Next Article in Journal
Mordellidae (Coleoptera) Research: A Review Based on the Zoological Record from 1864 through 2013
Previous Article in Journal
Quiescence in Aedes aegypti: Interpopulation Differences Contribute to Population Dynamics and Vectorial Capacity
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Previous Interspecific Courtship Impairs Female Receptivity to Conspecifics in the Parasitoid Wasp Nasonia longicornis But Not in N. vitripennis

Institute of Zoology, University of Regensburg, Universitätsstraße 31, D-93053 Regensburg, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Insects 2018, 9(3), 112; https://doi.org/10.3390/insects9030112
Submission received: 27 July 2018 / Revised: 18 August 2018 / Accepted: 22 August 2018 / Published: 1 September 2018

Abstract

:
Interspecific sexual interactions are not uncommon in animals. In sympatry, females often face the risk of accidentally mating with a heterospecific male. Based on the actual risks imposed by the environment at a given time and place, females should be able to adjust their mate acceptance in order to avoid interspecific copulations as well as accidentally refusing to mate with a conspecific. We investigate the ability of females of the two parasitoid wasp species Nasonia vitripennis (Nv) and N. longicornis (Nl) to adjust their mate acceptance in response to previous unsuccessful courtship by heterospecific males. We show that Nl females are more reluctant to mate with a conspecific male when having been courted previously by a heterospecific male, but Nv females are not. We argue that this strategy is reasonable for Nl females but not for Nv females, which follow a different strategy to avoid the fitness costs imposed by heterospecific copulations.

1. Introduction

Reproductive interference is common among a wide variety of animal taxa [1]. Due to incomplete species recognition during mate acquisition, signals sometimes attract individuals of the wrong species [2,3], courtship is directed towards the wrong mating partners [4], males try to copulate with heterospecific females [5], and females occasionally become receptive to heterospecific males [6]. To avoid the fitness costs arising from interspecific copulations, the ability to discriminate between conspecific and heterospecific courtship partners usually evolves in the choosing sex (usually females) [7]. During mate acquisition, a female assesses the species of the courting male, and refrains from copulation if the male does not belong to the same species. Mate discrimination acts as an important prezygotic hybridisation barrier [8,9], and is particularly important in species in which females mate only once during their lifetime and post-mating reproductive isolation is complete [10]. In insects, mate discrimination usually involves chemical messengers [11], but it frequently includes additional signals, e.g., specific courtship displays, acoustic signals, or wing vibration patterns [12]. Closely related species often resemble each other in their courtship display, and mate discrimination is not absolutely accurate [13,14,15]. Females face both the risk of accidentally mating a heterospecific male and the risk of accidentally rejecting a conspecific. A trade-off thus arises between either becoming more selective in order to prevent interspecific copulations or broaden the range of stimuli that elicit receptivity in order to avoid accidentally rejecting a conspecific. Females should minimise both of these risks by adjusting their behaviour depending on the actual risks and costs imposed by the environment at a given time and place. If the chance of meeting and being courted by a heterospecific partner is low, e.g., in areas of allopatry, the costs of rejecting a conspecific become comparably large, and the range of partners accepted for copulation should be broadened. In contrast, in sympatry, the chance of being courted by a heterospecific male increases with the increasing population density of the interfering species, and interspecific copulations become more likely. In these situations, it is advantageous for females to become more selective by either establishing more accurate discrimination abilities or accepting the accidental rejection of a conspecific in order to avoid the much higher costs of consenting to interspecific copulation. It has been shown in various animal taxa that when animals are reared in sympatry with an interfering species, mate discrimination abilities can become more accurate through learning by experience either early in their life (e.g., sexual imprinting) or at later stages of their lives (e.g., contextual behavioural plasticity) [16,17,18]. One means by which females may learn about the risks of interspecific mating in a specific environment is the experience of being courted by a heterospecific male. However, nothing is known to date about the direct effects of heterospecific courtship experience on future female mate acceptance.
Nasonia vitripennis (Nv) and N. longicornis (Nl) are two species of parasitoid wasps that parasitise the pupae of cyclorrhaphous flies. In the western part of North America, Nv and Nl occur frequently in microsympatry, i.e., they develop within the same host individual [19,20,21,22]. As fly pupae often occur in a clumped distribution, and Nasonia wasps are gregarious, i.e., they lay more than one egg per host individual, males and females of Nv and Nl likely encounter after emergence at the same host patch. Courtship and copulation in Nasonia happen typically at the natal host patch, and females leave after mating to search for new oviposition sites [23,24,25]. In laboratory studies, males readily engage in courtship with females of the other species [25,26,27,28]. Males exhibit an elaborate courtship display, including specific movements of their forelegs over the female’s head accompanied by series of head-nodding movements during which a sex pheromone is transferred from the male’s oral glands to the female’s antennae [29,30]. The female shows receptivity by lowering her antennae and opening the genital orifice, and copulation follows. Although following the same general pattern, male courtship displays differ in detail between the different Nasonia species [29,31,32,33]. Females are able to discriminate between conspecific and heterospecific mating partners, but mistakes in mate discrimination occur [25,26,27,28,34]. In behavioural bioassays, Nv females usually discriminate more strongly against Nl males (more than 70% rejection of heterospecific males) than vice versa (less than 40% rejection of heterospecific males), and mate discrimination differs among different Nasonia strains [26,27]. However, differences in mate discrimination among Nv strains seem to be independent from the origin of these strains from areas of sympatry or allopatry with other Nasonia species [27]. Nv and Nl show complete postzygotic reproductive isolation due to Wolbachia-mediated cytoplasmic incompatibility, preventing the production of hybrid females [35]. Similar to most Hymenoptera, Nasonia are haplodiploid, and eggs fertilised by heterospecific sperm either die or develop into male offspring, similar to unfertilised eggs [35,36]. In addition, Nasonia females mate only once during their lifetime in nature [24]. Females having copulated with a conspecific male usually refrain from mating again [37]. Females consenting to interspecific copulation thus face particularly high fitness costs [10]. Nv females counteract these costs by increased remating with a conspecific male after having copulated with a heterospecific [25]. However, this effect has not been shown for Nl. Considering the fitness costs imposed on the females of the less discriminating species, Nl, it would be advantageous for them to adjust their mate acceptance behaviour depending on the actual presence or absence of heterospecific males.
Here, we investigate the impact of previous unsuccessful heterospecific courtship on the females’ acceptance of conspecific mates in Nv and Nl. We hypothesize that females of Nl use the experience of heterospecific courtship to adjust their mate acceptance behaviour. In particular, we hypothesize that Nl females that have been unsuccessfully courted by a heterospecific male are subsequently more reluctant to mate in general in order to avoid accidentally copulating with the wrong male. We expect this reluctance to be reflected in both a decrease in conspecific mating rate and an increase of the duration of courtship necessary to induce receptivity in couples where copulation happens. For Nv females, we hypothesize that if they adjust their mating rates, they do so to a lesser degree because, firstly, Nv females show stronger mate discrimination in general, and secondly, they counteract costs of heterospecific matings by increased remating with a conspecific. We address these hypotheses by performing mating trials with Nv and Nl females without prior contact to any male, and with females that have been courted previously by a heterospecific male.
We found that Nl females, but not those of Nv, decreased conspecific mate acceptance after having been courted by a heterospecific male. We argue that this strategy is advantageous for Nl females to avoid future mismating and that the difference in behavioural plasticity between the two Nasonia species is reasonable, considering that Nv females, as shown in earlier studies, follow a different behavioural strategy to counteract the costs imposed by interspecific copulation through increased remating.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Strains, Rearing, and Preparation of Wasps

Experiments were performed with the Nv strain NvHVRx [38] and the Nl strain NLMN8510*. Wasps were reared on freeze-killed pupae of the green bottle fly Lucilia caesar at 25 °C under a 16:8 light:dark regime. For behavioural bioassays, wasps were isolated from host puparia at their pupal stage, separated, and kept singly in 1.5-mL microcentrifuge tubes until being used in experiments. By isolating wasps at this developmental stage, it was ensured that adult wasps were unmated and had not had any direct contact with other adult wasps when the experiments began. Each day in the morning, isolated wasps were checked for emergences. Emerged wasps were sexed and defined as zero days old. For bioassays, zero-day-old females and one to three-day-old males were used.

2.2. Behavioural Bioassays

Mating trials were conducted in a standard mating arena consisting of a round hole (10 mm diameter, 3 mm height) cut into an acrylic glass plate and covered by a cover slip (for a detailed description of the arena, see Ruther et al. in 2000 [39]). In each mating trial, a female and subsequently a male were put into the arena. The arena was closed with a cover slip, and the couple was observed for five minutes. The females of each species were subjected to one of two treatments: (1) Females were tested with conspecific males without prior contact to any other individual. (2) Females were exposed to a heterospecific male for five minutes, and were eventually courted by the male. If the female did not show receptivity to the heterospecific male, the heterospecific male was removed, and the female was subsequently exposed in a second mating trial to a conspecific male. For each conspecific couple, it was noted whether the female consented to mating or not. If copulation happened, the duration of preceding courtship, i.e., the time span between the male mounting the female and the female’s receptivity signal (opening the genital orifice), was recorded.
Trials in which males (conspecific or heterospecific) did not engage in courtship were discarded. Trials in which the female showed receptivity to the heterospecific male were excluded from further treatment, but were noted in order to assess whether the exclusion of these females led to a bias in the experimental females. Excluding females that consented to interspecific mating could have potentially resulted in testing only those females in treatment two that were in general more reluctant to mate. However, only three Nv females (3.6%) and four females of Nl (4.8%) copulated with heterospecific males. Thus, it is unlikely that excluding these females resulted in a significant bias among the experimental groups.
Some Nv males exhibited aggressive behaviours towards Nl females. For each trial with Nl females, it was therefore noted whether the female was treated aggressively or not. Females were defined as having been treated aggressively when males, after having started courtship, turned their wings into a vertical position, jumped towards the female, and repeatedly grabbed the female, occasionally involving injuries, i.e., tearing off parts of the female’s legs or antennae. In these occasions, females usually crouched down and tried to run away from the respective males.
Each individual was tested only once. The assignment of individuals to treatments was randomised, and the order of treatments followed a blocked design. In total, 80 replicates were performed for each of the four treatments (Nv courted previously, Nv without prior contact, Nl courted previously, and Nl without prior contact).

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Conspecific mate rejection rates were compared between females having been courted previously and those without prior contact to heterospecific males using a 2 × 2 Chi-square test for each of the species separately. Differences in the duration of courtship were compared between females having been courted previously and those without prior contact to heterospecific males with a Mann–Whitney U test. In addition, conspecific mate rejection rates and the duration of courtship were compared between Nl females that had been subjected to aggressive versus non-aggressive heterospecific contact with a 2 × 2 Chi-square test and a Mann–Whitney U test, respectively.

3. Results

3.1. Conspecific Mate Rejection

Nl females that have been courted by Nv males rejected conspecific males more often than females without prior heterospecific contact (Chi-square test: χ1 = 9.56, n = 80 each, p < 0.01; Figure 1A; all raw data are provided in Table S1). In Nv females, heterospecific courtship had no effect on subsequent conspecific mate rejection (Chi-square test: χ1 = 0.28, n = 80 each, p = 0.60: Figure 1B).

3.2. Duration of Courtship

Previously courted Nl females that subsequently consented to mating a conspecific male did so after prolonged courtship compared to females without prior heterospecific contact (Mann–Whitney U test: U = 2206.5, n = 53 (Nl courted) and 61 (Nl without prior contact), p < 0.001; Figure 1C). In Nv females, heterospecific courtship had no effect on the subsequent duration of courtship by conspecific males (Mann–Whitney U test: U = 2283, n = 69 (Nv courted) and 63 (Nv without prior contact), p = 0.62; Figure 1D).

3.3. Aggression

Aggression by heterospecific males had no effect on Nl female mate rejection (Chi-square test: χ1 = 0.24, N = 32 (aggressive contact) and 48 (non-aggressive contact), p = 0.62) or the duration of subsequent conspecific courtship (Mann–Whitney U test: U = 812, N = 21 (aggressive) and 32 (non-aggressive), p = 0.23).

4. Discussion

Nl females were more reluctant to mate a conspecific after having been courted by a heterospecific male. This was reflected in both an increased conspecific mate rejection rate and an increased duration of courtship needed to induce receptivity in couples in which copulation took place. This reluctance to mate a conspecific was not a result of the aggressive attacks exhibited by Nv males towards Nl females. Hence, it is likely that Nl females use previous contact with and/or courtship by heterospecific males to assess the future risk of mismating. Nl females alerted by previous heterospecific courtship adjust their general mating behaviour and show an increased rejection of even conspecific males in subsequent courtship encounters. However, whether the mere contact with a heterospecific male, e.g., antennal contact, or the exposure to heterospecific courtship induces this adjustment remains to be investigated.
Rejecting conspecific males more frequently and consenting to conspecific copulation only after prolonged courtship likely results in Nl females losing valuable time that could otherwise be spent on the location of new host patches. In addition, it potentially increases the risk of leaving the natal host patch unmated. However, several males usually emerge from a host patch, and unmated females are potentially courted by several males prior to dispersal. In addition, at least in Nv, females become restless and switch to host-seeking behaviour only after mating [23,25]. Consistently, unmated Nv females have been rarely found to oviposit on new host patches in nature [24].
In contrast to Nl females, conspecific mate acceptance in Nv females was not affected by prior heterospecific courtship. A possible reason for this difference between the two species is that Nv females follow a different strategy to counteract the costs arising from interspecific mating. Nv females that have copulated with a heterospecific (N. giraulti, Ng) male show an increased willingness to remate with a conspecific. By remating, Nv females are able to produce female offspring numbers that are similar to those found in females that mated only once with a conspecific [25]. However, whether females of Nl exhibit similar remating adjustments needs further investigation. Furthermore, the costs of heterospecific copulation imposed on females of the two species likely differ in another aspect. While cytoplasmic incompatibility induced by the infection with strains of Wolbachia leads to a conversion to male offspring in Nv, it predominantly leads to mortality in Nl [36]. As a result, Nl females that have copulated with a heterospecific male most likely suffer higher fitness costs than Nv females that consented to interspecific copulation.
Mate discrimination is a plastic behavioural trait in Nasonia. Females adjust their mate acceptance rate according to their internal state and previous experience: they become less choosy with age (Nv and Ng) [25,28], refrain from mating when they have already mated with a conspecific (Nv) [37], show increased remating frequencies when having mated with a heterospecific male before (Nv) [25], and as shown here, are able to adjust mate acceptance in response to having experienced courtship by heterospecific males (Nl).
Plasticity in mate discrimination in response to being held in the presence of heterospecific mating partners has been demonstrated in various animal taxa. Females of the silverleaf whitefly Bemisia tabaci increase conspecific mate acceptance when males of a different biotype are present in the environment [16]. Females and males of three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus spp.) become more discriminating against heterospecific partners when reared in mixed populations [17]. Males of the Trinidadian guppies Poecilia reticulata and P. picta as well as males of the fruit fly Drosophila persimilis learn to distinguish between conspecific and heterospecific females when held in mixed populations [40,41]. However, in contrast to Nl, individuals in these studies developed more accurate mate discrimination abilities in response to the presence of a reproductively interfering species. As females of Nl usually mate only once during their lifetime, a single mistake in mate choice is extremely costly for them, particularly if it is not, similar to in Nv, counteracted by increased remating after interspecific copulation. Instead of relying on the relatively slow process of learning by repeated experience, it may thus be more advantageous for Nl females to become more selective the moment that they recognize the presence of the interfering species. Recent studies indicate that the four Nasonia species differ in far more behavioural and ecological aspects than assumed so far, and that species interactions are far more complex than described to date [25,28,42,43,44,45,46]. In general, the potential role that the behavioural plasticity of mate discrimination can play in interactions and dynamics among co-occurring species has until now been widely neglected. The Nasonia model system offers fruitful opportunities for future investigations of plastic mate discrimination and species interactions on evolutionary, ecological, and behavioural levels, as well as on the level of underlying neurobiological and physiological processes.

5. Conclusions

After having been unsuccessfully courted by a heterospecific male, females of Nl are more reluctant to mate with a conspecific. In contrast, females of Nv do not change their mate acceptance behaviour following unsuccessful heterospecific courtship. This difference in the plasticity of mate acceptance between the two species is reasonable, because Nl females likely face higher fitness costs when consenting to interspecific copulation, and Nv females counteract the costs of mismating additionally through increased re-mating rates.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2075-4450/9/3/112/s1, Table S1: Raw data.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.R.; Methodology, M.M.M. and J.R.; Validation, M.M.M., J.R., and N.S.; Formal Analysis, M.M.M.; Investigation, N.S.; Writing-Original Draft Preparation, M.M.M.; Writing-Review & Editing, M.M.M. and J.R.; Visualization, M.M.M.; Funding Acquisition, J.R.

Funding

This research was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) grant number Ru-717-10/2.

Acknowledgments

We thank Melanie Schloßberger for rearing the wasps and B. Pannebakker for providing the wasp strains. Three anonymous reviewers gave helpful comments on the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Gröning, J.; Hochkirch, A. Reproductive interference between animal species. Q. Rev. Biol. 2008, 83, 257–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Groot, A.T.; Claßen, A.; Staudacher, H.; Schal, C.; Heckel, D.G. Phenotypic plasticity in sexual communication signal of a noctuid moth. J. Evol. Biol. 2010, 23, 2731–2738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Doherty, J.A.; Howard, D.J. Lack of preference for conspecific calling songs in female crickets. Anim. Behav. 1996, 51, 981–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Andrews, R.H.; Petney, T.N.; Bull, C.M. Reproductive interference between three parapatric species of reptile tick. Oecologia 1982, 52, 281–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Singer, F. Reproductive costs arising from incomplete habitat segregation among three species of Leucorrhinia dragonflies. Behaviour 1990, 115, 188–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Takafuji, A.; Kuno, E.; Fujimoto, H. Reproductive interference and its consequences for the competitive interactions between two closely related Panonychus spider mites. Exp. Appl. Acarol. 1997, 21, 379–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Wirtz, P. Mother species–father species: Unidirectional hybridization in animals with female choice. Anim. Behav. 1999, 58, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Doi, M.; Matsuda, M.; Tomaru, M.; Matsubayashi, H.; Oguma, Y. A locus for female discrimination behavior causing sexual isolation in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 6714–6719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Noor, M.A.F. Reinforcement and other consequences of sympatry. Heredity 1999, 83, 503–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Liou, L.W.; Price, T.D. Speciation by reinforcement of premating isolation. Evolution 1994, 48, 1451–1459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Wyatt, T.D. Pheromones and Animal Behavior, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2014; pp. 65–103. [Google Scholar]
  12. Talyn, B.C.; Dowse, H.B. The role of courtship song in sexual selection and species recognition by female Drosophila melanogaster. Anim. Behav. 2004, 68, 1165–1180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Boake, C.R.B.; Andreadis, D.K.; Witzel, A. Behavioural isolation between two closely related Hawaiian Drosophila species: The role of courtship. Anim. Behav. 2000, 60, 495–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Gray, D.A. Does courtship behavior contribute to species-level reproductive isolation in field crickets? Behav. Ecol. 2005, 16, 201–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Chaplin, S.J. Reproductive isolation between two sympatric species of Oncopeltus (Hemiptera: Lygaeidae) in the tropics. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 1973, 66, 997–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Crowder, D.W.; Sitvarin, M.I.; Carrière, Y. Plasticity in mating behaviour drives asymmetric reproductive interference in whiteflies. Anim. Behav. 2010, 79, 579–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Kozak, G.M.; Boughman, J.W. Learned conspecific mate preference in a species pair of sticklebacks. Behav. Ecol. 2009, 20, 1282–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Irwin, D.E.; Price, T. Sexual imprinting, learning and speciation. Heredity 1999, 82, 347–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Whiting, A.R. The biology of the parasitic wasp Mormoniella vitripennis [=Nasonia vitripennis] (Walker). Q. Rev. Biol. 1967, 42, 333–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Darling, D.; Werren, J.H. Biosystematics of Nasonia (Hymenoptera: Pteromalidae): Two new species reared from birds’ nests in North America. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 1990, 83, 352–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Grillenberger, B.K.; van de Zande, L.; Bijlsma, R.; Gadau, J.; Beukeboom, L.W. Reproductive strategies under multiparasitism in natural populations of the parasitoid wasp Nasonia (Hymenoptera). J. Evol. Biol. 2009, 22, 460–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Raychoudhury, R.; Grillenberger, B.K.; Gadau, J.; Bijlsma, R.; van de Zande, L.; Werren, J.H.; Beukeboom, L.W. Phylogeography of Nasonia vitripennis (Hymenoptera) indicates a mitochondrial-Wolbachia sweep in North America. Heredity 2010, 104, 318–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. King, B.H.; Grimm, K.; Reno, H. Effects of mating on female locomotor activity in the parasitoid wasp Nasonia vitripennis (Hymenoptera: Pteromalidae). Environ. Entomol. 2000, 29, 927–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Grillenberger, B.K.; Koevoets, T.; Burton-Chellew, M.N.; Sykes, E.M.; Shuker, D.M.; van de Zande, L.; Bijlsma, R.; Gadau, J.; Beukeboom, L.W. Genetic structure of natural Nasonia vitripennis populations: Validating assumptions of sex-ratio theory. Mol. Ecol. 2008, 17, 2854–2864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Ruther, J.; McCaw, J.; Böcher, L.; Pothmann, D.; Putz, I. Pheromone diversification and age-dependent behavioural plasticity decrease interspecific mating costs in Nasonia. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e89214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Buellesbach, J.; Greim, C.; Raychoudhury, R.; Schmitt, T. Asymmetric assortative mating behaviour reflects incomplete pre-zygotic isolation in the Nasonia species complex. Ethology 2014, 120, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Giesbers, M.C.W.G.; Gerritsma, S.; Buellesbach, J.; Diao, W.; Pannebakker, B.A.; van de Zande, L.; Schmitt, T.; Beukeboom, L.W. Prezygotic isolation in the parasitoid wasp genus Nasonia. In Speciation: Natural Processes, Genetics and Biodiversity; Pawel, M., Ed.; Nova Science Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 165–191. [Google Scholar]
  28. Mair, M.M.; Kmezic, V.; Huber, S.; Pannebakker, B.A.; Ruther, J. The chemical basis of mate recognition in two parasitoid wasp species of the genus Nasonia. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2017, 164, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Van den Assem, J.; Jachmann, F.; Simbolotti, P. Courtship behaviour of Nasonia vitripennis (Hym., Pteromalidae): Some qualitative, experimental evidence for the role of pheromones. Behaviour 1980, 75, 301–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ruther, J.; Thal, K.; Blaul, B.; Steiner, S. Behavioural switch in the sex pheromone response of Nasonia vitripennis females is linked to receptivity signalling. Anim. Behav. 2010, 80, 1035–1040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Van den Assem, J.; Werren, J.H. A comparison of the courtship and mating behavior of three species of Nasonia (Hymenoptera: Pteromalidae). J. Insect Behav. 1994, 7, 53–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Van den Assem, J.; Jachmann, F.; de Jong, K.A.G. Courtship behaviour of Nasonia vitripennis: Head nodding, mouth-part extrusion and pheromone discharge by abdomectomized males. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 1981, 30, 215–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Van den Assem, J.; Vernel, C. Courtship behaviour of Nasonia vitripennis (Hym.: Pteromalidae): Observations and experiments on male readiness to assume copulatory behaviour. Behaviour 1979, 68, 118–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Bordenstein, S.R.; Werren, J.H. Effects of A and B Wolbachia and host genotype on interspecies cytoplasmic incompatibility in Nasonia. Genetics 1998, 148, 1833–1844. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  35. Bordenstein, S.R.; O’Hara, F.P.; Werren, J.H. Wolbachia-induced incompatibility precedes other hybrid incompatibilities in Nasonia. Nature 2001, 409, 707–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Tram, U.; Fredrick, K.; Werren, J.H.; Sullivan, W. Paternal chromosome segregation during the first mitotic division determines Wolbachia-induced cytoplasmic incompatibility phenotype. J. Cell Sci. 2006, 119, 3655–3663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Van den Assem, J.; Visser, J. Aspects of sexual receptivity in female Nasonia vitripennis. Biol. Behav. 1976, 1, 37–56. [Google Scholar]
  38. Van de Zande, L.; Ferber, S.; de Haan, A.; Beukeboom, L.W.; van Heerwaarden, J.; Pannebakker, B.A. Development of a Nasonia vitripennis outbred laboratory population for genetic analysis. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 2014, 14, 578–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Ruther, J.; Homann, M.; Steidle, J.L.M. Female-derived sex pheromone mediates courtship behaviour in the parasitoid Lariophagus distinguendus. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2000, 96, 265–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Dukas, R. Learning decreases heterospecific courtship and mating in fruit flies. Biol. Lett. 2008, 4, 645–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Magurran, A.E.; Ramnarine, I.W. Learned mate recognition and reproductive isolation in guppies. Anim. Behav. 2003, 67, 1077–1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Giesbers, M.C.W.G.; Pannebakker, B.A.; van de Zande, L.; Beukeboom, L. Within-Host-Mating in the Nasonia Genus is Largely Dependent on Male Behaviour. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  43. Leonard, J.E.; Boake, C.R.B. Site-dependent aggression and mating behaviour in three species of Nasonia (Hymenoptera: Pteromalidae). Anim. Behav. 2006, 71, 641–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Leonard, J.E.; Boake, C.R.B. Associations between male courtship and female polyandry in three species of wasp, Nasonia (Hymenoptera: Pteromalidae). Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 637–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Niehuis, O.; Buellesbach, J.; Gibson, J.D.; Pothmann, D.; Hanner, C.; Mutti, N.S.; Judson, A.K.; Gadau, J.; Ruther, J.; Schmitt, T. Behavioural and genetic analyses of Nasonia shed light on the evolution of sex pheromones. Nature 2013, 494, 345–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Mair, M.M.; Ruther, J. Territoriality and behavioural strategies at the natal host patch differ in two microsympatric Nasonia species. Anim. Behav. 2018, 143, 113–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Females of Nasonia longicornis (A) rejected conspecific males more often and (C) consented to mating a conspecific male only after prolonged courtship when having been courted previously by a heterospecific male. In N. vitripennis females, heterospecific courtship had no effect on (B) female conspecific mate rejection, or (D) on the duration of courtship of the conspecific males. Boxplots display median (horizontal line within the box), lower and upper quartile (box margins), maximum/minimum range (whiskers; <1.5× above box height), and outliers (dots; ≥1.5× above box height). Asterisks indicate significant differences: Chi-square test (A,B) and Mann–Whitney U test (C,D): ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, NS: Not significant.
Figure 1. Females of Nasonia longicornis (A) rejected conspecific males more often and (C) consented to mating a conspecific male only after prolonged courtship when having been courted previously by a heterospecific male. In N. vitripennis females, heterospecific courtship had no effect on (B) female conspecific mate rejection, or (D) on the duration of courtship of the conspecific males. Boxplots display median (horizontal line within the box), lower and upper quartile (box margins), maximum/minimum range (whiskers; <1.5× above box height), and outliers (dots; ≥1.5× above box height). Asterisks indicate significant differences: Chi-square test (A,B) and Mann–Whitney U test (C,D): ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, NS: Not significant.
Insects 09 00112 g001

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mair, M.M.; Seifert, N.; Ruther, J. Previous Interspecific Courtship Impairs Female Receptivity to Conspecifics in the Parasitoid Wasp Nasonia longicornis But Not in N. vitripennis. Insects 2018, 9, 112. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects9030112

AMA Style

Mair MM, Seifert N, Ruther J. Previous Interspecific Courtship Impairs Female Receptivity to Conspecifics in the Parasitoid Wasp Nasonia longicornis But Not in N. vitripennis. Insects. 2018; 9(3):112. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects9030112

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mair, Magdalena M., Nicole Seifert, and Joachim Ruther. 2018. "Previous Interspecific Courtship Impairs Female Receptivity to Conspecifics in the Parasitoid Wasp Nasonia longicornis But Not in N. vitripennis" Insects 9, no. 3: 112. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects9030112

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop