1. Introduction
It is common for occupational health and safety (H&S) research to discuss the dynamic environments of high-risk workplaces, including mining, construction, forestry, oil and gas, and transportation. Although the ongoing presence of hazards is real, in some ways, the risks and hazards present in a workplace are fairly static. For example, hazards consistently identified at mine operations include slips, trips, and falls, as well as electrical issues, guarding, or noise and dust exposure [
1,
2,
3].
Despite a specific hazard or group of hazards being constant in many mine environments, workers often are not. Rather, general occupational research has found that the workforce is constantly changing due to issues such as employee turnover, new people coming in, or job reassignment [
4]. These workforce issues are particularly prevalent in industries that have suffered a higher turnover for the past number of years, such as mining, which will continue as baby boomers retire [
5]. In response, understanding the knowledge, perceptions, and behaviors of workers, based on any individual differences, may be important for identifying the likelihood of preventing and introducing risks on the job for certain subgroups of workers. In this case, we focus on workers’ experience levels in different job tasks, with their company, and within the industry as individual factors of interest.
To date, even the largest meta-analyses have revealed mixed findings pertaining to the relationship of job tenure and job performance, with more attention being focused on the risks that new employees bring to the job site [
6]. Specifically, nearly a century’s worth of research has consistently shown that workers new to their profession are more likely to be injured on the job than those with longer job tenures [
7,
8,
9]. A commonly referenced landmark study by Leigh and colleagues [
8] asserted that length of time on the job (tenure) was negatively associated with accidents (i.e., new employees had a higher accident rate). Similarly, research has also confirmed that job tenure can be positively related to task performance over time [
10].
Older studies have shown similar results in coal mining, specifically, where job tenure was found to be a significant factor in accidents that occurred between 1976 and 1977 [
11]. These researchers found that 40% of reported injuries had occurred during workers’ first year of employment. More recent research by Groves and colleagues [
12] used data from the Mine Safety and Health Administration (MSHA) between 1995 and 2004 to conclude that 28% of injuries and 31% of fatalities occurred among employees who were in their first year on the job. MSHA is the regulatory entity for U.S. mining, mandating specific Codes of Federal Regulations (CFRs) which organizations must follow. Similarly, Kecojevic and colleagues [
13] analyzed the relationship between work-related fatalities and worker experience to find that 44% of workers killed in equipment-related incidents had less than five years of mining experience.
Although these numbers seem convincing, there are other factors to consider. First, the statistics can be recontextualized. For example, Kecojevic’s results [
13] could communicate that 56% of those workers killed in equipment-related incidents had more than five years of mining experience, indicating that this more experienced population is overlooked when considering specific risk management interventions. Specifically, many studies do not examine the spectrum of the workforce and often focus on workers who tend to be inexperienced (e.g., under one year on the job). One research study showed a strong inverse relationship between job tenure and injury claim rates, but job tenure only ranged from 0 to 13 months [
14]. Importantly, these inferences are all made based on reported incidents, whereas we know that underreporting is a problem in high-risk occupations [
15]. In light of these issues, a more diverse research portfolio that includes all levels of experience, from novice to veteran, is needed to better understand this phenomenon.
As this review demonstrates, there is still much disagreement on the impact of workers’ demographic factors, specifically experience, on performance outcomes. Therefore, more recent and even self-reported data that offer the ability to study a variety of experience factors, as well as to determine accurate occurrences of incidents, are necessary to uncover any trends in potential indicators of incidents, if they exist. To that end, the National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) researchers analyzed several quantitative datasets that were collected by working with miners who worked in either underground coal, surface stone, sand, and gravel, or metal/non-metal to reveal any underlying trends among differing levels of experience. These case studies include several sectors of the mining industry. The mining industry remains an important part of economies both domestically and internationally. Regardless of the sector (e.g., coal, metals, minerals, etc.) and whether materials are extracted on the surface or underground, the working conditions are consistently acknowledged as physically demanding, and workers are exposed to numerous risks [
16]. In 2007, National Public Radio [
17] interviewed mineworkers to better understand their job and work environment, and a common theme among employees was that the work is “dark, dirty, and dangerous.” Due to the numerous hazards present both underground and on the surface, research to understand ways to improve the H&S workers through protective technologies and evolving risk management processes remains important.
Mineworkers completed respective surveys over a two-year period between 2016 and 2018. These data, collected in three separate case studies, used time in job, time at the mine, and time in the mining industry as predictor variables in an attempt to find associations with workers’ perceived knowledge, confidence, and job compliance. These specific case studies provide results that show the necessity of understanding how experience, on multiple levels, can impact worker perceptions and performance in order to improve hazard recognition, compliance to safety rules, and if necessary, rapid response in the event of an emergency. Although the results in this study are specific to mining, the trends are still relevant to any high-risk industry to help develop and implement aspects of a tailored risk management program to segments of the workforce at opportune time periods. Whether focusing on the development and implementation of more accurate hazard recognition and training, adequate preparation and effective response during a mine emergency, or improved safety climate through targeted leadership and engagement in H&S, all are efforts that can benefit from more information about the workforce at varying points in their careers.
2. Applied Research Methodology
Prior to the start of each respective data collection, NIOSH researchers obtained Institutional Review Board (IRB) and Office of Management and Budget (OMB) approval. In total, 3428 surveys were completed with mineworkers over a two-year period. These data, collected in three separate case studies, use time in job, time at the mine, and industry tenure as predictor variables in an attempt to find associations between workers’ varying experience and performance outcomes of interest (i.e., hazard recognition, self-escape confidence, and compliant behavior). Each of these case studies are presented in turn and utilize similar analysis methods to convey the results related to experience. Regarding how experience was measured within each case study, the categories measured were under one year, 1–5 years, 6–10 years, 11–15 years, 16–20 years, and more than 20 years. These categories are consistent with the demographic breakdown included in NIOSH’s Quality of Worklife Questionnaire and General Social Survey. These surveys have generated reports since 1972, exhibiting reliability and consistency in demographic breakdowns. Additionally, MSHA asserts that a novice worker has under one year of experience, further supporting our justification for the inexperienced group.
Prior to running analyses within each case study, an assessment of the normality of the data was completed using either a visual inspection (i.e., Case Study 1) or the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (i.e., Case Studies 2 and 3). For Case Studies 2 and 3, the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test rendered a result of less than 0.05, showing a deviation from normal distribution. This is normal and even common for large datasets [
18]. In response, a non-parametric option, the Kruskall-Wallis, was used as an alternative to a one-way between groups ANOVA for all three case studies. Similarly, in order to understand exactly where each significant relationship existed, Mann-Whitney tests were completed for each group to further investigate differences between the various experience groups and outcomes of interest. To better account for a potentially inflated type I error, due to multiple testing, the researchers adopted a more stringent significance value of 0.01 as an alternative to Bonferonni tests for each potential relationship. Further, we report the skewness and Shapiro-Wilks test of normality to demonstrate the non-normal distribution of the results, further justifying the use of nonparametric tests.
2.1. Case Study 1: Time on the Job and Hazard Recognition Accuracy
The first case study reflects on whether mineworkers’ time in their current position affects their ability to accurately recognize worksite hazards. Hazard recognition is fundamental to every safety activity, and hazards that go unrecognized and unmanaged can potentially result in catastrophic accidents and injuries [
19]. This is especially true for the mining industry because the environment is dynamic and often unpredictable, and mineworkers perform a variety of tasks in close proximity to heavy machinery [
20]. Despite the importance of hazard recognition, recent research indicates that a large proportion of hazards go undetected by mineworkers [
1,
21].
While previous research has mainly focused on determining the effect of overall tenure within an industry on hazard recognition ability [
1,
22], other types of experience are also critical. As an example, the specific job a worker does at a mine site has been shown to affect the number of hazards found during a hazard identification task, with exploration workers finding a greater number of hazards than those who perform maintenance jobs [
21]. This difference is presumably because workers in an exploration role are exposed to more and varied areas at the mine. An alternative way to capture the impact of this type of experience is to look at the effect of time in current position on hazard recognition ability. As an industry, mining has experienced periods of turnover, because of retirement or layoffs, and as a consequence, the overall industry demographics have changed (e.g., [
5,
23]). At the mine site level, this has caused workers to change job positions. Therefore, the purpose of this case study was to identify the influence of time in current position on hazard recognition ability.
2.1.1. Case Study 1 Materials and Methods
After researchers obtained IRB and OMB approval, study participants were recruited to travel to the NIOSH Pittsburgh Mining Research Division in Bruceton, PA, where the study took place at the facility’s Virtual Immersion and Simulation Laboratory (hereafter referred to as the VISLab).
VISLab and Panormic Images for Hazard Identification. The VISLab contained a 360° panoramic projection screen that measured 10 m in diameter by 3 m tall. Imagery was front-projected onto this screen from six high-definition projectors to create a seamless image. For the purposes of this study, the VISLab was equipped with 10 motion-tracking cameras and used in conjunction with eye-tracking glasses to record participant movement and resolve their point of regard within the display space, and study image sequencing was controlled by an in-house application. Research materials for the laboratory case study included 32 panoramic images of four locations typically found at any surface stone operation: pit, plant, roadway, and shop. These are all plant location areas that are included in the planning processes of controlling major hazards at surface operations, followed by identifying material unwanted events (MUEs), which then leads to the critical control management process [
24].
Eight panoramic images were taken at each of the four mine-specific locations. Six images for each of the four locations were experimental images. These 24 experimental images contained hazards that were staged by the researchers based on common citations and incidents recorded in MSHA’s 2015 database [
25]. Two images for each of the four locations, totaling eight images, were control images that contained no hazards. The number of hazards per experimental image ranged from two to seven, totaling 101 hazards among the 24 images. The overall breakdown of the hazards was 19 in the pit, 25 at the plant, 26 on the roadways, and 31 in the shop. Hazards varied by type, size, location, and risk. Researchers did not focus specifically on one type of hazard (e.g., slips, trips, or falls, or electrical). Instead, we included hazards that were appropriate given the context and location. Choosing to create scenes this way means that some hazard types included several representations, while other hazard types included one or two representations. This approach makes it difficult to compare hazard groups, but that was not the primary goal. The goal was to create panoramic images that were realistic and accurately reflected hazards and situations that mineworkers encounter during their workday. Other publications provide a more detailed explanation of the panoramic images and the hazards within them (e.g., [
1,
26]).
Participants. Participants consisted of mine H&S professionals, mineworkers, and students enrolled in a mining engineering program. All mine H&S professionals and mineworkers had experience with surface stone, sand, and gravel mining operations and procedures and reported having completed at least 24 h of MSHA New Miner Training and those with more than one year of mining experience reported having completed the additional eight hours of MSHA Annual Refresher Training as necessary. These trainings are mandated per MSHA’s Code of Federal Regulations (30 CFR Part 46) [
27].
Fifty-two participants volunteered to take part in the study. However, three participants were excluded from the analyses because of technical difficulties; therefore, the final dataset included 49 participants. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision and were screened to verify their visual abilities. None of these participants received payment for their participation, although some were able to participate during a normal workday with permission from their supervisors. Of the 49 participants, 19 (39%) were 18–24 years old; 12 (24%) were 25–34 years old; 6 (12%) were 35–44 years old; 9 (18%) were 45–54 years old; and 3 (6%) were 55–64 years old. Participants were also asked to report time in their current job and total time in the mining industry. Because of the small sample size, the participants initially included in the 16–20 year job experience group were combined with the 11–15 year group because their time in position was similar and provided the opportunity for enhanced statistical analyses (see
Table 1).
Procedure. After researchers obtained informed consent, participants were outfitted with eye-tracking glasses that were connected to a small laptop placed in a backpack, also worn by the participant. Participants were given a handheld joystick in their dominant hand that was connected to wireless data streaming hardware that was placed in the backpack. The eye-tracking glasses had passive motion-tracking markers to track head position, and several additional markers were placed on the participant’s torso to resolve head motion relative to the body frame. A series of calibration tests for the motion-tracking system and eye-tracking glasses was conducted in the simulator to ensure that the data collection software was accurately capturing the participant’s gaze within the screen space. Once the data collection instruments were calibrated, researchers presented two panoramic images to the participants to allow them to familiarize themselves with the 360° simulator and button press control.
Once acclimated, participants were presented with the 32 images in four sets of eight grouped by location category (pit, plant, roadway, and shop). Blocks were randomized across participant categories, and images were randomized within each location category. Participants were given up to two minutes to view each image and were instructed to press the joystick button as quickly as possible when they identified a hazard. If they decided they had identified all the hazards in an image, participants could press a second button on the hand grip to end that trial early. Once all images were complete, the glasses, markers, and backpack were removed.
2.1.2. Case Study 1 Results
The Kruskal-Wallis Test was applied to the data to determine the effect of time in position on hazard recognition accuracy. The raw averages are presented in
Table 1, while
Figure 1 visually shows the difference in hazard recognition accuracy and job experience. As the results visually show, the hazard recognition accuracy was lowest for those with under one year of experience in their current job position. Recognition accuracy gradually increased until workers exceeded 20 years in their same job role; at this point, the recognition accuracy dropped significantly.
The Kruskal-Wallis Test revealed a significant difference in hazard recognition accuracy scores across the experience groups, χ2 (5,
n = 49) = 11.832,
p = 0.01. To further identify where these relationships existed, Mann-Whitney tests were completed between each of the experience groups. Of the possible comparisons, one was statistically significant, using 0.01 as a strict significance value to account for possible inflated Type I errors (see
Appendix A for significant relationship). The Mann-Whitney tests indicated that the group with less than one year of experience in their current position found significantly fewer hazards than all other groups in their current position. Obviously, a majority of the participants are in the low experience amount groups. Even though the distribution is uneven, the results are consistent with previous research which shows that increasing experience improves hazard recognition (highlighted in the discussion section of this paper).
2.2. Case Study 2: Time at Current Mine and Confidence in Risk Response
The second case study reflects on underground coal miners’ tenure at their current mine and applies this variable as a potential indicator of mineworkers’ self-escape competence. Because mine disasters are high-severity, low-probability events, there are limited data related to post-disaster survival of underground miners. Although these events are fortunately rare, preparedness deficiencies often come to light when they occur. Even if a miner is fortunate enough to survive an initial catastrophic event (e.g., explosion, gas or water inundation, roof fall), he or she may still be required to successfully execute critical physical and cognitive tasks to escape from the mine unaided.
Limited research into such circumstances suggests that a lack of competence in the non-routine tasks required of mineworkers during emergency situations can have tragic consequences (e.g., [
28,
29,
30]). While significant efforts to improve mine emergency responses have been made in the last decade, it is impossible to know with any certainty whether they have been effective or whether active underground coal miners possess the knowledge and skills required for successful self-escape. Therefore, after researchers identified critical tasks and required skills through detailed task analyses (see [
31,
32]), the question of mineworker competence was addressed.
2.2.1. Case Study 2 Materials and Methods
Because it is difficult and dangerous to simulate the dynamic conditions of an actual mine emergency and because standard self-escape competency and assessment protocols are yet to be developed, it was necessary for researchers to frame the questions in a way that gaps in competence could most readily be identified and quantified. A large body of research suggests that when competence is difficult or impossible to measure, self-reported confidence in one’s ability to perform a task can serve as a reliable “proxy” measurement of competence [
33,
34].
Self-escape Competence Survey. This survey, administered by NIOSH, consisted of questions that measured mineworker self-escape competence. To identify gaps in the critical self-escape knowledge and skills among mineworkers, 28 critical self-escape tasks that were identified in previous NIOSH research and preliminary task analysis results [
31,
32] were phrased into confidence questions, which were subsequently reviewed by mine emergency response subject matter experts for content validity. The 28 items were deemed to be skills that all miners, regardless of the self-escape role, should be able to confidently demonstrate or explain in the event of a mine emergency [
35]. Participating mineworkers were asked, “On a scale of 0–100%, how confident are you that you could correctly demonstrate or explain the following to a brand new miner?”
Table 2 lists the items that participants responded to using an 11-point scale that ranged from not at all confident to extremely confident [
36]. When doing an internal reliability test of these 28 variables, the Cronbach’s
α = 0.96, demonstrating high internal consistency. Additionally, the inter-item correlations are strong, which further justifies using average confidence as a summary variable in this case study. Questions also captured demographic data including age, time in mining, time in job, and time in current mine, and responses to other background questions related to leadership experience, specialized training, and emergency response experience.
Procedure. Upon receiving IRB and OMB approval, NIOSH researchers invited underground coal mine operations across the United States to participate in the survey using a variety of convenience and purposive sampling methods [
37]. Although the hope was to visit mines in all geographic regions, interest in the survey came primarily from mine operators in the Eastern U.S. and, as a result, all data were collected in the Appalachian Region.
Participants. Researchers traveled to the eight mines and administered a paper-pencil survey. Potential participants were instructed that the survey was not only voluntary, but also completely confidential and that their responses would not be shared with their supervisors, except in aggregate form. On average, the survey took about 10 min to complete. Eight mines participated in this data collection effort in 2016 and 2017, with 696 hourly workers completing the survey (note, salaried workers also participated, but are not included in this analysis because they are not likely to have to lead or be able to escape during an underground emergency). Of the 696 hourly participants at the eight mines, the range of participants per mine was 16 to 213 (M = 87).
2.2.2. Case Study 2 Results
For this analysis, the main outcome variable of interest was self-escape confidence. This variable is an average of each individual respondent’s self-reported confidence on each of the 28 self-escape competencies included on the survey. Because the variable is highly skewed (skewness = −1.81, standard error = 0.10) and not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilks statistic = 0.84,
p = 0.000), researchers used nonparametric tests to measure associations between self-escape confidence and experience. The self-escape confidence median and range within each experience group are shown in
Table 3.
Figure 2 plots the distribution of the self-escape confidence variable by level of experience.
As illustrated by
Figure 1, the self-escape confidence variable is highly skewed to the left for each level of worker experience, with self-reported confidence more likely to be on the higher end of the spectrum.
Figure 2 also highlights the presence of outliers, which might allude to a more nuanced association between worker experience and self-escape confidence that might be clearer when taking other individual factors into account (e.g., age, education, workgroup or experience in emergency response, etc.).
The Kruskal-Wallis Tests for each of the 28 competence questions revealed that six were statistically significant across the experience groups.
Table 4 shows these items and their respective results. In order to understand where exactly these significant relationships existed, Mann-Whitney tests were completed between each group for each item to further investigate differences between experience groups. In completing these tests for each possible comparison group, 32 were statistically significant, using 0.01 as a strict significance value to account for possible inflated Type I errors (see
Appendix B).
These results indicate that time at one’s current mine can significantly impact workers’ self-reported confidence on several self-escape competency items. Also of interest is that almost all of the items that rendered statistical significance were mine-specific competencies, such as where to find self-escape self-rescuers (SCSR), how to read a mine map, and knowing the mine’s emergency response plan. Alternatively, some of the items that did not render statistical significance were more mining-specific rather than site-specific, such as how to don (or deploy) an SCSR, how to use nonverbal communication, or how to identify the explosive atmosphere with a gas meter. Additionally, these items of significance are often covered in mine annual refresher trainings.
The trend in these results was somewhat unexpected. Specifically, the results showed that self-confidence was, in most cases, stronger among those workers who were newer to the mine site. Then, worker confidence tends to decrease over time, often hitting the lowest point around 16–20 years. Then, the confidence for those who were employed on site for more than 20 years often started to increase again. These initial results speak to the potential impact of site-specific training for new mineworkers. Specifically, most mineworkers must go through site-specific training upon starting at a new jobsite. These results lend themselves to supporting the initial value of any on-the-job training, communication, or experience that mineworkers gain early on at the mine. However, the results also illustrate that some of these knowledge and skill-building efforts may wane over time as workers’ confidence levels in performing these critical self-escape activities decrease.
2.3. Case Study 3: Time in the Mining Industry, Safety Compliance, and Risk Avoidance
The last case study examines time in the mining industry and workers’ perceived compliance to following rules and avoiding risks. In this particular case, a survey dataset was used that was populated by NIOSH researchers who administered a safety climate survey to the industry. Safety climate has been linked to many safety-related outcomes [
38,
39,
40]. However, as Clarke [
38] and others have pointed out, the links have not been consistent across all safety climate research. In response to overarching questions about safety climate and worker performance, NIOSH developed a safety climate survey for the mining industry. Through an extensive literature review of safety climate assessments in other high-risk occupational industries, perception-based organizational value and characteristic constructs were identified and presumed to be important in fostering H&S knowledge, motivation, behaviors, and outcomes.
2.3.1. Case Study 3 Materials and Methods
As a part of the safety climate assessment completed by participants, compliance was one factor, or scale, contained within the survey, as well as risk avoidance. These data were used to answer whether there is a difference in compliance and risk avoidance across experience, or tenure, in the mining industry.
Compliance Survey Scale. Safety compliance is related to workers’ participation in safety-related activities, including the completion of work in a safe manner [
41]. More specifically, Griffin and Neal [
42] contend that safety compliance is a function of knowledge, skills, and motivation to comply with safety policies and processes. A safety compliance scale was adapted from Neal and colleagues [
43] and Zachataros and colleagues [
44] to measure compliance with safety procedures. The original scale had a Cronbach’s
α = 0.94. In the current survey, the scale was adapted to a four-item measure which workers were asked to complete using a six-point Likert scale (strongly disagree to strongly agree), with six being the highest value, indicating a high level of compliance. Our shortened, four-scale version had Cronbach’s
α = 0.85, demonstrating high internal reliability [
44,
45]. The four questions were prefaced with “When I’m at work I…” and were phrased as follows:
don’t take risks that could result in an accident;
use all necessary H/S equipment to do my job;
use the correct H/S procedures for carrying out my job;
always report all health/safety-related incidents.
Risk Avoidance Survey Scale. Measuring risk avoidance can help predict the types of at-risk behaviors in which workers are willing to participate [
46] and as a result are used to measure an individual’s general tendency to take risks and general avoidance of risks on site [
47,
48]. Researchers adapted items from Meertens and Lion’s [
48] risk propensity scale. The original scale contained nine items to tap into difference aspects of risk-taking and yielded a Cronbach’s
α = 0.80. We adapted four of these items and used a six-point Likert scale (strongly disagree to strongly agree), with six being the highest value, indicating a high avoidance of risks. Within the current sample, these questions rendered a Cronbach’s
α = 0.72, which is an acceptable level of internal consistency [
44,
45]. Participants were prompted with, “As far as day to day work…”, and then answered the following items:
Procedure. After IRB and OMB approval, the survey was validated [
49] and data collection occurred between February 2016 and March 2018. Upon contacting or being contacted by a corporate H&S leader, mine operator, or H&S manager and explaining the study, a mutually agreed-upon time was chosen to travel to the mine and administer the survey. If upcoming MSHA annual refresher training was scheduled, researchers often visited the mine that day in order to have everyone together at one time. If upcoming annual refresher or other training was not on the mine’s schedule in the near future, researchers worked with the mine to pick one or two days that were convenient to attend pre-shift safety meetings to collect the survey data.
Prior to participating, mine management and hourly workers were briefed about the purpose of the survey, that their participation was voluntary, that their responses would be anonymous, and that their answers would not be seen by their supervisors. To our knowledge, no one refused to participate and it took approximately 15 min for participants to complete the survey. Researchers collected the hard copy surveys and subsequently, they were entered into an SPSS file for cleaning and analysis.
Participants. Participants consisted of 2683 mineworkers—both salaried and hourly—at 39 mine sites. Three of these mines were in Canada and the remaining 36 were dispersed throughout 17 states in the United States. The 39 mines represented nine major companies and three mined sectors (i.e., coal, stone, sand, and gravel, and industrial minerals). To our knowledge, everyone who was present on site during data collection completed the survey. The breakdown of participation by sector was stone, sand, and gravel (n = 1418, 53%); industrial minerals (n = 907, 34%); and coal (n = 358, 13%). The range of participants at each mine was 7–280 (M = 69). Additionally, n = 569 (22%) of the participants were salaried workers and n = 2020 (78%) were hourly workers.
2.3.2. Case Study 3 Results
For this analysis, there were two main outcome variables of interest: safety compliance and risk avoidance. Because these variables are highly skewed (skewness = −1.63, standard error = 0.048 and skewness = 1.38, standard error = 0.048, respectively) and not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk statistic = 0.82, p = 0.000 and 0.85, p = 0.000, respectively), researchers used nonparametric tests to measure associations between the outcome variables and industry experience.
The median and range of participants’ responses on the compliance scale and risk avoidance scale within each experience group are presented in
Table 5, while
Figure 2 plots these averages on a line graph for a visual comparison of the patterns by experience groups.
A Kruskal-Wallis Test revealed a statistically significant difference in compliance across the six different experience groups, also shown in
Table 6 (Under 1 year,
n = 234; 1–5 years,
n = 465; 6–10 years,
n = 441; 11–15 years,
n = 389; 16–20 years,
n = 239; 20+ years,
n = 761), χ2 (5,
n = 2529) = 32.002,
p < 0.000. The less and more experienced groups recorded higher mean rank scores than the middle-range experience groups, particularly in the 6–10, 11–15, and 16–20 year ranges. A trend toward significant differences in risk avoidance across the six different experience groups was rendered (Under 1 year,
n = 239; 1–5 years,
n = 468; 6–10 years,
n = 444; 11–15 years,
n = 395; 16–20 years,
n = 245; 20+ years,
n = 770), χ2 (5,
n = 2561) = 10.551,
p = 0.061), but does not meet the significance requirements established for this study, so detailed results are not shown.
These results indicate that experience in the mining industry does significantly impact workers’ compliant behaviors on the job. In completing these tests for each possible comparison group, several were statistically significant for compliance (see
Appendix C for results). Because risk avoidance was not statistically significant, Mann-Whitney tests were not completed for these between-group relationships.
The Mann-Whitney tests show that the group with under one year of mining experience had significantly higher levels of compliance compared to each experience comparison group. In addition, the group that had 1–5 years of mining experience had significantly higher levels of compliance than those with 6–10 and 16–20 years of experience. However, the experience group with more than 20 years in the industry did have higher levels of compliance than one experience group—the 6–10 year group. Most noticeably and notably within these results is that the group with under one year of experience in the mining industry perceives that they follow the rules, report incidents, and use the correct procedures to complete their jobs.
4. Conclusions and Limitations
It is an obvious limitation that we are drawing upon patterns using three varying datasets that are all based on behavioral lab or self-reported data. If researchers were able to coordinate data collections and specific instruments to a more rigorous degree, stronger relationships and results may have emerged. For example, being able to track the same groups of people to compare how they perceive and process risks that stem from routine and non-routine work practices, would be an area of future exploration that could occur with a common sample. However, the results for each separate case study all support findings in previous studies, supporting the validity of all three studies and their potential influence on future organizational research. In our case, synthesizing the results of the three studies together allowed for some sort of triangulation, particularly among the second and third case studies, which provide support for increased communication and on-the-job experience and self-assessments that can support the ongoing development and maintenance of procedural knowledge. However, future research should aim to better coordinate big data collection efforts with overlapping populations in order to draw more cause-effect results. In addition, such data should be linked to lagging indicator data [
67] to better support the conclusions made throughout this paper. The current case studies did not collect accident or fatality data so these concrete links cannot be made.
Despite all the limitations, this paper was able to contribute to the occupational H&S literature in several ways. First, we were able to show that organizations need to consider worker factors other than age and blanket experience as possible predictors of worker performance. The main takeaway is that even if longer job tenure is associated with safer job performance, as shown in Case Study 1, this may not be the case as workers’ tenure increases at the mine site and within the industry as an overall career path. More specifically, our results demonstrated that communication with more intermediate to veteran workers (i.e., 6–20 years) is necessary to maintain worker engagement and execution of the organization’s health, safety, and risk practices.
In summary, these three case studies show the value of being able to identify various levels of workers’ experience, and that it is important for an organization to continually reengage with workers about specific safety issues relevant to their job tasks, at their mine, and within their mining sector. In doing so, managers may be able to parse out important differences in their workforce’s knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors based on varying levels of experience, in order to reduce negative consequences for managers and their organization while improving the knowledge, motivation, and overall resources for the workforce.