Next Article in Journal
Causal Relationships Between Iron Deficiency Anemia, Gut Microbiota, and Metabolites: Insights from Mendelian Randomization and In Vivo Data
Next Article in Special Issue
Magnetic Resonance Imaging Predictors of Surgical Difficulty in Transforaminal Endoscopic Lumbar Discectomy for Far-Lateral Disc Herniation Under Local Anesthesia
Previous Article in Journal
Severe Asthma and Active SARS-CoV-2 Infection: Insights into Biologics
Previous Article in Special Issue
Beyond the Surface: Nutritional Interventions Integrated with Diagnostic Imaging Tools to Target and Preserve Cartilage Integrity: A Narrative Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

White Matter Microstructural Abnormalities in Children with Familial vs. Non-Familial Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

1
Department of Biomedical Engineering, New Jersey Institute of Technology, Newark, NJ 07102, USA
2
Graduate School of Biomedical Sciences, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ 07102, USA
3
School of Biomedical Sciences and Engineering, South China University of Technology, Guangzhou International Campus, Guangzhou 511436, China
4
Department of Psychology, Queens College, City University of New York, New York, NY 11367, USA
5
Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, New Jersey Institute of Technology, Newark, NJ 07102, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Biomedicines 2025, 13(3), 676; https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030676
Submission received: 3 January 2025 / Revised: 28 February 2025 / Accepted: 7 March 2025 / Published: 10 March 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Applications of Imaging Technology in Human Diseases)

Abstract

:
Background: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a highly prevalent, heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorder. Methods: This study presents, for the first time, a comprehensive investigation of white matter microstructural differences between familial ADHD (ADHD-F) and non-familial ADHD (ADHD-NF) using advanced diffusion tensor imaging analyses in a large community-based sample. Results: Children with ADHD-F exhibited significantly greater volume in the right anterior thalamic radiations and the left inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus compared to controls, and greater volume in the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus relative to ADHD-NF. The ADHD-NF group showed reduced fractional anisotropy in the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus compared to the controls. In both the ADHD-F and ADHD-NF groups, a greater volume of anterior thalamic radiation significantly contributed to reduced ADHD symptoms. Conclusions: Our findings suggest that white matter microstructural alterations along the frontal-thalamic pathways may play a critical role in hereditary factors among children with ADHD-F and significantly contribute to elevated inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive behaviors in the affected children.

1. Introduction

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a high prevalence, inheritability, and heterogeneity [1]. It is characterized by age-inappropriate inattention and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity for at least 6 months in multiple settings, such as home and school, with three different clinical presentations: predominantly inattentive, predominantly impulsive-hyperactive, or combined [2,3]. The onset of ADHD in children typically occurs before the age of 12, affecting nearly 11.4% of US children aged 3–17, and approximately 15% to 65% of individuals with a childhood diagnosis continue to be affected by this disorder into adulthood [4]. This persistence is reflected either through a continued ADHD diagnosis or by the presence of significant symptoms that do not fully meet the criteria for a clinical diagnosis [5]. ADHD is considered a major public health issue in the US, with an overall incremental cost of USD 143–266 billion. It is associated with multiple health-related behaviors, including addiction, sleep difficulties, anxiety-related issues, mood disorders, increased rates of divorce, higher rates of suicidal ideation and attempts, criminality, academic underachievement, employment challenges, physical injuries, and accidents [6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15]. Identifying etiological risk factors for ADHD, particularly during childhood, could facilitate timely and more effective prevention and interventions for affected children before they enter adolescence, potentially preventing the persistence of ADHD symptoms into adulthood.
Based on numerous investigations, it has been consistently observed that among the complicated and interactive biological and environmental components contributing to ADHD, familial history emerges as one of the most significant and influential risk factors for the emergence and persistence of ADHD [16,17,18]. One study demonstrated that children with a positive family history of ADHD have a significantly higher risk (approximately 5-fold) of developing the condition compared to those without a family history [19]. Another study estimated the heritability of ADHD to be approximately 70% to 90% [20]. Furthermore, studies have reported that relative to those without a family history of ADHD (ADHD-NF), a child with ADHD with a positive family history (ADHD-F) has a 4-fold higher risk for ADHD symptoms to persist into adulthood [21,22]. These findings suggest that ADHD-F may represent a biologically distinct and more homogeneous subgroup of ADHD [23]. Studying common and distinct neural substrates of ADHD-F vs. ADHD-NF could facilitate the development of more tailored early preventions and interventions that target the differential underlying neural anomalies characteristic of ADHD-F and ADHD-NF.
Over the last three decades, existing imaging and clinical/behavioral studies in ADHD without controlling for familial factors have shown widely inconsistent results [24]. Recent studies applying advanced machine learning (ML) and deep learning (DL) techniques have exhibited similar inconsistencies. For example, recent studies that applied advanced ML/DL techniques in the same imaging modality and measurement metrics, and reporting frontal lobe anomalies in ADHD, have shown incongruent results in terms of altered locations, tissue types, and anomaly directions of the metrics [25,26,27,28,29,30]. Similarly, although multiple studies have supported impairments in executive functions (EFs) in ADHD mediated by the prefrontal lobe and frontoparietal circuits [31,32,33], broader investigations have found that only about 50% of those with ADHD were impaired on the most sensitive EF measures (i.e., Stop Signal Reaction Time) [33,34,35]. Without considering the significant impact of the neurobiological heterogeneity of the disorder, decades of research so far have not established any specific neural substrates that are essential to move the field toward neurobiologically targeted diagnoses and interventions of ADHD.
Despite the inconsistencies in the findings of neuroimaging studies in children with ADHD, there remains a significant gap in the literature related to the influence of family history. Only a limited number of clinical and neuroimaging studies have investigated the effects of family history, particularly in siblings or twins, highlighting the need for further research in this area. Nevertheless, neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies controlling for etiological and biological heterogeneities have reported relatively more consistent results. Chen et al. showed that in a sample of 14 monozygotic (MZ) twin pairs, children with ADHD had smaller right striatum and thalamus volumes compared to their co-twins, while no significant change was observed in the cerebral cortex volume [36]. In another neuroanatomical twin study, Castellanos et al. found lower caudate volume in the affected children relative to their unaffected co-twins in nine discordant twin pairs [37]. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), Van’t Ent et al. found that during the Stroop and Flanker task, 27 MZ twin pairs that were highly concordant for attention problems had lower activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal, parietal, and temporal brain regions [38]. Godinez et al. showed lower activations in the frontoparietal network regions in children with ADHD relative to their co-twins [39]. A study investigating neuroanatomical anomalies in individuals with ADHD and their unaffected siblings, compared to groups of matched healthy control subjects, reported alterations in gray matter (GM) volume in the region of the right inferior frontal gyrus [40]. An fMRI study revealed lower functional connectivity within the fronto-striatal network during go/no-go tasks in cohorts with ADHD and their immediate family members in comparison to typically developed controls [41]. These findings suggest that GM neuroanatomical and functional underdevelopment associated with the frontal lobe and its related pathways can be a heritable pattern in individuals with a positive family history of ADHD. However, none of these neural anomalies were necessarily linked to ADHD symptoms in the study samples. More importantly, these small-sample studies, without an independent clinical control group for comparison, could not robustly identify familial risk-specific neurobiological mechanisms in ADHD.
On the other hand, childhood ADHD has recently been characterized by widespread white matter (WM) alterations in the developmental brain [42]. Multiple studies have reported that children with ADHD exhibited significantly abnormal fractional anisotropy (FA) and radial diffusivity in specific brain regions, including the fronto-striatal tracts, the superior longitudinal fasciculus, the cingulum bundle, the cerebral peduncle, the posterior limb of the internal capsule, etc. [43,44,45]. Additionally, multiple other diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies have reported reduced WM volume and FA in the fronto-parietal, fronto-limbic, corona-radiate, and temporo-occipital tracts in children with ADHD [46,47,48,49,50]. While these studies have indicated anomalies in the WM structure in children with ADHD, the findings are again largely inconsistent. Despite the technical variations in data acquisition/analyses and sample-related differences, the etiological heterogeneity of ADHD can play a critical role in WM neurobiological differences in ADHD samples. Unfortunately, the WM microstructural characteristics of children with familial vs. non-familial ADHD have not yet been sufficiently investigated [51].
To address these gaps in the field, the present study aimed to leverage a large sample of three independent groups of children (ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and controls) from the baseline data of the ongoing Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study to investigate the WM microstructural substrates characterizing ADHD-F vs. ADHD-NF. Our recent structural MRI and fMRI studies in independent groups of AHDH-F, ADHD-NF, and control children from the ABCD Study cohort found that children with ADHD-F have some unique patterns of neural alterations, i.e., abnormal GM structural network properties associated with precuneus and paracentral gyrus [52], as well as reduced activation laterality of inferior frontal gyrus during working memory, thalamic, and frontal GM abnormalities [53,54], all associated with elevated ADHD symptoms in ADHD-F. On the basis of the findings from our team and other groups [24,46,52,54,55,56], we hypothesized that relative to matched controls and children with ADHD-NF, preadolescent children with ADHD-F would exhibit WM morphometrical and microstructural deficits in the frontal lobe and the major WM tracts connecting the frontal lobe and other brain regions, particularly the thalamus and thalamus-related pathways. These deficits may potentially affect the functional and behavioral integrity related to attention and play a significant role in the elevated inattentive and/or hyperactive/impulsive symptoms in the affected children.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

For the current study, de-identified clinical and neuroimaging data were obtained from the ABCD Study baseline pool. This 10-year longitudinal investigation is currently the largest prospective study to comprehensively track cognitive brain development and child health in the US. The study initially enrolled 11,785 children (ages 9–10) from a major collaboration between 21 research sites across the country and is following them into early adulthood. The study’s recruitment and design process carefully considered age, sex, and socioeconomic variables to better mirror the US sociodemographic levels to ensure that the sample reflected the nation’s diversity, which significantly strengthens the generalizability of the findings [57]. The data are publicly acquirable and downloadable through the National Institute of Mental Health Data Archive [58]. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board as well as the central IRB at the University of California, San Diego. All the parents provided written informed consent, and informed assent was obtained from the children.
The original exclusion criteria of the ABCD Study baseline enrollment included a current diagnosis of schizophrenia, autism spectrum disorder (moderate or severe), mental retardation/intellectual disability, or alcohol/substance use disorder [58]. Additional exclusion criteria of the present study included a history of traumatic brain injury based on the Modified Ohio State University TBI Screen-Short Version [59], a gestational age below 28 weeks, a birth weight under 1.2 kg, or the use of non-stimulant psychotropic medication within the past three months. Subjects involved in the present study met all inclusion criteria: proficiency in English for the participants and either English or Spanish for their parents, intelligence quotient (IQ) above 80 measured by the NIH Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Task T-score [60], and the provision of complete and high-quality MRI data needed. Approval for the study was not required in accordance with local/national legislation (https://www.ecfr.gov/current/title-28/chapter-I/part-46 (accessed on 6 March 2025)).

2.2. ADHD Assessments

The ADHD diagnoses and symptoms were assessed using parent or guardian responses to the recently validated Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophrenia (KSADS-5) according to the DSM-5 criteria [58,61]. Children who were marked according to the screening questions for a high risk of having an ADHD diagnosis were further administered with the KSADS-5 ADHD supplementary scales for diagnoses. The diagnosis of ADHD was based on the threshold of endorsing at least six out of nine symptoms of inattention (ADHD inattentive presentation), at least six out of nine symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity (ADHD impulsive/hyperactivity presentation), or those who met the criteria in both domains (ADHD combined presentation).
After applying the general inclusion, exclusion, and ADHD symptomatology criteria, children with ADHD were further divided into two distinct groups: ADHD-F and ADHD-NF. The ADHD-NF group comprised 137 children who met at least one ADHD presentation criterion, had no biological parents with a current diagnosis or past diagnosis of ADHD, or significant inattentive and/or hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (as indicated by the T-score exceeding 65 on any of the three DSM-5 ADHD-oriented scales in the Adult Self-Report (ASR)) [62], and who provided high-quality imaging data for analyses. Conversely, the ADHD-F group included 114 children who exhibited at least one ADHD presentation, had at least one biological parent with a current or past ADHD diagnosis or a T-score above 65 on one of the three DSM-5 ADHD-oriented scales based on ASR, and provided high-quality imaging data for analyses. Children with ADHD with unknown family medical conditions or poor imaging quality identified from the preprocessing steps were removed from the study.
Additionally, the third group, serving as the control group, included 150 children from the baseline pool of the ABCD study who did not present any symptoms of ADHD. This group was carefully matched for age, sex, handedness, IQ, and puberty category scale with the other two groups, ADHD-F and ADHD-NF. The detailed inclusion and exclusion steps are shown in Figure 1.

2.3. Demographic, Neurocognitive, and Clinical/Behavioral Measures

In this study, the Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Task (TPVT) was used as a measure of language ability, and an estimate of verbal intellectual was used as the IQ reference [60,63]. The Edinburgh Handedness Inventory was utilized to assess handedness [64,65]. The parent report of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) was used for assessing behavioral and emotional problems [66]. Information about each participant’s medication was collected through the Parent Medication Survey Inventory, in which the parents were asked to provide the names and dosages of all medications their children had taken in the previous two weeks. All other demographic data, including age, sex, race, household income, and parental education, were collected from the demographic survey completed by each participant’s parent or guardian.
As we all know, the period from the pre-pubertal to post-pubertal stages is a critical time of human brain maturational changes, especially involving frontal GM and the WM tracts connecting cortical and subcortical structures [67,68,69,70]. These developmental changes in the human brain can introduce between-subject variability in studies of children with ADHD [68,69,71,72]. Therefore, this study proposed to use both age and the Puberty Category Score (PCS) to account for any imbalances in brain maturational stages across groups. The caregiver’s report of the ABCD Youth Pubertal Development Scale and Menstrual Cycle Survey History was used to assess the PCS [73,74].

2.4. Imaging Data Acquisition Protocol

The ABCD neuroimaging data were acquired at 21 different sites using 3T scanners (either Siemens, General Electric, or Philips, all with a 32-channel head). The neuroimaging data acquisitions and parameters have been previously described in detail [75]. The current study focused on diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI), but two other modalities of imaging, including T1- and T2-weighted structural MRI data, were used in the analyses as well. The following sequence parameters were used in the high angular resolution DWI: Repetition time/Echo time (TR/TE) of 4100/88 (msec) and 4100/81.9 (msec) for Siemens and General Electric, voxel size = 1.7 mm isotropic, 96 diffusion encoding directions, including 6 directions at b = 500 s/mm2, 15 directions at b = 1000 s/mm2, 15 directions at b = 2000 s/mm2, and 60 directions at b = 3000 s/mm2 repeated with inverted phase encoding (AP and PA), and field of view (FOV) = 256 × 256 mm2. T1-weighted structural MRI was acquired using an inversion prepared RF-spoiled gradient echo pulse sequence with the following parameters: Repetition time/Echo time (TR/TE) of 2500/2.88 (ms) and 6.31/2.9 (msec) for Siemens and General Electric, voxel size = 1.0 mm isotropic, field of view (FOV) = 256 × 256 mm2, matrix size = 256 × 256, flip angle = 8 (degree), and slice numbers = 176 and 208 for Siemens and General Electric scanners, respectively.

2.5. Individual-Level Imaging Data Preprocessing

The DWI from all participants was visually checked for any moderate to severe motion or any other artifacts, leading to the exclusion of those with such issues. Additionally, subjects who failed to score at least 3 on a 4-point scale (poor, fair, good, and excellent) for quality check, following the criteria of the Human Connectome Project Structural QC assessing image clarity, blurriness, and motion in T1-weighted structural images, were also removed from further analyses [76]. We used ABCD DTI-processed data. Diffusion MRI preprocessing steps include Eddy current correction [77], head motion correction [78], and spatial and intensity distortion correction [79]. Model-based Eddy current correction was used, focusing on displacements in the phase-encode direction. This approach models these displacements using 12 parameters, adjusting them based on gradient orientation and strength. To avoid errors caused by head motion resulting in dark slices and signal loss, a robust tensor fitting method was used to remove dark slices from the distortion estimation process. Those slices including signal loss (dark slices) were identified by calculating the root mean square (RMS) of residual errors. These values were then compared to the median value for every slice (frames with an RMS above a threshold were excluded from tensor fitting). This fitting process takes three iterations to stabilize, replacing the frame with signal loss (dark frames) with the synthesized images. Then, to optimize the Eddy current correction, Newton’s approach was used to minimize the RMS error between the corrected images and the synthesized images. And finally, following the correction, the tensor was re-estimated, removing the dark frames. This process was iterated five times for precision.
To correct for head motion, each frame was registered to a synthesized post-Eddy current correction volume. Slices with signal loss (dark frames) were excluded and replaced with interpolated values. And finally, the diffusion gradient matrix was adjusted for rotation.
To correct distortions from B0 field inhomogeneity, a robust technique involving phase-encoding polarity reversal is applied by aligning pairs of non-diffusion images with opposite encoding, a fast, nonlinear registration to adjust distortion across all frames [80]. The resulting displacement field is used to adjust each diffusion-weighted frame, followed by gradient nonlinearity correction. Then, the processed images are registered to T1-weighted structural images using mutual information after an initial alignment to a standard brain atlas and resampled to match the acquisition resolution, with adjustments for rotation and translation ensuring that all images fit a standard orientation across participants. Moreover, head rotation is accounted for during this process, and cubic interpolation is applied at each resampling step. And finally, a registration matrix records the rigid-body transformation between the dMRI and T1-weighted images, which allows for consistent diffusion orientations and improved data accuracy for subsequent analysis across the cohort.

2.6. Individual-Level Imaging Data Processing and Analyses

Briefly, AtlasTrack, a probabilistic atlas-based tool, was used to label major WM tracts, including additional pathways such as cortico-striate and cortico-cortical connections [78]. This atlas-based tool incorporates prior probabilities and orientation information for various long-range projection fibers. Structural MRI images for each subject are nonlinearly registered to the atlas using discrete cosine transforms, and the DTI orientations for each subject are compared to the atlas fiber orientations. This alignment process refines the a priori tract location probabilities and individualizes the fiber tract regions of interest (ROIs). Freesurfer’s “recon-all” function is used for automated brain segmentation, including skull stripping, volume registration, intensity normalization, WM segmentation, surface atlas registration, surface extraction, volumetric alignment to the MNI305 atlas, and final labeling of volume [81]. Then, multiple diffusion tensor measures are calculated, including FA and mean diffusivity (MD), using log-transformed diffusion weighted signals, providing important insights into the microstructural properties of the WM tracts [82]. Both the DTI inner shell model, which excludes data from frames with higher b-values (1000 s/mm2), and the DTI full shell, which includes a broader range of gradient strength, were fitted to ensure accurate assessments of the tissue’s microstructural properties across participants.

2.7. Secondary Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS) Analysis

FA maps were processed using FSL (version 5) following a series of steps that have been described in detail in prior studies [83]. Briefly, the FMRIB58_FA template in the MNI common space served as the reference image for non-linear registration of all participants’ FA maps, employing the FNIRT tool (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/docs/#/structural/index (accessed on 6 March 2025)). Then, the transformed FA maps in MNI space were averaged and skeletonized to form a primary WM skeleton for all the participants. A threshold of FA ≥ 0.2 was applied to the averaged FA map to capture the inclusion of key WM tracts while eliminating non-WM ones. And finally, each participants’ aligned FA map was projected onto this skeleton, where each voxel was assigned the maximum FA value from a line perpendicular to the local skeleton. Figure 2 presents the graphical abstract and a summary of the study’s methods for individual-level analyses.

2.8. Group-Level Statistical Analyses

For the demographic, neurocognitive, and behavioral measures reported in Table 1, we performed one-way ANOVA or chi-square tests depending on whether the variable was continuous or categorical. For continuous variables meeting normality (Shapiro–Wilk test) and homogeneity of variances (Levene’s test), one-way ANOVA was used across the three groups (ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and control). Categorical variables were analyzed using chi-square tests when Cochran’s rule was met, and Fisher’s exact test when it was not. For all RxC tables, pairwise comparisons were performed using Fisher’s exact test in addition to chi-square tests, as detailed in Supplementary Table S1. All analyses were conducted in SPSS (IBM Corp., Released 2020, Version 27.0, Armonk, NY, USA (with statistical significance set at p < 0.05)). In our primary ROI-based analyses, a mixed-model analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted to compare the FA and volume of each WM fiber tract among the three groups (ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and control), with age, sex, handedness, IQ, and estimated intracranial volume (ETIV) as covariates. Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (at a corrected α = 0.05) was applied to control for potential false positives in these group comparisons. Brain–behavioral relationships in the ADHD probands were performed using Pearson correlation between the ROI-based WM measures that showed significant between-group differences and the ADHD symptom measures derived from CBCL and ksads-5 (p-values below 0.05 were considered statistically significant). These ADHD symptom measures included the t-score for ADHD-related problems on DSM-oriented CBCL scales and the raw scores for inattentive and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptom scores collected from KSADS-5. To compare the voxel-based maps of fractional anisotropy (FA) in white matter (among the three groups of ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and control), we utilized Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS) analysis with a non-parametric, permutation-based threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) approach in FSL. Specifically, the averaged FA skeleton (with a threshold at FA ≥ 0.2) served as the analysis mask, and we conducted 1000 permutations to build the null distribution. A significance threshold of p < 0.05 (family-wise error [FWE] corrected) was then applied, ensuring that the Type I error rate remained appropriately controlled for multiple comparisons. This TFCE-based FWE correction is a standard procedure in neuroimaging research and helps mitigate the risk of false positives across voxel-wise tests [78,79]. To assess the distributional properties of white matter tract measures, Hartigan’s dip test for unimodality was conducted on all significant white matter tracts across the ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and control groups (using R package “dip.test”) [84].

3. Results

The demographic and clinical characteristics of the study participants are displayed in Table 1. The group comparisons did not reveal any significant between-group differences among the ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and control groups in terms of age, sex, handedness, puberty category score, IQ, race, income, parental education, or ADHD presentations. Regarding medication status, the majority of participants in both the ADHD-F and ADHD-NF groups were either on no medication or on stimulant medication, with no significant differences in medication distribution between the two ADHD subgroups (p = 0.608). A smaller proportion of individuals in these groups were on non-stimulant medication or mixed medications, highlighting a comparable pattern of medication usage across the ADHD-F and ADHD-NF groups.
As displayed in Table 2, there were notable differences in WM tract fractional anisotropy values between the ADHD-F and control groups, particularly in the Forceps major (F(1,285) = 11.03, p = 0.023). Additionally, significant differences in fractional anisotropy were observed between the ADHD-NF and control groups in the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus (F(1,249) = 13.19, p = 0.001), indicating specific structural variations in this tract among the groups.
The WM tracts that exhibited significant between-group differences in volume are outlined in Table 3. The ADHD-F group demonstrated significant differences in the volume of the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus (F(1,249) = 11.34, p = 0.008) and right anterior thalamic radiation (F(1,262) = 13.80, p = 0.001) compared to the ADHD-NF and control groups, respectively. Furthermore, significant volumetric differences were detected in the left anterior thalamic radiation and left inferior-fronto-occipital fasciculus between the ADHD-F group and the control group (F(1,262) = 10.69, p = 0.007 and F(1,262) = 11.10, p = 0.005, respectively). These results highlight distinct structural differences in the WM tracts of children with ADHD-F relative to those with ADHD-NF and the control groups.
In order to visualize the distribution of FA and volume measures in regions that exhibited significant differences between groups (ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and control), we used Raincloud plots. These plots show the distribution of these measures with jittering, median, interquartile range (IQR) boxes, and 95% confidence intervals (CI95). Figure 3 presents these Raincloud plots, which provide a clear depiction of how the ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and control groups compare in terms of FA and volume measures across the significantly different regions. The plots also highlight the spread of the data and include the CI95 intervals, allowing for a better understanding of the variability and precision of these measures across the groups.
Brain–behavioral correlation analyses showed that in both groups of children with ADHD-F and ADHD-NF, higher volumes of the right anterior thalamic radiation (r = −0.193, p = 0.039 ADHD-F; r = −0.176, p = 0.039 for ADHD-NF) and left anterior thalamic radiation (r = −0.206, p = 0.028 for ADHD-F; r = −0.183, p = 0.033 for ADHD-NF) were significantly associated with lower T-score for ADHD-related problems on DSM-oriented CBCL scales (Figure 4).
The secondary whole-brain TBSS analysis revealed no significant differences between the three groups of ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and controls.
Hartigan’s dip test for unimodality revealed no significant evidence of bimodality in any of the significant white matter regions (p > 0.05 for all tests). The full results of the dip test are presented in Supplementary Table S2.

4. Discussion

The present study aimed to explore the WM microstructural substrates linked to symptom manifestations in children with ADHD-F vs. ADHD-NF by leveraging a statistically robust sample of three independent groups—ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and control—drawn from the baseline data pool of the ongoing ABCD Study. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first in the field to assess the common and distinct WM microstructural substrates associated with symptom onset in ADHD-F vs. ADHD-NF.
Compared to matched controls, we found that the ADHD-F group had significantly increased WM volume of both the right and left anterior thalamic radiation tracts. While the ADHD-NF group showed an opposite trend of decreased WM volume anterior thalamic radiation compared to the ADHD-NF and control groups, the wide within-group variance demeaned the significance of the between-group differences. Furthermore, in both the ADHD-F and ADHD-NF groups, the higher WM volumes of both right and left anterior thalamic radiation tracts were significantly correlated with lower scores of ADHD problems based on the DSM-oriented T-score of the CBCL scale. The anterior thalamic radiation fiber bundle includes pathways that connect the mediodorsal thalamic nuclei and the frontal cortical regions, such as the prefrontal cortex, and is part of the fronto-striato-thalamic circuit, which plays a critical role in executive functioning, planning, complex behaviors, and social behaviors [85,86,87]. Substantial structural MRI studies have suggested that children with ADHD exhibit inconsistent anomalies in the anterior thalamic radiation, including alterations in the volume and fractional anisotropy (FA) of this WM tract [55,88,89,90,91]. A number of clinical and multi-modal neuroimaging studies in children with ADHD have demonstrated that structural and functional anomalies associated with the thalamus and frontal lobe can significantly contribute to the emergence of ADHD symptoms [40,55,92,93,94]. Family-based studies of ADHD have reported more convergent findings in terms of the functional and anatomical alternations associated with the frontal-thalamo pathways. For instance, neuroanatomical studies in siblings have reported that, compared to controls, both patients with ADHD and their unaffected siblings exhibit smaller frontal GM volumes [40,95]. Additionally, studies investigating functional and structural pathways have found reduced activation in the frontal regions and associated networks, which correlate with lower WM integrity in the prefrontal cortex [41,96,97]. Taken together with these existing findings, our results suggest that WM disruptions in the frontal-thalamo pathways, along with the GM functional and structural alterations, have a strong heredity role in children with ADHD-F and are significantly linked to symptom onset.
Our results also demonstrated that compared to matched controls, children with ADHD-F exhibited significantly reduced WM volume of the left inferior-fronto-occipital fasciculus. The inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus is a critical structural component of spatial attention. Anatomically, it originates from the posterior area of the occipital lobe, extends through the temporal lobe, and terminates in orbitofrontal and prefrontal cortices, connecting substantial brain regions of the ventral attention network [98,99,100]. Numerous clinical and neuroimaging studies have suggested that abnormalities in the WM tract contribute to behavioral symptoms of inattention in both children and adults with ADHD. For instance, a recent DTI study by Zhou et al. reported significantly reduced FA in children with ADHD compared to healthy control individuals [101], while Tremblay et al. observed a trend of increased FA in children with ADHD compared to controls [102]. Another family-based multi-modal neuroimaging study in ADHD reported significant differences in FA associated with the inferior fronto-occipital and uncinate fasciculi, suggesting a phenotypical correlation and shared genetic determination of the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus WM tract [103]. Conversely, another imaging study using refined DTI tractography and investigating WM microstructure in subjects with ADHD and their unaffected siblings found no significant difference in DTI microstructural measurements of the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus WM tract in children with ADHD, unaffected siblings, and controls [51]. Building on prior research, which has yielded inconsistent findings, our study, conducted in three large and independent groups of subjects, suggests that WM microstructural alterations in the left inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus may have a strong hereditary origin that characterizes a biologically unique long-distance WM connection pattern in children with ADHD-F.
Our results also revealed that the ADHD-NF group exhibited significantly reduced FA in the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus compared to the matched control group. This ventral associative WM tract, which connects the occipital and temporal lobes, plays a crucial role in the ventral semantic system, supporting functions such as visual processing, memory, and language. The observed FA reduction in this tract aligns with findings from previous studies. For instance, Gonzalez-Madruga et al. reported lower FA in the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus among patients with ADHD compared to healthy controls [104], while Svatkova et al. identified higher FA in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus in ADHD cohorts compared to healthy controls [90]. Again, discrepancies in the findings associated with WM abnormality of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus in ADHD can be sourced from technical variations in data acquisition and analyses, differences in sample characteristics, as well as heterogeneity in ADHD. Derived from comparisons in independent groups of ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and controls, our findings suggest that underdeveloped WM structure of the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus may exhibit a more common neuroanatomical profile in children with ADHD who do not carry the family/genetic risk factor. While this neurobiological alteration in ADHD-NF may be linked to other cognitive or behavioral impairments, it does not necessarily play a role in inattention and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity in the affected children that characterize the disorder of ADHD.

5. Limitations and Future Directions

The results of this study should be considered while taking potential limitations into account. One limitation of this study is that only one parent was interviewed and participated in the ADHD-related assessments in the ABCD Study. Detailed medical information on other first-degree family relatives was acquired based on the responses of the interviewed parents to the family history questionnaire. To reduce the potential impact of inaccurate biological parents’ medical history information on our grouping of ADHD-F and ADHD-NF children, we excluded nearly half of the ADHD children originally identified from the ABCD Study baseline pool. Second, the ADHD assessments, based on parent-reported KSADS and supplementary scales, were intended for research purposes and were not clinically verified. Consequently, the findings should be further validated in a clinical sample. In addition, children with ADHD have diverse developmental trajectories and outcomes due to uneven brain development statuses and complex environmental interactions. Therefore, future longitudinal studies are expected to test the role of these early childhood neural markers of ADHD-F vs. ADHD-NF in different developmental stages, such as late adolescence and early adulthood, in affected individuals.

6. Conclusions

In conclusion, together with existing findings, the results of this novel research indicate that WM microstructure in tracts that connect regions of the prefrontal cortex and thalamus may have a strong heredity component in ADHD-F and play a critical role in symptom manifestations in affected children. Meanwhile, disrupted WM microstructures of the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus may represent a common neural deficit in ADHD-NF, which is significantly vulnerable to the environmental risk factors of ADHD. These findings are derived from large independent groups of subjects, providing more than sufficient statistical power for our findings. Future longitudinal research could build upon these insights to further understand the developmental trajectories of these specific neural substrates linked to ADHD-F vs. ADHD-NF.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/biomedicines13030676/s1, Table S1: Pairwise 95% confidence intervals for proportions and Fischer exact test results for categorical variables in Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study cohort. Table S2: Hartigan’s Dip Test results for unimodality assessment across significant white matter tracts. The test was conducted for Fractional Anisotropy and Volume measures in ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and Control groups. The table presents the dip statistic (D) and associated p-value for each group.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.B. and X.L.; methodology, R.B. and X.L.; formal analysis, R.B.; writing—original draft preparation, R.B.; writing—review and editing, R.B., K.W., J.M.H. and X.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was partially supported by research grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (R15MH117368, R01MH126448) and the New Jersey State Department of Health (CBIR17PIL012, CBIR22PIL022).

Informed Consent Statement

All the parents provided written informed consent, and informed assent was obtained from the children.

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data for this work are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org (accessed on 6 March 2025)), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children aged 9–10 years and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html (accessed on 6 March 2025). A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members (accessed on 6 March 2025). The ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data, but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or the ABCD consortium investigators.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

ADHDAttention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
ADHD-FADHD with positive family history
ADHD-NFADHD without a family history of ADHD
MLMachine learning
DLDeep learning
EFsExecutive functions
MZMonozygotic
GMGray matter
WMWhite matter
FAFractional anisotropy
DTIDiffusion tensor imaging
ABCDAdolescent Brain Cognitive Development
IQIntelligence quotient
KSADS-5Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophrenia
ASRAdult Self-Report
TVPTToolbox Picture Vocabulary Task
PCSPuberty Category Score
CBCLChild Behavior Checklist
DWIDiffusion-weighted imaging
TRRepetition time
TEEcho time
FOVField of view
RMSroot mean squared
MDMean diffusivity
TBSSTract-based spatial statistics
TFCEThreshold-free cluster enhancement

References

  1. Faraone, S.V.; Perlis, R.H.; Doyle, A.E.; Smoller, J.W.; Goralnick, J.J.; Holmgren, M.A.; Sklar, P. Molecular Genetics of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 57, 1313–1323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Barkley, R.A. Major life activity and health outcomes associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2002, 63, 10–15. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  3. Edition, F. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Am. Psychiatr. Assoc. 2013, 21, 591–643. [Google Scholar]
  4. Polanczyk, G.V.; Willcutt, E.G.; Salum, G.A.; Kieling, C.; Rohde, L.A. ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: An updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2014, 43, 434–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Faraone, S.V.; Biederman, J.; Mick, E. The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychol. Med. 2006, 36, 159–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Doshi, J.A.; Hodgkins, P.; Kahle, J.; Sikirica, V.; Cangelosi, M.J.; Setyawan, J.; Erder, M.H.; Neumann, P.J. Economic impact of childhood and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the United States. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2012, 51, 990–1002.e1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Chang, Z.; Lichtenstein, P.; Larsson, H. The effects of childhood ADHD symptoms on early-onset substance use: A Swedish twin study. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2012, 40, 425–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Dalsgaard, S.; Mortensen, P.B.; Frydenberg, M.; Thomsen, P.H. ADHD, stimulant treatment in childhood and subsequent substance abuse in adulthood—A naturalistic long-term follow-up study. Addict. Behav. 2014, 39, 325–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Ogundele, M.O.; Yemula, C. Management of sleep disorders among children and adolescents with neurodevelopmental disorders: A practical guide for clinicians. World J. Clin. Pediatr. 2022, 11, 239–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. León-Barriera, R.; Ortegon, R.S.; Chaplin, M.M.; Modesto-Lowe, V. Treating ADHD and Comorbid Anxiety in Children: A Guide for Clinical Practice. Clin. Pediatr. 2023, 62, 39–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Dorani, F.; Bijlenga, D.; Beekman, A.T.F.; van Someren, E.J.W.; Kooij, J.J.S. Prevalence of hormone-related mood disorder symptoms in women with ADHD. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2021, 133, 10–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Faraone, S.V.; Asherson, P.; Banaschewski, T.; Biederman, J.; Buitelaar, J.K.; Ramos-Quiroga, J.A.; Rohde, L.A.; Sonuga-Barke, E.J.; Tannock, R.; Franke, B. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 2015, 1, 15020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Dalsgaard, S.; Leckman, J.F.; Mortensen, P.B.; Nielsen, H.S.; Simonsen, M. Effect of drugs on the risk of injuries in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A prospective cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry 2015, 2, 702–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Curry, A.E.; Yerys, B.E.; Metzger, K.B.; Carey, M.E.; Power, T.J. Traffic Crashes, Violations, and Suspensions Among Young Drivers With ADHD. Pediatrics 2019, 143, e20182305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Wymbs, B.T.; Pelham, W.E., Jr.; Molina, B.S.; Gnagy, E.M.; Wilson, T.K.; Greenhouse, J.B. Rate and predictors of divorce among parents of youths with ADHD. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2008, 76, 735–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Larsson, H.; Asherson, P.; Chang, Z.; Ljung, T.; Friedrichs, B.; Larsson, J.O.; Lichtenstein, P. Genetic and environmental influences on adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms: A large Swedish population-based study of twins. Psychol. Med. 2013, 43, 197–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Uchida, M.; Spencer, T.J.; Faraone, S.V.; Biederman, J. Adult Outcome of ADHD: An Overview of Results From the MGH Longitudinal Family Studies of Pediatrically and Psychiatrically Referred Youth With and Without ADHD of Both Sexes. J. Atten. Disord. 2018, 22, 523–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Bonvicini, C.; Faraone, S.V.; Scassellati, C. Common and specific genes and peripheral biomarkers in children and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 2018, 19, 80–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Biederman, J.; Faraone, S.V.; Keenan, K.; Knee, D.; Tsuang, M.T. Family-genetic and psychosocial risk factors in DSM-III attention deficit disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1990, 29, 526–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Thapar, A.; Cooper, M.; Eyre, O.; Langley, K. Practitioner Review: What have we learnt about the causes of ADHD? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2013, 54, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Johnston, B.A.; Mwangi, B.; Matthews, K.; Coghill, D.; Konrad, K.; Steele, J.D. Brainstem abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder support high accuracy individual diagnostic classification. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014, 35, 5179–5189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Riglin, L.; Collishaw, S.; Thapar, A.K.; Dalsgaard, S.; Langley, K.; Smith, G.D.; Stergiakouli, E.; Maughan, B.; O’Donovan, M.C.; Thapar, A. Association of Genetic Risk Variants With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Trajectories in the General Population. JAMA Psychiatry 2016, 73, 1285–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Faraone, S.V.; Biederman, J.; Friedman, D. Validity of DSM-IV subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A family study perspective. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2000, 39, 300–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Luo, Y.; Weibman, D.; Halperin, J.M.; Li, X. A Review of Heterogeneity in Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Makris, N.; Biederman, J.; Valera, E.M.; Bush, G.; Kaiser, J.; Kennedy, D.N.; Caviness, V.S.; Faraone, S.V.; Seidman, L.J. Cortical thinning of the attention and executive function networks in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17, 1364–1375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Zhang-James, Y.; Helminen, E.C.; Liu, J.; Group, E.-A.W.; Franke, B.; Hoogman, M.; Faraone, S.V. Evidence for similar structural brain anomalies in youth and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A machine learning analysis. Transl. Psychiatry 2021, 11, 82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Luo, Y.; Alvarez, T.L.; Halperin, J.M.; Li, X. Multimodal neuroimaging-based prediction of adult outcomes in childhood-onset ADHD using ensemble learning techniques. Neuroimage Clin. 2020, 26, 102238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Yao, D.; Guo, X.; Zhao, Q.; Liu, L.; Cao, Q.; Wang, Y.; Calhoun, V.D.; Sun, L.; Sui, J. Discriminating ADHD From Healthy Controls Using a Novel Feature Selection Method Based on Relative Importance and Ensemble Learning. Annu. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2018, 2018, 4632–4635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Riaz, A.; Asad, M.; Alonso, E.; Slabaugh, G. DeepFMRI: End-to-end deep learning for functional connectivity and classification of ADHD using fMRI. J. Neurosci. Methods 2020, 335, 108506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Chen, Y.; Tang, Y.; Wang, C.; Liu, X.; Zhao, L.; Wang, Z. ADHD classification by dual subspace learning using resting-state functional connectivity. Artif. Intell. Med. 2020, 103, 101786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Willcutt, E.G.; Nigg, J.T.; Pennington, B.F.; Solanto, M.V.; Rohde, L.A.; Tannock, R.; Loo, S.K.; Carlson, C.L.; McBurnett, K.; Lahey, B.B. Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2012, 121, 991–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. O’Neill, S.; Rajendran, K.; Mahbubani, S.M.; Halperin, J.M. Preschool Predictors of ADHD Symptoms and Impairment During Childhood and Adolescence. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2017, 19, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Curchack-Lichtin, J.T.; Chacko, A.; Halperin, J.M. Changes in ADHD symptom endorsement: Preschool to school age. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2014, 42, 993–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Overgaard, K.R.; Oerbeck, B.; Friis, S.; Pripp, A.H.; Aase, H.; Biele, G.; Ingeborgrud, C.B.; Polanczyk, G.V.; Zeiner, P. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder from preschool to school age: Change and stability of parent and teacher reports. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2022, 32, 1947–1955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Luciana, M.; Bjork, J.M.; Nagel, B.J.; Barch, D.M.; Gonzalez, R.; Nixon, S.J.; Banich, M.T. Adolescent neurocognitive development and impacts of substance use: Overview of the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) baseline neurocognition battery. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 67–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Chen, Y.C.; Sudre, G.; Sharp, W.; Donovan, F.; Chandrasekharappa, S.C.; Hansen, N.; Elnitski, L.; Shaw, P. Neuroanatomic, epigenetic and genetic differences in monozygotic twins discordant for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 2018, 23, 683–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Castellanos, F.X.; Sharp, W.S.; Gottesman, R.F.; Greenstein, D.K.; Giedd, J.N.; Rapoport, J.L. Anatomic Brain Abnormalities in Monozygotic Twins Discordant for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2003, 160, 1693–1696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Van’t Ent, D.; van Beijsterveldt, C.E.; Derks, E.M.; Hudziak, J.J.; Veltman, D.J.; Todd, R.D.; Boomsma, D.I.; De Geus, E.J. Neuroimaging of response interference in twins concordant or discordant for inattention and hyperactivity symptoms. Neuroscience 2009, 164, 16–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Godinez, D.A.; Willcutt, E.G.; Burgess, G.C.; Depue, B.E.; Andrews-Hanna, J.R.; Banich, M.T. Familial risk and ADHD-specific neural activity revealed by case-control, discordant twin pair design. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2015, 233, 458–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Pironti, V.A.; Lai, M.C.; Muller, U.; Dodds, C.M.; Suckling, J.; Bullmore, E.T.; Sahakian, B.J. Neuroanatomical abnormalities and cognitive impairments are shared by adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected first-degree relatives. Biol. Psychiatry 2014, 76, 639–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Casey, B.J.; Epstein, J.N.; Buhle, J.; Liston, C.; Davidson, M.C.; Tonev, S.T.; Spicer, J.; Niogi, S.; Millner, A.J.; Reiss, A.; et al. Frontostriatal Connectivity and Its Role in Cognitive Control in Parent-Child Dyads With ADHD. Am. J. Psychiatry 2007, 164, 1729–1736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Connaughton, M.; Whelan, R.; O’Hanlon, E.; McGrath, J. White matter microstructure in children and adolescents with ADHD. Neuroimage Clin. 2022, 33, 102957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Gau, S.; Tseng, W.-L.; Tseng, W.-Y.; Wu, Y.-H.; Lo, Y.-C. Association between microstructural integrity of frontostriatal tracts and school functioning: ADHD symptoms and executive function as mediators. Psychol. Med. 2015, 45, 529–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Chiang, H.-L.; Chen, Y.-J.; Shang, C.-Y.; Tseng, W.-Y.; Gau, S.-F. Different neural substrates for executive functions in youths with ADHD: A diffusion spectrum imaging tractography study. Psychol. Med. 2016, 46, 1225–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Tung, Y.-H.; Lin, H.-Y.; Chen, C.-L.; Shang, C.-Y.; Yang, L.-Y.; Hsu, Y.-C.; Tseng, W.-Y.I.; Gau, S.S.-F. Whole brain white matter tract deviation and idiosyncrasy from normative development in autism and ADHD and unaffected siblings link with dimensions of psychopathology and cognition. Am. J. Psychiatry 2021, 178, 730–743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Aoki, Y.; Cortese, S.; Castellanos, F.X. Research Review: Diffusion tensor imaging studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Meta-analyses and reflections on head motion. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2018, 59, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Chiang, H.L.; Tseng, W.I.; Tseng, W.L.; Tung, Y.H.; Hsu, Y.C.; Chen, C.L.; Gau, S.S. Atypical development in white matter microstructures in ADHD: A longitudinal diffusion imaging study. Asian J. Psychiatr. 2023, 79, 103358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Qiu, M.-g.; Ye, Z.; Li, Q.-y.; Liu, G.-j.; Xie, B.; Wang, J. Changes of brain structure and function in ADHD children. Brain Topogr. 2011, 24, 243–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Peterson, D.J.; Ryan, M.; Rimrodt, S.L.; Cutting, L.E.; Denckla, M.B.; Kaufmann, W.E.; Mahone, E.M. Increased regional fractional anisotropy in highly screened attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). J. Child Neurol. 2011, 26, 1296–1302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Hoogman, M.; Bralten, J.; Hibar, D.P.; Mennes, M.; Zwiers, M.P.; Schweren, L.S.J.; van Hulzen, K.J.E.; Medland, S.E.; Shumskaya, E.; Jahanshad, N.; et al. Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: A cross-sectional mega-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2017, 4, 310–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Lawrence, K.E.; Levitt, J.G.; Loo, S.K.; Ly, R.; Yee, V.; O’Neill, J.; Alger, J.; Narr, K.L. White matter microstructure in subjects with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their siblings. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2013, 52, 431–440.e434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Baboli, R.; Cao, M.; Martin, E.; Halperin, J.M.; Wu, K.; Li, X. Distinct structural brain network properties in children with familial versus non-familial attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Cortex 2024, 179, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Li, X.; Motwani, C.; Cao, M.; Martin, E.; Halperin, J.M. Working Memory-Related Neurofunctional Correlates Associated with the Frontal Lobe in Children with Familial vs. Non-Familial Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 1469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Baboli, R.; Cao, M.; Halperin, J.M.; Li, X. Distinct Thalamic and Frontal Neuroanatomical Substrates in Children with Familial vs. Non-Familial Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Brain Sci. 2022, 13, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Xia, S.; Li, X.; Kimball, A.E.; Kelly, M.S.; Lesser, I.; Branch, C. Thalamic shape and connectivity abnormalities in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2012, 204, 161–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Saad, J.F.; Griffiths, K.R.; Korgaonkar, M.S. A Systematic Review of Imaging Studies in the Combined and Inattentive Subtypes of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Garavan, H.; Bartsch, H.; Conway, K.; Decastro, A.; Goldstein, R.Z.; Heeringa, S.; Jernigan, T.; Potter, A.; Thompson, W.; Zahs, D. Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 16–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Barch, D.M.; Albaugh, M.D.; Avenevoli, S.; Chang, L.; Clark, D.B.; Glantz, M.D.; Hudziak, J.J.; Jernigan, T.L.; Tapert, S.F.; Yurgelun-Todd, D.; et al. Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: Rationale and description. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 55–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Bogner, J.A.; Whiteneck, G.G.; MacDonald, J.; Juengst, S.B.; Brown, A.W.; Philippus, A.M.; Marwitz, J.H.; Lengenfelder, J.; Mellick, D.; Arenth, P.; et al. Test-Retest Reliability of Traumatic Brain Injury Outcome Measures: A Traumatic Brain Injury Model Systems Study. J. Head. Trauma. Rehabil. 2017, 32, E1–E16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Gershon, R.C.; Wagster, M.V.; Hendrie, H.C.; Fox, N.A.; Cook, K.F.; Nowinski, C.J. NIH toolbox for assessment of neurological and behavioral function. Neurology 2013, 80, S2–S6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Kobak, K.A.; Kratochvil, C.; Stanger, C.; Kaufman, J. Computerized screening of comorbidity in adolescents with substance or psychiatric disorders. Presented at the 33rd Annual Anxiety Disorders and Depression Conference, La Jolaa, CA, USA, 6 April 2013. [Google Scholar]
  62. Achenbach, T.M.; Verhulst, F. Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA); University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families: Burlington, VT, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  63. Gershon, R.C.; Cook, K.F.; Mungas, D.; Manly, J.J.; Slotkin, J.; Beaumont, J.L.; Weintraub, S. Language measures of the NIH toolbox cognition battery. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2014, 20, 642–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Oldfield, R.C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9, 97–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Veale, J.F. Edinburgh handedness inventory–short form: A revised version based on confirmatory factor analysis. Laterality Asymmetries Body Brain Cogn. 2014, 19, 164–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Achenbach, T.M.; Ruffle, T.M. The Child Behavior Checklist and related forms for assessing behavioral/emotional problems and competencies. Pediatr. Rev. 2000, 21, 265–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Constantinidis, C.; Luna, B. Neural Substrates of Inhibitory Control Maturation in Adolescence. Trends Neurosci. 2019, 42, 604–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Gogtay, N.; Giedd, J.N.; Lusk, L.; Hayashi, K.M.; Greenstein, D.; Vaituzis, A.C.; Nugent, T.F., 3rd; Herman, D.H.; Clasen, L.S.; Toga, A.W.; et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2004, 101, 8174–8179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Tamnes, C.K.; Herting, M.M.; Goddings, A.L.; Meuwese, R.; Blakemore, S.J.; Dahl, R.E.; Guroglu, B.; Raznahan, A.; Sowell, E.R.; Crone, E.A.; et al. Development of the Cerebral Cortex across Adolescence: A Multisample Study of Inter-Related Longitudinal Changes in Cortical Volume, Surface Area, and Thickness. J. Neurosci. 2017, 37, 3402–3412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Goddings, A.L.; Roalf, D.; Lebel, C.; Tamnes, C.K. Development of white matter microstructure and executive functions during childhood and adolescence: A review of diffusion MRI studies. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2021, 51, 101008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Baron Nelson, M.; O’Neil, S.H.; Wisnowski, J.L.; Hart, D.; Sawardekar, S.; Rauh, V.; Perera, F.; Andrews, H.F.; Hoepner, L.A.; Garcia, W.; et al. Maturation of Brain Microstructure and Metabolism Associates with Increased Capacity for Self-Regulation during the Transition from Childhood to Adolescence. J. Neurosci. 2019, 39, 8362–8375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Moura, L.M.; Crossley, N.A.; Zugman, A.; Pan, P.M.; Gadelha, A.; Del Aquilla, M.A.G.; Picon, F.A.; Anes, M.; Amaro, E., Jr.; de Jesus Mari, J.; et al. Coordinated brain development: Exploring the synchrony between changes in grey and white matter during childhood maturation. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017, 11, 808–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Carskadon, M.A.; Acebo, C. A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development. J. Adolesc. Health 1993, 14, 190–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Herting, M.M.; Uban, K.A.; Gonzalez, M.R.; Baker, F.C.; Kan, E.C.; Thompson, W.K.; Granger, D.A.; Albaugh, M.D.; Anokhin, A.P.; Bagot, K.S. Correspondence between perceived pubertal development and hormone levels in 9–10 year-olds from the adolescent brain cognitive development study. Front. Endocrinol. 2021, 11, 549928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Casey, B.J.; Cannonier, T.; Conley, M.I.; Cohen, A.O.; Barch, D.M.; Heitzeg, M.M.; Soules, M.E.; Teslovich, T.; Dellarco, D.V.; Garavan, H.; et al. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 43–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Marcus, D.S.; Harms, M.P.; Snyder, A.Z.; Jenkinson, M.; Wilson, J.A.; Glasser, M.F.; Barch, D.M.; Archie, K.A.; Burgess, G.C.; Ramaratnam, M.; et al. Human Connectome Project informatics: Quality control, database services, and data visualization. Neuroimage 2013, 80, 202–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Zhuang, J.; Hrabe, J.; Kangarlu, A.; Xu, D.; Bansal, R.; Branch, C.A.; Peterson, B.S. Correction of eddy-current distortions in diffusion tensor images using the known directions and strengths of diffusion gradients. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging Off. J. Int. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. 2006, 24, 1188–1193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Hagler, D.J., Jr.; Ahmadi, M.E.; Kuperman, J.; Holland, D.; McDonald, C.R.; Halgren, E.; Dale, A.M. Automated white-matter tractography using a probabilistic diffusion tensor atlas: Application to temporal lobe epilepsy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 30, 1535–1547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Morgan, P.S.; Bowtell, R.W.; McIntyre, D.J.; Worthington, B.S. Correction of spatial distortion in EPI due to inhomogeneous static magnetic fields using the reversed gradient method. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2004, 19, 499–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Holland, D.; Kuperman, J.M.; Dale, A.M. Efficient correction of inhomogeneous static magnetic field-induced distortion in Echo Planar Imaging. Neuroimage 2010, 50, 175–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Fischl, B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 2012, 62, 774–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Alexander, A.L.; Lee, J.E.; Lazar, M.; Field, A.S. Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics 2007, 4, 316–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Smith, S.M.; Jenkinson, M.; Woolrich, M.W.; Beckmann, C.F.; Behrens, T.E.; Johansen-Berg, H.; Bannister, P.R.; De Luca, M.; Drobnjak, I.; Flitney, D.E.; et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 2004, 23 (Suppl. 1), S208–S219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Hartigan, J.A.; Hartigan, P.M. The dip test of unimodality. Ann. Stat. 1985, 13, 70–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Masterman, D.L.; Cummings, J.L. Frontal-subcortical circuits: The anatomic basis of executive, social and motivated behaviors. J. Psychopharmacol. 1997, 11, 107–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Tekin, S.; Cummings, J.L. Frontal–subcortical neuronal circuits and clinical neuropsychiatry: An update. J. Psychosom. Res. 2002, 53, 647–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Biesbroek, J.M.; Kuijf, H.J.; van der Graaf, Y.; Vincken, K.L.; Postma, A.; Mali, W.P.T.M.; Biessels, G.J.; Geerlings, M.I.; on behalf of the SMART Study Group. Association between Subcortical Vascular Lesion Location and Cognition: A Voxel-Based and Tract-Based Lesion-Symptom Mapping Study. The SMART-MR Study. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e60541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Bouziane, C.; Caan, M.W.A.; Tamminga, H.G.H.; Schrantee, A.; Bottelier, M.A.; de Ruiter, M.B.; Kooij, S.J.J.; Reneman, L. ADHD and maturation of brain white matter: A DTI study in medication naive children and adults. Neuroimage Clin. 2018, 17, 53–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Hung, Y.; Dallenbach, N.T.; Green, A.; Gaillard, S.; Capella, J.; Hoskova, B.; Vater, C.H.; Cooper, E.; Rudberg, N.; Takahashi, A.; et al. Distinct and shared white matter abnormalities when ADHD is comorbid with ASD: A preliminary diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Res. 2023, 320, 115039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Svatkova, A.; Nestrasil, I.; Rudser, K.; Goldenring Fine, J.; Bledsoe, J.; Semrud-Clikeman, M. Unique white matter microstructural patterns in ADHD presentations-a diffusion tensor imaging study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016, 37, 3323–3336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Ercan, E.S.; Suren, S.; Bacanlı, A.; Yazıcı, K.U.; Callı, C.; Ardic, U.A.; Aygunes, D.; Kosova, B.; Ozyurt, O.; Aydın, C.; et al. Altered structural connectivity is related to attention deficit/hyperactivity subtypes: A DTI study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2016, 256, 57–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Shaw, P.; Sudre, G.; Wharton, A.; Weingart, D.; Sharp, W.; Sarlls, J. White matter microstructure and the variable adult outcome of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015, 40, 746–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Cortese, S.; Imperati, D.; Zhou, J.; Proal, E.; Klein, R.G.; Mannuzza, S.; Ramos-Olazagasti, M.A.; Milham, M.P.; Kelly, C.; Castellanos, F.X. White matter alterations at 33-year follow-up in adults with childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2013, 74, 591–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Mills, K.L.; Bathula, D.; Dias, T.G.; Iyer, S.P.; Fenesy, M.C.; Musser, E.D.; Stevens, C.A.; Thurlow, B.L.; Carpenter, S.D.; Nagel, B.J.; et al. Altered cortico-striatal-thalamic connectivity in relation to spatial working memory capacity in children with ADHD. Front. Psychiatry 2012, 3, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Bralten, J.; Greven, C.U.; Franke, B.; Mennes, M.; Zwiers, M.P.; Rommelse, N.N.; Hartman, C.; van der Meer, D.; O’Dwyer, L.; Oosterlaan, J. Voxel-based morphometry analysis reveals frontal brain differences in participants with ADHD and their unaffected siblings. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. JPN 2016, 41, 272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Mulder, M.J.; Baeyens, D.; Davidson, M.C.; Casey, B.J.; Van Den Ban, E.; Van Engeland, H.; Durston, S. Familial vulnerability to ADHD affects activity in the cerebellum in addition to the prefrontal systems. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2008, 47, 68–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Rapoport, J.L.; Shaw, P. Defining the contribution of genetic risk to structural and functional anomalies in ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2008, 47, 2–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Sarubbo, S.; De Benedictis, A.; Maldonado, I.L.; Basso, G.; Duffau, H. Frontal terminations for the inferior fronto-occipital fascicle: Anatomical dissection, DTI study and functional considerations on a multi-component bundle. Brain Struct. Funct. 2013, 218, 21–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Hattori, T.; Ito, K.; Nakazawa, C.; Numasawa, Y.; Watanabe, M.; Aoki, S.; Mizusawa, H.; Ishiai, S.; Yokota, T. Structural connectivity in spatial attention network: Reconstruction from left hemispatial neglect. Brain Imaging Behav. 2018, 12, 309–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Martino, J.; Brogna, C.; Robles, S.G.; Vergani, F.; Duffau, H. Anatomic dissection of the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus revisited in the lights of brain stimulation data. Cortex 2010, 46, 691–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Zhou, R.; Dong, P.; Chen, S.; Qian, A.; Tao, J.; Zheng, X.; Cheng, J.; Yang, C.; Huang, X.; Wang, M. The long-range white matter microstructural alterations in drug-naive children with ADHD: A tract-based spatial statistics study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2022, 327, 111548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Tremblay, L.K.; Hammill, C.; Ameis, S.H.; Bhaijiwala, M.; Mabbott, D.J.; Anagnostou, E.; Lerch, J.P.; Schachar, R.J. Tracking Inhibitory Control in Youth With ADHD: A Multi-Modal Neuroimaging Approach. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 00831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Sudre, G.; Choudhuri, S.; Szekely, E.; Bonner, T.; Goduni, E.; Sharp, W.; Shaw, P. Estimating the Heritability of Structural and Functional Brain Connectivity in Families Affected by Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 2017, 74, 76–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. González-Madruga, K.; Staginnus, M.; Fairchild, G. Alterations in Structural and Functional Connectivity in ADHD: Implications for Theories of ADHD. In New Discoveries in the Behavioral Neuroscience of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; Stanford, S.C., Sciberras, E., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2022; pp. 445–481. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the study cohort. ABCD: Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development; ADHD-F: familial ADHD; ADHD-NF: non-familial ADHD.
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the study cohort. ABCD: Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development; ADHD-F: familial ADHD; ADHD-NF: non-familial ADHD.
Biomedicines 13 00676 g001
Figure 2. Overview of the study methods.
Figure 2. Overview of the study methods.
Biomedicines 13 00676 g002
Figure 3. Raincloud plots of the (a) fractional anisotropy and (b) volume measures in white matter tracts showing significant group differences among the ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and control groups. The half-violins illustrate each group’s data distribution, while the boxes depict the median and the 25th and 75th percentiles. Whiskers extend to 1.5 times the interquartile range beyond the box boundaries and cover 95% of observed data.
Figure 3. Raincloud plots of the (a) fractional anisotropy and (b) volume measures in white matter tracts showing significant group differences among the ADHD-F, ADHD-NF, and control groups. The half-violins illustrate each group’s data distribution, while the boxes depict the median and the 25th and 75th percentiles. Whiskers extend to 1.5 times the interquartile range beyond the box boundaries and cover 95% of observed data.
Biomedicines 13 00676 g003
Figure 4. Regions that showed significant brain–behavior correlations in the ADHD-F and ADHD-NF groups. Each scatter plot depicts the relationship between the DSM-oriented ADHD T-score and the corresponding volumetric measure, along with a regression line and a 95% confidence interval (shaded band).
Figure 4. Regions that showed significant brain–behavior correlations in the ADHD-F and ADHD-NF groups. Each scatter plot depicts the relationship between the DSM-oriented ADHD T-score and the corresponding volumetric measure, along with a regression line and a 95% confidence interval (shaded band).
Biomedicines 13 00676 g004
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study cohort (continuous variables as mean [±95% CI]; categorical variables as N (%) [CI range]).
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study cohort (continuous variables as mean [±95% CI]; categorical variables as N (%) [CI range]).
ADHD-F
Mean (±95% CI) or N
ADHD-NF
Mean (±95% CI) or N (%) [CI Range]
Control
Mean (±95% CI) or N (%) [CI Range]
F or χ2p-Value
Age (months)119.90 [118.51, 121.29]118.07 [116.77, 119.37]119.16 [117.97, 120.35]1.910.148
Sex: 2.540.280
   Female38 (34) [24.6, 42.0]59 (43) [36.8, 49.4]60 (40) [32.2, 47.8]
   Male76 (66) [58.0, 75.4]78 (57) [50.6, 63.2]90 (60) [52.2, 67.8]
Handedness: 2.400.662
   Right-Handed86 (76) [ 67.5, 83.37]110 (80) [73.6, 86.9]124 (83) [76.7, 88.7]
   Left-Handed7 (6) [1.7, 10.6]8 (6) [1.9, 9.8]8 (5) [1.7, 9.3]
   Both-Handed21 (18) [11.3, 25.6]19 (14) [8.1, 19.6]18 (12) [6.8, 17.2]
Puberty category score: -0.548
   Pre-Pubertal65 (57) [47.9, 66.1]80 (58) [50.1, 66.7]91 (61) [52.8, 68.5]
   Early-Pubertal30 (26) [18.2, 34.4]26 (19) [12.4, 25.6]36 (24) [17.2, 30.8]
   Mid-Pubertal18 (16) [9.0, 22.5]30 (22) [14.9, 28.8]21 (14) [8.5, 19.5]
   Late-Pubertal1 (1) [0.0, 2.6]1 (1) [0.0, 2.1]2 (1) [0.0, 3.2]
IQ (Picture Vocabulary)109.85 [106.29, 113.42]105.39 [102.41, 108.37]107.43 [104.87, 109.99]2.020.133
Race: 1.720.943
   Caucasian85 (75) [67.0, 82.2]101 (74) [66.1, 81.3]112 (75) [67.5, 81.9]
   African-American11 (10) [4.3, 14.9]18 (13) [7.4, 18.8]14 (9) [4.6, 14.0]
   More than one race12 (10) [5.0, 16.1]12 (9) [4.0, 13.6]15 (10) [5.2, 14.8]
   Other races6 (5) [1.2, 9.4]6 (4) [0.9, 7.9]9 (6) [2.2, 9.8]
Annual income: 6.500.164
   <USD 50,00051 (45) [35.7, 53.6]44 (32) [24.3, 39.9]60 (40) [32.2, 47.8]
   USD 50,000–10,000024 (21) [3.7, 28.5]28 (20) [16.5, 32.3]24 [(16) [10.2, 21.8]
   >USD 100,00039 (34) [25.3, 43.1]65 (48) [39.3, 55.5]66 (44) [36.4, 51.6]
Parental education: 6.120.633
   No high school diploma4 (4) [0.1, 6.9]8 (6) [1.8, 9.8]11 (7) [3.0, 11.5]
   High school diploma6 (5) [1.2, 9.4]9 (7) [2.4, 10.8]16 (11) [5.7, 15.7]
   Some college40 (35) [26.3, 43.8]45 (33) [24.7, 40.9]47 (31) [23.8, 38.8]
   Bachelor’s degree31 (27) [19.0, 35.5]42 (31) [22.7, 38.6]42 (28) [20.5, 35.7]
   Graduate degree33 (29) [20.6, 37.3]33 (24) [16.9, 31.4]34 (23) [15.7, 29.7]
Medication status: -0.619
   No medication81 (71) [62.7, 79.3]104 (76) [68.5, 83.3]-
   Stimulant medication31 (27) [18.6, 35.8]29 (21) [14.0, 28.4]-
   No stimulant medication1 (1) [0.0, 2.6]3 (2) [0.0, 4.7]-
   Mixed medications1 (1) [0.0, 2.6]1 (1) [0.0, 2.1]-
ADHD symptom presentation: 1.970.373
   Inattentive48 (42) [32.9, 51.3]48 (35) [27.0, 43.0]-
   Hyperactive-Impulsive12 (11) [4.8, 16.1]21 (15) [9.1, 21.6]-
   Combined54 (47) [37.9, 56.9]68 (50) [41.3, 58.0]-
ADHD-F: familial ADHD; ADHD-NF: non-familial ADHD; IQ: intelligence quotient; CI: confidence Interval. Continuous variables (age, IQ) were compared by one-way ANOVA; categorical variables were compared by chi-square if Cochran’s rule was met, or Fisher’s exact (Freeman–Halton) if not.
Table 2. White matter tracts that showed significant between-group differences in fractional anisotropy with age, sex, handedness, IQ, and ETIV controlled for confounding factors.
Table 2. White matter tracts that showed significant between-group differences in fractional anisotropy with age, sex, handedness, IQ, and ETIV controlled for confounding factors.
White Matter TractFractional Anisotropyp-Value After Bonferroni Correction
ADHD-F
(±95% CI)
ADHD-NF
(±95% CI)
Control
(±95% CI)
ADHD-F
vs.
ADHD-NF
ADHD-F
vs.
Control
ADHD-NF
vs.
Control
Left inferior longitudinal fasciculus0.492 (0.487, 0.496)0.486 (0.482, 0.491)0.499 (0.495, 0.503)0.1450.0760.001
Forceps major0.631 (0.626, 0.637)0.633 (0.628, 0.639)0.642 (0.638, 0.647)0.3760.0230.057
ADHD-F: familial ADHD; ADHD-NF: non-familial ADHD; IQ: intelligence quotient; ETIV: estimated intracranial volume; CI: confidence interval.
Table 3. White matter tracts that showed significant between-group differences in volume with age, sex, handedness, IQ, and ETIV controlled for confounding factors.
Table 3. White matter tracts that showed significant between-group differences in volume with age, sex, handedness, IQ, and ETIV controlled for confounding factors.
White Matter TractVolumep-Value After Bonferroni Correction
ADHD-F
(±95% CI)
ADHD-NF
(±95% CI)
Control
(±95% CI)
ADHD-F
vs.
ADHD-NF
ADHD-F
vs.
Control
ADHD-NF
vs.
Control
Left inferior longitudinal fasciculus13,529 (13,157, 13,902)12,796 (12,506, 13,086)13,571 (13,288, 13,854)0.0080.1500.064
Right anterior thalamic radiations13,273 (12,950, 13,596)12,843 (12,607, 13,061)13,123 (12,872, 13,374)0.1880.0010.193
Left anterior thalamic radiations13,639 (13,309, 13,969)13,202 (12,945, 13,459)13,553 (13,287, 13,818)0.2090.0070.283
Left inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus14,643 (14,309, 14,977)14,202 (13,942, 14,463)14,599 (14,338, 14,860)0.3050.0050.567
ADHD-F: familial ADHD; ADHD-NF: non-familial ADHD; IQ: intelligence quotient; ETIV: estimated intracranial volume; CI: confidence interval.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Baboli, R.; Wu, K.; Halperin, J.M.; Li, X. White Matter Microstructural Abnormalities in Children with Familial vs. Non-Familial Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Biomedicines 2025, 13, 676. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030676

AMA Style

Baboli R, Wu K, Halperin JM, Li X. White Matter Microstructural Abnormalities in Children with Familial vs. Non-Familial Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Biomedicines. 2025; 13(3):676. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030676

Chicago/Turabian Style

Baboli, Rahman, Kai Wu, Jeffrey M. Halperin, and Xiaobo Li. 2025. "White Matter Microstructural Abnormalities in Children with Familial vs. Non-Familial Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)" Biomedicines 13, no. 3: 676. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030676

APA Style

Baboli, R., Wu, K., Halperin, J. M., & Li, X. (2025). White Matter Microstructural Abnormalities in Children with Familial vs. Non-Familial Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Biomedicines, 13(3), 676. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines13030676

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop