Next Article in Journal
Using Mind–Body Modalities via Telemedicine during the COVID-19 Crisis: Cases in the Republic of Korea
Previous Article in Journal
Gratitude and Emotional Intelligence as Protective Factors against Cyber-Aggression: Analysis of a Mediation Model
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Examining the Moderation Effect of Political Trust on the Linkage between Civic Morality and Support for Environmental Taxation

1
Community Wellbeing Research Center, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, Korea
2
Department of Public Administration, Inha University, Incheon 22212, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(12), 4476; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124476
Submission received: 12 May 2020 / Revised: 5 June 2020 / Accepted: 11 June 2020 / Published: 22 June 2020

Abstract

:
Climate change and pollution are threatening sustainable environments and human life. To mitigate and adapt to the effects of such threats, governments around the world need significant financial resources. Accordingly, this study focuses on which factors are associated with individuals’ support for taxation to protect the environment and pays special attention to the direct effects of civic morality and political trust, as well as their joint effects on support for environmental taxation. Ordered probit results with a sample size of 760 demonstrate that civic morality is positively associated with individuals’ support for environmental taxation; political trust works in the same way. More importantly, political trust moderates and enhances the linkage between civic morality and support for environmental taxation, demonstrating that it can serve as a powerful tool in a government’s efforts to protect the environment.

1. Introduction

The latest Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) report explored the meaning behind a 1.5 °C increase in the global environmental temperature. Specifically, the experts on the IPCC warned that human activities, if continued unchanged, would result in a 1.5 °C increase between 2030 and 2052 [1]. This portends a significant number of extreme weather and associated events. Even within a 0.5 °C increase, the world has experienced an unusual number of environmental disasters [1]. Australia and the United States are already struggling with prolonged wildfires, while storms are ravaging countries from all angles and during all seasons [2,3,4]. As such, humans should adapt to and mitigate the temperature increase by changing political, social, and ecological systems, as well as making efforts to reduce greenhouse gases [5,6]. To assist with these efforts, governments need resources and taxation to support those activities, which is more urgent now than ever before. Supporting taxation for environmental protection can be categorized as pro-environmental behavior, and the factors associated with such behavior have important implications that warrant a scholarly investigation.
In recent decades, research on environmental behavior and psychology has flourished, and scholars have identified several factors as critical in explaining individuals’ pro-environmental behaviors. These include individuals’ values, norms, beliefs, attitudes, perception of threats, knowledge, locus of control (or self-control), demographic characteristics, and external factors [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16]. Nonetheless, a few underdeveloped research areas include joint effects of multiple factors in explaining pro-environmental behaviors. Consequently, this study focuses on two of the factors and their joint effects on individuals’ support for environmental taxation: citizens’ norms related to good citizenship (i.e., civic morality) and citizens’ perceptions of their trust in politics (i.e., political trust), a major external element. As political trust plays a key role in how government policies are perceived and evaluated by citizens [17,18], it can moderate the linkage between citizens’ civic morality and their pro-environmental behaviors.
Civic morality refers to having honesty and respect toward others and the public good [19]. Individuals with a high degree of civic morality take rules, institutions, and social norms seriously and feel responsible for and obedient to them [19,20]. Because the respect is directed to those rules and norms, civic morality enables citizens to comply with them [19,20]. They do so in the public interest, even if such rules and norms are unpopular and evading them is not only legally inconsequential but also vastly advantageous [19]. Since civic morality entails civic duty and obligations, scholars have naturally found close associations between civic morality and compliance with rules and social norms, such as not evading taxes [21,22]. Others have explored the link between civic morality and pro-environmental behaviors [23,24,25,26]. Because civic morality is concerned with the public good, of which the environment is a vital part, those with a high degree of civic morality are expected to exhibit pro-environmental behaviors and support the taxation needed to protect the environment.
Political trust has relevance in contemporary society due to its decline in many countries [17]. Citizens have become increasingly cynical toward the workings of the government, and this has grave implications for how the government operates and pursues its policies. Defined as citizens’ feelings toward government relative to their expectations of how it ought to be [17,27], political trust serves as a heuristic, a mental shortcut for individuals who may be deficient in knowledge and have little time and few resources to understand what is happening around them [17,18,28]. For them, political trust operates as a cue by which individuals judge governments’ policies, initiatives, and legislation. Thus, high-trusting individuals are likely to benefit from doubt in the form of support of a given governmental policy without actually understanding what it does and entails. Consequently, political trust would enable high-trusting citizens to support a government action tailored to environmental protection [29,30,31,32,33], even though such action may require taxation.
More importantly, political trust in this study functions as the moderator of the linkage between individuals’ civic morality and their support for environmental taxation. Both civic morality and political trust are functional equivalents [20], and both serve the common function of engendering an obligation toward government action or policy [19,20]. Political trust as a heuristic helps individuals support government action, as they find the government trustworthy [18]. Similarly, civic morality compels individuals to follow social norms, such as paying taxes [19,20]. Thus, political trust operates as an added force that enhances potentially positive relationships between civic morality and support for taxation.
By bringing political trust to the front of explaining the relationship between civic morality and support for environmental taxation, this study combines two factors—values and perceptions induced by an external factor—in explaining pro-environmental behavior. In doing so, this study helps advance a scholarly understanding of what influences individuals’ pro-environmental behaviors.
This work will proceed as follows: First, it will introduce the theoretical mechanisms behind the direct effects of civic morality and political trust, as well as their joint effects on individuals’ support for environmental taxation. Subsequently, it develops hypotheses for an empirical analysis, followed by the presentation of the results and their implications for policymakers.

2. Civic Morality and Support for Environmental Taxation

Civic morality forms a foundation for a democratic society and its vitality. Also called norms of citizenship [20], civic morality is a sense of obligation and duty to social rules and norms [19]. This compliance is indispensable for maintaining social order; otherwise, unpopular rules and norms could only be enforced by coercive power [20]. In democratic decision-making processes, citizens’ willingness to comply voluntarily is pivotal in generating peace in everyday life [20].
In essence, civic morality implies having honesty and respect toward other citizens for the sake of the public good [19]. Because of this obligation to uphold the public interest, citizens with a greater degree of civic morality feel responsible for and compelled to follow social rules and norms [19]. Civic morality is designed to maximize public gains rather than personal gains [19]. It accepts duties owed to society and its members [19], and a keen sense of duty compels citizens to possess compliant behavior [34]. This also helps a society avoid exorbitant enforcement and deterrence costs with respect to catching and punishing law-breaking citizens [19].
Civic morality entails following a diverse set of imagined norms to be a good citizen [35,36]. These norms include regular voting, complying with the laws, being active in community affairs, and being sympathetic to the poor and unfortunate [35,36]. Moreover, rules need not be popular to be constituted as civic morality. Civic morality also applies to unpopular rules, such as paying taxes [22]. In many parts of the world, tax authorities are not equipped to detect every transaction of tax evasion. A predominant majority of citizens in developed countries pay taxes regularly, even though tax evasion seems tempting and advantageous [22]. While citizens may have an economic rationale for compliance with taxation in considering what evasion may cost them, normative considerations go beyond the economic calculus [20,37]. Citizens concerned about normative considerations prioritize ethical and moral standards that should be met [20]. By complying with a series of norms that pave the way for being good citizens, civic morality defines “an ideal citizen in a world deprived of practical limitations” [20].
Scholars have consistently found close associations between civic morality and pro-social behaviors. For instance, Torgler used the term tax morale to explain taxation and how it relates to tax compliance [22]. Environmental education studies have consistently linked civic education with environmental morale and activism [23]; an environmental civic duty nurtured in schools encourages individuals to form positive attitudes about sustainable development [24]. A high degree of ecological citizenship facilitates individuals’ environmentally friendly behaviors in everyday life when compared to those with a lower degree of environmental citizenship [25]. Civic cooperation is also positively associated with individuals’ willingness to pay higher taxes for environmental protection [38], as individuals possessing altruistic values influence the formation of pro-environmental attitudes [26].
These studies point to the linkage between individuals’ civic morality and their attitudes toward environmental taxation to protect the environment, making the following hypothesis plausible for an empirical examination.
Hypothesis 1.
Individuals’ civic morality is associated with their level of support for environmental taxation.

3. Moderating Role of Political Trust on the Linkage between Civic Morality and Support for Environmental Taxation

Political trust emerged as a vital scholarly concept in the 1970s, when countries such as the United States experienced a precipitous decline in political trust. Naturally, scholars have explored the factors leading to such decline [39,40]. Soon after, however, scholars shifted their focus to what political trust can do. As part of this, studies have begun to examine the dynamic roles of political trust as a predictor or moderator in explaining citizens’ policy preferences [17,18,27,28,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48]. In line with this, the current study narrows its focus to the moderating effect of political trust on the linkage between individuals’ civic morality and their support for taxes to protect the environment.
Political trust can be defined in terms of three approaches: a performance-based approach, a process-based approach, and a probity-based approach. A performance-based definition focuses on citizens’ feelings toward government based on their expectations of what it ought to be [17]. For instance, governmental performance in handling disasters or economies can affect citizens’ confidence in political institutions. A process-based approach emphasizes the processes that lead to performance [49]. However, citizens may dislike conflicts and gridlocks implicated in the processes affecting government performance. A probity-based approach focuses on government integrity and transparency [41,45,50]. In this approach, political trust plummets amidst government scandals, but it is heightened in the absence of such scandals and corruption. Thus, political trust can be conceptualized so that it stems from people’s perceptions of government performance, processes, and integrity.
Scholars have focused on the positive roles of political trust in enhancing individuals’ perceptions of government and its policies [17,18,28]. This stems from viewing political trust as a heuristic [17,18,28]. Political trust as a heuristic serves as a simple decision rule for individuals [17,18,28]. Individuals do not possess the necessary knowledge or philosophical understanding to analyze and understand government programs nor do they often make a cognitive effort to process information sufficiently [51,52]. Thus, individuals need “cognitive shortcuts” or “rules of thumb” [18]. These helps simplify complex information and problem-solving into digestible, evaluative operations [18,53]. In the context of evaluating government policies, political trust becomes a mental cue by which individuals support or oppose government action; in so doing, individuals can then make effortless judgements [18]. With high trust levels, people will exhibit positive attitudes toward new government programs or actions; because they find the government trustworthy, they deem its actions trustworthy, and thus support them.
This heuristic function exists because people were satisfied with government performance in the past [17,18]. The heuristic is also prospective that it will facilitate individuals’ support of government action in the future [17,18]. Moreover, political trust is not always activated for individuals with respect to any government action. Rather, such action should involve some sacrifice (such as material interest) or risk from individuals [18,28,47].
Several studies have examined the relationships between political trust and individuals’ preferences for government actions or policies, and found positive associations between political trust and redistributive programs, such as welfare aids and racially targeted programs [17,28,43]. Scholars have also found similar results between political trust and individuals’ pro-environmental behaviors [29,30,31,32,33]. For instance, high-trusting individuals are likely to make sacrifices for the environment [29], as well as favor pro-environmental policies [30,31,32]. Political trust also helps individuals take ameliorative actions toward climate change [33]. These findings lend strong support to the theoretical premise that political trust functions as a heuristic for individuals, and thus would be positively associated with individuals’ support for taxation to protect the environment, particularly given that taxation involves individual risks or sacrifices activating political trust.
More importantly, this study contends that political trust can moderate and augment the relationship between individuals’ civic morality and support for environmental taxation. Political trust is a heuristic that helps individuals become willing to support government action, even if they are not fully aware of its details and consequences [17,18,28]. In essence, political trust facilitates individuals’ compliance with such action. In a similar vein, civic morality keeps a democratic society and its vitality intact by enabling citizens to comply with commonly accepted rules and norms. However unpopular, unwanted, or excruciating taxes might be, a predominant majority of citizens do not evade taxes [20,22]. Political trust and civic morality, then, serve the common goal and function of compliance with government action or policy [20]. Thus, political trust and civic morality are functional equivalents [20]. Because political trust engenders obligations toward political institutions due to heuristics giving legitimacy to the authority, it can enhance the sense of obligation individuals with a high degree of civic morality feel toward supporting environmental taxation. Conversely, if citizens are distrustful of the authority, this will erode the confidence those with a high level of civic morality will have toward taxation to protect the environment [54].
Studies on political trust show that its heuristic force is potent as a moderator between ideology and policy preference, issue salience, and policy preferences, and government involvement and policy preferences [18,27,47]. Given the potency of political trust as a heuristic to help individuals support policies as well as political trust and civic morality as functional equivalents inducing citizens’ compliance with government action, political trust is likely to moderate and augment how individuals with a strong sense of civic morality perceive supporting taxation for environmental protection. These rationales form the basis for the following hypotheses for an empirical examination. Figure 1 provides the conceptual framework used in this study.
Hypothesis 2.
Political trust is positively associated with individuals’ support for environmental taxation.
Hypothesis 3.
Political trust moderates and enhances the relationship between individuals’ civic morality and their support for environmental taxation so that the relationship becomes stronger when the level of political trust is higher.

4. Data and Measurement

The empirical model in this study relied on the 2014 Korean General Social Survey (KGSS) [55]. The KGSS began in 2003. Its core questions and format resemble those of the U.S. General Social Survey [55]. As such, the survey targets a core group of questions repeated in every survey, complemented by topical questions repeated in every several surveys [55]. The KGSS itself has been implemented every year between 2003 and 2014, and biennially since 2014 [55]. This study used the 2014 survey because it features question items most relevant to the theory.
The response rate for the 2014 survey was 55%. The KGSS survey relies on an area probability sampling that reflects proportional population size. The sampling’s main components are the smallest administrative districts in Korea and resident registry statistics. Based on these components, the survey randomly selects individuals who are 18 years or older. The survey also excludes individuals who are institutionalized [55].
In terms of respondents’ demographics, 42% of respondents were female, while 58% were male. The average of respondent age was approximately 45 years-old. The average income level of respondents was 6.46, earning before-tax income ranging between KRW 2,500,000 and KRW 2,990,000. The average educational level of respondents was 4 (technical college education that provides 2 years of higher education).

4.1. Dependent Variable

The dependent variable in this study is support for environmental taxation. The variable relies on a single, subjective item: “How willing would you be to pay much higher taxes to protect the environment?” [55]. Scoring for the item ranges from 1 to 5. Signaling strong support, 5 indicates a “great willingness”, while 1 indicates a “great unwillingness,” signaling strong opposition. The mean was 3.33, slightly above 3, showing that, in general, respondents possessed slightly more favorable attitudes toward paying taxes for environmental protection.
While a single item is not ideal due to a lack of psychometric properties that multi-item measures can afford, several studies have demonstrated that a single-item measure can approximate a multi-item measure due to high correlations between the two [56,57]. Similarly, a subjective item denoting behavioral intentions is not equal to an objective item denoting actual behaviors, but some studies have displayed high correlations between the two measures [7,58], suggesting that a subjective item can be an alternative to an objective item when objective measures are not readily available.

4.2. Explanatory Variable

4.2.1. Civic Morality

Civic morality consists of six items, the scoring for each of which ranges from 1 to 7. Respondents were asked to reveal how important each of the following six items is to being a good citizen: voting in every election, no tax evasion, monitoring government actions, understanding others with different views, choosing products ethically, and helping other worse-off Koreans. The measure was reliable, with Cronbach’s α equaling 0.768. The mean of the variable was 5.57, suggesting that respondents generally possessed a relatively higher degree of citizenship norms. As hypothesized, civic morality was expected to be positively associated with individuals’ support for environmental taxation.

4.2.2. Political Trust

Political trust comprises four items, the scoring for each of which ranges from 1 to 3. Respondents were asked to reveal their confidence in the following four institutions: central agencies, the National Assembly of Korea, the president’s office (Cheongwadae), and the local government. The measure was highly reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.809). The mean of the variable was 1.45, closer to the minimum level of trust than the maximum, suggesting that respondents generally did not possess a high degree of confidence in the country’s four major political institutions. As hypothesized, political trust is expected to be positively associated with individuals’ support for environmental taxation and to moderate and enhance the relationship between individuals’ civic morality and their support for environmental taxation.

4.3. Controls

The model also accounts for several control variables. First, the model included political participation. Political participation touches on individuals’ self-assessment of their political life. The measure consists of four items. Respondents were asked to assess their participation in (1) political rallies, (2) demonstrations, (3) product boycotts, and (4) online forums. The measure was highly reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.951). Several studies have found that political participation generates a strong sense of locus of control and self-efficacy, with which individuals are likely motivated to pursue desirable individual, social, and political outcomes [59,60]. Thus, individuals with a greater level of political participation are more likely to pursue ameliorative actions with respect to the environment [13,61,62]. Second, the model accounts for individuals’ political interests. The measure relies on a single item; respondents were asked how interested they personally are in politics. Individuals with a strong interest in politics are more likely to gain in-depth knowledge on contemporary problems, such as the degree to which pollution and climate change threaten the environment, and to support an action to mitigate them [63]. In this sense, they would likely be well-disposed to support taxation to protect the environment. Third, the model accounts for a perceptual measure of environmental threat. The measure comprises six items: respondents’ perceptions of vehicle-induced air pollution, industry-induced air pollution, farming-induced pollution, water pollution, global warming, and gene-modified crops. The measure was highly reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.984). Several studies have demonstrated that individuals exhibiting a high degree of threat perception display positive orientations toward resolving such threats [64,65]. Since environmental protection is gaining urgency in South Korea due to frequent occurrences of fine dust and temperature warming [66], those feeling threatened by such events would be supportive of ameliorative actions, such as taxation, to protect the environment. Fourth, the model includes additional perceptual measures about how individuals perceive environmental threats in their community. The measure consists of three items: Respondents were asked how they perceive (1) air, (2) water, and (3) noise pollution in their communities. There exist strong connections between individuals’ direct experiences with the environment near their homes and their support for a remedial action [12]. This prompts the expectation that individuals perceiving a greater degree of threat near where they live are more likely to be motivated to support taxation that addresses environmental protection. The measure was also highly reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.989).
Finally, the model accounts for several demographic variables. Studies have shown that women tend to support environmental causes and display pro-environmental behaviors more than men [67]; as people grow older, they find themselves closer to the environment [68]; the well-educated possess necessary knowledge and understanding with which they display positive attitudes toward efforts to protect the environment [69]; and high-income earners are likely to be equipped with sufficient resources and time to invest more in environmental causes than low-income earners [30]. Income and education are expressed as levels. Respondents were asked to choose a level to which they belonged for their education and income (before-tax average monthly income). In terms of education, 0 indicates “no formal schooling”; 1, “elementary school”; 2, “junior high school”; 3, “high school”; 4, “junior college” (two-year course); 5, “college” (four-year course); 6, “graduate school (master’s degree)”; 7, “graduate school (doctoral degree).” In terms of monthly income, 0 indicates “no income”; 1, “less than KRW 500,000”; 2, “KRW 500,000-990,000”; 3, “KRW 1,000,000–1,490,000”; 4, “KRW 1,500,000–1,990,000”; 5, “KRW 2,000,000–2,490,000”; 6, “KRW 2,500,000–2,990,000”; 7, “KRW 3,000,000–3,490,000”; 8, “KRW 3,500,000–3,990,000”; 9, “KRW 4,000,000–4,490,000”; 10, “KRW 4,500,000–4,990,000”; 11, “KRW 5,000,000–5,490,000”; 12, “KRW 5,500,000–5,990,000”; 13, “KRW 6,000,000–6,490,000”; 14, “KRW 6,500,000–6,990,000”; 15, “KRW 7,000,000–7,490,000”; 16, “KRW 7,500,000–7,990,000”; 17, “KRW 8,000,000–8,490,000”; 18, “KRW 8,500,000–8,990,000”; 19, “KRW 9,000,000–9,490,000”; 20, “KRW 9,500,000–9,990,000”; and 21, “more than KRW 10,000,000.” Table 1 displays the variables’ descriptive statistics.

4.4. Measurement Validity

Because the study relies on a single-source dataset, it is susceptible to common method variance. Thus, we have employed a series of checks to ensure that the model does not suffer from common source bias. First, Harman’s single-factor test was implemented to find whether a single factor dominated respondents’ answers to other factors included in the model. The test only found 17.33% of one factor in explaining the covariance in the model. Second, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed to identify a model fit. Included in the analysis were five factors: civic morality, political trust, political participation, perceived local pollution, and perceived environmental threats. The analysis produced the following fit indices: χ 2 = 876.985, RMSEA = 0.047, SRMR = 0.034, CFI = 0.939, and TLI = 0.930. The numbers meet generally accepted threshold values [70], suggesting that the five-factor model fit the data well.

5. Results

The empirical analysis employed an ordered probit. Because the dependent variable contains ordinal values, running a linear regression can produce biased estimates. A binary probit is an alternative method, but it shrinks information contained in the ordinal values. Thus, an ordered probit was used for the empirical analysis. Finally, the analysis accounted for robust standard errors, as well as a weight that represented the population.
The results are displayed in Table 2. The empirical analysis proceeded in two steps. In the first step, only direct relationships between the explanatory variables and dependent variable, as well as between the controls and dependent variable, were considered. The second step was centered on identifying the joint effects of civic morality and political trust on individuals’ support for environmental taxation.
The results confirmed the three hypotheses. First, individuals’ civic morality was positively associated with their support for environmental taxation. Those who hold citizenship norms in high esteem are likely to exhibit compliance with taxation and demonstrate positive attitudes toward the environment. Second, high-trusting individuals were also positively associated with their support for environmental taxation. High-trusting individuals are more likely to view governmental causes in a favorable light and be supportive of taxation that funds such causes. More importantly, the study focused on whether political trust moderates and augments the linkage between individuals’ perceptions of civic morality and their support for environmental taxation. The results showed that political trust enhanced and amplified the linkage. Because political trust functions as a heuristic to compel individuals to support environmental taxation, it enhances the sense of obligation to comply with environmental taxation for those possessing a higher level of civic morality.
In terms of the controls, several variables behaved as previous findings have suggested. Individuals’ degree of political participation and interest was positively associated with supporting environmental taxation [59,60,63]. Individuals with a heightened perception of environmental threat were also positively associated with support for environmental taxation [64,65]. Similarly, individuals showed more favorable attitudes toward supporting environmental taxation when they possessed a greater perception of local pollution near their places of residence [12]. Two demographic variables—income and education—were also positively associated with supporting taxes for environmental protection [30,69].
Table 3 displays the predicted probabilities of individuals strongly supporting environmental taxation (the value of the dependent variable = 5). This demonstrates the difference with respect to “strong support” between when a variable is set at the minimum and when it is set at the maximum. For the interaction, the difference is between when both variables are at their minimum and when they are at their maximum. The heightened sense of civic morality shifted “strong support” for environmental protection by 10.79%. The influence of political trust was less substantial than that of civic morality, yet it was more than 7%. The focus of the study on the moderation effects of political trust, however, was justified when examining the joint effects of civic morality and political trust on “strong support” for environmental taxation. The shift yielded more than a 34% change for “strong support.” Thus, both variables became more powerful in their combination with “strong support.”
Figure 2 visualizes the joint effects of civic morality and political trust on having strong support for environmental taxation. The solid line refers to the relationship between civic morality and “strong support” for environmental taxation when the level of political trust is at its maximum (high trust). The dashed line denotes the linkage when the level of political trust is at its minimum (low trust). The slope becomes much steeper when the level of political trust is high, whereas the slope changes little when the level of political trust is low. Once again, the joint effects of civic morality and political trust are noticeable.

6. Discussion

The study focused on the direct effects of civic morality and political trust as well as their joint effects on individuals’ support of environmental taxation. The results showed that civic morality and political trust were positively associated with support for environmental taxation, and more importantly, they jointly exerted a powerful influence on support for environmental taxation. The results help expand on the existing body of environmental behavior research. Studies on individuals’ pro-environmental behaviors have explored a wide array of factors. For instance, they have identified values and beliefs, perceptions, loci of control (internal vs. external), attitudes, knowledge, demographic characteristics, and external circumstances [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16]. While these have helped illuminate why individuals behave in a positive manner toward the environment, few have examined the role politics plays in terms of how many individuals place their trust in major political institutions. If civic morality can be characterized as valuing others, political trust can be considered values or perceptions induced by an external circumstance known as politics. The study brings together two elements that make up the factors associated with the determinants of pro-environmental behaviors. Examining political trust is also relevant in that individuals’ behavior is significantly dampened or stimulated by their perceptions of politics. When ideological battles or gridlocks seem to intensify across countries, political trust has significant implications for how citizens approach major issues, such as climate change and pollution, which are vital for a sustainable environment and human life.
The results suggest important implications for policymakers. First, the results proved that civic morality is a critical element in citizens’ support for pro-environmental behaviors. Thus, it is important for policymakers to direct their attention to improving citizens’ sense of civic morality. Scholars have been concerned about nurturing civic duty among citizens for quite some time [23,25,26]. One of the ways they have argued to boost citizens’ civic morality begins with developing their political self-efficacy. This is based on the premise that citizens isolated from political life develop skeptical worldviews and negatively view values and principles that can hold society together [29,50]. Improving citizens’ self-efficacy can start in the school setting [12,13]; students need to learn about facilitating their self-efficacy. They can be nudged toward this through various mechanisms, including open-ended discussions to learn how to freely express themselves, working with other politically oriented groups to feel a sense of bonding for a common purpose, and participating in simulated activities, such as mock elections and demonstrations [13]. Studies have repeatedly shown that individuals with a stronger sense of self-efficacy develop internal efficacy (i.e., the belief that they can affect personal and political changes) and external efficacy (i.e., the belief that they can demand change from the government) to bring about positive changes to themselves and their society [59,60]. Pro-environmental policies and behaviors are an urgent need in this age of global warming, and they can emerge from people’s moral principles about being an active citizen.
Second, the results suggested that political trust plays a crucial role in facilitating citizens’ support for environmental taxation and augmenting the linkage between citizens’ civic morality and their support for environmental taxation. Increasing citizens’ confidence in their country’s political institutions can bring positive effects to environmental protection efforts. Scholars have extensively studied the factors associated with political trust [17,28,39,40], identifying factors such as government corruption, perception of the legislative body, and performance-based explanations [17,28,39,40,42]. For instance, government corruption dampens public confidence in mainstream politics. In South Korea, the corruption-ridden Park administration led to massive candlelight demonstrations for months and the president’s eventual ousting in 2017 [71,72]. Government performance is also a crucial element in explaining political trust. Citizens’ perceptions of economic performance are identified positively with political trust [17]. Economic performance is also asymmetrical, in that its salience is more pronounced in a sour economy than in a healthy economy [17]. This explains why a sour economy frequently leads to the defeat of incumbent administrations, whereas a healthy economy does not necessarily correspond to the public holding incumbent administrations in high esteem [17]. U.S.-based studies have also revealed that relative to perceptions of a president and central government, perceptions of a legislative body explain political trust to a higher degree. Parliamentary activities are exposed to the public every day, making a legislative body vulnerable to public anger [17,28]. Thus, it is important for political trust levels to be high, so all major political institutions work together, maintain integrity, and serve the public good. Indeed, this is a tall order.
A major problem lies in how to improve citizens’ political trust when it is at its all-time low in countries such as the United States [73]. It is difficult to foresee a reversing trend anytime soon; however, even with a general declining trend, a glimpse of hope remains in relation to frequent fluctuations in political trust sparked by major natural or human-caused events. For instance, the degree of political trust in the central government in South Korea was plummeting, an ominous sign for the upcoming parliamentary election, due to slow economic growth and scandals involving a presidential appointee [74]. Then, coronavirus emerged, and thousands of COVID-19 cases further dampened citizens’ confidence in the central government [75]. Within a few months, however, thousands of cases have turned into dozens, mostly induced by inbound visitors, producing a remarkable turnaround in political trust just in time for the government party to win a landslide victory, the largest such gain in modern Korean history [76]. How to maintain and improve citizens’ political trust is up to the government in coming months, but the Korean episode points to the fact that many events can influence citizens’ political trust. Citizens’ political trust levels are not fixed, but malleable by unforeseen events, and political leaders should be motivated to pursue their favorite policies when such opportunity strikes.

7. Conclusions

Global warming signals difficulties for human life in the years ahead [1,5]. Countries around the world have observed frequent environmental degradation and devastation, with storms, heat waves, and wildfires filling newspaper pages and TV programs [2,3,4]. An IPCC report pictured what the world will look like with a 1.5 °C increase [1]. Some of its effects will include a decline in the number of communities relying on small islands, coasts, and farming [1]. Developing countries—compared to developed ones—are especially vulnerable to the effects of global warming [1]. Some in hot-weather regions will undergo slow economic growth and a shortage of water supply [1]. All of these demand urgent responses to mitigate and adapt to global warming, with government resources directed toward the environment.
Finally, it should be noted that the study has its limitations. First, the study relied on a single-year cross-sectional dataset. Thus, the causal chain between the explanatory and dependent variables is much weaker than when using panel or experimental data. This may be inherent in cross-sectional surveys such as the KGSS, in which the same individuals are not tracked across years. Second, the dataset was collected during the same time period. Because there was no time difference between the explanatory and dependent variables, the study could not resolve any potential endogeneity between them. Third, the study relied on a single-source dataset. While a few cautionary tests (Harman’s single-factor and CFA tests) were undertaken to mitigate concerns about common method variance, the threat of common method bias may always be present when using a single-source dataset. Fourth, the study examined a single-country survey, and its applicability may be limited when compared to what a comparative survey covering several countries can offer. Finally, the dependent variable denoted behavioral intentions. While some claim close correlations between behavioral intentions and actual behavior [7], the utility of the results would be further preserved if actual behaviors were observed and recorded.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.Y.L. and K.-K.M.; data curation, J.Y.L. and K.-K.M.; analysis, J.Y.L. and K.-K.M.; methodology, J.Y.L. and K.-K.M.; writing, reviewing, and editing, J.Y.L. and K.-K.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Global Warming of 1.5 °C: An Ipcc Special Report on the Impacts of Global Warming of 1.5 °C Above Pre-Industrial Levels and Related Global Greenhouse Gas Emission Pathways. Available online: https://www.ipcc.ch/sr15/ (accessed on 15 March 2020).
  2. The End of Australia as We Know it. The New York Times. 2020. Available online: https://www.nytimes.com/2020/02/15/world/australia/fires-climate-change.html (accessed on 16 February 2020).
  3. Puerto Rico Spend 11 Months Turning the Power Back on. They Finally Got to her. The New York Times 2018. 14 August. Available online: https://www.nytimes.com/2018/08/14/us/puerto-rico-electricity-power.html (accessed on 12 January 2020).
  4. Minder, R. Powerful Winter Storm in Spain kills at least 10. The New York Times. 2020. Available online: https://www.nytimes.com/2020/01/23/world/europe/storm-gloria-spain-floods.html (accessed on 13 March 2020).
  5. VijayaVenkataRaman, S.; Iniyan, S.; Goic, R. A review of climate change, mitigation and adaptation. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 2012, 16, 878–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wallace-Wells, D. The Uninhabitable Earth: Life after Warming; Tim Duggan Books: New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  7. Ajzen, I.; Fishbein, M. Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior; Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  8. Axelrod, L.J.; Lehman, D.R. Responding to environmental concerns: What factors guide individual action? J. Environ. Psychol. 1993, 13, 149–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Fransson, N.; Gärling, T. Environmental concern: Conceptual definitions, measurement methods, and research findings. J. Environ. Psychol. 1999, 19, 369–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Fujii, S. Environmental concern, attitude toward frugality, and ease of behavior as determinants of pro-environmental behavior intentions. J. Environ. Psychol. 2006, 26, 262–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Grob, A. A structural model of environmental attitudes and behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 1995, 15, 209–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kollmuss, A.; Agyeman, J. Mind the gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 2002, 8, 239–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Levy, B.L.M.; Zint, M.T. Toward fostering environmental political participation: Framing an agenda for environmental education research. Environ. Educ. Res. 2012, 19, 553–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Liobikienė, G.; Juknys, R. The role of values, environmental risk perception, awareness of consequences, and willingness to assume responsibility for environmentally-friendly behaviour: The Lithuanian case. J. Clean. Prod. 2016, 112, 3413–3422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Newman, T.P.; Fernandes, R. A re-assessment of factors associated with environmental concern and behavior using the 2010 General Social Survey. Environ. Educ. Res. 2015, 22, 153–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Nordlund, A.M.; Garvill, J. Effects of values, problem awareness, and personal norm on willingness to reduce personal car use. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 339–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hetherington, M.J.; Rudolph, T.J. Why Washington Won’t Work: Polarization, Political Trust, and the Governing Crisis; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  18. Rudolph, T.J. Political trust as a heuristic. In Handbook on Political Trust; Zmerli, S., van der Meer, T., Eds.; Edward Elgar Publishing: Northampton, MA, USA, 2017; pp. 197–211. [Google Scholar]
  19. Letki, N. Investigating the roots of civic morality: Trust, social capital, and institutional performance. Polit. Behav. 2006, 28, 305–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Van Deth, J. Compliance, trust and norms of citizenship. In Handbook on Political Trust; Zmerli, S., van der Meer, T., Eds.; Edward Elgar Publishing: Northampton, MA, USA, 2017; pp. 212–227. [Google Scholar]
  21. Alm, J.; Torgler, B. Culture differences and tax morale in the United States and in Europe. J. Econ. Psychol. 2006, 27, 224–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Torgler, B. Tax Compliance and Tax Morale: A Theoretical and Empirical Analysis; Edward Elgar Publishing, Inc.: Northampton, MA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  23. Dresner, M.; Handelman, C.; Braun, S.; Rollwagen-Bollens, G. Environmental identity, pro-environmental behaviors, and civic engagement of volunteer stewards in Portland area Parks. Environ. Educ. Res. 2015, 21, 991–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Dobson, A. Environmental citizenship: Towards sustainable development. Sustain. Dev. 2007, 15, 276–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Jagers, S.C.; Martinsson, J.; Matti, S. Ecological citizenship: A driver of pro-environmental behaviour? Environ. Polit. 2014, 23, 434–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Ojea, E.; Loureiro, M. Altruistic, egoistic and biospheric values in willingness to pay (WTP) for wildlife. Ecol. Econ. 2007, 63, 807–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Rudolph, T.; Popp, E. Bringing the ideological divide: Trust and support for social security privatization. Polit. Behav. 2009, 31, 331–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Hetherington, M. Why Trust Matters: Declining Political Trust and the Demise of American Liberalism; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  29. Taniguchi, H.; Marshall, G.A. Trust, political orientation, and environmental behavior. Environ. Polit. 2018, 27, 385–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Fairbrother, M. Rich people, poor people, and environmental concern: Evidence across nations and time. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 2012, 29, 910–922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Harring, N. Understanding the effects of corruption and political trust on willingness to make economic sacrifices for environmental protection in a cross-national perspective. Soc. Sci. Q. 2013, 94, 660–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Harring, N. Trust and state intervention: Results from a Swedish survey on environmental policy support. Environ. Sci. Policy 2018, 82, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Smith, E.K.; Mayer, A. A social trap for the climate? Collective action, trust and climate change risk perception in 35 countries. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2018, 49, 140–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Orviska, M.; Hudson, J.W. Tax evasion, civic duty and the law abiding citizen. Eur. J. Polit. Econ. 2002, 19, 83–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Van Deth, J. Norms of citizenship. In The Oxford Handbook of Political Behavior; Dalton, R.J., Klingemann, H.D., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007; pp. 402–417. [Google Scholar]
  36. Dalton, R. Citizenship norms and the expansion of political participation. Polit. Stud. 2008, 56, 76–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Tyler, T. Why People Obey the Law; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  38. Owen, A.L.; Videras, J. Civic cooperation, pro-environment attitudes, and behavioral intentions. Ecol. Econ. 2006, 58, 814–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Citrin, J. Comment: The political relevance of trust in government. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 1974, 68, 973–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Miller, A.H. Political issues and trust in government: 1964–1970. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 1974, 68, 951–972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Chanley, V.A.; Rudolph, T.J.; Rahn, W.M. The origins and consequences of public trust in government. Public Opin. Q. 2000, 64, 239–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hetherington, M. The political relevance of political trust. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 1998, 92, 791–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hetherington, M.J.; Globetti, S. Political trust and racial policy preferences. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 2002, 46, 253–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hetherington, M.J.; Husser, J.A. How trust matters: The changing political relevance of political trust. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 2012, 56, 312–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Hetherington, M.J.; Rudolph, T.J. Priming, performance, and the dynamics of political trust. J. Polit. 2008, 70, 498–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Popp, E.; Rudolph, T.J. A tale of two ideologies: Explaining public support for economic interventions. J. Polit. 2011, 73, 808–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Rudolph, T.; Evans, J. Political trust, ideology, and public support for government spending. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 2005, 49, 660–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Rudolph, T.J. Political trust, ideology, and public support for tax cuts. Public Opin. Q. 2009, 73, 144–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Hibbing, J.R. What Is It About Government that Americans Dislike? Hibbing, J., Theiss-Morse, E., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  50. Keele, L. Social capital and the dynamics of trust in government. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 2007, 51, 241–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Chaiken, S. Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 39, 752–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Chaiken, S.; Trope, Y. (Eds.) Dual-Process Theories of Social Psychology; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  53. Fiske, S.T.; Taylor, S.E. Social Cognition, 2nd ed.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  54. Hooghe, M.; Zmerli, S. Introduction: The context of political trust. In Political Trust: Why Context Matters; Zmerli, S., Hooghe, M., Eds.; ECPR Press: Colchester, UK, 2011; pp. 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  55. Kim, J.; Kang, J.; Kim, S.; Kim, C.; Park, W.; Lee, Y.; Choi, S.; Kim, S. Korean General Social Survey 2003–2016; Sungkyunkwan University: Seoul, South Korea, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  56. Wanous, J.P.; Reichers, A.E.; Hudy, M.J. Overall job satisfaction: How good are single-item measures? J. Appl. Psychol. 1997, 82, 247–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Wanous, J.P.; Hudy, M.J. Single-Item reliability: A replication and extension. Organ. Res. Methods 2001, 4, 361–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Dess, G.G.; Robinson, R.B. Measuring organizational performance in the absence of objective measures: The case of the privately-held firm and conglomerate business unit. Strateg. Manag. J. 1984, 5, 265–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Finkel, S.E. Reciprocal effectis of participation and political efficacy: A panel analysis. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 1985, 29, 891–913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Pollock, P.H. The participatory consequences of internal and external political efficacy: A research note. West. Polit. Q. 1983, 36, 400–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Carvalho, A.; van Wessel, M.; Maeseele, P. Communication practices and political engagement with climate change. Environ. Commun. 2017, 11, 122–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Schild, R. Fostering environmental citizenship: The motivations and outcomes of civic recreation. J. Environ. Plan. Manag. 2018, 61, 924–949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Bradley, J.C.; Waliczek, T.M.; Zajicek, J.M. Relationship between environmental knowledge and environmental attitude of high school students. J. Environ. Educ. 1999, 30, 17–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Keshavarz, M.; Karami, E. Farmers’ pro-environmental behavior under drought: Application of protection motivation theory. J. Arid Environ. 2016, 127, 128–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Kim, S.; Jeong, S.-H.; Hwang, Y. Predictors of pro-environmental behaviors of American and Korean students: The application of the theory of reasoned action and protection motivation theory. Sci. Commun. 2012, 35, 168–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Harris, B.; Kang, B. South Korea Joins Ranks of World’s Most Polluted Countries. Financial Times, 29 March 2017. Available online: https://www.ft.com/content/b49a9878-141b-11e7-80f4-13e067d5072c(accessed on 19 March 2020).
  67. McCright, A.M. The effects of gender on climate change knowledge and concern in the American public. Popul. Environ. 2010, 32, 66–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Wiernik, B.M.; Ones, D.S.; Dilchert, S. Age and environmental sustainability: A Meta-analysis. J. Manag. Psychol. 2013, 28, 826–856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Meyer, A. Heterogeneity in the preferences and pro-environmental behavior of college students: The effects of years on campus, demographics, and external factors. J. Clean. Prod. 2016, 112, 3451–3463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 3rd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  71. Choi, S. South Koreans Rally in Largest Protest in Decades to Demand President’s Ouster. The New York Times, 12 November 2016. Available online: https://www.nytimes.com/2016/11/13/world/asia/korea-park-geun-hye-protests.html(accessed on 2 February 2020).
  72. Choi, S. South Korea Removes President Park Geun-hye. The New York Times, 9 March 2017. Available online: https://www.nytimes.com/2017/03/09/world/asia/park-geun-hye-impeached-south-korea.html(accessed on 2 February 2020).
  73. Pew Research Center. Little Public Support for Reductions in Federal Spending. 11 April. Available online: https://www.people-press.org/2019/04/11/little-public-support-for-reductions-in-federal-spending/ (accessed on 14 March 2020).
  74. Lee, J. South Korea’s Moon Sees Approval Rating Hit New Low Amid Scandal. Bloomberg, 18 October 2019. Available online: https://www.bloomberg.com/news/articles/2019-10-18/south-korea-s-moon-sees-approval-rating-hit-new-low-amid-scandal(accessed on 12 March 2020).
  75. Shin, H. Ratings of South Korea’s Moon Take a Hit from Virus Outbreak. Reuters. 28 February 2020. Available online: https://www.reuters.com/article/us-china-health-southkorea-moon/ratings-of-south-koreas-moon-take-a-hit-from-virus-outbreak-idUSKCN20M0RK (accessed on 15 March 2020).
  76. McCurry, J. South Korea’s Ruling Party Wins Election Landslide Amid Coronavirus Outbreak. The Guardian. 16 April 2020. Available online: https://www.theguardian.com/world/2020/apr/16/south-koreas-ruling-party-wins-election-landslide-amid-coronavirus-outbreak (accessed on 16 April 2020).
Figure 1. The Conceptual Framework.
Figure 1. The Conceptual Framework.
Ijerph 17 04476 g001
Figure 2. Predicted probabilities of strong support for environmental taxation.
Figure 2. Predicted probabilities of strong support for environmental taxation.
Ijerph 17 04476 g002
Table 1. Descriptive statistics.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics.
VariablesNMeanS.D.Min.Max.
Support for environmental taxation7603.331.1015
Civic morality7605.571.011.337
Political trust7601.450.4613
Political participation7601.670.6614
Political interest7602.440.7814
Perceived local pollution7602.450.6314
Perceived environmental threats7603.620.631.675
Age76044.9813.191883
Female7600.420.4901
Income7606.464.24021
Education7604.001.3907
Table 2. Regression results.
Table 2. Regression results.
VariablesSupport for Environmental Taxation
Step 1Step 2
Coef.(S.E.)Coef.(S.E.)
Civic morality0.190.05 ***−0.110.13
Political trust0.240.09 ***−0.980.48 **
Civic morality × political Trust 0.220.08 ***
Political participation0.250.07 ***0.270.07 ***
Political interest0.140.06 **0.120.06 *
Perceived local pollution0.130.07 *0.130.07 *
Perceived environmental threats0.160.08 **0.150.08 *
Age0.000.000.000.00
Female−0.040.09−0.030.09
Income0.020.01 **0.020.01 **
Education0.080.03 **0.080.03 **
τ12.080.450.350.82
τ22.980.451.250.82
τ33.470.461.750.83
τ45.170.473.460.83
Log likelihood−985.82−982.53
Wald test 117.68130.83
Number of cases760
Note: * p < 0.1, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01.
Table 3. Predicted probabilities of strong support for environmental taxation.
Table 3. Predicted probabilities of strong support for environmental taxation.
VariablesStrong Support for
Environmental Taxation
MinimumMaximumDifference
Civic morality × political trust1.98%36.68%34.70%
Civic morality0.89%11.68%10.79%
Political trust5.55%13.21%7.66%

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lim, J.Y.; Moon, K.-K. Examining the Moderation Effect of Political Trust on the Linkage between Civic Morality and Support for Environmental Taxation. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4476. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124476

AMA Style

Lim JY, Moon K-K. Examining the Moderation Effect of Political Trust on the Linkage between Civic Morality and Support for Environmental Taxation. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(12):4476. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124476

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lim, Jae Young, and Kuk-Kyoung Moon. 2020. "Examining the Moderation Effect of Political Trust on the Linkage between Civic Morality and Support for Environmental Taxation" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 12: 4476. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124476

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop