality Scores
3.2. Charactersits of Studies
3.2.1. Participants
3.2.2. Intervention
3.2.3. Controls and Study Designs
3.2.4. Outcomes
3.3. Overall Functions and Effects of Hearing Protection Devices
3.4. Subgroup Analysis
4. Discussion
4.1. Do HPDs Effectively Attenuate the Level of Intensity of Unwanted Noise?
4.2. Does Wearing HPDs Affect Sound Localization Ability for Their Users?
4.3. Does Wearing an HPD Affect a User’s Speech Perception Ability?
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Chung, I.S.; Chu, I.M.; Cullen, M.R. Hearing effects from intermittent and continuous noise exposure in a study of Korean factory workers and firefighters. BMC Public Health 2012, 12, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- MacGregor, A.J.; Joseph, A.R.; Walker, G.J.; Dougherty, A.L. Co-occurrence of hearing loss and posttraumatic stress disorder among injured military personnel: A retrospective study. BMC Public Health 2020, 20, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smalt, C.J.; Calamia, P.T.; Dumas, A.P.; Perricone, J.P.; Patel, T.; Bobrow, J.; Collins, P.P.; Markey, M.L.; Quatieri, T.F. The Effect of Hearing-Protection Devices on Auditory Situational Awareness and Listening Effort. Ear Hear. 2020, 41, 82–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, A.D.; Beemer, B.T.; Greene, N.T.; Argo IV, T.; Meegan, G.D.; Tollin, D.J. Effects of active and passive hearing protection devices on sound source localization, speech recognition, and tone detection. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0136568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Abel, S.M.; Sass-Kortsak, A.; Kielar, A. The effect on earmuff attenuation of other safety gear worn in combination. Noise Health 2002, 5, 1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Song, H.; Jeong, S.; Lee, E.; Alsabbagh, N.; Lee, J.; You, S.; Kwak, C.; Kim, S.; Han, W. Types of hearing protection devices and application. Korean J. Otorhinolaryngol.-Head Neck Surg. 2019, 62, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tufts, J.B.; Jahn, K.N.; Byram, J.P. Consistency of attenuation across multiple fittings of custom and non-custom earplugs. Ann. Occup. Hyg. 2013, 57, 571–580. [Google Scholar]
- Abel, S.M.; Paik, J.S. Sound source identification with ANR earmuffs. Noise Health 2005, 7, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carmichel, E.L.; Harris, F.P.; Story, B.H. Effects of binaural electronic hearing protectors on localization and response time to sounds in the horizontal plane. Noise Health 2007, 9, 83–95. [Google Scholar]
- Talcott, K.A.; Casali, J.G.; Keady, J.P.; Killion, M.C. Azimuthal auditory localization of gunshots in a realistic field environment: Effects of open-ear versus hearing protection-enhancement devices (HPEDs), military vehicle noise, and hearing impairment. Int. J. Audiol. 2012, 51, S20–S30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Byrne, D.C.; Palmer, C.V. Comparison of speech intelligibility measures for an electronic amplifying earmuff and an identical passive attenuation device. Audiol. Res. 2012, 2, 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dastpaak, H.; Alimohammadi, I.; jalal Sameni, S.; Abolghasemi, J.; Vosoughi, S. Effects of earplug hearing protectors on the intelligibility of Persian words in noisy environments. Appl. Acoust. 2019, 148, 19–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tufts, J.B.; Frank, T. Speech production in noise with and without hearing protection. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2003, 114, 1069–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic revies. BMJ 2021, 372, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
- PROSPERO, Centre for Reviews and Dissemination. Title of subordinate document. International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews. Centre for Reviews and Dissemination. 2014. Available online: http://cdn.elsevier.com/promis_misc/PROSPEROAnimal.pdf (accessed on 5 April 2021).
- Macleod, M.R.; O’Collins, T.; Horky, L.L.; Howells, D.W.; Donnan, G.A. Systematic review and metaanalysis of the efficacy of FK506 in experimental stroke. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 2005, 25, 713–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Group, G.W. Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ 2004, 328, 1490–1494. [Google Scholar]
- Alali, K.A.; Casali, J.G. The challenge of localizing vehicle backup alarms: Effects of passive and electronic hearing protectors, ambient noise level, and backup alarm spectral content. Noise Health 2011, 13, 99–112. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Plyler, P.N.; Klumpp, M.L. Communication in noise with acoustic and electronic hearing protection devices. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 2003, 14, 260–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abel, S.M.; Lam, Q. Sound attenuation of the indoor/outdoor range EAR plug. Mil. Med. 2004, 169, 551–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Casali, J.G.; Robinson, G.S.; Dabney, E.C.; Gauger, D. Effect of electronic ANR and conventional hearing protectors on vehicle backup alarm detection in noise. Hum. Factors 2004, 46, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Faria, C.A.R.; Suzuki, F.A. Pure tone audiometry with and without specific ear protectors. Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 2008, 74, 417–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bolia, R.S.; D’Angelo, W.R.; Mishler, P.J.; Morris, L.J. Effects of hearing protectors on auditory localization in azimuth and elevation. Hum. Factors 2001, 43, 122–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simpson, B.D.; Bolia, R.S.; McKinley, R.L.; Brungart, D.S. The impact of hearing protection on sound localization and orienting behavior. Hum. Factors 2005, 47, 188–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zimpfer, V.; Sarafian, D. Impact of hearing protection devices on sound localization performance. Front. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Giguère, C.; Laroche, C.; Vaillancourt, V. The interaction of hearing loss and level-dependent hearing protection on speech recognition in noise. Int. J. Audiol. 2015, 54, S9–S18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Manning, C.; Mermagen, T.; Scharine, A. The effect of sensorineural hearing loss and tinnitus on speech recognition over air and bone conduction military communications headsets. Hear. Res. 2017, 349, 67–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgins, J.P.; Thompson, S.G.; Deeks, J.J.; Altman, D.G. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analysis. BMJ 2003, 327, 557–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Simes, R.J. Confronting publication bias: A cohort design for meta-analysis. Stat. Med. 1987, 6, 11–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- The R Foundation. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. The R Foundation. 2018. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 7 April 2021).
- Henshaw, H.; Ferguson, M.A. Efficacy of individual computer-based auditory training for people with hearing loss: A systematic review of the evidence. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e62836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rawool, V.W. Conservation and management of hearing loss in musicians. In Hearing Conservation in Occupational, Recreational, Educational, and Home Settings, 1st ed.; Rawool, V.W., Ed.; Thieme: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 201–223. [Google Scholar]
Parameter | Inclusion Criteria |
---|---|
Participants | Adults 18 years or older with and without hearing loss |
Intervention | Functions and effects of hearing protection devices (i.e., comparison of wearing or not wearing hearing protection devices or a comparison of the types of hearing protection devices) |
Control | Comparison to control group or repeated measures (experiments with additional purposes) |
Outcomes | Outcome measure(s) related to functions and/or effects of hearing protection devices (i.e., sound attenuation, sound localization, and speech perception) |
Study Designs | Randomized controlled trials, non-randomized controlled trials, and repeated measures (experiments with additional purposes) |
Study | Scientific Study Validity Criteria | Study Quality Score | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Randomization | Controls | Sample Size Calculation | Publication after Peer Review | Outcome Measure | Statement of Potential Conflict of Interest | ||
Smalt et al. (2019) [3] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 4 |
Brown et al. (2015) [4] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 4 |
Abel et al. (2002) [5] | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 4 |
Tufts et al. (2012) [7] | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 2 |
Abel and Paik (2005) [8] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 4 |
Carmichel et al. (2007) [9] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 4 |
Talcott et al. (2012) [10] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 4 |
Byrne and Palmer (2012) [11] | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 3 |
Dastpaak et al. (2019) [12] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 4 |
Tufts and Frank (2003) [13] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 3 |
Alali and Casali (2011) [18] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 4 |
Plyler and Klumpp (2003) [19] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 3 |
Abel and Lam (2004) [20] | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 2 |
Casali et al. (2004) [21] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 3 |
de Faria and Suzuki (2008) [22] | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 2 |
Bolia et al. (2001) [23] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 3 |
Simpson et al. (2005) [24] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 3 |
Zimpfer and Sarafian (2014) [25] | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 4 |
Giguère et al. (2015) [26] | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 5 |
Manning et al. (2016) [27] | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 4 |
Study | Participants | Test Materials and Conditions | Study Design | Major Function | Outcome Measures | Main Findings | Effects of HPD |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Abel et al. (2002) [5] | Twenty-four young adults with normal hearing (aged under 40 years), twenty-four older adults with normal hearing (aged over 40 years), and twenty-four older adults with hearing loss | A total of five ear conditions were adjusted: (1) ears under unoccluded conditions, (2) ears with Class A muffs, (3) ears with muffs on hard hat and air-purifying half-mask respirators, (4) ears with muffs on hard hat and safety glasses, and (5) ears with muffs on hard hat, safety glasses, and respirators. Attenuation measurements were conducted for eight one-third octave noise bands centered from 0.25 to 8 kHz. | Repeated measures | Attenuation | Amount of attenuation (dB SPL) | The results of ANOVA with repeated measures showed significant effects for group [F(5,61) = 4.3, p < 0.002], protector condition [F(3,183) = 104.2, p < 0.0001], and frequency [F(7,427) = 387.7, p < 0.0001]. The interaction of the protector condition by group [F(35,427) = 1.8, p< 0.006], protector condition by group [F(21,1281) = 12.2, p < 0.0001], protector condition by frequency by group [F(105,1281) = 6.8, p < 0.04]. The interaction with protector condition by group was not significant. | Averaged across protector conditions, attenuation showed significant increases at frequency increased from 0.25 to 1 kHz and then remained constant, except for a dip at 6.3 kHz. Averaged across groups and frequencies, the least attenuation was achieved with the muff on hard hat in combination with the glasses and respirator. The muff on hard hat alone condition showed highest attenuation. The range in attenuation in order to the conditions was greater at 0.25 and 0.5 kHz (9 dB) and at its lowest 2 and 3.15 kHz (3–4 dB). |
Abel and Lam (2004) [20] | Sixteen adults ages from 21 to 53 years (8 men and 8 women) with normal hearing | The Indoor and Outdoor E-A-R plugs manufactured by the Aero Company were used. The hearing thresholds were measured on nine one-third-octave noise bands (i.e., 0.125, 0,25, 0,5, 1, 2, 3.15, 4, 6.3, and 8 kHz) for three conditions (i.e., unoccluded, indoor plug, and outdoor plug) | Repeated measures | Attenuation | Amount of attenuation (dB). | The results of ANOVA with repeated measures showed significant effects of ear condition, frequency and ear by frequency (p < 0.0001). Post hoc comparison showed that the differences in attenuation for the two devices was significant for all testing frequencies (p < 0.001 or better). | The attenuation values in the indoor plug increased from 21 dB at 0.125 kHz to 40 dB at 8 kHz. Additionally, the results for the outdoor plug attenuated the thresholds by 5 and 14 dB at 0.5 kHz and 1 kHz. |
Abel and Paik (2005) [8] | Twelve young adults (age range—18 to 30 years) and twelve older adults (age range—40 to 55 years) with normal hearing | Three ear conditions (i.e., with and without ANR earmuff operational and unoccluded ear) were used. A total of eight speakers formed eight azimuth angles (i.e., 15, 75, 105, 165, 195, 255, 285, and 345 degrees). For the sound source identification task, three different signals, such as 0.5 kHz, 4 kHz, and broadband noise, were presented. | Repeated measures | Sound localization | Response time (ms) for sound source identification | An ANOVA indicated that there were significant effects for ear condition [F(2,40) = 5.6, p < 0.007] and stimulus frequency [F(2,40) = 9.0, p < 0.001]. | Averaged across the four groups and three stimuli, the mean median response times ranged from 712 ms for ANR On to 805 ms in the unoccluded condition. Averaged across the four groups and three ear conditions, the mean median response times ranged from 667 ms for the broadband noise to 803 ms for the 0.5 kHz stimulus. |
Alali and Casali (2011) [18] | Twelve adults (older than 18 years) with normal hearing | Within-subject design (8 HPDs × 2 signals × 2 presentation levels) were adjusted. The HPDs consisted of: (1) unoccluded, (2) foam earplug, (3) pre-molded earplug, (4) flat attenuation HPD, (5) level-dependent HPD, (6) passive earmuff, (7) dichotic sound transmission earmuff, and (8) custom-made diotic sound transmission earmuff. The signals consisted of a standard backup alarm and spectrally modified one. Presentation levels of pink noise were both 60 dBA and 90 dBA. | Repeated measures | Sound localization | Percentage correct (%) of sound identification. | The ANOVA revealed the significant main effects of HPD for a percentage correct localization [F(7,77) = 59.2, p < 0.0001], percentage of right–left localization errors [F(7,77) = 42.78, p < 0.0001], percentage of front-rear localization errors [F(7,77) = 43.03, p < 0.0001], and localization absolute deviation in degrees [F(7,77) = 92.67, p < 0.0001]. The interaction between HPD and background noise level on percentage correct localization response was also significant [F(7,77) = 9.51, p < 0.0001]. | The mean values of percentage correctness localization showed that flat attenuation HPD (83.9%) was the most correct in terms of percentage. Unoccluded (82.2%), pre-molded earplug (81.5%), and passive earmuff followed, but were not significant. The custom-made diotic sound transmission earmuff (66.3%) and foam earplug (64.6%) showed similar percentages of correctness. The dichotic sound transmission earmuff (15.8%) showed the lowest percentage correctness for localization. |
Bolia et al. 2001 [23] | Six adults (aged 18 to 34 years) with normal hearing | Three hearing protector conditions (i.e., earplugs, earmuffs, and unoccluded ear) were adjusted factorially with three source elevation conditions (upper hemisphere, peri-horizontal region, and lower hemisphere) for a total of nine experimental conditions. | Repeated measures | Sound localization | Azimuth error (°) | Mean azimuth errors were analyzed using a 3 (hearing protector) × 3 (source elevation) repeated-measures ANOVA, employing the Huynh–Feldt correction, which revealed significant main effects of hearing protector, [F(2,10) = 29.94, p < 0.05], and source elevation, [F(2,10) = 19.96, p < 0.05], and a Hearing Protector × Source Elevation interaction, [F(4,20) = 10.86, p < 0.05]. | All simple main effects of the hearing protector factor were statistically significant (p < 0.01), implying that at one or more levels of the source elevation factor, performance varies as a function of the HPD factor. |
Brown et al. (2015) [4] | Ten normal hearing adults (mean age: 29.5 years) | Four HPDs and one control (unoccluded) were used in the experiment: (1) passive HPD, (2) active HPD, (3) hybrid HPD, and (4) ShotShields HPD. | Repeated measures | Sound localization | Response angle (°) | The ANOVA indicated a significant main effect of device condition [F(4,36) = 13.64, p < 0.001] and target angle [F(11,99) = 5.67, p < 0.001]. A significant interaction between the device and target angle [F(44,396) = 5.67, p < 0.001] was revealed. | All tested HPDs significantly degraded localization performance relative to control (unoccluded) condition. However, post hoc analysis of HPD-related distortions of sound source localization cues demonstrated that the ShotShields produced both the best behavioral performance and the greatest preservation of sound source cues. |
Byrne and Palmer (2012) [11] | Fifteen adults with normal hearing (age range—21 to 60 years) | In the four testing conditions, two types of HPDs were used, an electronic hearing protector and conventional passive earmuff. The electronic hearing protector had three different settings (i.e., off, low, and high). | Repeated measures | Speech intelligibility | Correct percentage of HINT test score (%) | The results of repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant main effect [F(2,28) = 1014.50, p < 0.0001] for the SNR condition. The highest scores were obtained under the +5 dB SNR condition, while the lowest scores were obtained under the −5 dB SNR condition. A significant main effect was also found for the earmuff condition [F(3,42) = 57.19, p < 0.0001]. Additionally, the interaction effect was significant [F(6,84) = 6.94, p < 0.0001]. | For the types of HPDs, passive muff showed the highest correctness percentage for the all SNR conditions. While the off and low settings of electronic hearing protector showed similar correctness percentages, and the high setting of hearing protector showed the lowest correctness percentage, regardless of the SNRs. |
Carmichel et al. (2007) [9] | Eight normal hearing listeners (aged 20 to 40 years) | Four stimuli (i.e., two successive transient clicks of a semiautomatic handgun being loaded, the double ring of a telephone with a mechanical ringer, and electronically generated FM tone bursts at 0.5 and 4 kHz) and four HPDs (i.e., electronic earmuffs, dynamic level compression protectors, action ear sport, and unoccluded) were used. | Repeated measures | Sound localization | Localization performance (%) and response time (sec) | Differences in localization among the conditions were significant for the firearm [F(3,21) = 15.30, p < 0.05], telephone [F(3,21) = 15.40, p < 0.05], 4 kHz tone burst [F(3,21) = 1.13, p < 0.05] stimuli, but not for the 0.5 kHz tone burst [F(3,21) = 1.13, p = 0.36]. The mean response time showed that there were differences in the conditions for the broadband stimuli (firearm, [F(3,21) = 6.15, p < 0.05], telephone [F(3,21) = 5.21, p < 0.05]. | The mean correct responses for localization to broadband stimuli in the unoccluded condition were 97.2% (firearm) and 98.6% (telephone). Mean scores for localization to these stimuli ranged from 67.4 to 54.2% correctness in the HPD conditions when averaged across all locations. When averaged across all stimuli and locations, the mean response time for correct responses in the unoccluded condition (mean: 1.58s, SD: 0.76) was not different from the mean response time to errors (mean: 1.64s, SD: 0.81). In the HPD conditions, the mean response time to incorrect response was less (mean: 1.88 s, SD: 0.89) than to correct responses (mean: 1.90 s, SD: 0.81) but these differences were not significant. |
Casali et al. (2004) [21] | Ten adults with normal hearing, wuth age ranging from 18 to 49 years | The signal stimulus was a standard vehicle back-up alarm at a presentation level of 85 and 100 dBA. The experimental conditions consisted of four different ear conditions (i.e., earplug, passive earmuff, ANR earmuff, and unoccluded ear). | Repeated measures | Attenuation | Mean masked thresholds (dBA) | In the comparisons of HPD analyzed by ANOVA with repeated measures, interactions including ear condition x noise level [F(2,18) = 19.38, p = 0.0001], ear condition x noise spectrum [F(2,18) = 8.51, p = 0.0053] were significant. In comparisons of HPDs and an unoccluded condition, there were no statistically significant interactions for ear condition x noise spectrum. However, the main effects of the ear condition at 85 dBA [F(3,27) = 7.91, p = 0.0006] were significant. | Given 100 dBA noise, earplugs showed significantly lower masked thresholds (91.9 dBA) than either the ANR muff (93.8 dBA) or the passive earmuff (95.5 dBA). Additionally, the ANR muff produced significantly lower masked thresholds than the passive muff did. |
Dastpaak et al. (2019) [12] | Thirty-two adults (mean age: 26.12 years) with normal hearing | Three ear conditions (unoccluded, HPD with 25 NRR, and HPD with 32 NRR) and three SNR condition (−10, 0, and +10 dB) were used. | Repeated measures | Speech intelligibility | Averaged intelligibility score (%) | The results showed maximum speech intelligibility without HPDs and without noise and minimum average of speech intelligibility when the background noise was greater than the −10 dB SNR. | For the comparisons of types of HPDs, 32 of NRR HPD showed lower averaged intelligibility scores in all testing SNR conditions. While the without HPD condition showed 8.13% (SD: 9.11), 35.44% (SD: 19.11), and 74.94% (SD: 7.67) for three SNR conditions, HPD with 32 NRR showed 11.38% (SD: 9.64), 43.25% (SD: 17.54), and 78.06% (SD: 9.57). The HPD with 25 NRR showed highest average intelligibility score for −10 dB SNR (mean: 22.75%, SD: 12.04), 0 dB SNR (mean: 55.31%, SD: 14.41), and −10 dB SNR (mean: 78.38%, SD: 13.39). |
de Faria and Suzuki (2008) [22] | Thirty adults with normal hearing (age range—20 to 58 years) | Two HPDs with 21 and 17 dB NRR, respectively, were used to measure attenuation. | Repeated measures | Attenuation | Noise attenuation (decibels) | The mean noise attenuation for 60 ears at all frequencies was lower than the manufacturer’s rating. No ear achieved the expected attenuation, except at 500 Hz (35 dBNA). | The observed noise attenuation measurements at each frequency were: (1) 500 Hz (mean: 22.8 dB, SD: 3.8), (2) 1000 Hz (mean: 23.4 dB, SD: 3.7), (3) 2000 Hz (mean: 27.3 dB, SD: 4.5), and (4) 4000 Hz (mean: 29.4 dB, SD: 4.3). |
Giguère et al. (2015) [26] | A total of forty-five adults (age of 23 to 81 years) participated. There were four groups based on t hearing loss: (1) normal hearing (n = 12), (2) slight-to-mild (n = 12), (3) mild-to-moderate (n = 12), and (4) moderate/severe (n = 9) | Three ear conditions were used (unoccluded and two HPDs). In the HPDs, one was an analogue device, and the other was a digital device. Two noises (95.3 dBA and 89.5 dBA) were used. | Repeated measures | Speech intelligibility | Speech recognition (% word recognition) | In a comparison of protected versus unprotected speech recognition scores by hearing loss, use of the higher level-dependent function almost fully restored speech recognition to unprotected values or provided significant improvements over an unprotected performance for all participants. | The passive HPD showed a mean decrease in performance of 27–29% across the noises compared to the unoccluded condition. However, when the level-dependent function was activated, it yielded average overall benefits of 11–15% and 23–24% across the two noises, respectively. The active HPD showed a mean decrement in performance of 17–25% compared to unprotected listening across the two noises. Unlikely for the passive HPD, an active HPD with activation of level-dependent function showed only a small effect on performance and a mean benefit of 6–7%. |
Manning et al. (2016) [27] | A total of 47 adults (19 with normal hearing, 15 with SNHL, and 13 with SNHL and tinnitus). | A total of 300 items consisting of six lists were used. The background noise was presented in three different SNR conditions (i.e., quiet, −6 and −12 dB). Two types of TCAPS (i.e., in-ear headset for air-conduction, and headset for bone-conduction) and two types of talker (male and female) were used. | Repeated measures | Speech intelligibility | Mean rationalized arcsine units (corrected) | The mean rationalized arcsine units measured for each of the TCAPS under test were marginally, but significantly better for the bone-conduction headset [F(1,528) = 29.90, p < 0.01]. For the types of hearing loss, the effects of hearing loss were found to be statistically significant [F(2,528) = 3.74, p < 0.024]. There was no significant interaction of hearing loss with TCAPS [F(2,528) = 0.99, p = 0.372]. In the SNR conditions, performance increased, as the SNR increased [F(2,528) = 1881.47, p < 0.01]. | The normal hearing listeners performed best, and SNHL listeners performed worst. The degrading effect of noise on speech recognition was approximately the same for the normal hearing and hearing impaired listeners; there was no measurable benefit for either TCAPS in noise. |
Plyler and Klump (2003) [19] | Fourteen females (age 21 to 24 years) with normal hearing | A total of three ear conditions including unoccluded were used. In the HPDs, no HPDs which was custom acoustic HPD and custom electronic HPD were used. For the speech perception test, two levels of noise (75 and 90 dB SPL) were used. The HINT sentence lists were used as the stimuli. | Repeated measures | Speech intelligibility | Relative HINT score | The analysis revealed a significant main effect for HPD [F(1,13) = 10, p < 0.05]. However, main effects for sentence presentation level [F(1,13) = 1, p > 0.05] were significant. The interaction between sentence presentation level x HPD [F(1,13) = 2, p > 0.05] was not significant. These results indicated that communication during noise was significantly better when utilizing the acoustic HPD than when utilizing the electronic HPD at each sentence presentation level. | The averaged relative HINT scores indicated that the acoustic HPD showed 1.1 (SD: 1.9) relative HINT score which was higher than the electronic HPD (mean: −0.4, SD: 1.2) at 75 dB SPL. In the 90 dB SPL condition, acoustic HPD (mean: 0.1, SD: 1.1) also showed higher relative HINT score than that for the electronic HPD (mean: −0.6, SD: 1.3). |
Simpson et al. (2005) [24] | Seven adults (age 18 to 39 years) with normal hearing | Four ear conditions (i.e., no HPD (unoccluded), foam earplugs, earmuffs, combination of earplugs and earmuffs) and five auditory cue conditions (four HPDs condition and no cue condition) were used. Stimuli were visual targets with continuous broadband (70 Hz to 16 kHz) pink noise. | Repeated measures | Sound localization | Response accuracy (%) and search time (sec) | For response accuracy, repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that neither of the main effects nor the interaction was found to be statistically significant (p > 0.05). However, for search time, repeated ANOVA showed significant main effects on the auditory condition [F(4,24) = 239.11, p < 0.05] and set size [F(2,12) = 328.75, p < 0.05]. The interaction between auditory condition and set size [F(8,48) = 93.84, p < 0.05] was significant. | The unoccluded condition had the lowest search time, regardless of the set size. Earmuff and earplug conditions showed similar search times as a function of set size. The combination of earplugs and earmuffs had a higher search time than for other ear conditions. |
Smalt et al. (2019) [3] | Thirteen adults average age of 31 years with normal hearing, except for one subject which showed 30 dB HL in one ear | A total of four HPDs, two passive types and two active types, were used. For the speech intelligibility test, Modified Rhyme Test using three background noises (i.e., 60, 75, and 80 dBA) was used. | Repeated measures | Speech intelligibility | Percentage correct (%) | A two-way ANOVA demonstrated that there were significant main effects for HPD [F(4,48) = 3.716, p = 0.010] and noise level [F(2,24) = 1737, p < 2 × 10−16. However, the interaction between HPD and noise level was not significant. For the results of the post hoc test, only the HPD pair showed a significant difference between active HPDs (p = 0.017). | Speech intelligibility was somewhat reduced for all the hearing protectors, but the differences induced by increasing the background noise level were much greater than the differences among the HPDs for a single noise level. |
Talcott et al. (2012) [10] | Thirteen adults age 22 to 54 years with normal hearing | Five ear conditions (i.e., unoccluded, earplug with 21 dB NRR and gain switch, ear tips with 21 dB NRR, earplugs with 7 dB NRR, earmuff with 21 dB NRR) and two noise conditions (i.e., 45–50 dBA ambient noise and 82 dBA diesel truck noise) were used. | Repeated measures | Sound localization | Percentage correctness response (%) and mean response time (sec) | The ANOVA showed a significant main effect of the listening condition (F = 17.22, p < 0.0001). For the mean response time, the results of ANOVA showed that there was a significant main effect of the listening condition (F = 11.11, p < 0.0001). | The percent of correct responses was significantly lower for the earmuff (21 ± 10%) than all listening conditions and significantly greater for the unoccluded ear (55 ± 17%) than all listening conditions. No other differences were significant between the HPD conditions. In addition, the mean response time was significantly higher for the earmuff condition (2.9 ± 1.4 s) than for all other HPDs and the unoccluded condition. |
Tufts et al. (2012) [7] | Thirty adults with normal hearing (mean age: 21 years, SD: 2.7) | Two types of HPDs were used (i.e., custom-molded and non-custom earplugs). For five measurements, the results of trained and untrained groups were compared. | Repeated measures | Attenuation | Mean per-frequency attenuation (dB) | Repeated-measures of ANOVA conducted on mean PAR showed the training to be statistically significant [F(1,29) = 11.77, p = 0.002], but neither earplug type [F(1,29) = 1,91, p = 0.178] nor the interaction of earplug type x training [F(1,29) = 0.29, p = 0.596] was statistically significant. | Mean per-frequency attenuation and mean PAR for each earplug type and training condition showed that the mean PAR of untrained non-custom plug produced 3.2 dB less attenuation than the trained non-custom plug condition. For the custom plug, the untrained group showed 4.1 dB less mean PAR than the trained group. For the frequency comparison, the trained group showed higher attenuation values than the untrained group did for both custom and non-custom plugs. |
Tufts and Frank (2003) [13] | Thirty-two adults (mean: 25.4 years, SD: 4.9) with normal hearing | Two types of HPDs (i.e., custom-molded foam ear plug and pre-molded earplug) were used. As stimuli, the 12 passages from the 20 passages of the Speech Intelligibility Rating test were chosen. | Repeated measures | Speech intelligibility | Signal-to-noise ratio (dB) | In the SNR analysis, the main effects of background noise [F(4,112) = 3041.646, p < 0.001] and ear condition [F(1,28) = 4965.188, p < 0.001] were significant. Further, the interactions of background noise x ear condition [F(4,112) = 60.568, p < 0.001] and background noise x ear condition x sex [F(4,112) = 2.776, p = 0.034] were significant. | For background noise at 100 dB SPL, the overall speech levels were 84.4 dB for an unoccluded condition, and 71.9 dB and 74.3 dB for the custom-molded and pre-molded ear plugs. The corresponding SNRs were −15.6, −28.1, and −25.7 dB. |
Zimpfer and Sarafian (2014) [25] | Twenty listeners (age 24–51 years) with normal hearing | Five HPDs (four earplugs and one earmuff) were used where two were passive HPD and three were active HPD. | Repeated measures | Sound localization | Number of correct responses and number of confusions | Two-way repeated-measure ANOVA revealed the significant main effect of the test condition factor [F(2.9,10.6) = 68.33, p < 0.001]. | The unoccluded condition showed a significantly higher number of correct responses than any other HPDs conditions. Moreover, the active systems yielded lower scores (53 and 40%) than did the passive systems (63%). |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kwak, C.; Han, W. The Effectiveness of Hearing Protection Devices: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11693. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182111693
Kwak C, Han W. The Effectiveness of Hearing Protection Devices: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(21):11693. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182111693
Chicago/Turabian StyleKwak, Chanbeom, and Woojae Han. 2021. "The Effectiveness of Hearing Protection Devices: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 21: 11693. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182111693
APA StyleKwak, C., & Han, W. (2021). The Effectiveness of Hearing Protection Devices: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(21), 11693. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph182111693