Next Article in Journal
Design and Experimental Approach to the Construction of a Human Signal-Molecule-Profiling Database
Previous Article in Journal
Radioactivity of Drinking-Water in the Vicinity of Nuclear Power Plants in China Based on a Large-Scale Monitoring Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

The Evolutionary Puzzle of Suicide

1
Hopital Paul Brousse, AP-HP, INSERM U 669, University Paris-Sud 11, 12 Avenue, Paul-Vaillant-Couturier, Villejuif 94800, France
2
Pitié-Salpêtrière, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Université P&M Curie-Faculté de Médecine, INSERM U894, 47 Bd de l'Hôpital, Paris 75013, France
3
Laboratoire de Psychologie Médicale, Université Libre de Bruxelles, CHU Brugmann, Place van Gehuchten 4, Bruxelles 1020, Belgium
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10(12), 6873-6886; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph10126873
Submission received: 18 October 2013 / Revised: 29 November 2013 / Accepted: 29 November 2013 / Published: 9 December 2013

Abstract

:
Mechanisms of self-destruction are difficult to reconcile with evolution’s first rule of thumb: survive and reproduce. However, evolutionary success ultimately depends on inclusive fitness. The altruistic suicide hypothesis posits that the presence of low reproductive potential and burdensomeness toward kin can increase the inclusive fitness payoff of self-removal. The bargaining hypothesis assumes that suicide attempts could function as an honest signal of need. The payoff may be positive if the suicidal person has a low reproductive potential. The parasite manipulation hypothesis is founded on the rodent—Toxoplasma gondii host-parasite model, in which the parasite induces a “suicidal” feline attraction that allows the parasite to complete its life cycle. Interestingly, latent infection by T. gondii has been shown to cause behavioral alterations in humans, including increased suicide attempts. Finally, we discuss how suicide risk factors can be understood as nonadaptive byproducts of evolved mechanisms that malfunction. Although most of the mechanisms proposed in this article are largely speculative, the hypotheses that we raise accept self-destructive behavior within the framework of evolutionary theory.

Natural selection will never produce in a being any structure more injurious than beneficial to that being, for natural selection acts solely by and for the good of each. No organ will be formed for the purpose of causing pain or for doing an injury to its possessor.
Charles Darwin

1. Introduction

Evolutionary theory is typically concerned with explaining life. Within this framework, selection is expected to promote the evolution of various biological mechanisms that increase the individual’s ability to avoid death. The evolution of mechanisms of self-destruction is more difficult to envision [1].
The estimated global burden of suicide is one million deaths per year, with a great inter-country variability [2]. Historically, suicide risk increased with age, with older men identified as the group at highest risk. However, in the 1970s, suicide became increasingly common in young adults, especially young men, in some high-income countries [3]. Distal risk factors include genetic loading, personality characteristics (e.g., impulsivity and aggression), restricted fetal growth and perinatal circumstances, early traumatic life events, and neurobiological disturbances. Proximal risk factors include psychiatric disorders, including substance use disorders, physical disorders, psychological crisis, availability of means, and exposure to suicidality models [2]. Heritability of completed suicide has been estimated at about 43% [4].
Most of the research on suicidal behavior has been conducted in an atheoretical context. However, diverse theories of suicide have been proposed. Biological theories suggest that suicidal behavior results from the dual presence of a biologically based vulnerability and an activating psychosocial stressor [5,6]. Psychodynamic theories propose that the suicidal crisis is the consequence of affect deluge, efforts to master affective flooding, loss of control and disintegration, and grandiose survival and body jettison [7]. Cognitive-behavioral theories posit causal roles of hopelessness, autobiographical memory deficits, perceptions of entrapment, and emotion dysregulation [8]. Developmental/systems theories posit causal roles of disturbed social forces and family systems [8]. The broader interpersonal theory of suicide proposes that the most dangerous form of suicidal desire depends on two constructs: (1) thwarted belongingness and (2) perceived burdensomeness. Suicide is likely to occur when the subject experiences hopelessness about these states and when he acquires the capability to engage in suicidal behavior [8]. Thwarted belongingness may result in self-defeating behaviors, perceptions of burdensomeness may lead to self-hate regarding those perceptions, and acquired capability is a multidimensional emergent variable that involves the dimensions of lowered fear of death and increased tolerance of physical pain.
Suicide and homosexuality are the two phenomena that have been most difficult to reconcile with evolutionary theory, as both directly affect human reproductive fitness [9,10]. Selection pressures modify physical characteristics and behavioral patterns to adapt to environmental changes and challenges. Evolutionary psychology has proposed that the brain was shaped by natural selection to solve adaptive problems that were repeatedly encountered by humans throughout their evolutionary history. The environment of evolutionary adaptedness refers to the problems hunter-gatherers had to solve and the conditions under which they solved them; the functional architecture of the mind was not selected for because it solved the problems faced in our modern environment, with its permanent settlements, agriculture, large interacting populations. Instead, it was shaped by how well it solved adaptive problems among our hunter-gatherers ancestors. Thus evolution by natural selection resulted in specialized neurocognitive mechanisms that successfully responded to distinct adaptive problems that occurred before the emergence of our modern environment [10,11]. Specialized mechanisms have been favored over more general mechanisms because they solve a number of problems that more generalized mechanisms cannot, e.g., functional incompatibility between two functions will favor specialized solutions rather than common general mechanism, combinatorial explosion paralyzes any truly domain-general system when encountering real-world complexity [12,13]. These specialized mechanisms have been referred to as “darwinian algorithms” [12]. Examples of specialized mechanisms designed for specific adaptive functions include fear of snakes and spiders, the capacity to learn spoken languages, preferences for particular characteristics in a mate, and the detection of cheaters [12].This theoretical framework contrasts with “blank slate” frameworks, which argue that a relatively few general-purpose mechanisms can account for humans’ reasoning and learning abilities [14].
Darwin postulated that variant traits are inherited by the offspring and that traits that are beneficial for survival and reproduction are transmitted to future generations at greater frequencies than alternatives. This process results in the following three products [10]: (a) adaptations, inherited traits that favor survival and reproduction; (b) byproducts, artifacts without adaptive value but that are inherently coupled with adaptations; and (c) noise, variations due to random environmental events or genetic mutations.
Evolutionary theorists stress the distinction between proximate and ultimate causation. Proximal causation explores how the mechanism works, whereas the higher-level ultimate causation explores why the phenomenon exists. The purpose of evolutionary psychology is to provide clues on ultimate causation. As natural selection favors genes that increase the ability to survive and reproduce, this mechanism should promote the emergence and maintenance of selfish behavior. However, humans are social animals that evolved with a high level of cooperation. Some situations present choices between alternatives that are beneficial to an individual and alternatives that are less beneficial—or even harmful—to the individual but would nevertheless be beneficial if chosen by many individuals. Such alternatives characterize problems of social cooperation; selection of the latter alternative is generally considered to be altruistic [15]. In other words, in an altruistic case, the altruistic subject experiences a fitness loss whereas the receiving subject benefits from a fitness gain.
Relatives often carry the same genes. Therefore, we can expect that a hypothetical gene that generates a behavior that increases the kin fitness at the expense the individual's own fitness will increase in frequency in the population. According to this fundamental principle of kin selection theory, at times, the fitness loss induced by the behavior can be compensated by an enhanced fitness of relatives. Kin selection is a special case of inclusive fitness, a more general principle that refers simply to the fact that identical gene copies are likely to be found in other subjects that are not necessarily kin [16]. Fitness criteria may show some degree of fluidity; for example, traits that have been transmitted through generations (with adaptive value in the ancestral environment) may lose their fitness importance in the modified modern environment. This phenomenon is called mismatch theory: traits that were adaptive in the ancestral ecosystem may not only use their adaptive value, but even have pathological consequence in the modern ecosystem [17] (for example an efficient energy storage system, essential in the ancestral environment, may lead to metobolic syndroms in the western modern environment).
The purpose of this narrative review is to present several non-mutually exclusive hypotheses that propose adaptive mechanisms that lead to suicidal behavior.

2. The Altruistic Suicide Hypothesis

De Catanzaro developed a mathematical formula on the basis of the inclusive fitness theory and the concept of reproductive value to express the residual capacity of an individual to influence his inclusive fitness. This formula involves “the individual's remaining reproductive potential in his expected natural lifetime plus the summated impacts of his continued existence on the remaining reproductive potential of each of his kin, each weighted by the coefficient of relationship” [18]. This measure (i.e., the residual capacity to influence ones inclusive fitness) is believed to predict the degree to which self-preservation is optimally expressed. For highly social species, such as humans, it has been shown that the association of burdensomeness toward kin and low residual reproductive potential can lead to negative values [18].
Altruistic self-destructive behavior has been found in a wide variety of organisms, ranging from unicellular organisms [1] and parasites [19] to social insects [20,21,22], and is debated in mammals [23]. Self-destructive defensive behavior has evolved independently in a number of social insect species in response to natural enemies. Self-sacrificing behavior has mainly been found in the context of nest protection [24]. Defensive self-sacrifice persists in social insects because of its positive impact on the fitness of the colony’s reproductive individuals. As with other forms of altruistic traits, the genes involved will pass on to subsequent generations. Social insects’ suicidal defense behaviors can be divided into the following three main categories [24]: (1) instantaneous defense—the enemy is actively engaged, with a fatal outcome for the defender; (2) preemptive suicidal defensive behavior—death is the price of the repair or occultation of the nest, that can occur preventively, even before the confrontation to the enemy; and (3) altruistic self-removal—sick and most likely infected individuals leave the nest, therefore reducing the risk of contaminating nestmates.
Although culture and pathology clearly bear on human suicide, some of its emotional concomitants could reflect an erosion of self-preservative genetic expression in conjunction with low reproductive potential and burdensomeness toward kin [25]. Social isolation and perceived burdensomeness toward family has been correlated with suicidal ideation [25]. Moreover, some subjects justify their suicidal behavior as a sacrifice to help their relatives [26,27]. Historically, suicide among Inuits was considered to be altruistic. Old or very ill adults would kill themselves or be killed, typically at their own request, during times of famine during poor hunting [28]. This differs from suicide martyrdom, such as performed by the kamikaze or the Hezbollah, which is believed to be primarily motivated by parochial altruism in the form of moral commitments to collective sacred values rather than by selective incentives such as financial compensation to their family [29,30]. It has been shown that parochial altruist groups have higher reproductive success than groups with fewer parochial altruists [31]. However, one could also hypothesize that in some cultural contexts, suicide martyrdom is the only avenue to suicide that does not damage afterlife prospects (i.e., suicide is expressly forbidden in some religions) [32].

3. The Bargaining Hypothesis

It has been proposed that suicidal behaviors are costly cries for help that can be functional for those enmeshed in interpersonal conflicts [33,34]. It may be a warning strategy for subjects to signal to their social group in which they have a conflict that it may suffer costs if assistance or change is not provided [26]. It could function as an honest signal of need, motivating people with a pre-existing interest in helping an individual who honestly signals the need for help, but not those who falsely exaggerate their need [33]. In this view, suicide attempts are gambles. Needy individuals win if they survive and receive the help needed; however, they lose in the case of a fatal outcome. The suicidal signaling strategy could have evolved if the average benefits received by genes coding for the warning strategy exceed the average costs. Important for this hypothesis is the fact that most suicide attempts are nonfatal [35].

4. The Parasite Manipulation Hypothesis

Parasite-induced modifications of host behavior are known from a wide range of host-parasite associations [36]. In many cases, these behavioral changes are considered to be adaptive and benefit the parasite by increasing its probability of successful transmission [37]. Food chain transmission of parasites is typical of intermediate host manipulations, i.e., the parasite is immature in the intermediate host; in order to attain maturity, and to achieve its life cycle, it must be ingested by a predatory definitive host [38,39].
Some parasites induce suicide-like behaviors in hosts [36]. Schistocephalus solidus-infected fish approach the water surface and are inattentive to predatory birds that serve as the parasite’s definitive hosts. A particular nematode must be transmitted from ants to fruit-eating birds; infected ants develop an inflated, red abdomen, looking like fruit, and perch motionlessly among red berries, waiting for predation by birds; terrestrial arthropods seek and fatally jump into water when infected by a nematode that needs to complete its cycle in aquatic worms. Such parasite manipulations engage the host in “suicidal” behaviors, for the sole parasite reproductive benefit.
The host-parasite system Rattus novergicus-Toxoplasma gondii provides an interesting model for a behavior manipulation that could affect humans. T. gondii is an intracellular protozoan that is capable of infecting all mammals. It has an indirect life cycle. The parasites definitive hosts are the felids (cat family); they are the only mammals emit T. gondii oocysts through their feces. When another mammal such as a wild rodent ingests oocysts, cysts grow in the intermediate host, most frequently in the brain, and are viable for the life of the host [38].
Several experimental studies have shown that T. gondii has evolved the capacity to turn the innate fear that rodents have of cats into a “suicidal” feline attraction. Whereas non-infected rats display a strong aversion to cat odor, infected rats are specifically attracted to cat odor [38,40,41,42]. In addition, T. gondii causes an increase in activity and a decrease in neophobic (the innate fear of novelty) and predator vigilance behavioral traits [43,44,45,46]. This is believed to increase the likelihood of the rodent being predated, thereby completing the parasite's life cycle. Other rodent fears do not seem to be altered [38,39,40,47]. Although T. gondii cysts are observed in a variety of brain regions within rats, the density of cysts may be significantly higher in the amygdala, a brain region that is important for both defensive behaviors and the conditioning of fear, and in the prefrontal cortex [42,48]. Changes in dopamine metabolism are believed to mediate the altered behavior [49]. Anti-T. gondii drugs and antipsychotic treatments of infected rats prevent host-specificity behavioral alterations from being displayed [40,41].
The prevalence levels of T. gondii infection are very high across species. For example, the prevalence is 56% in domestic and wildcats, 43% in lions, 20%–60% in wild rodents, and 13%–67% in wild birds [39,50]. In humans, the seroprevalence of T. gondii infection increases with age, does not vary greatly between sexes, and is lower in cold regions, hot and arid areas, or at high elevations. In general, incidence of the infection varies with the population group and geographic location. Seropositivity can be up to 75% by the fourth decade of life in El Salvador vs. an overall seroprevalence of 23% in the USA [51]. Primary infection with T. gondii is typically subclinical. Infection may however be seriously hazardous in immunocompromised patients or if acquired during pregnancy [52]. Mental disorders, mainly schizophrenia, have been related to latent infection with T. gondii [53,54,55,56,57]. Behavioral alterations (often subtle) that are similar to those observed in T. gondii-infected rodents have been observed in latently infected humans, such as increased activity, decreased reaction times and altered personality profiles [39,58,59].
Latent infection by T. gondii has been linked to a higher risk of suicidal behavior in humans [57,60,61,62,63,64,65]. T. gondii infection is also linked to a significantly increased risk of car accidents [66]. Interestingly, it has been reported that more than 2% of traffic accidents are suicidal behaviors [67]. Large felids (tigers, leopards, lions) were the primary mammalian predators of humans in the 20th century [68,69], and these species (along with other large felids that co-existed in our ancestral environment) are likely to have been significant predators of early humans. The inference that T. gondii manipulates human behavior as it manipulates rodents to increase the likelihood to being predated by its definitive host is speculative. However, we have displayed a set of arguments that lead to the hypothesis that T. gondii may manipulate human behavior to increase suicidality, thus serving an evolutionary purpose (Table 1).

5. Suicide Risk Factors in Light of Evolutionary Psychology

Although burdensomeness to kin may provide a plausible explanation for some suicides among the elderly, it could be more difficult to accept that suicide may enhance the fitness of relatives in the case of a healthy adolescent. Instead, such suicides are likely to be non adaptive byproducts of evolved mechanisms that malfunction [10].

5.1. Psychiatric Disorders

Psychological autopsies of suicide have shown that a context of mental disorder is present in the vast majority of cases. Some aversive states are the product of natural selection. For instance, pain, fever, nausea are known to be useful responses in some circumstances. Likewise, the aversiveness of negative emotional states can also be useful. Many adaptive functions have been suggested for low mood or depression: facilitating disengagement from unreachable responsibility, expressing a need for help, and signaling the act of yielding in a hierarchy conflict [70]. An increased fitness may result from sadness, pessimism and lack of motivation, in the way that they lead to the inhibition of certain behaviors, for example actions in the absence of a practicable plan, dangerous or futile challenges to dominant figures, efforts that would eventually damage the organism [70]. Crying elicits empathy, comforting behaviors in observers and may help strengthen bonds and result in an increase in social support. Feelings of worthlessness and guilt might motivate introspective understanding of how its actions were problematic and may also elicit forgiveness from others involved in the situation [71].
Prominent cognitive theories propose that negative cognitive biases, characterized by a tendency to preferentially process negatively valenced information, play a central role in depression [72]. Clinical and subclinical levels of depression are associated with difficulty in disengaging attention from negative self-relevant stimuli and attentional avoidance of positive stimuli [73]. Thus, although depression may have adaptive features in some circumstances, the negative cognitive bias that accompanies this condition is likely to result in an overvaluation of one’s perceived burdensomeness and, therefore, increase the risk of altruistic suicidal behavior (Table 1).
Even without the specific cognitive bias found in depressive disorders, the emotional distress that accompanies most psychiatric disorders is likely to increase perceived burdensomeness to some extent.
Table 1. Whose genes may benefit from the suicidal behavior?
Table 1. Whose genes may benefit from the suicidal behavior?
Suicidal Behavior Type Ancestral EcosystemWestern Modern Ecosystem
Physical Disorders Kin’s genes could benefit from the suicidal behavior of an individual imposing high burdensomeness and having low reproductive potentialAs burdensomeness is shared by social welfare, virtually no genes benefit from the suicidal behavior
Depression No genes directly benefit from the suicidal behavior. Suicide is a by-product of negative cognitive bias (overevaluation of perceived burdensomeness)No genes directly benefit from the suicidal behavior: no significant change in our current environment
Suicide Martyrdom Ethnic group genes could benefit from a parochial altruistic suicidal behaviorEthnic group genes: no significant change in our current environment
Bargaining Suicidal BehaviorThe suicidal individual’s genes could benefit if the suicide fails and the help needed providedThe suicidal individual’s genes: no significant change in our current environment
Males Impulsive Suicidal Behavior No genes directly benefit from the suicidal behavior. Suicide is a by-product of risk-takingNo genes directly benefit from the suicidal behavior: however risk-taking is likely to be less beneficial to the genes involved in the western modern than in the ancestral ecosystem
Parasite ManipulationT. gondii genes benefit from the suicidal behavior No genes benefit anymore from the suicidal behavior (some exceptions might occur in India and Africa *)
Note: * Large felids (tigers, leopards, lions) were the primary mammalian predators of humans in the 20th century, especially in India and Africa [68].

5.2. Physical Disorders

Severe physical disorders are obvious conditions in which burdensomeness is increased, especially when the disorders are chronic and disabling. According to the pathogen host defense theory, risk alleles for depression originated and have been retained in the human genome because these alleles promote pathogen host defense, which includes a suite of immunological and behavioral responses to infection [74]. One of these responses could be self-removal from kin to prevent the transmission of infection [75] (Table 1).This phenomenon has been observed in other social animals [20,24].

5.3. Gender

One of the most robust patterns in suicide is that men commit suicide more frequently than do women [2,3]. Sexual selection and the subsequent differential parental investment of the two sexes have shaped the manner in which men and women engage in intra-sexual competitions within the mating arena. Given that men face greater variance in reproductive fitness, they have evolved a strong penchant for risk-taking across numerous domains, such as physical and financial domains. Risk-taking can be construed as an adaptive response to the stronger sexual selection pressures faced by men [9]. Risk-taking is supported by some personality traits such as impulsivity-aggressiveness, which predisposes individuals to suicide, especially young men [76]. Impulsivity, which is believed to enhance inclusive fitness by increasing readiness to fight or flee in dangerous circumstances [26], may have been adaptive in the environment of evolutionary adaptedness [77,78] (Table 1). Compared to women, the stronger sexual selection pressures faced by men yield a more pronounced assortment of “winners” and “losers” in the mating field. Accordingly, some suicides can be conceived as the most extreme form of defeatism in the context of mating competition [9]. Men are more likely to commit suicide as a result of loss of status (employment, wealth, marriage), whereas women are more likely to do so because of romantic, emotive or familial concerns [79,80]. It has been hypothesized that social defeats that result in one’s degradation within the relevant dominance hierarchy can be sufficiently injurious to lead to suicide [9]. Interestingly, it has been recently shown that local sex ratio, a proxy for mating market conditions, is strongly linked to male suicide [80].

6. Conclusions

Although not all of the accumulated findings on suicide are within the purview of evolutionary theory, many are congruent with Darwinian-based expectations. We have shown that some features are compatible with the view that suicide can have adaptive values. In particular, the altruistic suicide hypothesis proposes that when both poor reproductive potential and perceived burdensomeness are present, self-removal can increase inclusive fitness. This ultimate explanation of increased suicide rates observed with senescence, psychiatric disorders, physical disorders, and homosexuality is compatible with the interpersonal theory of suicide proposed by van Orden et al. [8]. This theory, with no reference to evolutionary mechanisms, postulates that suicidal desire is particularly hazardous when caused by the simultaneous presence of two interpersonal constructs—thwarted belongingness and perceived burdensomeness. Our evolutionary views harbours the cognitive-behavioral interventions based on this model, e.g., restructuring of irrational negative beliefs and cognitive distortions related to burdensomeness over-perception, social skills training to decrease isolation. The bargaining hypothesis of suicide also proposes an adaptive value of suicidal behavior. Interestingly, in some circumstances, suicidal behavior may not be adaptive for the subject displaying suicidal behavior, but for the parasite it hosts. The higher suicide rate observed in males can be understood as a byproduct of mating competition that results in high-risk taking behavior and the struggle to increase one’s social status.
Our economic and cultural landscape has evolved exponentially. Individuals cooperate on an increasing scale and rely less on kin than in the past. We believe that the social welfare developed in many countries dramatically altered the adaptive value of suicidal behavior, which can be understood today as a mismatch: following our environmental change, the adaptive value of suicide has mainly been lost (Table 1).
In his Letter to the American People, the Surgeon General David Satcher asserted that suicide should be viewed as a serious public health problem [81]. Prevention efforts should be reinforced at the population level and in high-risk groups. Unless psychological models of human health behavior are infused with the evolutionary paradigm, they may risk the continued delivery of programs that offer only brief, suboptimal accounts of its causes [82]. A deeper understanding of the adaptive significance of suicide could aid in improving prevention strategies, notably through reducing perceived burdensomeness and perceived defeatism in the mating arena.

Conflicts of Interest

Henri-Jean Aubin has received in the last three years sponsorship to attend scientific meetings, speaker honoraria and consultancy fees from Pfizer (Paris, France), Lundbeck (Issy-les Moulineaux, France), D&A Pharma (Paris, France), and Merck-Serono (Lyon, France). Ivan Berlin has received during the last three years sponsorship to attend scientific meetings, speaker honoraria and consultancy fees from Pfizer Ltd. (Paris, France). Charles Kornreich declares no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Nedelcu, A.M.; Driscoll, W.W.; Durand, P.M.; Herron, M.D.; Rashidi, A. On the paradigm of altruistic suicide in the unicellular world. Evolution 2011, 65, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Hawton, K.; van Heeringen, K. Suicide. Lancet 2009, 373, 1372–1381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Pitman, A.; Krysinska, K.; Osborn, D.; King, M. Suicide in young men. Lancet 2012, 379, 2383–2392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. McGuffin, P.; Marusic, A.; Farmer, A. What can psychiatric genetics offer suicidology? Crisis 2001, 22, 61–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Mann, J.J. Neurobiology of suicidal behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2003, 4, 819–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. van Heeringen, K. Stress-diathesis Model of Suicidal Behavior. In The Neurobiological Basis of Suicide; Dwivedi, Y., Ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  7. Fowler, J.C.; Hilsenroth, M.J.; Groat, M.; Biel, S.; Biedermann, C.; Ackerman, S. Risk factors for medically serious suicide attempts: Evidence for a psychodynamic formulation of suicidal crisis. J. Amer. Psychoanal. Assn. 2012, 60, 555–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. van Orden, K.A.; Witte, T.K.; Cukrowicz, K.C.; Braithwaite, S.R.; Selby, E.A.; Joiner, T.E., Jr. The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 117, 575–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Saad, G. Suicide triggers as sex-specific threats in domains of evolutionary import: Negative correlation between global male-to-female suicide ratios and average per capita gross national income. Med. Hypotheses 2007, 68, 692–696. [Google Scholar]
  10. Confer, J.C.; Easton, J.A.; Fleischman, D.S.; Goetz, C.D.; Lewis, D.M.; Perilloux, C.; Buss, D.M. Evolutionary psychology: Controversies, questions, prospects, and limitations. Amer. Psychol. 2010, 65, 110–126. [Google Scholar]
  11. Tooby, J.; Cosmides, L. Conceptual Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology. In The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology; Buss, D., Ed.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  12. Laland, K.N.; Brown, G.R. Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cosmides, L.; Tooby, J. Cognitive Adaptations for Social Exchange. In The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture; Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  14. Pinker, S. The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature; Penguin Books: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  15. Rachlin, H. Altruism and selfishness. Behav. Brain Sci. 2002, 25, 239–250. [Google Scholar]
  16. West, S.A.; Griffin, A.S.; Gardner, A. Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Raubenheimer, D.; Simpson, S.J.; Tait, A.H. Match and mismatch: Conservation physiology, nutritional ecology and the timescales of biological adaptation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 2012, 367, 1628–1646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. de Catanzaro, D. Evolutionary limits to self-preservation. Ethology Sociobiol. 1991, 12, 13–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Pompili, M.; Serafini, G.; Innamorati, M.; Montebovi, F.; Palermo, M.; Campi, S.; Stefani, H.; Giordano, G.; Telesforo, L.; Amore, M.; et al. Car accidents as a method of suicide: A comprehensive overview. Forensic Sci. Int. 2012, 223, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Reece, S.E.; Pollitt, L.C.; Colegrave, N.; Gardner, A. The meaning of death: Evolution and ecology of apoptosis in protozoan parasites. PLoS Pathog. 2011, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Rueppell, O.; Hayworth, M.K.; Ross, N.P. Altruistic self-removal of health-compromised honey bee workers from their hive. J. Evol. Biol. 2010, 23, 1538–1546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Sobotnik, J.; Bourguignon, T.; Hanus, R.; Demianova, Z.; Pytelkova, J.; Mares, M.; Foltynova, P.; Preisler, J.; Cvacka, J.; Krasulova, J.; et al. Explosive backpacks in old termite workers. Science 2012, 337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Jones, T.H.; Clark, D.A.; Edwards, A.A.; Davidson, D.W.; Spande, T.F.; Snelling, R.R. The chemistry of exploding ants, Camponotus spp. (cylindricus complex). J. Chem. Ecol. 2004, 30, 1479–1492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Ramsden, E.; Wilson, D. The nature of suicide: Science and the self-destructive animal. Endeavour 2010, 34, 21–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Shorter, J.R.; Rueppell, O. A review on self-destructive defense behaviors in social insects. Insect. Soc. 2012, 59, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. de Catanzaro, D. Reproductive status, family interactions, and suicidal ideation: Surveys of the general public and high-risk groups. Ethology Sociobiol. 1995, 16, 385–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Blasco-Fontecilla, H.; Lopez-Castroman, J.; Gomez-Carrillo, A.; Baca-Garcia, E. Towards an evolutionary framework of suicidal behavior. Med. Hypotheses 2009, 73, 1078–1079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Nolle, A.P.; Gulbas, L.; Kuhlberg, J.A.; Zayas, L.H. Sacrifice for the sake of the family: Expressions of familism by latina teens in the context of suicide. Amer. J. Orthopsychiat. 2012, 82, 319–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kral, M.J. Postcolonial suicide among Inuit in arctic Canada. Cult. Med. Psychiat. 2012, 36, 306–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ginges, J.; Atran, S.; Sachdeva, S.; Medin, D. Psychology out of the laboratory: The challenge of violent extremism. Amer. Psychol. 2011, 66, 507–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ginges, J.; Atran, S. What motivates participation in violent political action: Selective incentives or parochial altruism? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009, 1167, 115–123. [Google Scholar]
  32. Choi, J.K.; Bowles, S. The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science 2007, 318, 636–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Gearing, R.E.; Lizardi, D. Religion and suicide. J. Relig. Health 2009, 48, 332–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Watson, P.J.; Andrews, P.W. Toward a revised evolutionary adaptationist analysis of depression: The social navigation hypothesis. J. Affect. Disorders 2002, 72, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hagen, E.H. Evolutionary theories of depression: A critical review. Can. J. Psychiatry 2011, 56, 716–726. [Google Scholar]
  36. Borges, G.; Nock, M.K.; Abad, J.M.H.; Hwang, I.; Sampson, N.A.; Alonso, J.; Andrade, L.H.; Angermeyer, M.C.; Beautrais, A.; Bromet, E.; et al. Twelve-month prevalence of and risk factors for suicide attempts in the World Health Organization world mental health SURVEYS. J. Clin. Psychiat. 2010, 71, 1617–1628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lagrue, C.; Poulin, R. Manipulative parasites in the world of veterinary science: Implications for epidemiology and pathology. Vet. J. 2010, 184, 9–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Poulin, R. The evolution of parasite manipulation of host behaviour: A theoretical analysis. Parasitology 1994, 109, S109–S118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Berdoy, M.; Webster, J.P.; Macdonald, D.W. Fatal attraction in rats infected with Toxoplasma gondii. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2000, 267, 1591–1594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Webster, J.P. Rats, cats, people and parasites: The impact of latent toxoplasmosis on behaviour. Microbes Infect. 2001, 3, 1037–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Lamberton, P.H.; Donnelly, C.A.; Webster, J.P. Specificity of the Toxoplasma gondii-altered behaviour to definitive vs. non-definitive host predation risk. Parasitology 2008, 135, 1143–1150. [Google Scholar]
  42. Webster, J.P.; Lamberton, P.H.; Donnelly, C.A.; Torrey, E.F. Parasites as causative agents of human affective disorders? The impact of anti-psychotic, mood-stabilizer and anti-parasite medication on Toxoplasma gondii’s ability to alter host behaviour. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2006, 273, 1023–1030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Vyas, A.; Kim, S.K.; Giacomini, N.; Boothroyd, J.C.; Sapolsky, R.M. Behavioral changes induced by Toxoplasma infection of rodents are highly specific to aversion of cat odors. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 6442–6447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hutchison, W.M.; Aitken, P.P.; Wells, W.P. Chronic Toxoplasma infections and familiarity-novelty discrimination in the mouse. Ann. Trop. Med. Parasitol. 1980, 74, 145–150. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hutchinson, W.M.; Bradley, M.; Cheyne, W.M.; Wells, B.W.; Hay, J. Behavioural abnormalities in Toxoplasma-infected mice. Ann. Trop. Med. Parasitol. 1980, 74, 337–345. [Google Scholar]
  46. Hay, J.; Aitken, P.P.; Hair, D.M.; Hutchison, W.M.; Graham, D.I. The effect of congenital Toxoplasma infection on mouse activity and relative preference for exposed areas over a series of trials. Ann. Trop. Med. Parasitol. 1984, 78, 611–618. [Google Scholar]
  47. Hay, J.; Hutchison, W.M.; Aitken, P.P.; Graham, D.I. The effect of congenital and adult-acquired Toxoplasma infections on activity and responsiveness to novel stimulation in mice. Ann. Trop. Med. Parasitol. 1983, 77, 483–495. [Google Scholar]
  48. House, P.K.; Vyas, A.; Sapolsky, R. Predator cat odors activate sexual arousal pathways in brains of Toxoplasma gondii infected rats. Plos One 2011, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Vyas, A.; Kim, S.K.; Sapolsky, R.M. The effects of toxoplasma infection on rodent behavior are dependent on dose of the stimulus. Neuroscience 2007, 148, 342–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Webster, J.P.; McConkey, G.A. Toxoplasma gondii-altered host behaviour: Clues as to mechanism of action. Folia Parasitol. 2010, 57, 95–104. [Google Scholar]
  51. Kamga-Waladjo, A.R.; Gbati, O.B.; Kone, P.; Lapo, R.A.; Dombou, E.; Chatagnon, G.; Bakou, S.N.; Diop, P.E.H.; Pangui, L.J.; Tainturier, D.; et al. Neospora caninum and Toxoplasma gondii in Lion (Panthera leo) from Senegal, West Africa. Asian J. Anim. Vet. Adv. 2009, 4, 346–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Jones, J.L.; Kruszon-Moran, D.; Wilson, M.; McQuillan, G.; Navin, T.; McAuley, J.B. Toxoplasma gondii infection in the United States: Seroprevalence and risk factors. Amer. J. Epidemiol. 2001, 154, 357–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Montoya, J.G.; Liesenfeld, O. Toxoplasmosis. Lancet 2004, 363, 1965–1976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Fekadu, A.; Shibre, T.; Cleare, A.J. Toxoplasmosis as a cause for behaviour disorders—Overview of evidence and mechanisms. Folia Parasitol. 2010, 57, 105–113. [Google Scholar]
  55. Pedersen, M.G.; Stevens, H.; Pedersen, C.B.; Norgaard-Pedersen, B.; Mortensen, P.B. Toxoplasma infection and later development of schizophrenia in mothers. Amer. J. Psychiat. 2011, 168, 814–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Mortensen, P.B.; Norgaard-Pedersen, B.; Waltoft, B.L.; Sorensen, T.L.; Hougaard, D.; Torrey, E.F.; Yolken, R.H. Toxoplasma gondii as a risk factor for early-onset schizophrenia: Analysis of filter paper blood samples obtained at birth. Biol. Psychiat. 2007, 61, 688–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Torrey, E.F.; Bartko, J.J.; Lun, Z.R.; Yolken, R.H. Antibodies to Toxoplasma gondii in patients with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bull. 2007, 33, 729–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Henriquez, S.A.; Brett, R.; Alexander, J.; Pratt, J.; Roberts, C.W. Neuropsychiatric disease and Toxoplasma gondii infection. Neuroimmunomodulation 2009, 16, 122–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Flegr, J.; Hrdy, I. Influence of chronic toxoplasmosis on some human personality factors. Folia Parasitol. 1994, 41, 122–126. [Google Scholar]
  60. Flegr, J. Effects of toxoplasma on human behavior. Schizophrenia Bull. 2007, 33, 757–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Arling, T.A.; Yolken, R.H.; Lapidus, M.; Langenberg, P.; Dickerson, F.B.; Zimmerman, S.A.; Balis, T.; Cabassa, J.A.; Scrandis, D.A.; Tonelli, L.H. Toxoplasma gondii antibody titers and history of suicide attempts in patients with recurrent mood disorders. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2009, 197, 905–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Okusaga, O.; Langenberg, P.; Sleemi, A.; Vaswani, D.; Giegling, I.; Hartmann, A.M.; Konte, B.; Friedl, M.; Groer, M.W.; Yolken, R.H.; et al. Toxoplasma gondii antibody titers and history of suicide attempts in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Res. 2011, 133, 150–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Pedersen, M.G.; Mortensen, P.B.; Norgaard-Pedersen, B.; Postolache, T.T. Toxoplasma gondii infection and self-directed violence in Mothers Toxoplasma gondii and self-directed violence. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 2012, 69, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  64. Ling, V.J.; Lester, D.; Mortensen, P.B.; Langenberg, P.W.; Postolache, T.T. Toxoplasma gondii seropositivity and suicide rates in women. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2011, 199, 440–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Yagmur, F.; Yazar, S.; Temel, H.O.; Cavusoglu, M. May Toxoplasma gondii increase suicide attempt-preliminary results in Turkish subjects? Forensic Sci. Int. 2010, 199, 15–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Zhang, Y.; Traskman-Bendz, L.; Janelidze, S.; Langenberg, P.; Saleh, A.; Constantine, N.; Okusaga, O.; Bay-Richter, C.; Brundin, L.; Postolache, T.T. Toxoplasma gondii immunoglobulin G antibodies and nonfatal suicidal self-directed violence. J. Clin. Psychiat. 2012, 73, 1069–1076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Flegr, J.; Havlicek, J.; Kodym, P.; Maly, M.; Smahel, Z. Increased risk of traffic accidents in subjects with latent toxoplasmosis: A retrospective case-control study. BMC Infect. Dis. 2002, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Loe, J.; Roskaft, E. Large carnivores and human safety: A review. Ambio 2004, 33, 283–288. [Google Scholar]
  69. Seidensticker, J.; McDougal, C. Tiger predatory behaviour, ecology and conservation. Symposium Zool. Soc. London 1993, 65, 105–125. [Google Scholar]
  70. Nesse, R.M. Is depression an adaptation? Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 2000, 57, 14–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Keller, M.C.; Nesse, R.M. Is low mood an adaptation? Evidence for subtypes with symptoms that match precipitants. J. Affect. Disorders 2005, 86, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Mathews, A.; MacLeod, C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 1, 167–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Bradley, B.P.; Mogg, K.; Lee, S.C. Attentional biases for negative information in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria. Behav. Res. Ther. 1997, 35, 911–927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Raison, C.L.; Miller, A.H. The evolutionary significance of depression in pathogen host defense (PATHOS-D). Mol. Psychiat. 2012, 18, 15–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Tanaka, M.; Kinney, D.K. An evolutionary hypothesis of suicide: Why it could be biologically adaptive and is so prevalent in certain occupations. Psychol. Rep. 2011, 108, 977–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. McGirr, A.; Renaud, J.; Bureau, A.; Seguin, M.; Lesage, A.; Turecki, G. Impulsive-aggressive behaviours and completed suicide across the life cycle: A predisposition for younger age of suicide. Psychol. Med. 2008, 38, 407–417. [Google Scholar]
  77. Brody, J.F. Evolutionary recasting: ADHD, mania and its variants. J. Affect. Disorders 2001, 65, 197–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Jensen, P.S.; Mrazek, D.; Knapp, P.K.; Steinberg, L.; Pfeffer, C.; Schowalter, J.; Shapiro, T. Evolution and revolution in child psychiatry: ADHD as a disorder of adaptation. J. Amer. Acad. Child. Adolescent Psychiat. 1997, 36, 1672–1679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Zacharakis, C.A.; Hadjivassilis, V.; Madianos, M.G.; Papadimitriou, G.N.; Stefanis, C.N. Suicide in Cyprus 1988–1999. Eur. Psychiat. 2005, 20, 110–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Kuroki, M. The effect of sex ratios on suicide. Health Economics 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. The Surgeon General’s Call to Action to Prevent Suicide; United States Public Health Service: Washington DC, USA, 1999.
  82. Graham, R.G.; Martin, G.I. Health behavior: A darwinian reconceptualization. Amer. J. Prev. Med. 2012, 43, 451–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Aubin, H.-J.; Berlin, I.; Kornreich, C. The Evolutionary Puzzle of Suicide. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10, 6873-6886. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph10126873

AMA Style

Aubin H-J, Berlin I, Kornreich C. The Evolutionary Puzzle of Suicide. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2013; 10(12):6873-6886. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph10126873

Chicago/Turabian Style

Aubin, Henri-Jean, Ivan Berlin, and Charles Kornreich. 2013. "The Evolutionary Puzzle of Suicide" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 10, no. 12: 6873-6886. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph10126873

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop